Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Pediatr., 22 November 2024
Sec. Children and Health
This article is part of the Research Topic ADHD and Anxiety: Causality Sequences Through a Biopsychosocial Model View all articles

Association between reported ADHD symptom and motor development delay in preschool children

\r\nZhijun CuiZhijun Cui1Shijie LiShijie Li1Aimin LiangAimin Liang1Hongmei Huang
Hongmei Huang1*Xin Ni
\r\nXin Ni2*
  • 1Department of Child Health Care Center, Beijing Children's Hospital, Capital Medical University, Beijing, China
  • 2Department of Otolaryngology Head and Surgery, Beijing Children's Hospital, Capital Medical University, Beijing, China

Objective: To explore whether the motor developmental level is associated with the attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) symptoms severity reported by parents in preschool children.

Methods: Preschool children aged 4–6 years old with the chief complaint of reported inattention or hyperactivity by kindergarten teachers or parents were recruited in this study. All participants were consulted by at least one experienced developmental behavior pediatrician, according to DSM-V diagnostic criteria of ADHD. Their neuromotor developments were assessed by the Children's Neuropsychological and Behavior Scale and recorded as developmental quotient (DQ) score in gross motor, fine motor, and other domains. Regarding the evaluation of ADHD symptoms, parents of the 4-year-old group completed the Conners' Parent Symptom Questionnaire, while parents of the 5-year-old group completed The Vanderbilt ADHD Diagnostic Parent Rating Scale.

Results: A total of 137 preschool children aged 4–4.9 years (4-year-old group) and 252 were aged 5.0–5.9 years (5-year-old group) were included in the study. Children exhibiting ADHD symptoms were at a much higher risk of fine motor delays compared to gross motor delays, particularly among the younger age group. Correlation analysis and hierarchical regression showed that in the 4-year-old ADHD group, better gross motor development was associated with increased severity of parent-reported ADHD symptoms. In the 5-year-old ADHD group, poorer fine motor development was linked to higher ADHD symptom severity. For children who do not meet ADHD diagnostic criteria, no significant correlations were found between gross or fine motor developmental quotients (DQ) and the severity of ADHD symptoms.

Conclusions: Preschool children exhibiting ADHD symptoms are at a notable high risk of fine motor delays. Motor development in preschool children who meet ADHD diagnostic criteria is related to the severity of their symptoms. It is important to monitor both fine and gross motor development in preschool children with ADHD.

Introduction

Attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is a neurodevelopmental disorder characterized by developmentally inappropriate levels of inattention, hyperactivity, and impulsiveness (1). ADHD symptoms often emerge in preschool children, with prevalence estimated about 5.5% (95% CI, 3.3–7.7) for preschoolers (2). Understanding the early characteristics of children with ADHD symptoms is of great significance for early identification and intervention.

Studies have noted motor skill delays in preschoolers with ADHD (35). However, the relationship between motor skills and ADHD symptoms remains unclear. Motor skills encompass gross and fine motor abilities, both significantly linked to executive function (6). Good gross motor skills were a risk factor for ADHD symptoms in infancy (7), and specific gross motor behaviors at early ages correlate with later ADHD severity (8, 9). Impaired fine motor skills were consistently observed in school-age children with ADHD (10, 11). However, little study research on the relationship between motor development in preschool children and ADHD symptoms.

Preschool years witness rapid sensorimotor brain area development (12), paralleling cognitive and motor skill maturation. Children with ADHD often exhibited atypical sensorimotor and cognitive brain network development (13). Increased ADHD severity was associated with altered white matter organization which support motor skills and cognitive abilities (14). It was also reported a strong correlation between motor skills and cognitive abilities (e.g., executive function) in typically developing preschool children (15, 16). Motor development is integral to academic, social, and communication skills, and delays or imbalances in motor development are common in neurological disorders like ADHD. The exact mechanisms undelaying ADHD remained unclear, but hypotheses suggested immature or dysfunctional brain connectivity involving motor areas and high order cognitive related brain areas.

Previous research noted motor delays in children with ADHD and their association with future symptom severity and brain development (14, 17). But the relationships between gross/fine motor development and ADHD symptoms in preschoolers remain unexplored. This study aimed to address this gap in children aged 4–6 years. Given the high activity levels of preschoolers, not all active children meet ADHD diagnostic criteria. This study included ADHD and ADHD symptom-like groups aged 4.0–5.9 years to examine the motor-symptom relationship. Hypothesis 1 proposed a delayed fine and gross motor development in preschool ADHD children. Hypothesis 2 proposed that symptom severity was associated with respective motor development levels. Exploring the motor-symptom association aids in understanding ADHD brain development mechanisms and differentiating symptom severity.

Materials and methods

Participants

All participants were recruited from the developmental behavior clinic in the same hospital between March 2021 and January 2024. The children met the following inclusion criteria were included: right-handed; aged from 4.0 to 5.9 years, with the chief complaint of inattention or hyperactivity reported by kindergarten teachers or parents. Participants were excluded for developmental quotient (DQ) below 70 on the Children Neuropsychological and Behavior Scale, language disorder, visual or hearing impairment, psychoactive medication use, and other neurological or psychiatric disorders except ADHD.

There were 137 children aged 4.0–4.9 years and 252 children aged 5.0–5.9 years, referred to as the 4-year-old group and the 5-year-old group, respectively. For each age group, children were categorized into either the ADHD group or the ADHD symptom-like group by one or two clinicians based on the DSM-5 criteria. During the diagnostic process, clinicians relied on at least two sources of information, such as interviews and observations conducted by the clinician, parent questionnaires, behavioral observations performed by trained professionals, and videos of the child's daily activities provided by parents. Children were classified into the ADHD group if the clinician's interviews and observations met DSM-5 criteria, and the parent questionnaire scores were above the threshold. If the clinician's interviews or behavioral observations did not meet DSM-5 criteria, the child was categorized into the ADHD symptom-like group. Thus, based on both age and diagnosis, there were a total of four categories of children. The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki, and approved by the Medical Research Ethics Committee of the authors' institution.

Clinical and neuropsychological assessments

All patients were interviewed by one or two experienced developmental behavior pediatricians. Overall developmental levels were assessed by the Children Neuropsychological and Behavior Scale. Regarding the evaluation of ADHD symptoms, parents of the 4-year-old group completed the Conners' Parent Symptom Questionnaire, while parents of the 5-year-old group completed the Vanderbilt ADHD Diagnostic Parent Rating Scale.

The Children Neuropsychological and Behavior Scale (CNBS) is a diagnostic assessment tool which is widely used in Chinese hospitals to assess the DQ of children aged 0–6 years (18, 19). It includes five separate subscales: gross motor, fine motor, adaptive behavior, language, and personal-social. DQ is calculated by the following formula: DQ = (mental age/chronological age) × 100. Children were tested one-on-one by a trained nurse, which takes about 20 min. Full DQ refers to the average value of the five subscales. For each subscale, a DQ less than 70 indicates a developmental delay, a DQ between 70 and 79 is slightly below the threshold for developmental delay, and a DQ of 80 or above is considered to be within the normal range (18).

The Conners' Parent Symptom Questionnaire (PSQ) is a 48-item parent-report assessment for children aged 3–17 years (20). The PSQ contains 6 subscales: conduct problem, learning problem, psychosomatic problem, impulsivity-hyperactivity, anxiety, and hyperactivity index. Symptom items are rated using a 4-point Likert scale (never to very often). Fan and colleagues developed the Chinese urban norms, which has been widely used in Chinese hospitals (21).

The Vanderbilt ADHD Diagnostic Parent Rating Scale (VADPRS) is a 55-item parent-report assessment for children aged 5–12 years (22). It contains six dimensions of symptoms: inattention (9 items), hyperactivity (9 items), oppositional defiant disorder (8 items), conduct disorder (14 items), anxiety/depression (7 items) and functional impairment (8 items). Symptom items are also rated using a 4-point Likert scale (never to very often). The VADPRS score is calculated for each subscale as sum score of parent ratings (23).

Statistical analyses

To address the main research problem, the following analyses were conducted:

(a) Descriptive statistics were utilized to compute the mean and standard deviation of all measures.

(b) To explore the motor developmental delays among children with primary ADHD symptoms. A DQ below 70 was used as the criterion for developmental delay, while a DQ of 70 or above indicated mild or no delay (24). The rates of delayed gross motor DQ and fine motor DQ were calculated across the four categories of children.

(c) To explore the relationship between ADHD symptoms and motor development, independent sample t-tests were employed to compare differences in various indicators between ADHD group and ADHD symptom-like group for each age group. Pearson's correlation coefficients were calculated among all measures to explore potential correlations between different domains of DQ and ADHD symptoms. Hierarchical regression was used to examine the relationship between symptom severity and motor DQ for each group, with age, gender, adaptive behavior DQ, language DQ, and personal-social DQ as control variables.


For the 4-year-old group, symptom severity was determined by the sum of scores on the impulsive-hyperactive dimension and the hyperactivity index dimension in the PSQ. For the 5-year-old group, the severity of attention-deficit/hyperactivity symptoms was assessed by the sum of scores on the attention-deficit symptoms dimension and the hyperactive-impulsive symptoms dimension in the VADPRS.

Results

Demographic characteristics of all the groups were shown in Table 1, including age, gender, birth weight, gestational age and birth delivery mode. The study involved 137 participants in the 4-year-old group, with 45 in ADHD group 1 (mean age 4.53 ± 0.26, 31 boys) and 92 in symptom-like group 1 (mean age 4.54 ± 0.28, 81 boys). After excluding missing values, there were 4 participants (10.81%) in ADHD Group 1 and 6 participants (8.96%) in Symptom-Like Group 1 with a gestational age of less than 37 weeks. In the 5-year-old group, there were 252 participants, with 120 in ADHD group 2 (mean age 5.47 ± 0.29, 94 boys) and 132 in symptom-like group 2 (mean age 5.50 ± 0.28, 90 boys). After excluding missing values, there were 7 participants (7.87%) in ADHD Group 2 and 5 participants (5.56%) in Symptom-Like Group 2 with a gestational age of less than 37 weeks.

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Demographic characteristics of different groups.

Table 2 presents scores on various indicators of the CNBS and PSQ for the 4-year-old group. Both the ADHD gourp1 and symptom-like group 1 demonstrated normal gross motor development, with gross motor delay rates of 2.2% and 0%, respectively (see Table 3). However, both groups exhibited significant delays in fine motor development. The average fine motor DQ were 81.34 and 79.80, respectively, with fine motor delay rates of 20.0% and 30.4%, respectively (see Table 3). Table 4 presents scores on various indicators of the CNBS and VADPRS for the 5-year-old group. Although t-tests did not reveal significant differences between the two groups on CNBS scores, the symptom-like group 2 had higher rates of gross motor delay (7.6%) and fine motor delay (13.6%) compared to ADHD group 2 (6.7% and 8.3%, respectively). Similar to the 4-year-old group, t-tests showed that ADHD group 2 exhibited more severe oppositional defiant disorder, conduct disorder, and anxiety/depression symptoms (see Table 2).

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. The score of the 4-year-old group on CNBS and PSQ.

Table 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Number of children with a motor developmental delay according to the subscale DQ of the CNBS (N, %).

Table 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. The score of the 5-year-old group on CNBS and VADPRS.

Pearson correlation analysis indicated a weak correlation between gross motor DQ and hyperactivity index (r = 0.35, p = 0.020), as well as symptom severity (r = 0.32, p = 0.030) in ADHD group 1. However, fine motor DQ showed no correlation with hyperactivity index, impulsivity, or overall symptom severity in 4-year-old group (see Figure 1). Pearson correlation analysis also revealed a weak correlation between fine motor DQ and hyperactive-impulsive symptoms (r = −0.25, p = 0.006), as well as symptom severity (r = −0.22, p = 0.016) in ADHD group 2. However, gross motor DQ showed no correlation with hyperactive-impulsive symptoms, attention deficit symptoms, or overall symptom severity in 5-year-old group (see Figure 1).

Figure 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Scatter plots of gross motor DQ, fine motor DQ and symptom severity. aSymptom severity = Impulsivity-hyperactivity + Hyperactivity index. bSymptom severity = Inattentive + Hyperactive. The dashed line represents the regression line depicting the relationship between the x-axis and y-axis variables for each group.

Due to the four comparisons made within each age group during the hierarchical regression analysis, the alpha value was adjusted to 0.05/4 ≈ 0.017, corrected with Bonferroni correction. The hierarchical regression results indicated that, after including control variables (age, gender, adaptive behavior DQ, language DQ, and personal-social DQ), gross motor DQ in ADHD group 1 remained positively correlated with symptom severity (β = 0.46, p < 0.05, Bonferroni corrected; see Table 5). In ADHD group 2, fine motor DQ exhibited a negative correlation with symptom severity (β = −0.35, p < 0.05, Bonferroni corrected; see Table 6). The relationships between motor DQ and symptom severity were not significant in all other cases (p > 0.05, Bonferroni corrected).

Table 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Hierarchical regression models to predict symptom severity from gross motor DQ and fine motor DQ in 4-year-old group.

Table 6
www.frontiersin.org

Table 6. Hierarchical regression models to predict symptom severity from gross motor DQ and fine motor DQ in 5-year-old group.

Discussion

The present study explored the relationship between motor development and ADHD symptom severity in preschool children aged 4.0–5.9 years. Consistent with Hypothesis 1, we found that preschool children identified by parents or teachers as exhibiting ADHD-related symptoms, regardless of whether they met the full diagnostic criteria, often showed delays in fine motor development. Contrary to Hypothesis 1, significant gross motor delays were only observed in the 5-year-old group.

Our findings regarding the rate of motor development delays in preschool children with ADHD are consistent with previous research. For instance, a nationwide epidemiological study in Egypt reported that among children aged 3–6, 2.2% experienced gross motor delays, and 2.8% experienced fine motor delays (25). However, in children with ADHD symptoms or at risk for ADHD, there is a noticeable difference in the prevalence of gross and fine motor delays. A Spanish epidemiological study reported that 22.6% of children aged 3–7 years who were likely to have ADHD had fine motor difficulties (26). In contrast, only 3.6% of children who were unlikely to have ADHD experienced similar issues (26). However, there was no significant difference in the prevalence of gross motor difficulties between the two groups, with both around 3.6% (26). This study similarly found that children exhibiting ADHD symptoms had a much higher risk of fine motor delays compared to gross motor delays, particularly among the younger children.

Consistent with Hypothesis 2, our study found a relationship between motor development and symptom severity in preschool children with ADHD. Specifically, only in the 4-year-old group, there was a positive correlation between better gross motor DQ and higher ADHD symptom severity. Conversely, only in the 5-year-old group, the ADHD group exhibited a negative correlation between poorer fine motor DQ and higher severity of parent-reported hyperactive-impulsive symptoms. No significant correlations were observed between gross motor DQ, fine motor DQ, and parent-reported ADHD symptoms in the ADHD symptom-like group.

Although ADHD symptoms are considered continuous, our study's results tend to support the interpretation that ADHD may involve specific brain developmental structures (2730). The observed associations between motor development and symptoms in both age groups may stem from specific changes in brain structure or function during the developmental process in children with ADHD. For instance, neuroimaging studies have indicated impaired inhibitory mechanisms in the sensorimotor brain network and structural and functional alterations in the corpus callosum among children with ADHD (31, 32). The bilateral corticospinal tract (CST), which transmits motor signals from the primary motor cortex down the spinal cord to the trunk and limb muscles, may also serve as a crucial brain structure linking motor skills and ADHD symptom severity. Previous study on children aged 9–11 has found that increased ADHD symptom severity is associated with reduced white matter organization in fronto-pontine fibers projecting to and from the supplementary motor area (14). Furthermore, increased fiber density in the CST during adolescence and early adulthood has been linked to a greater reduction in hyperactivity and impulsivity symptoms over the preceding 3–4 years (17).

Furthermore, the inconsistency in the motor-symptom associations between fine and gross motor skills across different age groups may arise from differences in the developmental rates of these two types of movements, leading to atypical development of motor-related brain networks. The negative correlation between fine motor skills and symptom severity, consistent with previous findings in school-aged children, suggests that fine motor impairments correlate with ADHD symptoms (33). However, the positive correlation between gross motor skills and symptom severity indicates that this atypical motor development cannot solely be attributed to fine motor impairment. Unlike in school-aged children (34), the results of this study suggest that, for younger children, higher levels of gross motor development may not provide a protective effect against ADHD symptoms.

The present study may inform future interventions targeting motor skills in preschool children exhibiting ADHD symptoms. For younger children, it is important to ensure they have sufficient opportunities for gross motor development while simultaneously encouraging participation in fine motor activities, such as using chopsticks or buttoning clothing. For older preschoolers, a comprehensive approach should be taken to monitor both gross and fine motor development. Activities such as ball games, climbing, stringing beads, and playing with playdough can be beneficial for the development of either gross or fine motor skills. Previous research on school-aged children has also suggested that motor interventions can reduce ADHD symptoms and enhance cognitive and academic outcomes (3, 10, 35, 36). When designing interventions to address the motor abilities of children with ADHD, it is vital to consider their developmental stage and the specific processes affecting the corresponding motor-related brain networks.

Limitations and strengths

Interpretation of the results should be tempered by the limitations of this study. The sample was drawn from a clinic population of Chinese preschool children with reported attention problems, lacking healthy controls. The existing data derived from the clinic-based sample may introduce bias and may not accurately represent the broader preschool population. Furthermore, the utilization of different ADHD parent questionnaires for the two preschool groups complicates the comparison of ADHD symptom severity between them and may impact result interpretation.

Despite these limitations, the study possesses several strengths. It filled a gap in previous research concerning the relationship between fine motor development and ADHD in preschool children. By considering the continuum of ADHD symptoms and investigating both children meeting diagnostic criteria and those with symptomatic presentations, the study results are more generalizable. By distinguishing between fine and gross motor development, this study offers insights for future motor intervention strategies targeting preschool children with ADHD.

As a retrospective cross-sectional study, the present study offers preliminary insights into the relationship between ADHD symptoms and motor DQ in children across two age groups. However, we acknowledge that further longitudinal studies would be valuable in exploring the developmental trajectories of motor skills in children with ADHD symptoms. Additionally, exploring the brain structures and functions of motor-related brain regions will also enhance our understanding of this research field.

Conclusion

The results of present study indicate that preschool children with ADHD symptoms exhibit high rates of motor developmental delays, particularly in fine motor development, regardless of whether they meet diagnostic criteria. However, whether in the 4-year-old group or the 5-year-old group, correlations between ADHD symptom severity and motor development were found only within the ADHD group. Therefore, the study indicates a need for increased focus from both parents and healthcare professionals on the motor development status of children meeting diagnostic criteria for ADHD.

Data availability statement

The datasets presented in this article are not readily available because these medical data involve the privacy of the child. Requests to access the datasets should be directed to Aimin Liang, liang-aimin@163.com.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by Medical Ethics Committee of Beijing Children's Hospital, Capital Medical University. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. Written informed consent for participation in this study was provided by the participants' legal guardians/next of kin.

Author contributions

ZC: Data curation, Formal Analysis, Methodology, Software, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. SL: Investigation, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. AL: Funding acquisition, Investigation, Project administration, Resources, Supervision, Writing – review & editing. HH: Conceptualization, Formal Analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Software, Supervision, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. XN: Funding acquisition, Project administration, Resources, Supervision, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. This work is supported by STI 2030—Major Projects 2021ZD0200508, National Natural Science Foundation of China.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

1. Danielson ML, Bitsko RH, Ghandour RM, Holbrook JR, Kogan MD, Blumberg SJ. Prevalence of parent-reported ADHD diagnosis and associated treatment among U.S. Children and adolescents, 2016. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. (2018) 47(2):199–212. doi: 10.1080/15374416.2017.1417860

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Liu A, Xu Y, Yan Q, Tong L. The prevalence of attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder among Chinese children and adolescents. Sci Rep. (2018) 8(1):11169. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-29488-2

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Gurevitz M, Geva R, Varon M, Leitner Y. Early markers in infants and toddlers for development of ADHD. J Atten Disord. (2014) 18(1):14–22. doi: 10.1177/1087054712447858

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Pant SW, Skovgaard AM, Ammitzboll J, Holstein BE, Pedersen TP. Motor development problems in infancy predict mental disorders in childhood: a longitudinal cohort study. Eur J Pediatr. (2022) 181(7):2655–61. doi: 10.1007/s00431-022-04462-3

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Shephard E, Zuccolo PF, Idrees I, Godoy PBG, Salomone E, Ferrante C, et al. Systematic review and meta-analysis: the science of early-life precursors and interventions for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. (2022) 61(2):187–226. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2021.03.016

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Oberer N, Gashaj V, Roebers CM. Motor skills in kindergarten: internal structure, cognitive correlates and relationships to background variables. Hum Mov Sci. (2017) 52:170–80. doi: 10.1016/j.humov.2017.02.002

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Jaspers M, De Winter AF, Buitelaar JK, Verhulst FC, Reijneveld SA, Hartman CA. Early childhood assessments of community pediatric professionals predict autism Spectrum and attention deficit hyperactivity problems. J Abnorm Child Psychol. (2013) 41(1):71–80. doi: 10.1007/s10802-012-9653-4

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Ali JB, Charman T, Johnson MH, Jones EJH, BASIS/STAARS Team. Early motor differences in infants at elevated likelihood of autism Spectrum disorder and/or attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. J Autism Dev Disord. (2020) 50(12):4367–84. doi: 10.1007/s10803-020-04489-1

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Goodwin A, Hendry A, Mason L, Bazelmans T, Begum Ali J, Pasco G, et al. Behavioural measures of infant activity but not attention associate with later preschool ADHD traits. Brain Sci. (2021) 11(5):524. doi: 10.3390/brainsci11050524

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Scott H, Shoulberg EK, Dennis M, Krasner A, Smith AL, Hoza B. Associations of ADHD-risk and motor competence with school functioning. J Atten Disord. (2024) 28(1):31–42. doi: 10.1177/10870547231197214

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Mendes LST, Manfro GG, Gadelha A, Pan PM, Bressan RA, Rohde LA, et al. Fine motor ability and psychiatric disorders in youth. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. (2018) 27(5):605–13. doi: 10.1007/s00787-017-1091-y

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Casey B, Tottenham N, Liston C, Durston S. Imaging the developing brain: what have we learned about cognitive development? Trends Cogn Sci (Regul Ed). (2005) 9(3):104–10. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.01.011

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Soman SM, Vijayakumar N, Thomson P, Ball G, Hyde C, Silk TJ. Functional and structural brain network development in children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Hum Brain Mapp. (2023) 44(8):3394–409. doi: 10.1002/hbm.26288

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Fuelscher I, Hyde C, Anderson V, Silk TJ. White matter tract signatures of fiber density and morphology in ADHD. Cortex. (2021) 138:329–40. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2021.02.015

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Liu J, Li Y, Zhou T, Lu Y, Sang M, Li L, et al. Relationship between gross motor skills and inhibitory control in preschool children: a pilot study. Front Hum Neurosci. (2022) 16:848230. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2022.848230

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Stuhr C, Hughes CML, Stöckel T. The role of executive functions for motor performance in preschool children as compared to young adults. Front Psychol. (2020) 11:1552. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01552

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Damatac CG, Soheili-Nezhad S, Blazquez Freches G, Zwiers MP, de Bruijn S, Ikde S, et al. Longitudinal changes of ADHD symptoms in association with white matter microstructure: a tract-specific fixel-based analysis. Neuroimage Clin. (2022) 35:103057. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2022.103057

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Jin C. Children Neuropsychological and Behavior Scale, Revision 2016. Beijing: Beijing press (2016).

Google Scholar

19. Jin C, Li R, Zhang L, Zhang R, Li N, Wang J, et al. The revision and according validity research of China developmental scale for China. Chin J Child Health Care. (2014) 22(12):1242–6. doi: 10.11852/zgetbjzz2014-22-12-04

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Goyette CH, Conners CK, Ulrich RF. Normative data on revised Conners parent and teacher rating scales. J Abnorm Child Psychol. (1978) 6(2):221–36. doi: 10.1007/BF00919127

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Fan J, Du Y, Wang L. The norm and reliability of the conners parent symptom questionnaire in Chinese urban children. Shanghai Arch Psychiatry. (2006) 17(6):321–3. doi: 10.3969/j.issn.1002-0829.2005.06.001

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Wolraich ML, Lambert W, Doffing MA, Bickman L, Simmons T, Worley K. Psychometric properties of the Vanderbilt ADHD diagnostic parent rating scale in a referred population. J Pediatr Psychol. (2003) 28(8):559–67. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/jsg046

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Anderson NP, Feldman JA, Kolko DJ, Pilkonis PA, Lindhiem O. National norms for the Vanderbilt ADHD diagnostic parent rating scale in children. J Pediatr Psychol. (2022) 47(6):652–61. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/jsab132

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Li HH, Feng JY, Wang B, Zhang Y, Wang CX, Jia FY. Comparison of the children neuropsychological and behavior scale and the Griffiths mental development scales when assessing the development of children with autism. PRBM. (2019) 12:973–81. doi: 10.2147/PRBM.S225904

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Metwally AM, Abdallah AM, El-Din EMS, Zeid DA, Khadr Z, Elshaarawy GA, et al. Screening and determinant of suspected developmental delays among Egyptian preschool-aged children: a cross-sectional national community-based study. BMC Pediatr. (2023) 23(1):521. doi: 10.1186/s12887-023-04335-0

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Marín-Méndez JJ, Borra-Ruiz MC, Álvarez-Gómez MJ, Soutullo Esperón C. Psychomotor development and learning difficulties in preschool children with probable attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: an epidemiological study in Navarre and La Rioja. Neurología. (2017) 32(8):487–93. doi: 10.1016/j.nrl.2016.02.009

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Chiang HL, Tseng WYI, Tseng WL, Tung YH, Hsu YC, Chen CL, et al. Atypical development in white matter microstructures in ADHD: a longitudinal diffusion imaging study. Asian J Psychiatr. (2023) 79:103358. doi: 10.1016/j.ajp.2022.103358

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Wu ZM, Bralten J, Cao QJ, Hoogman M, Zwiers MP, An L, et al. White matter microstructural alterations in children with ADHD: categorical and dimensional perspectives. Neuropsychopharmacol. (2017) 42(2):572–80. doi: 10.1038/npp.2016.223

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Hoogman M, Bralten J, Hibar DP, Mennes M, Zwiers MP, Schweren LSJ, et al. Subcortical brain volume differences in participants with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in children and adults: a cross-sectional mega-analysis. Lancet Psychiatry. (2017) 4(4):310–9. doi: 10.1016/S2215-0366(17)30049-4

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Hoogman M, Muetzel R, Guimaraes JP, Shumskaya E, Mennes M, Zwiers MP, et al. Brain imaging of the cortex in ADHD: a coordinated analysis of large-scale clinical and population-based samples. Am J Psychiatry. (2019) 176(7):531–42. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2019.18091033

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Chen C, Lidstone D, Crocetti D, Mostofsky SH, Nebel MB. Increased interhemispheric somatomotor functional connectivity and mirror overflow in ADHD. Neuroimage-Clinical. (2021) 31:102759. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2021.102759

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Gilbert DL, Isaacs KM, Augusta M, MacNeil LK, Mostofsky SH. Motor cortex inhibition: a marker of ADHD behavior and motor development in children. Neurology. (2011) 76(7):615–21. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e31820c2ebd

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Kaiser ML, Schoemaker MM, Albaret JM, Geuze RH. What is the evidence of impaired motor skills and motor control among children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD)? systematic review of the literature. Res Dev Disabil. (2015) 36:338–57. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2014.09.023

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

34. D’Anna C, Carlevaro F, Magno F, Vagnetti R, Limone P, Magistro D. Gross motor skills are associated with symptoms of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in school-aged children. Children. (2024) 11(7):757. doi: 10.3390/children11070757

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Lelong M, Zysset A, Nievergelt M, Luder R, Götz U, Schulze C, et al. How effective is fine motor training in children with ADHD? A scoping review. BMC Pediatr. (2021) 21(1):490. doi: 10.1186/s12887-021-02916-5

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Neudecker C, Mewes N, Reimers AK, Woll A. Exercise interventions in children and adolescents with ADHD: a systematic review. J Atten Disord. (2019) 23(4):307–24. doi: 10.1177/1087054715584053

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: preschool children, attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), fine motor, gross motor, developmental quotient

Citation: Cui Z, Li S, Liang A, Huang H and Ni X (2024) Association between reported ADHD symptom and motor development delay in preschool children. Front. Pediatr. 12:1480488. doi: 10.3389/fped.2024.1480488

Received: 14 August 2024; Accepted: 7 November 2024;
Published: 22 November 2024.

Edited by:

Hongbao Cao, George Mason University, United States

Reviewed by:

Feng Li, National Institutes of Health (NIH), United States
Xiaorong Guo, Feinstein Institute for Medical Research, United States

Copyright: © 2024 Cui, Li, Liang, Huang and Ni. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Hongmei Huang, hhm15@tsinghua.org.cn; Xin Ni, nixin@bch.com.cn

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.