Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Pediatr., 30 November 2023
Sec. Neonatology

Neurodevelopmental outcome of low-risk moderate to late preterm infants at 18 months

  • 1INFANT Centre, University College Cork, Cork, Ireland
  • 2Department of Paediatrics and Child Health, University College Cork, Cork, Ireland
  • 3Department of Neonatology, Cork University Maternity Hospital, Cork, Ireland

Background: Of the 15 million preterm births that occur worldwide each year, approximately 80% occur between 32 and 36 + 6 weeks gestational age (GA) and are defined as moderate to late preterm (MLP) infants. This percentage substantiates a need for a better understanding of the neurodevelopmental outcome of this group

Aim: To describe neurodevelopmental outcome at 18 months in a cohort of healthy low-risk MLP infants admitted to the neonatal unit at birth and to compare the neurodevelopmental outcome to that of a healthy term-born infant group.

Study design and method: This single-centre observational study compared the neurodevelopmental outcome of healthy MLP infants to a group of healthy term control (TC) infants recruited during the same period using the Griffith's III assessment at 18 months.

Results: Seventy-five MLP infants and 92 TC infants were included. MLP infants scored significantly lower in the subscales: Eye-hand coordination (C), Personal, Social and Emotional Development (D), Gross Motor Development (E) and General Developmental (GD) (p < 0.001 for each) and Foundations of Learning (A), (p = 0.004) in comparison to the TC infant group with Cohen's d effect sizes ranging from 0.460 to 0.665. There was no statistically significant difference in mean scores achieved in subscale B: Language and Communication between groups (p = 0.107).

Conclusion: MLP infants are at risk of suboptimal neurodevelopmental outcomes. Greater surveillance of the neurodevelopmental trajectory of this group of at-risk preterm infants is required.

Introduction

The World Health Organisation (WHO) has acknowledged that whilst reducing mortality for newborns is the priority there is also a need to prioritise improving health, psychosocial well-being and the learning potential of children, particularly in the early years of life (1). Over the past decades improvement in NICU care and technology has led to increased survival rates of preterm infants. However, despite this success, gains in survival have not been paralleled by improvements in morbidity.

Of the 15 million preterm births that occur worldwide each year, approximately 75% to 84% occur between 32 and 36 + 6 weeks gestational age (GA) and these infants are defined as moderate to late preterm (MLP) (2, 3). Current trends have shown that the rise in the rate of preterm birth is primarily attributed to the MLP infant group (4). This may in part reflect the increased use of artificial reproductive technology and its association with multiple pregnancies and preterm delivery and consequently an increased demand for neonatal unit services (5).

In the last six weeks of pregnancy, there is a 35% to 40% increase in brain weight with a five-fold increase in brain volume (6). After birth, the MLP infant is no longer protected by the uterine environment and this critical period of accelerated development takes place externally in the NICU or postnatal wards (7). A challenging external environment may influence neural connectivity (8). Disruption of the typical process of brain development may expose preterm infants to long-term neurodevelopmental impairment. Whilst a more positive outcome is associated with the low-risk uncomplicated preterm infant (9, 10) defining “uncomplicated” may prove problematic as preterm birth itself is atypical and its aetiology may involve deteriorating fetal or maternal health, which in itself may lead to suboptimal outcomes. Maturity at birth has a direct impact on outcome; the lower the GA of the preterm infant, the greater the impact on mortality and morbidity (1114). Low birth weight associated with preterm birth further increases risk factors for the preterm infant (15).

The availability of neurodevelopmental outcome data for healthy MLP infants is often hindered by the variation in follow-up between institutions internationally (16) and the belief that these infants are low risk, and hence require little developmental surveillance. This limits our ability to assess the prevalence of impairments in this healthy MLP infant group. This study aims to establish the natural neurodevelopmental baseline scores for a healthy homogenous group of MLP infants admitted to the neonatal unit at birth. Secondly, we aim to compare the neurodevelopmental outcome of the MLP group to that of a healthy term-born infant's group at 18 months postmenstrual age, recruited during the same period.

Methods

Cork University Maternity Hospital is the largest Maternity Hospital in the south of the Republic of Ireland, serving the local and surrounding areas, and is a referral centre for high risk pregnancies.

Participants

All participating MLP infants were recruited from the neonatal unit between July 2017 and September 2018 as part of a single-centre observational sleep study. Inclusion criteria required infants to be born between 32 and 36 + 6 weeks GA, admitted to the neonatal unit at birth, a normal physical/neurological exam for GA conducted by the clinical team at birth, clinically stable, no actual or suspected chromosomal abnormalities and a normal cranial ultrasound if performed.

As this study places a narrow focus on the natural neurodevelopment of a healthy MLP infant group, this study specifically excluded infants with a complicated neonatal stay requiring an escalation in medical care including surgery, treatment of sepsis, requiring prolonged invasive respiratory support, requiring a blood transfusion or persistent hypoglycaemia or hyponatraemia. There is no universally accepted threshold for safe blood glucose concentrations (17) despite the fact that hypoglycaemia has been identified as a risk factor for suboptimal development (18). No MLP infant had a diagnosis of hypoglycaemia based on discharge summary reports. Transient hypoglycaemia may have occurred as part of the normal adaption to extrauterine life (19) in some infants but hypoglycaemia was not a concern for any MLP infant (local protocol defines hypoglycaemia as laboratory blood sugar level <2.8 mmol/L (45 mg/dl).

Infants requiring treatment for physiological jaundice which resolved with phototherapy were included in this study and were identified in the discharge summary report. No MLP infant required prolonged phototherapy or phototherapy for reasons other than normal physiological jaundice. All infant's enteral feeds progressed in volume as per local feeding regime and were tolerated well, with full oral feeds (breast or bottle) established for a minimum of 48 h prior to discharge. In 2016, the year prior to the commencement of this study, 64,097 infants were born in Ireland and 3.8% were multiple births. Forty six percent of multiple births were born between 32 and 36 weeks GA (20). We therefore included MLP infants from singleton and multiple pregnancies in this study. This percentage has since increased to 49.8% in 2020 (21).

During the same period, a cohort of healthy term-born infants (37–42 weeks birth GA) were recruited on the postnatal wards as part of a randomised clinical trial of a massage intervention (ENRICH study—Clinicaltrials.gov NCT03381027). The non-intervention arm of this clinical trial served as the term control (TC) group for the current study. Parents of infants with no metabolic or genetic anomalies, not requiring specialist ongoing care and not requiring admission to the neonatal unit, were invited to participate. The TC group excluded infants from multiple pregnancies.

Outcome

As per hospital criteria, cranial ultrasounds (CRUS) were routinely performed on MLP infants <1,500 g or if clinically indicated.

Infants from both groups (MLP and TC) returned to the INFANT Early Life Lab for a neurodevelopmental assessment at 18 months PMA (+2 weeks). Identifying, quantifying and predicting suboptimal development requires a valid and reliable standardised neurodevelopmental assessment tool. The Griffiths lll mental developmental scale (MDS), is a norm-based validated neurodevelopmental assessment and was used in this study. The Griffith's III was validated on a representative sample of infants born in Ireland and the UK (22). Children were seated at a low table beside their parent/guardian during the assessment which took on average approximately 45 min. The same group of experienced assessors performed assessments for both study groups. Due to the nature of the study and the assessment used, we were unable to blind assessors to group membership.

The Griffiths lll provides an overall general neurodevelopmental score and an individual profile of strengths and weaknesses across five areas or subscales of neurodevelopment (23). Subscales of development include: Foundations of Learning (curiosity, communication creativity, learning and memory), Hearing/speech and communication, Eye-hand coordination coordination, Personal–social performance, and Gross Motor development. Each subscale is equally weighted and is a complete and separate scale in itself. Developmental scores were calculated for each subscale of development based on the mean score obtained for each group (24). Input from primary caregivers is required for this assessment with some subscales requiring more input than others (25). Caregiver input confirms the infant's ability to carry out specific activities in the home that may not be completed on the day of assessment. In calculating scores achieved with Griffiths lll, “ratio transformation” is used which divides developmental age in months by psychologists age giving rise to slightly different means and standard deviations for each subscale of development (26).

Using the Irish Central Statistics Office criteria (27) we categorised employment as: professional, lower professional, manager/technical/administration, non-manual or skilled. Data relating to maternal occupation for the TC group data was obtained from a maternal questionnaire completed as part of the original clinical trial. Data relating to maternal occupation of the MLP group was obtained from the maternal electronic health record system if documented. No data was available on paternal occupation.

Research ethics

Ethical approval was granted by the Clinical Research Ethics Committee (CREC) of the Cork Teaching Hospitals. Parents of all infants included gave informed consent for participation and publication of the results. Consent was sought as per good clinical practice (GCP) ICH GCP E6 (R2) guidelines. Data that was recorded followed General Data Protection and Regulations (GDPR 2016/679).

Statistical analysis

Demographic and clinical characteristics of the MLP and TC groups were described using the median and inter-quartile range (IQR) for continuous variables and frequencies and percentages for categorical variables. Comparisons of demographical variables between the MLP and TC groups were made using the Mann–Whitney U-test for continuous variables and using the chi-squared test or Fisher's exact test (in the case of small expected counts) for categorical variables.

Griffiths DQ scores were described using the mean and standard deviation (SD) and the independent samples t-test was used to compare scores between MLP and TC groups. Cohen's d formula was used to determine the standardized mean difference between the MLP and TC groups. A d of 0.2 was considered a small effect size, 0.5 a medium effect size and 0.8 a large effect size. To adjust for the potential confounding effect of sex, linear regression analysis was performed with sex and group included as independent variables in the model. As the type of pregnancy (singleton vs. multiple) was also a potential confounder and there were no multiples included in the TC group, linear regression analysis, unadjusted and adjusted for sex was also performed using the singleton infants only.

Within the MLP infant group analysis, a comparison of DQ scores between two groups (e.g., sex and pregnancy type) was performed using an independent samples t-test and more than two groups [e.g., gestational age groups (week)] using a one-way ANOVA. Infants in the MLP group were defined as having a normal outcome if their general development DQ score was within 1 SD of the mean score of the healthy term control group [mean (SD): 119 (10)].

Statistical analysis was performed using IBM SPSS Statistics (version 25.0, IBM Corp, Armonk, NY, USA). All tests were two-sided and a p-value <0.05 was considered statistically significant.

Results

One hundred and one MLP infants and ninty seven term control (TC) infants were recruited at birth and enrolled in this study. The median (IQR) birth GA (weeks) was 34.0 (33.0–35.0) for the MLP group and 39.9(38.9–40.9) for the TC group. There was no difference in maternal age between groups (p = 0.785). As expected the TC group had significantly higher birthweight and larger head circumference than the MLP group (p < 0.001 for both) (Table 1). A higher percentage of infants in both groups were male (MLP 52.5% and TC 57.7%). Participants from TC and MLP groups were predominantly Irish Caucasian (89.7% and 93.1% respectively) which reflects the local population. Whilst 96.7% of mothers of TC group were employed, maternal occupation was not documented for two participants (2.2%). In the MLP group, 74.7% of mothers were employed with maternal occupation not documented in 18.7% of the group. There was no significant difference in employment category between TC and MLP infant groups (p = 0.784).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Demographics.

Within the MLP group, 52.5% (53/101) were from singleton pregnancies. MLP infants from a singleton pregnancy(n = 53) had significantly higher birthweight than infants from a multiple pregnancy(n = 48) (median (IQR): 2.27(1.95–2.65) kg vs. 1.97(1.78–2.14) kg, p < 0.001); however, this difference in weight between infants from a singleton(n = 36 and multiple pregnancy(n = 35 did not persist at 18 months (median(IQR): 11.29(10.80–12.49) kg vs. 11.37(10.60–12.24) kg, p = 0.666). In the MLP group, nine infants had a birth weight < 10th centile for GA i.e., small for gestational age. Cranial ultrasounds were performed on three infants (all <1,500 g), as per local guidelines, all of which were normal prior to discharge.

At 18 months (+2 weeks) PMA 74.3% (75/101) of the MLP infant group returned for follow up. The large fall out rate (n = 26) may in part have been associated with Covid 19 and some families chose to withdraw and some parents were not contactable or known to have moved overseas. Assessments were delayed for a number of MLP infants (n = 14). These infants were assessed at 22 months PMA as soon as restrictions were lifted and Griffiths scores were adjusted according to PMA. There was no significant difference in birth demographics between MLP infants who were lost to follow up and those who returned at 18 months. Follow up of the TC group was completed prior to the global pandemic and hence study retention was not impacted by same. In the TC group 94.8% (92/97) returned for a Griffiths developmental assessment at 18 months(+2 weeks) (Table 1).

Comparison of neurodevelopmental outcome between MLP and TC groups

General development DQ scores and the subscales of development scores for the MLP and TC groups are described and compared in Table 2. The mean(SD) GD DQ score in our healthy TC group was 119.45 (10.78) and the MLP infant group was 112.12(13.67). DQ values for the TC group were significantly higher than published values of 109(19). These higher scores seen in the control group may be due to the fact that these infants were low risk, term infants with normal Apgar scores and did not require admission to the NICU. It may also be due to the fact that this is one of the first studies published using the Griffiths III and may present valuable data on real world use in the Irish population.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Neurodevelopmental outcome of term control (TC) and moderate to late preterm (MLP) infant group at 18 months.

The mean DQ score in all subscales of development and overall general development were significantly higher in the TC group compared to the MLP group with the exception of subscale B: Language and Communication. The greatest differences were in Gross Motor development (p < 0.001, Cohen's d = 0.665) and Eye-hand co-ordination (p < 0.001, Cohen's d = 0.648). Being within +/− 1 SD from the mean is considered normal for data.

Adjustment for sex

As sex was a potential confounder in the relationship between DQ scores and group, an adjusted analysis was performed (Table 3). Larger differences were found between the two groups in the adjusted analysis compared to the unadjusted analysis.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Comparison of mean scores at 18 months between TC and MLP infants unadjusted and adjusted for sex.

After adjustment for sex, mean scores were significantly higher in the TC group compared to the MLP group for all subscales, including subscale B: Language and Communication (p = 0.028).

Adjustment for pregnancy type (singleton v multiple) and sex

Pregnancy type was a potential confounder in the relationship between DQ scores and group. As the TC group only included singleton infants, we performed an analysis restricted to singleton infants (n = 92 in the TC group and n = 38 in the MLP group, Table 4). In the unadjusted and adjusted analysis, mean scores were significantly higher in the TC group compared to the MLP group for the general development DQ score and all subscales of development except for subscale B: Language and Communication.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Comparison at 18 months between TC (n = 92) and MLP (n = 38) groups, singleton infants only.

MLP group only—investigation of variables associated with neurodevelopmental outcome

There were no significant differences in outcomes by birth gestational age group. (Table 5) Female infants outperformed male infants subscale B: Language and Communication (p < 0.001), subscale C: Eye-hand coordination (p = 0.003), subscale D: Personal, Social and Emotional (p < 0.001), and overall general development (GD) (p < 0.001). (Table 5) There was no difference in overall general developmental (GD) scores between infants born from singleton and multiple pregnancies (p = 0.319). Significant differences by pregnancy type were only evident in subscale B: Language and Communication (p = 0.040) in which singleton infants outperformed infants born from a multiple pregnancy. All SGA infants, (four infants from singleton pregnancies and 5 infants from multiple pregnancies) achieved normal Griffiths III General Development DQ score at 18 months.

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Relationships between the degree of prematurity (GA week group), sex, pregnancy type and outcome of MLP infants at 18 months, n = 75.

Discussion

In two carefully categorised prospective healthy cohorts we have shown that MLP infants had significantly lower scores compared to TC infants in the Griffiths lll MDS at 18 months. This study shows that MLP infants are vulnerable to suboptimal neurodevelopment. There were no significant differences in scores found in Subscale B (Language and Communication) between the two groups (p = 0.107) however, when we controlled for sex, a significant difference was evident between the groups (p = 0.028). In a study by Putnick et al. inclusive of very preterm infants (n = 204), moderate-late preterm (n = 276) and term born infants (n = 268), language was assessed at 5 months, 20 months, 4 years, 6 years and 8 years of age (28). Very preterm children consistently performed less well than term-born children, with moderate-late preterm children also identified as being at risk for poorer language performance compared to term-born children. By comparison, Perez-Pereira et al. compared language development of low risk preterm infants (n = 150, mean GA of 32.62 weeks) and term born children (n = 49, mean GA of 39.70 weeks) up to 30 months and concluded that low risk preterm children were not at risk of delayed language development (29).

Both MLP and TC infant groups scored high in Subscale E (Gross Motor). However, statistically significant differences were evident between the groups (p < 0.001) with the TC group out-performing the MLP infant group. When the MLP group was subdivided by pregnancy type, differences were evident in Subscale B (Language and Communication) in which MLP infants from singleton pregnancies significantly outperformed MLP infants born from a multiple pregnancy (p = 0.040). A systematic review comparing the outcome of infants from twin and singleton pregnancies described differences in language and communication but proposed that the likely cause was due to the parental demands of managing two infants (30). Thorpe identified language delay in twins as mild but prevalent especially in male infants (31). It is not certain if low birth weight in multiple pregnancies may be a potential confounder which is associated with 60% of twin pregnancies (32).

Capobianco et al. compared the outcome of preterm infants (average birth GA of 32 weeks and birth weight of 2,200 g) with no neurological sequela (n = 20) and a normal Bayley's lll score(>85) at 18 and 24 months with term born infants (average birth GA of 39 weeks and birth weight of 3,500 g) (n = 20). The preterm infant group demonstrated significantly slower language acquisition but these differences were only significant at 16 months (p < 0.001) and 18 months (p < 0.005) but not at 20, 22, 24 months of age (33). However, in the Copabianco study, assessments were based on chronological age rather than corrected age. Using the Bayley's ll developmental assessment, Gould et al. described differences in scores achieved by preterm infants (n = 554) based on chronological age vs. corrected age as 17.3 points lower on the mental scale (79.5 vs. 96.8 respectively) and 11.8 points lower on the motor scale (84.8 vs. 96.6, respectively) at 18 months (34).

In the MLP group, we did not find any significant differences in outcome related to birth GA. This may be because of low numbers at each gestational age group meaning that we were underpowered to examine this effect. In a similar study investigating the neurodevelopmental outcome and social-emotional competence of MLP infants vs. a term control infant group at 2 years, Cheong et al. also found little evidence of an association with gestational age (7).

As infants in both studies had an uncomplicated neonatal stay / hospital stay on post-natal wards, factors such as suboptimal brain development and/or a high frequency of mild brain injuries (35), maternal well-being, obstetric/fetal complications warrant consideration in explaining the differences in developmental outcome between the groups. Pre-eclampsia, male sex and interventional delivery have been identified as antenatal/perinatal risk factors for poor school performance, which Johnson et al. also identified as independent risk factors for cognitive impairment (14).

Other determinants of neurobehavior and intellectual development include level of parental education, socioeconomic status, cultural background and maternal mental health conditions which may be considered additional risks for suboptimal outcome (3641).

Socioeconomic metrics may be derived from many different aspects including maternal education (40). Maternal education is the most strongly associated dimension of a child's neurocognitive development (42). Detailed data on maternal education were not available for our MLP cohort. Of those employed similar profiles of maternal occupation were seen, with 71.4% of mothers of MLP infants identified as professionals requiring a minimum of a university degree whilst 64.0% of TC group fell into the same category. These high percentages may in part reflect the Organisation for Economic Co-operation and Development (OECD) findings that 60% of women in Ireland between 25 and 34 years and 51% of women between 25 and 64 years have a tertiary education (43). It also may reflect the local community of 210,000 persons from which the cohort was recruited which is home to world leading information technology and software development organisations, large pharmaceutical companies, the largest acute tertiary hospital outside the country's capital and a high ranking university, all of which have high employment of professionals. Finally, it may also be reflective of the professional mothers who agree to take part in birth cohort studies, which is generally higher than the general population. We have reported similar high rates of tertiary education in a previous cohort study (44).

As 18.7% of mothers in MLP group (vs. 2.2% of TC group) had an undocumented occupation this led to employment analysis of a smaller group (n = 56) of MLP infants. This may have impacted the differences in percentage of employed professionals in the MLP infant group (71.4%) versus TC group 64.0% (57/89). It is reassuring that the differences in neurodevelopmental outcome are not due to an imbalance in maternal employment between the two groups (p = 0.784).

The results of our study are consistent with those of Johnson et al. (n = 638 MLP infants and n = 765 TC infants) (14) describing female infants outperforming male infants in the MLP group with significantly higher scores in overall general development (p < 0.001). However, the Johnson study was based on parental questionnaires in comparison to our study which used a well-validated neurodevelopmental assessment tool.

A gender disparity is evident in the literature with male infants having a greater mortality and respiratory morbidity than female infants which is not explained by neonatal factors (45). In a review of outcomes of male and female preterm infants, O’Driscoll et al. described an increased vulnerability of male preterm infants which they suggest is likely to be multifactorial and perhaps includes hormonal and genetic factors and immunological differences between males and females (46). Romeo et al. described significant gender differences in low-risk preterm infant groups (n = 69 very preterm and n = 71 late preterm infants) and no gender differences amongst term born infants (n = 48) based on the scores achieved in the Bayley's scales of Infant Development, 2nd edition at two years (47). However no gender differences were reported by Romeo et al. at 12 and 24 months using the Griffiths ll standardised assessment tool which the authors suggested was attributed to a higher ratio of female to male participants (F:M = 1.6:1) (48).

The risk of preterm birth and infant mortality rates are higher in multiple pregnancies (49, 50). Almost half of the MLP infants in this study were from a multiple pregnancy (49.3%). General development DQ scores of MLP infants from singleton (n = 38) and multiple pregnancies (n = 37) at 18 months were comparable (p = 0.319). Restricting the analysis to singletons in both groups did not change our findings (Table 4).

Overall cognitive performance of children born moderately preterm (MPT) and MLP have been described as consistently lower in comparison to the term-born group (51). A study by Cheong et al. (7) of MLP and term-born infants concluded that the greatest difference between the groups was in the language domain at 2 years (−11.4 (95% CI, −15.3 to −7.5). However, our study has not shown a statistically significant difference between the MLP and TC groups in the domain of language and communication (p = 0.107). These differences may in part be explained by the differences in the timing of the assessment and the assessment tool used; Cheong et al. used the Bayley Scales of Infant Development assessment (Third Edition) at 2 years corrected age.

Fernald and Marchman cite individual differences in language proficiency which can accelerate and abate at different time points for each infant (52). Our single assessment provides a snapshot of language development at one-time point at 18 months, which may be too early to detect significant language delay and the clinical relevance of our findings on long term outcomes has yet to be established. We accept that neurodevelopmental deficits may be quite subtle and frequently not become evident until preschool or school-age years and may change positively or negatively at an older age (53). All impairments, irrespective of degree, have the potential to limit professional options, life opportunities and overall quality of life.

In this study, MLP infants had significantly lower scores in the personal, social and emotional subscale of development (p < 0.001) with a moderate effect size (Cohen's d = 0.590) than the TC group. Research has shown an increased risk of delayed social competence, with social behavioural issues higher amongst MLP infants in comparison to term-born infants (54). MLP infants also have a predisposition to more challenging behavioural issues with an increased risk of hyperactivity and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) (51). However, the association between prematurity and hyperactivity has not been firmly established (55). Children with learning, social, motor and sensory impairments that challenge their performance prior to or during school-going years may be considered to have special educational needs (SEN). Increased SEN are well described in MLP -born groups (56). Odd et al. identified MLP infants as being 50% more likely to have SEN requiring support in comparison to healthy term-born infants (57).

A strong correlation has been shown between prematurity and a diagnosis of cerebral palsy (CP) with the absolute risk of CP increasing with decreasing birth GA. Pierrat et al. reported that 1.0% of infants born between 32 and 34 weeks GA weeks (n = 1,187) had a diagnosis of CP at 2 years of age (58). Although more mature preterm infants generally have better outcomes, MLP infants have a six times higher prevalence of CP than children born at term (7 per 1,000 vs. 1.1 per 1,000 live births) (59). This may be related to the incidence of brain lesions in MLP infants described previously on cranial ultrasound (35) the clinical relevance of which warrants further study including long-term follow-up.

Our study has shown that the greatest difference between the MLP and TC groups was in the area of gross motor development (p < 0.001, Cohen's d = 0.665) and eye-hand co-ordination (p < 0.001, Cohen's d = 0.648) with a moderate effect size for each subscale. In this study no MLP infants had a diagnosis of CP at 18 months based on neurological exam nor were there any concerns about motor impairment. However, mild motor impairment may only become apparent beyond 18 months. Williams et al. suggest that motor impairment (either CP or non-CP e.g. Developmental Coordination Disorder) may be subtle and not evident until early childhood years and may be the most common form of impairment in the preterm population (60).

Strengths and limitations

This study provides robust data to describe the developmental outcome of a large group of healthy MLP infants. Homogeneity is an important factor for identifying norms for specific groups of preterm children and a major strength of this study. All MLP infants had an uncomplicated journey through the neonatal unit with no neurological concerns and were considered healthy at discharge. Both the MLP and TC groups were recruited over a similar period of time (2017–2018) and infants were assessed by the same group of highly experienced assessors at the same time point of 18 months PMA with the exception of some MLP infants with a delayed assessment due to Covid restrictions.

Assessing infants based on PMA or corrected age decreases the transient developmental gap that MLP infants experience whilst they catch up to infants born at term (61). A failure to correct for GA at birth has implications for intervention programmes. Differences in scores may assist parents/schools in decision-making relating to the age of starting school. Placing the child in the correct year for PMA rather than the school year based on calendar age has led to greater academic success (57). The MLP infant group included infants from singleton and multiple pregnancies. With close to half MLP infants born from a multiple pregnancy, inclusion of multiples in our MLP group ensures a true representative group.

A neurological exam was normal in the MLP group and infants were considered healthy prior to discharge. At the time of this study (2017–2018) healthy preterm infants > 32 weeks had a cranial ultrasound performed if < 1,500 g (n = 3) or neurological concerns (n = 0) as CRUS was not standard practice in the care of MLP in our unit at the time of study recruitment. Boswinkel et al. identified minor brain lesions on CRUS and /or MRI in over 30% of MLP infants (35). As this was not standard practice in our unit at the time of study recruitment, we cannot rule out the presence of minor brain abnormalities in the MLP group, which may have influenced neurodevelopmental outcome. Neurological status was based on clinical neurological examination. Infants did not have a structured neurological exam such as the Hammersmith Infant Neurological Exam at follow up appointments in neonatal clinics. This is a limitation of this study. However, infants did have a clinical neurological examination by a neonatal consultant in outpatients and if any concerns arose, infants would have been referred for further assessment and/or intervention.

Participants were recruited from a single research site reflecting the local population. As a result, there was a lack of diversity in our population which limits the generalisability of our findings.

Follow up of the TC group was completed prior to Covid restrictions. These restrictions may be considered an unavoidable limitation of this study resulting in a delay in the assessment of a small group of MLP infants (n = 14). Multiple births were an exclusion-criteria for the TC group. All MLP participants were admitted to the neonatal unit to the exclusion of those cared for in postnatal wards. This may have had implications in terms of the level of stress or sensory exposure for the MLP infant in comparison to the TC group (62).

Research has shown that neurodevelopmental delay is more prevalent amongst MLP infants from disadvantaged socioeconomic groups. As this data was not available in our study, we utilised maternal education levels (where documented) as a proxy for socioeconomic status. This data was not recorded for 18.7% of MLP infants. Another possible limitation includes the potential for bias in scoring infants in neurodevelopmental assessments as assessors were not blinded to the study group.

Future research

There is limited data available on long-term neurodevelopmental outcome of MLP infants with an uncomplicated neonatal stay and a normal neurological exam on discharge. Institutional practices relating to pre-discharge screening including CRUS or MRI and post discharge neurological surveillance vary (16). Standardising screening practice based on findings from multicentre longitudinal research studies with expert input would identify best practice for follow up of this at risk group.

Accuracy in determining the presence and degree of suboptimal neurodevelopment is dependent on the reliability and validity of the assessment tool, the assessment time point and the experience of the assessors. Hence identifying a screening tool(s) and screening intervals appropriate to all infant groups is a priority. Surveillance of preterm infants presents a unique opportunity for early diagnosis of neurodevelopmental delay and intervention with targeted therapies. As neurodevelopmental delay can be transitory or sustained we do not know what these suboptimal scores at 18 months will mean for this MLP infant group in the future. Early assessment is not always predictive of school-age cognitive functioning, long-term learning disabilities or academic achievement (63). Ongoing neurodevelopmental surveillance and sequential screening are especially pertinent to infants who have a previous abnormal assessment.

Conclusion

MLP infants are at higher risk of suboptimal outcome at 18 months in comparison to term-born infants. This study highlights the effect of moderate to late preterm birth on neurodevelopmental outcome. Our findings support the need for a broader effort for longitudinal surveillance of the MLP infant group including those who appear healthy.

Data availability statement

For this current study, it is not possible to share data sets. The clinical data are collected under written proxy consent from the participants' guardians/parents that did not include permission for sharing or open data. To be allowed to share these data under Irish Health Research Regulations we are required to re-consent families or obtain approval from the Health Regulation Consent Declaration Committee.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by Clinical Research Ethics Committee (CREC) of Cork Teaching Hospital. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. Written informed consent for participation in this study was provided by the participants’ legal guardians/next of kin.

Funding

The author(s) declare financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.

This work was supported by a research grant from Science Foundation Ireland (SFI) under Grant Number 12/RC/2272 and 15/SP/3091.

Author contributions

MR: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. DM: Formal analysis, Methodology, Supervision, Writing – review & editing. ED: Formal analysis, Methodology, Supervision, Writing – review & editing. SM: Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Methodology, Supervision, Writing – review & editing. VL: Data curation, Formal analysis, Methodology, Supervision, Writing – review & editing. GB: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Resources, Writing – review & editing.

Acknowledgments

Special thanks to the guardians/parents who gave consent for their infant to participate in this study. Thanks to the ENRICH study team at the INFANT Research Centre, UCC who collected data for the term control group in this study and performed the neurodevelopmental assessments for both groups.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

The author(s) declared that they were an editorial board member of Frontiers, at the time of submission. This had no impact on the peer review process and the final decision.

Publisher's note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

1. Lancaster GA, McCray G, Kariger P, Dua T, Titman A, Chandna J, et al. Creation of the WHO indicators of infant and young child development (IYCD): metadata synthesis across 10 countries. BMJ Global Health. (2018) 3(5):e000747. doi: 10.1136/bmjgh-2018-000747

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Walani SR. Global burden of preterm birth. Int J Gynaecol Obstet. (2020) 150(1):31–3. doi: 10.1002/ijgo.13195

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Townley Flores C, Gerstein A, Phibbs CS, Sanders LM. Short-term and long-term educational outcomes of infants born moderately and late preterm. J Pediatr. (2021) 232:31–7.e2. doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2020.12.070

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Boyle EM, Johnson S, Manktelow B, Seaton S, Draper E, Smith L, et al. Neonatal outcomes and delivery of care for infants born late preterm or moderately preterm: a prospective population-based study. Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed. (2015) 100(6):F479–85. doi: 10.1136/archdischild-2014-307347

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Moore AM, O’Brien K. Follow-up issues with multiples. Paediatr Child Health. (2006) 11(5):283–6. doi: 10.1093/pch/11.5.283

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Guihard-Costa AM, Larroche JC. Differential growth between the fetal brain and its infratentorial part. Early Hum Dev. (1990) 23(1):27–40. doi: 10.1016/0378-3782(90)90126-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Cheong JL, Doyle LW, Burnett AC, Lee KJ, Walsh JM, Potter CR, et al. Association between moderate and late preterm birth and neurodevelopment and social-emotional development at age 2 years. JAMA Pediatr. (2017) 171(4):e164805. doi: 10.1001/jamapediatrics.2016.4805

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Kinney HC. The near-term (late preterm) human brain and risk for periventricular leukomalacia: a review. Semin Perinatol. (2006) 30(2):81–8. doi: 10.1053/j.semperi.2006.02.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Gurka MJ, LoCasale-Crouch J, Blackman JA. Long-term cognition, achievement, socioemotional, and behavioral development of healthy late-preterm infants. JAMA Pediatr. (2010) 164(6):525–32. doi: 10.1001/archpediatrics.2010.83

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Field D, Boyle E, Draper E, Evans A, Johnson S, Khan K, et al. Towards reducing variations in infant mortality and morbidity: a population-based approach. Programme Grants for Applied Research. Southampton (UK): NIHR Journals Library (2016). p. 92–200. Available from: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/books/NBK349414/

Google Scholar

11. Joseph RM, O’Shea TM, Allred EN, Heeren T, Hirtz D, Jara H, et al. Neurocognitive and academic outcomes at age 10 years of extremely preterm newborns. Pediatrics. (2016) 137(4):e20154343. doi: 10.1542/peds.2015-4343

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Petrini JR, Dias T, McCormick MC, Massolo ML, Green NS, Escobar GJ. Increased risk of adverse neurological development for late preterm infants. J Pediatr. (2009) 154(2):169–76. doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2008.08.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Chan E, Leong P, Malouf R, Quigley MA. Long-term cognitive and school outcomes of late-preterm and early-term births: a systematic review. Child Care Health Dev. (2016) 42(3):297–312. doi: 10.1111/cch.12320

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Johnson S, Evans TA, Draper ES, Field DJ, Manktelow BN, Marlow N, et al. Neurodevelopmental outcomes following late and moderate prematurity: a population-based cohort study. Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed. (2015) 100(4):F301–8. doi: 10.1136/archdischild-2014-307684

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Manuck TA, Rice MM, Bailit JL, Grobman WA, Reddy UM, Wapner RJ, et al. Preterm neonatal morbidity and mortality by gestational age: a contemporary cohort. Am J Obstet Gynecol. (2016) 215(1):103.e1–.e14. doi: 10.1016/j.ajog.2016.01.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Krüse-Ruijter M, Boswinkel V, Consoli A, Nijholt I, Boomsma M, Vries L, et al. Neurological surveillance in moderate-late preterm infants—results from a Dutch–Canadian survey. Children. (2022) 9:846. doi: 10.3390/children9060846

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Shah R, Harding J, Brown J, McKinlay C. Neonatal glycaemia and neurodevelopmental outcomes: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Neonatology. (2019) 115(2):116–26. doi: 10.1159/000492859

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Kerstjens JM, Bocca-Tjeertes IF, de Winter AF, Reijneveld SA, Bos AF. Neonatal morbidities and developmental delay in moderately preterm-born children. Pediatrics. (2012) 130(2):e265–72. doi: 10.1542/peds.2012-0079

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Adamkin DH. Neonatal hypoglycemia. Semin Fetal Neonatal Med. (2017) 22(1):36–41. doi: 10.1016/j.siny.2016.08.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Health Care Pricing Office HSE. Perinatal Statistics Report Dublin, Ireland: Economic and Social Research Institute, Division HRaI; 2016. Contract No.: 978-1-78602-107-6).

21. Health Care Pricing Office HSE. The Perinatal Statistics Report. Dublin, Ireland: The Economic and Social Research Institute, HSE (2020). Available from: https://www.hpo.ie/latest_hipe_nprs_reports/NPRS_2019/Perinatal_Statistics_Report_2019.pdf

22. Green E SL, Bloomfield S, Cronje J, Foxcroft C, Hurter K, Lane H, et al. Griffiths lll scales of Child Development: ARICD. 3 edn ed. Oxford, UK: Hogrefe (2016). Available at: https://www.aricd.ac.uk/about-the-griffiths-scales/

23. Association for Research in Infant and Child Development. What is Griffiths III? (2016). Available from: https://www.aricd.ac.uk/about-the-griffiths-scales

24. Olson TL, Roggman LA, Innocenti MS. Developmental research methods with infants & young children. In: Benson JB, editors. Encyclopedia of infant and early childhood development (second edition). Oxford: Elsevier (2020). p. 456–68.

25. Jacklin L, Cockcroft K. The griffiths mental development scales: an overview and a consideration of their relevance for South Africa. In: Merwe A, Thatcher A, Kanjee A, Shuttleworth-Edwards A, Prinsloo C, Young C, et al., editors. Psychological assessment in South Africa: Research and applications. Witwatersrand, Johannesburg: Wits University Press; 2013. p. 169–85.

26. Ivens J, Martin N. A common metric for the griffiths scales. Arch Dis Child. (2002) 87(2):109–10. doi: 10.1136/adc.87.2.109

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Central Statistics Office C. Vital Statistics Yearly Summary 2020. Report. Available at: https://www.cso.ie: CSO; 2021 28th May 2021.

28. Putnick DL, Bornstein MH, Eryigit-Madzwamuse S, Wolke D. Long-Term stability of language performance in very preterm, moderate-late preterm, and term children. J Pediatr. (2017) 181:74–9. e3. doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2016.09.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Pérez-Pereira M, Fernández P, Gómez-Taibo ML, Resches M. Language development of low risk preterm infants up to the age of 30 months. Early Hum Dev. (2014) 90(10):649–56. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2014.08.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Trombetta T, Brustia P, Curti L, Caldarera AM, Gerino E, Rollè L. Twins’ and Singletons’ linguistic environment: a systematic review. Front Psychol. (2019) 10:2005. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Thorpe K. Twin children’s language development. Early Hum Dev. (2006) 82(6):387–95. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2006.03.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Martin JA, Osterman MJ, Kirmeyer SE, Gregory EC. Measuring gestational age in vital statistics data: transitioning to the obstetric estimate. Natl Vital Stat Rep. (2015) 64(5):1–20. PMID: 26047089

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

33. Capobianco M, Cerniglia L. Early language development in preterm children without neurological damage: a longitudinal study. F1000Res. (2017) 6:2169. doi: 10.12688/f1000research.13314.1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Gould JF, Fuss BG, Roberts RM, Collins CT, Makrides M. Consequences of using chronological age versus corrected age when testing cognitive and motor development in infancy and intelligence quotient at school age for children born preterm. PLoS One. (2021) 16(9):e0256824. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0256824

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Boswinkel V, Krüse-Ruijter MF, Nijboer-Oosterveld J, Nijholt IM, Edens MA, Mulder-de Tollenaer SM, et al. Incidence of brain lesions in moderate-late preterm infants assessed by cranial ultrasound and MRI: the BIMP-study. Eur J Radiol. (2021) 136:109500. doi: 10.1016/j.ejrad.2020.109500

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. ElHassan NO, Bai S, Gibson N, Holland G, Robbins JM, Kaiser JR. The impact of prematurity and maternal socioeconomic status and education level on achievement-test scores up to 8th grade. PLoS One. (2018) 13(5):e0198083. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0198083

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Eliner Y, Gulersen M, Chervenak FA, Lenchner E, Grunebaum A, Phillips K, et al. Maternal education and racial/ethnic disparities in nulliparous, term, singleton, vertex cesarean deliveries in the United States. AJOG Glob Rep. (2022) 2(1):100036. doi: 10.1016/j.xagr.2021.100036

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Ene D, Der G, Fletcher-Watson S, O'Carroll S, MacKenzie G, Higgins M, et al. Associations of socioeconomic deprivation and preterm birth with speech, language, and communication concerns among children aged 27 to 30 months. JAMA Netw Open. (2019) 2(9):e1911027. doi: 10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2019.11027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. To T, Guttmann A, Dick PT, Rosenfield JD, Parkin PC, Cao H, et al. What factors are associated with poor developmental attainment in young Canadian children? Can J Public Health. (2004) 95(4):258–63. doi: 10.1007/BF03405127

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Dunlop AL, Essalmi AG, Alvalos L, Breton C, Camargo CA, Cowell WJ, et al. Racial and geographic variation in effects of maternal education and neighborhood-level measures of socioeconomic status on gestational age at birth: findings from the ECHO cohorts. PLoS One. (2021) 16(1):e0245064. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0245064

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Connelly R, Platt L. Cohort profile: uK millennium cohort study (MCS). Int J Epidemiol. (2014) 43(6):1719–25. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyu001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Jackson M, Kiernan K, McLanahan S. Maternal education, changing family circumstances, and children’s skill development in the United States and UK. Ann Am Acad Pol Soc Sci. (2017) 674(1):59–84. doi: 10.1177/0002716217729471

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. OECD. Education at a glance OECD 2019. Paris: OECD publishing (2019).

44. Casey T, Thachuthara AJ, Fogarty L, Livingstone V, De Haan M, Marlow N, et al. Validation of a touchscreen assessment tool to screen for cognitive delay at 24 months. Dev Med Child Neurol. (2023) 65(9):1206–14. doi: 10.1111/dmcn.15555

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Peacock JL, Marston L, Marlow N, Calvert SA, Greenough A. Neonatal and infant outcome in boys and girls born very prematurely. Pediatr Res. (2012) 71(3):305–10. doi: 10.1038/pr.2011.50

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. O’Driscoll DN, McGovern M, Greene CM, Molloy EJ. Gender disparities in preterm neonatal outcomes. Acta Paediatr. (2018) 107:1494–9. doi: 10.1111/apa.14390

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Romeo DM, Brogna C, Sini F, Romeo MG, Cota F, Ricci D. Early psychomotor development of low-risk preterm infants: influence of gestational age and gender. Eur J Paediatr Neurol. (2016) 20(4):518–23. doi: 10.1016/j.ejpn.2016.04.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Romeo DM, Ricci M, Mirra F, Venezia I, Mallardi M, Pede E, et al. Longitudinal cognitive assessment in low-risk very preterm infants. Medicina (Kaunas). (2022) 58(1):133–41.35056441

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

49. Vasak B, Verhagen JJ, Koenen SV, Koster MP, de Reu PA, Franx A, et al. Lower perinatal mortality in preterm born twins than in singletons: a nationwide study from The Netherlands. Am J Obstet Gynecol. (2017) 216(2):161–e1-.e9. doi: 10.1016/j.ajog.2016.10.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Shah PE, Poehlmann J, Weeks HM, Spinelli M, Richards B, Suh J, et al. Developmental trajectories of late preterm infants and predictors of academic performance. Pediatr Res. (2023). [Epub ahead of print]

Google Scholar

51. Allotey J, Zamora J, Cheong-See F, Kalidindi M, Arroyo-Manzano D, Asztalos E, et al. Cognitive, motor, behavioural and academic performances of children born preterm: a meta-analysis and systematic review involving 64 061 children. BJOG. (2018) 125(1):16–25. doi: 10.1111/1471-0528.14832

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Fernald A, Marchman VA. Individual differences in lexical processing at 18 months predict vocabulary growth in typically developing and late-talking toddlers. Child Dev. (2012) 83(1):203–22. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01692.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Quigley MA, Poulsen G, Boyle E, Wolke D, Field D, Alfirevic Z, et al. Early term and late preterm birth are associated with poorer school performance at age 5 years: a cohort study. Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed. (2012) 97(3):F167–73. doi: 10.1136/archdischild-2011-300888

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Cheong J, Cameron KLI, Thompson D, Anderson PJ, Ranganathan S, Clark R, et al. Impact of moderate and late preterm birth on neurodevelopment, brain development and respiratory health at school age: protocol for a longitudinal cohort study (LaPrem study). BMJ Open. (2021) 11(1):e044491. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2020-044491

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Huddy CL, Johnson A, Hope PL. Educational and behavioural problems in babies of 32-35 weeks gestation. Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed. (2001) 85(1):F23–8. doi: 10.1136/fn.85.1.F23

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. van Baar AL, Vermaas J, Knots E, de Kleine MJ, Soons P. Functioning at school age of moderately preterm children born at 32 to 36 weeks’ gestational age. Pediatrics. (2009) 124(1):251–7. doi: 10.1542/peds.2008-2315

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Odd D, Evans D, Emond A. Preterm birth, age at school entry and long term educational achievement. PloS one. (2016) 11(5):e0155157–e. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0155157

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Pierrat V, Marchand-Martin L, Arnaud C, Kaminski M, Resche-Rigon M, Lebeaux C, et al. Neurodevelopmental outcome at 2 years for preterm children born at 22 to 34 weeks’ gestation in France in 2011: EPIPAGE-2 cohort study. Br Med J. (2017) 358:j3448. doi: 10.1136/bmj.j3448

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Himmelmann K, Hagberg G, Beckung E, Hagberg B, Uvebrant P. The changing panorama of cerebral palsy in Sweden. IX. Prevalence and origin in the birth-year period 1995-1998. Acta Paediatr. (2005) 94(3):287–94. doi: 10.1111/j.1651-2227.2005.tb03071.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Williams J, Lee KJ, Anderson PJ. Prevalence of motor-skill impairment in preterm children who do not develop cerebral palsy: a systematic review. Dev Med Child Neurol. (2010) 52(3):232–7. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8749.2009.03544.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Harel-Gadassi A, Friedlander E, Yaari M, Bar-Oz B, Eventov-Friedman S, Mankuta D, et al. Developmental assessment of preterm infants: chronological or corrected age? Res Dev Disabil. (2018) 80:35–43. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2018.06.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Zhang X, Spear E, Hsu HL, Gennings C, Stroustrup A. NICU-based stress response and preterm infant neurobehavior: exploring the critical windows for exposure. Pediatr Res. (2022) 92(5):1470–8. doi: 10.1038/s41390-022-01983-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Lin CY, Hsu CH, Chang JH. Neurodevelopmental outcomes at 2 and 5 years of age in very-low-birth-weight preterm infants born between 2002 and 2009: a prospective cohort study in Taiwan. Pediatr Neonatol. (2020) 61(1):36–44. doi: 10.1016/j.pedneo.2019.05.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: preterm birth, premature infant, moderate to late preterm, developmental outcomes, Griffiths III

Citation: Ryan MA, Murray DM, Dempsey EM, Mathieson SR, Livingstone V and Boylan GB (2023) Neurodevelopmental outcome of low-risk moderate to late preterm infants at 18 months. Front. Pediatr. 11:1256872. doi: 10.3389/fped.2023.1256872

Received: 11 July 2023; Accepted: 30 October 2023;
Published: 30 November 2023.

Edited by:

Domenico Romeo, Agostino Gemelli University Polyclinic (IRCCS), Italy

Reviewed by:

Claudia Brogna, IRCCS, Italy
Gerda Meijler, Isala Women and Children’s Hospital, Netherlands

© 2023 Ryan, Murray, Dempsey, Mathieson, Livingstone and Boylan. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Mary Anne Ryan maryanne.ryan@ucc.ie Geraldine B. Boylan g.boylan@ucc.ie

Abbreviations MLP, moderate to late preterm; TC, term control; GA, gestational age.

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.