Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Pediatr., 16 January 2024
Sec. Pediatric Cardiology

A randomized, controlled trial of an innovative, multimedia instructional program for acquiring auditory skill in identifying pediatric heart murmurs

\r\nRobin W. Doroshow
Robin W. Doroshow1*Julie AldrichJulie Aldrich2Rebecca DornerRebecca Dorner3Laurie LyonsLaurie Lyons4Robert McCarter\r\nRobert McCarter5
  • 1Department of Cardiology, Children’s National Hospital and George Washington School of Medicine and Health Sciences, Washington, DC, United States
  • 2Department of Pediatrics, Children’s National Hospital and George Washington School of Medicine and Health Sciences, Washington, DC, United States
  • 3Department of Pediatrics, Georgetown University School of Medicine, Washington, DC, United States
  • 4Department of Instructional Design and Technology, George Washington University School of Medicine and Health Sciences, Washington, DC, United States
  • 5Division of Biostatistics, Children’s National Hospital and George Washington School of Medicine and Health Sciences, Washington, DC, United States

Objective: To create a brief, acceptable, innovative method for self-paced learning to enhance recognition of pediatric heart murmurs by medical students, and to demonstrate this method's effectiveness in a randomized, controlled trial.

Materials and methods: A curriculum of six 10-min online learning modules was designed to enable deliberate practice of pediatric cardiac auscultation, using recordings of patients' heart murmurs. Principles of andragogy and multimedia learning were applied to optimize acquisition of this skill. A pretest and posttest, given 4 weeks apart, were created using additional recordings and administered to 87 3rd-year medical students during their pediatric clerkship. They were randomized to have access to the modules after the pretest or after the posttest, and asked to use at least the first 2 of the modules.

Results: 47 subjects comprised the Intervention group, and 40 subjects the Control group. On our primary outcome, distinguishing innocent from pathological with at least moderate confidence, the posttest scores were significantly higher for the Intervention group (60.5%) than for the Control group (20.0%). For our secondary outcomes, the 2 groups also differed significantly in the ability to distinguish innocent from pathological murmurs, and in identifying the actual diagnosis. On all 3 outcomes, those Intervention group subjects who accessed 4–6 modules scored higher than those who accessed 0–3 modules, who in turn scored higher than the Control group.

Summary: Applying current principles of adult learning, we have created a teaching program for medical students to learn to recognize common pediatric murmurs. Its effectiveness was demonstrated in a randomized, controlled trial. The program results in a meaningful gain in this skill from 1 h of self-paced training with high acceptance to learners.

Introduction

Congenital heart defects (CHD) occur in approximately 1% of live births, and can have serious effects on a child's health. Critical CHD can be detected with high sensitivity in the newborn using pulse oximetry (1). Most non-critical CHD present in childhood with an asymptomatic heart murmur, detected by a primary care provider (PCP). Innocent, or normal, heart murmurs occur far more frequently, with incidence ranging from 50%–90% (2). Therefore, the PCP must be able to distinguish between innocent and pathological murmurs, in order to appropriately refer patients with potential CHD without referring an excessive number of children with innocent murmurs (37). Hence, education in pediatric cardiac auscultation (CA) is routinely included in undergraduate and graduate medical education, and this skill has now been incorporated into standardized medical testing (8, 9).

Unfortunately, non-cardiologists have been repeatedly shown to be lacking in the skill of CA (1016), suggesting that traditional methods of teaching it, such as lectures and bedside demonstration, are ill-suited to honing this sensory skill. Moreover, most non-cardiologists do not improve at CA after medical school (11, 1720).

The recent evolution of digital technologies for recording and sharing audio signals has led to the development of new methods of teaching CA (12, 21) of the adult patient. While several such innovative programs have been successful in teaching subjects to identify an adult's cardiac diagnosis from a recording or simulation (22, 23), they cannot be applied to pediatric murmur education due to major differences in disease processes, heart rates, diagnostic prevalence, and presenting symptoms of CHD. Therefore, similar innovative programs are called for in pediatric CA (11, 13, 1517, 2428).

To enhance competency in this skill, we have produced a user-friendly online program for undergraduate medical students, using actual recordings from pediatric patients and applying central principles of andragogy (29) and multi-sensory learning (30). We have placed more emphasis on differentiating between innocent and pathological murmurs, which is the task of the pediatric PCP, than on arriving at a specific diagnosis (31, 32). We hypothesized that our method would improve acquisition of, and confidence in, this skill, compared with traditional approaches.

Materials and methods

Six online modules, averaging 10 min each, were developed using multiple selected recordings from patients with confirmed cardiac diagnoses. The dominant principles of this teaching program are deliberate practice (14, 33), contiguity or linkage (3436), and avoidance of cognitive overload (3641).

Deliberate practice is a demonstrably effective method of improving performance; it is a form of practicing which is usually solitary, and requires considerable repetition and immediate feedback. Its most familiar application is in mastering a musical instrument. Contiguity refers to the learning advantage attained by placing closely related teaching points (e.g., auditory information coupled with visual information) near one another in time and/or space. Cognitive overload is a concept most simply represented as “too much information.” It is to be minimized in order to reduce extraneous processing of information, and has been well shown to interfere with acquisition of new skills.

The modules are accessed through the Internet with a computer or tablet, using standard earbuds. They are presented in a fixed sequence, starting with the Still's Murmur and Ventricular Septal Defects modules (Supplementary Table S1), allowing the learner to build upon each segment. To enable the application of deliberate practice, learners control the duration of playback of all recordings, and may review completed modules at will; and they participate in brief interactive activities scattered throughout the modules. Throughout the modules, they have free access to a set of recordings of 8 common murmurs and one of normal heart sounds for comparison (Supplementary Figures S1–S5).

To apply the principle of multimedia contiguity, we provided the learner with nonverbal content, such as the messages that (a) this starts with a simple binary decision, and (b) these children are normal and healthy. Additional relevant information depicted nonverbally includes (a) the common age of the child for that diagnosis, (b) the location of the stethoscope on the child's chest, (c) the side of the stethoscope used (bell or diaphragm), and (d) whether the child is upright or supine. This material is visually linked with each murmur heard, with the name of the diagnosis shown on the top of the screen, and with color-coding for each module. Limited, familiar voice narration (36) accompanies, but does not compete with, each murmur recording. Cognitive overload is minimized by targeting CA specifically, eliminating material targeting other goals of teaching in cardiology, such as knowledge of anatomy or pathophysiology.

We devised a 20-case pre-test, and a post-test containing the same 20 recordings in different order with pictures of another 20 child models. None of the test recordings was used in the teaching modules, which incorporated 115 different recordings. For each case, the learner was able to listen to the murmur as long as desired, and was then asked (a) Is this murmur innocent or not? (b) How confident are you in your answer? and (c) What do you think the cardiologist finds? (actual diagnosis). Demographic questions were included in the pre-test, and feedback questions in the post-test.

In a pilot study, these tests were administered preliminarily to 23 medical trainees and faculty in an iterative manner, to establish content validity and to provide a frame of reference for student performance. Recordings that were rarely identified correctly were eliminated, as were those felt by cardiologists not to be representative of the diagnosis.

The teaching program was offered to all 3rd-year medical students at George Washington School of Medicine and Health Sciences (GWU) over a 10-month period, during their required two-month pediatric clerkship. None had received previous instruction in pediatric auscultation. Students were randomized equally to a Control group and an Intervention group. All subjects took an online pre-test in person during a specified educational forum at the beginning of the clerkship, and a post-test similarly after 4 weeks. The Intervention subjects had access to the program between the two tests, and were asked to complete at least the first 2 modules. The Control subjects were given access after completion of the post-test. Entry into each module was automatically recorded. During the clerkship, all students had formal teaching and bedside instruction, and unlimited access to teaching programs available on the Internet (see Supplementary Table S2). All subjects received email-reminders during the interim to complete the modules or to individually learn about murmurs, depending upon group assignment.

Our primary outcome of interest was improvement in correct discrimination between innocent and pathological murmurs with at least moderate confidence (CDIP), as a surrogate for the decision of whether to refer the case for specialty evaluation. Confidence was rated on a 4-point Likert scale, 4 indicating “very confident.” Our secondary outcomes were improvement (a) in the simple distinction of innocent vs. pathological (DIP), and (b) in the correct identification of specific diagnoses. Total confidence scores were the sum of the 20 confidence responses.

To maintain confidentiality, all students were assigned study numbers. Results could therefore not affect grades for the clerkship. The Human Subjects Committee of Children's National Hospital approved this study as expedited.

Statistical methods first addressed the comparability of the 2 study groups. Methods used to evaluate evidence of baseline difference across study groups depended on the nature and distribution of the characteristic being compared. Categorical comparisons were implemented by chi square tests, and comparisons based on measurements by analysis of variance or nonparametric procedures for non-normally distributed data. Analyses of outcomes were based on linear regression analysis using bootstrapping methods, with 1,000 replications, to generate model parameter and variance estimates which do not depend on the normality assumption. The study was powered at 80% to detect a moderate effect size (0.5 SD) difference in scored results between 2 randomly assigned groups with a 2-tailed type 1 error of 5%. A sample size of at least 64 per group was planned to address study aims.

Results

All 3rd-year GWU medical students participating in their pediatric clerkship over a 10-month period were randomized 1:1 to the Control group (regular training) or the Intervention group (regular training + modules). Of these, 138 subjects took the pre-test; 87 also completed the post-test, yielding interpretable data regarding improvement. Of those completing both tests, 47 had been randomized to Intervention and 40 to Control (Figure 1).

Figure 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Subject enrollment.

Table 1 compares full participants and drop-outs by group on demographics, prior experience, self-assessment and pre-test performance. Full participants in the intervention and control groups were very similar in all comparisons.

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Sample characteristics.

Figure 2 compares post-test scores (95% confidence interval) by group, controlling for pre-test scores. In the primary outcome (CDIP), the Intervention group (I) received a score of 60.5% (51.8, 69.1) compared to the Control (C) group's 20.0% (14.1, 26.0) (p < 0.001). The comparable results for the two secondary outcomes, DIP (I group 81.2% (77.1, 85.3) vs. C group 63.9% (59.6, 68.2) (p < 0.001)) and specific diagnosis I group 50.7% (45.1, 56.2) vs. C group 27.5% (23.3, 31.7, p < 0.001), were also significantly different. Controlling for pre-test confidence scores, post-test confidence scores (range 20–80) were significantly higher in the Intervention group 57.7 (53.1, 62.2), compared to the Control group 41.4 (37.7, 45.1) (p < 0.001).

Figure 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Effect of intervention on 3 study outcomes.

Of the 47 participants in the Intervention group, 3 subjects did not access any modules. Twenty-six accessed more than the required 2 modules, and of these, 20 accessed all 6. Controlling for pre-test scores, those subjects who accessed more modules scored significantly higher on the post-test for all outcomes than those who did fewer, and those who accessed 0–3 modules scored significantly higher than those randomized to the Control group (Figure 3).

Figure 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Effect of intervention by number of modules accessed.

Satisfaction feedback was obtained anonymously from the subjects at the end of the post-test. Due to malfunction of the data-collection system, the Likert responses were not recorded. However, detailed responses were obtained from all subjects to the following questions: “Do you feel that your skill in evaluating heart murmurs has improved over the course of your clerkship?” and “Which if any of the following did you use to supplement your auscultatory learning during the clerkship?” The categorical results of the former are given in Table 2, demonstrating a strongly significant effect of the intervention (“Yes” in 70% of the intervention group v. 15% of controls). Sample comments are listed in Supplementary Table S3.

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Feedback regarding improvement.

Members of the Control group often responded to the effect, “I haven't had access to the modules yet”. Only one control subject reported improvement over the 4 weeks between the tests, attributed to a cardiology lecture given to all the students.

Responses from the Intervention group were largely very positive Four members of this group reported that they did not think they improved significantly, and two others that they did not have the time to do the modules.

There were no significant differences between the 2 groups in use of supplementary learning materials (Supplementary Table S2).

Discussion

This study demonstrates the substantial impact of a new teaching method on the skill of medical students in differentiating innocent murmurs from pathological murmurs in children. Subjects viewing even 1 or 2 modules performed significantly better on all 3 outcomes measured than the control group. Moreover, those subjects who viewed more modules showed progressively better acquisition of this skill.

We believe this is the first pediatric CA program evaluated with a randomized controlled trial comparing pre- and post-intervention scores on standardized tests. Finley et al. (28) designed a 1-hour audio-only training program for 124 medical students in Australia and Nova Scotia. Utilizing a complex 7-level interactive self-paced approach and asking for a binary innocent/pathological choice, they demonstrated a small but significant improvement from 75% (Australian) and 85% (Canadian) to 95% on the post-test. In choosing our primary outcome, we avoided such high baseline scores, which likely reflected educated guesses. Murmur files for both their training program and their tests were chosen randomly from one pool of 56 recordings; no 2 subjects and no 2 tests were the same, rendering comparisons problematic. Germanakis et al. (11) tested 106 primary care practitioners before and after an 8-hour intensive seminar including lectures and multimedia. The subjects showed substantial improvement in the binary choice, except in the recognition of innocent murmurs, rising from a baseline of 17% to only 26%. Their study was not randomized and had no control group.

We selected CDIP as our primary outcome as a surrogate for differentiating between those patients who would be referred to a cardiologist and those who would not. Thus, “correct” answers in which the subjects had little or no confidence were counted as incorrect, because these murmurs would have prompted a referral. The ability to make a specific auscultatory diagnosis (e.g., aortic stenosis), while significantly better in the Intervention group, is of less importance in the clinical setting, where the PCP must make a simple binary decision (31, 32). The emphasis on this binary decision has been embraced by several investigators studying pediatric CA (11, 15, 17, 24, 25) and adult cases (42).

Other pediatric murmur programs targeting students and trainees (16, 26, 27) have focused on the more challenging tasks of identifying auditory features (such as shape or quality) and/or reaching an actual diagnosis, which in practice is usually subsequently reached by a pediatric cardiologist with or without other data. We chose not to score our learners on features of the murmurs, as has been done in other such tests (18, 26, 43), in which identification of such features correlates poorly with recognition of the innocence, or the cause, of the murmur (22, 27). We believe that identification of these details does not aid the learner in recognizing murmurs, but adds another layer of cognitive material to master which is not essential to the purpose.

The effect size demonstrated here was clinically significant, with the Intervention subjects scoring 60.5% correct on the primary outcome, compared with 20.0% in the Control group. This is in contrast with published studies of pediatric murmur programs (11, 26) in which the improvement in this binary distinction was statistically significant but was not substantive relative to the investment in time (6 and 8 h, respectively), personnel, and funding.

We did not propose an arbitrary target score to define mastery. However, we noted that the post-test scores for the Intervention group on all outcomes fell between those of 11 cardiology faculty and fellows and those of 6 non-cardiology pediatric faculty members (mean scores for primary outcome 90.0% and 55.0%, respectively) studied in the pilot project. Furthermore, those subjects who accessed 4–6 modules performed similarly to the cardiologists with respect to our primary outcome. This result exceeded our expectations.

The pre- and post-tests evaluated the external validity of this skill, by providing “new” recordings which were not used in the teaching modules. In other studies (14, 28, 44) investigators have used the same recordings for both training and testing purposes, possibly testing the subject's ability to recognize the actual recording, which reflects only internal validity.

For the teaching modules, we used multiple recordings to illustrate each common murmur, in contrast to the work of Barrett et al. (14), in which precise repetition of a single cardiac cycle is deliberately employed, with the goal of forming an auditory template for the learner. To enable our learners to more accurately categorize a “new” murmur when it is encountered, we chose to train them with a range of different recordings of each murmur (33).

We approximated the clinical setting by providing more than one “case” of each common diagnosis on the tests, so that subjects could not use one answer to deduce another by elimination, which might allow learners to game the system. Similarly, we offered the same 8 options for diagnoses of pathological murmurs in all test questions, making a correct guess less likely than with the standard 4 or 5 options which vary between cases (45).

Limitations

Because our subjects were randomized prior to self-enrollment by completing the pre-test, we cannot determine whether those who opted out (18% of the total randomized group) were comparable to the population who participated. However, we found no significant differences between those who initiated but did not complete participation, and those who did complete the study (Table 1).

The quantitative effect of increasing exposure to the modules is an observation based on non-randomized self-selection of students within the Intervention group, and therefore subject to selection bias.

We did not assess for retention of this skill by re-testing the subjects after a time interval. This curriculum, being brief and readily accessible, was designed to be made available for reinforcement learning after the initial exposure (28).

Transferability to the clinical setting was not tested in this study, nor in any other published studies of teaching CA. Our choice of 3rd-year students as subjects was guided by a desire to have an early impact on the learning of this clinical skill, and did not permit that type of assessment.

Our data regarding learner satisfaction was limited, but largely positive. The fact that over half of the Intervention subjects chose to do more than the 2 “required” modules, and 43% of the group completed all 6 modules, demonstrates change in behavior and attests to its value to them.

We elected for simplicity (Supplementary Figures S1–S5) in designing the program, to optimize multisensory linkage while minimizing cognitive load, given the goal to provide opportunity for deliberate practice as a means of learning auditory recognition, not pathophysiology (46). This less cluttered approach also helps to avoid the intimidation which may be experienced by users of more complex teaching programs. However, we cannot determine which of the features of our program contributed most to the outcome.

Future directions

We plan to test this program on a different, more advanced group of learners, with late follow-up to evaluate both retention and the booster effect of the modules. At present, it is incorporated into the pediatric clerkship for 3rd-year medical students at GWU, and into the pediatric cardiology rotation of 2nd-year pediatric residents from the Children's National and Georgetown University programs. Further, we hope to disseminate it more widely to pediatric trainees and practitioners in the future.

Summary

We have created a novel teaching program using actual recordings of pediatric murmurs to aid medical trainees in learning to recognize common murmurs in children, incorporating major principles of andragogy and multimedia learning in an effort to avoid the pitfalls of previous such innovations and their assessments (19). We demonstrated its effectiveness in a randomized controlled trial to result in a meaningful gain in this skill from 1 h of self-paced individualized training with high acceptance to learners.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by Children's National Hospital Human Subjects Committee (IRB). The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The ethics committee/institutional review board waived the requirement of written informed consent for participation from the participants or the participants’ legal guardians/next of kin because it posed minimal risk to subjects.

Author contributions

RWD: Conceptualization, Formal Analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Supervision, Validation, Writing – original draft. JA: Data curation, Formal Analysis, Investigation, Project administration, Writing – review & editing. RD: Data curation, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Writing – review & editing. LL: Conceptualization, Resources, Software, Writing – review & editing. RM: Formal Analysis, Methodology, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.

Children's Academy of Pediatric Educators (CAPE), Children's National Hospital, Washington, DC—PI: RD.

Northeast Group on Education Affairs, Association of American Medical Colleges, Washington, DC—PI: RD.

The sponsors had no input into the study design; the collection, analysis, and interpretation of data; the writing of the report; or the decision to submit the paper for publication.

Acknowledgments

Jeffrey Sestokas designed and built the tests and half of the modules. Wilhelmina Bradford recruited and organized subjects. Ariana Lee-Kane randomized the subjects, assisted in administering the tests, and organized the initial data. Yao Iris Cheng contributed to the statistical analysis. Kurt Sidenstick helped prepare audiovisual materials.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fped.2023.1283306/full#supplementary-material

References

1. Martin GR, Cross RR, Hom LA, Klugman D. Updates in congenital heart disease: can outcomes improve? Am J Perinatol. (2019) 36(S 02):S22–8. doi: 10.1055/s-0039-1691773

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Advani N, Menahem S, Wilkinson J. The diagnosis of innocent murmurs in childhood. Cardiol Young. (2000) 10:340–2. doi: 10.1017/S104795110000963X

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Roy DL. The paediatrician and cardiac auscultation. Paediatr Child Health. (2003) 8(9):561–3. doi: 10.1093/pch/8.9.561

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Wong KK, Barker AP, Warren AE. Paediatricians’ validation of learning objectives in paediatric cardiology. Paediatr Child Health. (2005) 10(2):95–9. doi: 10.1093/pch/10.2.95

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Murugan SJ, Thomson J, Parsons JM, Dickinson DF, Blackburn ME, Gibbs JL. New outpatient referrals to a tertiary paediatric cardiac centre: evidence of increasing workload and evolving patterns of referral. Cardiol Young. (2005) 15(1):43–6. doi: 10.1017/S1047951105000090

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Montinari M, Minelli S. The first 200 years of cardiac auscultation and future perspectives. J Multidiscip Healthc. (2019) 12:183–9. doi: 10.2147/JMDH.S193904

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Barrett M, Mackie A, Finley J. Cardiac auscultation in the modern era: premature requiem or Phoenix rising? Cardiol Rev. (2017) 25:205–10. doi: 10.1097/CRD.0000000000000145

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Available at: https://www.usmle.org/sites/default/files/2021-10/Step_1_Sample_Items.pdf, updated June 2022 (Accessed February 20, 2023).

9. Holmboe E, Rizzolo MA, Sachdeva AK, Rosenberg M, Ziv A. Simulation-based assessment and the regulation of healthcare professionals. Simul Healthc. (2011) 6 Suppl:S58–62. doi: 10.1097/SIH.0b013e3182283bd7

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Butterworth JS, Reppert EH. Auscultatory acumen in the general medical population. JAMA. (1960) 174(1):32–4. doi: 10.1001/jama.1960.03030010034009

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Germanakis I, Petridou ET, Varlamis G, Matsoukis IL, Papadopoulou-Legbelou K, Kalmanti M. Skills of primary healthcare physicians in paediatric cardiac auscultation. Acta Paediatr. (2013) 102(2):e74. doi: 10.1111/apa.12062

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Vukanovic-Criley JM, Criley S, Warde CM, Boker JR, Guevara-Matheus L, Churchill WH, et al. Competency in cardiac examination skills in medical students, trainees, physicians, and faculty: a multicenter study. Arch Intern Med. (2006) 166(6):610–6. doi: 10.1001/archinte.166.6.610

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Roy D, Sargeant J, Gray J, Hoyt B, Allen M, Fleming M. Helping family physicians improve their cardiac auscultation skills with an interactive CD-ROM. J Contin Educ Health Prof. (2002) 22(3):152–9. doi: 10.1002/chp.1340220304

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Barrett MJ, Kuzma MA, Seto TC, Richards P, Mason D, Barrett DM, et al. The power of repetition in mastering cardiac auscultation. Am J Med. (2006) 119(1):73–5. doi: 10.1016/j.amjmed.2004.12.036

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Mattioli LF, Belmont JM, Davis AM. Effectiveness of teaching cardiac auscultation to residents during an elective pediatric cardiology rotation. Pediatr Cardiol. (2008) 29(6):1095–100. doi: 10.1007/s00246-008-9265-5

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Gaskin PR, Owens SE, Talner NS, Sanders SP, Li JS. Clinical auscultation skills in pediatric residents. Pediatrics. (2000) 105(6):1184–7. doi: 10.1542/peds.105.6.1184

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Kumar K, Thompson WR. Evaluation of cardiac auscultation skills in pediatric residents. Clin Pediatr (Phila). (2013) 52(1):66–73. doi: 10.1177/0009922812466584

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Vukanovic-Criley JM, Hovanesyan A, Criley SR, Ryan TJ, Plotnick G, Mankowitz K, et al. Confidential testing of cardiac examination competency in cardiology and noncardiology faculty and trainees: a multicenter study. Clin Cardiol. (2010) 33(12):738–45. doi: 10.1002/clc.20851

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Donato AA, Kaliyadan AG, Wasser T. Self-directed study using MP3 players to improve auscultation proficiency of physicians: a randomized, controlled trial. J Contin Educ Health Prof. (2014) 34(2):131–8. doi: 10.1002/chp.21220

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Mangione S, Nieman L, Gracely E, Kaye D. The teaching and practice of cardiac auscultation during internal medicine and cardiology training. Ann Intern Med. (1993) 119:47–54. doi: 10.7326/0003-4819-119-1-199307010-00009

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Barrett MJ, Lacey CS, Sekara AE, Linden EA, Gracely EJ. Mastering cardiac murmurs: the power of repetition. Chest. (2004) 126(2):470–5. doi: 10.1378/chest.126.2.470

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Mangrulkar R, Judge R, Chapman C, Grum C, Hagan P, Westfall J, et al. Professional skill builder: mastering cardiac auscultation in under 4 h. MedEdPORTAL. (2017) 13:10577. doi: 10.15766/mep_2374-9265.10577

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Issenberg SB, Petrusa ER, McGaghie WC, Felner JM, Waugh RA, Nash IS, et al. Effectiveness of a computer-based system to teach bedside cardiology. Acad Med. (1999) 74(10 Suppl):S93–5. doi: 10.1097/00001888-199910000-00051

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Rajakumar K, Weisse M, Rosas A, Gunel E, Pyles L, Neal WA, et al. Comparative study of clinical evaluation of heart murmurs by general pediatricians and pediatric cardiologists. Clin Pediatr (Phila). (1999) 38(9):511–8. doi: 10.1177/000992289903800902

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Haney I, Ipp M, Feldman W, McCrindle BW. Accuracy of clinical assessment of heart murmurs by office based (general practice) paediatricians. Arch Dis Child. (1999) 81(5):409–12. doi: 10.1136/adc.81.5.409

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Mahnke CB, Nowalk A, Hofkosh D, Zuberbuhler JR, Law YM. Comparison of two educational interventions on pediatric resident auscultation skills. Pediatrics. (2004) 113(5):1331–5. doi: 10.1542/peds.113.5.1331

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Dhuper S, Vashist S, Shah N, Sokal M. Improvement of cardiac auscultation skills in pediatric residents with training. Clin Pediatr (Phila). (2007) 46(3):236–40. doi: 10.1177/0009922806290028

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Finley JP, Caissie R, Nicol P, Hoyt B. International trial of online auditory training programme for distinguishing innocent and pathological murmurs. J Paediatr Child Health. (2015) 51(8):815–9. doi: 10.1111/jpc.12839

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Knowles MS, Holton EF, Swanson RA. The Adult Learner: The Definitive Classic in Adult Education and Human Resource Development. 6th ed. Burlington, MA, USA: Elsevier (2005). p. 378.

30. Mayer RE. Cognitive theory and the design of multimedia instruction: an example of the two-way street between cognition and instruction. New Dir Teach Learn. (2002) 2002(89):55–71. doi: 10.1002/tl.47

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Mackie A. Auscultation: a review of teaching methods. In: Finley JP, editors. Teaching Heart Auscultation to Health Professionals. Ottawa, ON: Canadian Pediatric Cardiology Association (2011). p. 13–31.

32. Finley JP. Teaching Heart Auscultation to Health Professionals. Ottawa, ON: Canadian Pediatric Cardiology Association (2011). p. 122.

33. Ericsson KA. Deliberate practice and the acquisition and maintenance of expert performance in medicine and related domains. Acad Med. (2004) 79(10 Suppl):S70–81. doi: 10.1097/00001888-200410001-00022

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

34. De Meo R, Matusz PJ, Knebel J-F, Murray MM, Thompson WR, Clarke S. What makes medical students better listeners? Curr Biol. (2016) 26(13):R519–20. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2016.05.024

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Shimojo S, Shams L. Sensory modalities are not separate modalities: plasticity and interactions. Curr Opin Neurobiol. (2001) 11(4):505–9. doi: 10.1016/S0959-4388(00)00241-5

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Mayer RE. Multimedia Learning. 3rd ed. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press (2021). p. 433.

37. Sweller J. Element interactivity and intrinsic, extraneous, and germane cognitive load. Educ Psychol Rev. (2010) 22:123–38. doi: 10.1007/s10648-010-9128-5

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

38. van Merrienboer J, Sweller J. Cognitive load theory in health professional education: design principles and strategies. Med Educ. (2010) 44:85–93. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2923.2009.03498.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Kalyuga S, Chandler P, Sweller J. When redundant on-screen text in multimedia technical instruction can interfere with learning. Hum Factors. (2004) 46(3):567–81. doi: 10.1518/hfes.46.3.567.50405

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Adesope OO, Nesbit JC. Verbal redundancy in multimedia learning environments: a meta-analysis. J Educ Psychol. (2012) 104(1):250–63. doi: 10.1037/a0026147

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Fraser K, Ma I, Teteris E, Baxter H, Wright B, McLaughlin K. Emotion, cognitive load and learning outcomes during simulation training. Med Educ. (2012) 46(11):1055–62. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2923.2012.04355.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Nielsen T, Mølgaard H, Ringsted C, Eika B. The development of a new cardiac auscultation test: how do screening and diagnostic skills differ? Med Teach. (2010) 32(1):56–61. doi: 10.3109/01421590802572767

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Binka EK, Lewin LO, Gaskin PR. Small steps in impacting clinical auscultation of medical students. Glob Pediatric Health. (2016) 3:2333794X16669013. doi: 10.1177/2333794X16669013

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Power JE, Toft LEB, Barrett M. The murmur online learning experience (MOLE) curriculum improves medical students’ ability to correctly identify cardiac murmurs. MedEdPORTAL. (2020) 16:10904. doi: 10.15766/mep_2374-8265.10943

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Veloski JJ, Rabinowitz HK, Robeson MR, Young PR. Patients don't present with five choices: an alternative to multiple-choice tests in assessing physicians’ competence. Acad Med. (1999) 74(5):539–46. doi: 10.1097/00001888-199905000-00022

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Finley JP. Assessment of performance. In: Finley JP, editors. Teaching Heart Auscultation to Health Professionals. Ottawa, ON: Canadian Pediatric Cardiology Association (2011). p. 32–43.

Keywords: cardiac auscultation, multimedia learning, pediatric murmurs, teaching auscultation, children's heart murmurs

Citation: Doroshow RW, Aldrich J, Dorner R, Lyons L and McCarter R (2024) A randomized, controlled trial of an innovative, multimedia instructional program for acquiring auditory skill in identifying pediatric heart murmurs. Front. Pediatr. 11:1283306. doi: 10.3389/fped.2023.1283306

Received: 25 August 2023; Accepted: 29 December 2023;
Published: 16 January 2024.

Edited by:

Benjamin Frank, University of Colorado, United States

Reviewed by:

Megan McFerson SooHoo, University of Colorado, United States
Daniel McLennan, Children's Hospital of Wisconsin, United States

© 2024 Doroshow, Aldrich, Dorner, Lyons and McCarter. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Robin W. Doroshow rdorosho@childrensnational.org

Abbreviations PCP, primary care provider; GWU, George Washington University School of Medicine and Health Sciences; CDIP differentiating between innocent and pathological with at least moderate confidence; DIP, differentiating between innocent and pathological; CA, cardiac auscultation.

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.