Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Pediatr., 18 May 2022
Sec. Children and Health
This article is part of the Research Topic Understanding the Socio-emotional and Socio-cognitive Developmental Pathways in Children with Sensory Impairment View all 8 articles

Emotional Prosodies Processing and Its Relationship With Neurodevelopment Outcome at 24 Months in Infants of Diabetic Mothers

  • 1Department of Pediatrics, Peking University First Hospital, Beijing, China
  • 2Department of Pediatrics, Beijing Friendship Hospital, Capital Medical University, Beijing, China

Gestational diabetes mellitus (GDM) is one of the most common complications of pregnancy. Hyperglycemia of pregnancy is a risk not only for later obesity of the offspring but also do harm to their neurodevelopment from fetus. An ERP research has shown that children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) was characterized by impaired semantic processing. In this study, we used event-related potential (ERP) to assess the procession of different emotional prosodies (happy, fearful, and angry) in neonates of diabetic mothers, compared to the healthy term infants. And to explore whether the ERP measure has potential value for the evaluation of neurodevelopmental outcome in later childhood. A total of 43 full-term neonates were recruited from the neonatology department of Peking University First Hospital from December 1, 2017 to April 30, 2019. They were assigned to infants of diabetic mothers (IDM) group (n = 23) or control group (n = 20) according to their mother's oral glucose tolerance test's (OGTT) result during pregnancy. Using an oddball paradigm, ERP data were recorded while subjects listened to deviation stimulus (20%, happy/fearful/angry prosodies) and standard stimulus (80%, neutral prosody) to evaluate the potential prognostic value of ERP indexes for neurodevelopment at 24 months of age. Results showed that 1) mismatch response (MMR) amplitudes in IDM group were lower than the control; 2) lower MMR amplitude to fearful prosody at frontal lobe was a high risk for increased Modified Checklist for Autism in Toddlers (M-CHAT) scores at 24 months. These findings suggests that hyperglycemia of pregnancy may influence the ability to process emotional prosodies in neonatal brain; it could be reflected by decreased MMR amplitude in response to fearful prosody. Moreover, the decreased MMR amplitude at the frontal lobe may indicated an increased risk of ASD.

Introduction

Gestational diabetes mellitus (GDM) is one of the most common complications of pregnancy. The International Diabetes Federation (IDF) estimates that 16.8% of newborns were born from mothers with hyperglycemia during pregnancy, with 84% of maternal hyperglycemia being gestational diabetes mellitus (1). In China, with the change in childbearing attitudes and the implementation of the “bearing two-children” policy, the proportion of elderly pregnant mothers is increasing year by year, leading to a parallel increase in the prevalence of GDM. Infants of diabetic mothers (IDMs) are babies born to mothers with gestational diabetes and mothers who had pre-existing diabetes. In addition to the increased risk of obesity and metabolic syndrome in IDMs, current research also shows that hyperglycemia at pregnancy can interfere with fetal brain development. Mechanisms include a hyperglycemic environment during pregnancy, chronic intrauterine hypoxia due to hyperglycemia and hyperinsulinemia, iron deficiency, and docosahexaenoic acid (DHA) deficiency (2, 3). Consequently, children exposed to GDM in utero are at an increased risk for acquiring neurodevelopmental disorders including autism spectrum disorder (ASD) (4). ASD is a neurodevelopmental disorder with biological basis that manifests as persistent deficits in social communication and social interaction, as well as restricted and repetitive patterns of behavior, interests, and activities. Typical features appear in most children within 2 years of age (511). About 14 to 13 of children with ASD reach early language milestones, but regression or stagnation in language, communication, and/or social skills occurs at 15–24 months of age (1217). Early diagnosis of these children is difficult due to the lack of specific neurological manifestations at birth and in the early postnatal period. In addition, due to the lack of specific medication, early identification and some early intervention may be helpful in improving the neurological prognosis of this group of children.

Emotional processing skills are extremely important in social interaction. However, children with ASD are unable to correctly understand the emotional responses of others (e.g., misinterpreting others' happiness as fear, failing to understand others' pain). They are also unable to share their interests or emotions (e.g., happiness, achievement, or apprehension) with social peers (e.g., caregivers or playmates) (18). Event-related potentials are cortical potentials generated during the brain's cognitive processing of stimulus information that can accurately reflect the brain's cognitive processing after stimulation. They also reflect the brain's ability to pay attention to external information as well as its ability to process memories. Cheng's research (19) shows that healthy full-term newborns produce different MMR amplitudes in response to different emotional prosodies (happy/fearful), and higher MMR amplitudes with negative (fearful) prosodies. Our previous study (20) showed that the MMR amplitude of neonates with brain injury decreased and was related to poor neurodevelopmental outcomes at 30 months (20). However, the previous study didn't analyze the relationship between MMR amplitude and the risk of ASD. Another ERP study has indicated that 5–11 years old children with ASD showed EEG evidence of delayed speed of semantic processing and limited integration with mental representations (21). While, did these children at risk already have abnormalities in processing emotional prosody at birth? There is lacking of knowledge about this.

Therefore, we applied auditory event related potential (AERP) in IDMs, who were at risk of ASD, to explore the change of MMR under the stimulation of different emotional prosodies and to analyze whether the change of MMR in neonates was an early index predicting the development of ASD in childhood.

Materials and Methods

Participants

Full-term newborns admitted to the pediatric neonatal ward of Peking University First Hospital from December 1, 2017 to April 30, 2019 were selected for the study. They were divided into the IDM group and the control group. Inclusion criteria for the IDM group: (1) Their mothers met the diagnosis of gestational diabetes, with the diagnostic criteria being: Pregnant women should be offered OGTT between 24–28 weeks using a standardized 2-h 75-g oral glucose tolerance test with fasting plasma glucose (FPG), 1-h plasma glucose (PG), 2-h PG (III-B). Gestational diabetes mellitus is diagnosed if one value is met or exceeded: i. FPG ≥5.1 mmol/L; ii. 1-h PG ≥10.0 mmol/L; iii. 2-h PG ≥8.5 mmol/L (22); (2) Gestational age ≥37 weeks and birth weight above the 10th percentile for the same gestational age and sex; (3) Passed otoacoustic emission (OAE) and automated auditory brainstem response (AABR) examinations; (4) Vital signs for all children were stable at the time of examination; (5) No sedation was applied within 48 h prior to the event-related potential data collection. Inclusion criteria for the control group (non-IDM group): (1) Full-term newborns admitted to the ward in the same period without severe diseases (sepsis, pneumonia, hyperbilirubinemia, congenital heart disease, necrotizing enterocolitis); (2) Gestational age ≥37 weeks and birth weight above the 10th percentile for the same gestational age and sex; (3) Passed otoacoustic emission (OAE) and automated auditory brainstem response (AABR) examinations; (4) Vital signs for all children were stable at the time of examination; (5) No sedation was applied within 48 h prior to the event-related potential data collection.

Exclusion Criteria

(1) The mother has conditions such as autoimmune diseases, hypothyroidism/hyperthyroidism, hypertension during pregnancy, or taking medicines except folic acid, multivitamin and calcium tablet; (2) History of perinatal hypoxic asphyxia; (3) Brain injury such as intraventricular hemorrhage of Grades III-IV, stroke, meningitis, neonatal seizure; (4) Congenital genetic metabolic disease; (5) Unstable vital signs combined with severe diseases such as respiratory failure and shock, requiring the application of treatment such as respiratory support, vasoactive drugs.

Informed consent was signed by the parent or legal guardian of the neonates to approve the use of clinical information and EEG data for scientific purpose. The research protocol was approved by the Ethics Committee of Peking University First Hospital.

Stimuli and Procedure

With reference to Cheng et al. study (19), the syllables “da-da” spoken by a young woman representing negativity (fear, anger), positivity (happiness) and neutrality were used as stimuli. The duration of each syllable was 350 ms.

The experiment was conducted in a soundproof room when the neonate was in relatively stable condition during hospitalization and during the natural sleep state half an hour after feeding. Emotional prosodies were played through a loudspeaker (HiFier-010A, Shenzhen, China), and EEG information was collected during the experiment. The sound intensity was 57–62 dB with an average of 59 dB, and the noise intensity of the surrounding environment was ≤ 30 dB.

The emotional prosodies were presented using an oddball paradigm, in which deviant stimuli (fearful, angry or happy prosody) were occasionally presented (20%) mixed among a series of repetitive standard stimuli (neutral prosody, 80%). The interval between stimuli ranged from 450 to 850 ms.

The experiment lasted for about 60 min on average. In this study, there were 6 blocks, each with 600 stimuli, including 480 “standard stimuli” and 120 “deviant stimuli”. The two types of stimuli were played randomly (Figure 1).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. The pattern of vocal voice broadcasting.

ERP Recording

An EEG recorder (Neusen.U, Borecon) was used to extract event-related potentials (ERP) according to the international standard 10–20 system electrode placement method. This was completed by acquiring EEG signals from F3 and F4 (frontal area), C3 and C4 (central area), P3 and P4 (parietal area) after different emotional voice stimuli while playing the voice signal. Electrodes were all Ag-AgCl disc electrodes with impedance <5 kQ. The EEG signals obtained after different emotional voice stimulation were extracted from the ERP data using EEGLAB with Matlab software. EEGs were filtered (1 to 30 Hz), segmented (−200 to 800 ms), baseline corrected (−200 to 0 ms), and trials with amplitudes exceeding ± 150 μV were excluded. MMR wave amplitude and latency were used to determine the subject's ability to discriminate between different emotional prososdies (angry, fearful, and happy vocal voice). Higher amplitude indicated that the brain was better at discriminating between such stimuli. Shorter latency indicated that the brain processed emotional information faster.

Neurodevelopment Assessment

Neurodevelopmental evaluations were performed using the Ages and Stages Questionnaire (ASQ) for all the enrolled children at 3, 6, 9, 12, 18 and 24 months postpartum and the Modified Checklist for Autism in Toddlers (M-CHAT) was tested at 24 months. ASQ-3, which covers five domains (communication, gross motor, fine motor, problem-solving, and personal-social), can be completed at home (23). Each domain represents a developmental milestone for that specific age group. Infant who scored higher than the cutoff was normal; individual who scored near the cutoff was borderline and required further evaluation; individual who scored below the cutoff was considered to have developmental abnormalities (24). ASQ-SE focuses on the emotional development and mental health of children. A total score equal to or above the cutoff indicated that the infant was at risk for social-emotional disorders and was recorded as a failed test. M-CHAT is a commonly used screening scale for autism in infants and toddlers in China at present (25). There are 23 items on the scale. The screening was considered failed if items 11, 18, 20, and 22 were scored “3” and other items were scored “0”. The infant was considered to be at risk for autism or other developmental disorders if two or more among items 2, 7, 9, 13, 14, and 15 were scored as “3”, or if there were three or more items out of all items on the list that were scored as “3”. A higher total score reflects a greater risk for ASD (26). A total score of ≥17 was considered failing to pass the test (see the attachment). Finally, 43 newborns (23 in IDM group and 20 in control group) were followed up to 24 months and ASQ-3, ASQ-SE and M-CHAT evaluations were performed by then.

Statistical Analysis

Statistical analyses were performed using SPSS 21.0 software. Repeated measures ANOVA was performed on the amplitude of ERP, with group (neonates born to a diabetic mother vs. not born to a diabetic mother) as the between-subject factor, emotional prosodies (negative: fearful and angry, positive: happy) and electrode position (F3, F4, C3, C4, P3, and P4) as within-subject factors. Significance level was set at p < 0.05. Effect sizes of independent variables were assessed using biased η2. Bonferroni correction was used for multiple comparisons. Greenhouse-Geisser correction for degrees of freedom was used.

Results

General Demographics

A total of 50 neonates were enrolled in this study. 7 cases were excluded due to poor data caused by crying of the infants, and 43 cases were included ultimately. Among them, there were 23 cases in IDM group (15 males and 8 females) and 20 neonates in the control group (10 males and 10 females). The IDM and the control groups did not differ by sex, gestational age, birth weight, time of ERP acquisition and neurodevelopmental assessment, and maternal age and education. Median maternal age was 33 years (ranged from 26–45) in the IDM group and 32 years (ranged from 24–44) in the control group. In our study, 87% of the parents (20/23) in IDM group and 90% of the parents (18/20) in control group achieved a university degree or above. Besides, cranial ultrasound was done in all infants of this study. 38 of them are normal and 5 of them got a subependymal hemorrhage at absorption period which didn't affect the neurodevelopment outcomes. The general demographics in both groups is summarized in Table 1.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Participant demographics.

ERPs

MMR Amplitude

The main effect of group was not significant [F(1, 42) = 3.069, p = 0.108, η2 = 0.218]: though the MMR amplitudes were smaller in the IDM group (1.05 ± 0.22 μV) compared to the control group (1.63 ± 0.23 μV). The main effect of emotion valence was significant [F(3, 126) = 6.487, p = 0.013, η2 = 0.684]: fearful prosody (1.75 ± 0.21 μV) evoked larger MMR amplitudes than happy and angry prosodies (1.38 ± 0.18 μV, 1.31 ± 0.29 μV). The main effect of electrode position was significant [F(5, 210) = 6.911, p = 0.012, η2 = 0.832]. In general, the MMR amplitudes declined from front to back (F3 = 1.33 ± 0.29 μV, F4 = 2.24 ± 0.23 μV, C3 = 1.59 ± 0.21 μV, C4 = 1.89 ± 0.34 μV, P3 = 0.32 ± 0.28 μV, P4 = 0.66 ± 0.36 μV). The most important finding was the significant interaction between emotion prosody × electrode position × group [F(1, 42) = 20.020, p = 0.001, η2 = 0.645]: while fearful prosody evoked larger MMR amplitudes at F3 in the control group (3.47 ± 0.47 μV) than the IDM group (1.23 ± 0.64 μV). There was no significantly statistic difference between other emotions in two groups. Detailed results are shown in Table 2 and Figure 2.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Comparison of MMR amplitudes produced by two groups at different electrode positions and with different emotional prosodies (± S, μV).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. The MMR of IDM and control group stimulated by fearful/angry/happy prosodies.

MMR Latency

The main effect of group was significant [F(1, 42) = 14.607, p = 0.003, η2 = 0.594]: the MMR latency were shorter in the IDM group (300 ± 9 ms) compared to the control group (346 ± 13 ms). Interaction between emotion prosody × electrode position × group showed: (1) fearful prosody evoked longer MMR latency at F3 in the control group (422.5 ± 31.9 ms) than the IDM group (288.2 ± 44.5 μV), [F(1, 42) = 65.316, p = 0.000, η2 = 0.867]. (2) angry prosody evoked longer MMR latency at F3/F4 in the control group (F3: 419.8 ± 38.2 ms, F4: 453.1 ± 25.6 ms) than the IDM group (284.0 ± 28.3 μV, F4: 329.0 ± 33.1 ms), [F3: F(1, 42) = 10.874, p = 0.008, η2 = 0.521; F4: F(1, 42) = 8.311, p = 0.016, η2 = 0.454]. There was no significantly statistic difference between two groups under happy prosody.

Neurological Prognosis

The results showed that no infant failed ASQ-3 and ASQ-SE at 24 months and no significant statistic difference was found between two groups in five domains (IDM vs Control: Communication: 52.8 ± 5.7 vs 51.5 ± 7.7, p = 0.074; Gross motor: 55.0 ± 4.3 vs 57.7 ± 3.9, p = 0.887; Fine motor: 57.8 ± 3.6 vs 55.4 ± 5.6, p = 0.308; Problem solving: 52.2 ± 5.1 vs 51.9 ± 6.0, p = 0.399; Personal-social: 54.4 ± 3.9 vs 53.1 ± 6.0, p = 0.058) and ASQ-SE scores (IDM vs Control: 27.3 ± 9.0 vs 23.5 ± 14.3, p = 0.114). There was no abnormality in the M-CHAT scores of two groups. No significant statistic difference was found between gestational age/sex and M-CHAT scores (p = 0.176, p = 0.663).

However, the ERP amplitudes at the frontal electrodes in response to fearful prosodies were negatively correlated with M-CHAT score (F3: r = −0.418, p = 0.005; F4: r = −0.420, p = 0.005; Figure 3).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Correlation between ERP amplitudes at F3/F4 in response to fearful prosody and M-CHAT score at 24 months.

Discussion

Abnormal maternal glucose metabolism during pregnancy leads to alterations in the intrauterine environment that can interfere with fetal brain development. Existing researches have found that the shape and volume of the hippocampus in IDM may be affected by intrauterine hyperglycemia (27). The hippocampus is a subcortical structure critical for learning, memory and emotion regulation (28), and hippocampus impairment may affect neurological function either directly or by breaking off neural connections. Increased risk of attention deficits, poor gross and fine motor skills, and reduced expressive language skills may occur during growth (29), but there are usually no specific neurological symptoms in the neonatal period or even in infancy. Neither early classical structural brain examinations such as MRI nor functional examinations like conventional EEG can detect abnormalities like that especially in neonates (3037). In this study, we found that emotional prosody discrimination was affected in IDM in the early postnatal period by ERP, as evidenced by a significant decrease in ERP amplitude in response to fearful prosody at F3 and shorter latency in response to fearful and angry prosodies. Follow-up revealed a negative correlation between fearful prosody wave amplitude at F3/F4 in the early postnatal period and M-CHAT scores at 2 years of age despite all of them act normal by then.

Limited studies (38, 39) showed changes of ERP in IDMs. In Cordón's study (39), impairments of emotional face recognition were observed in ERPs from 3 to 4 years old children born to diabetic mothers (CDM). It showed that CDM displayed longer latencies and lower amplitude when processing the fearful and angry expressions, while not at happy procession. In our study, decreased amplitudes of MMR were also found in fearful and angry prosodies, though it was statistically significant in fearful prosody at F3. The different processing between negative and positive information may due to less negative stimuli exposure than happy stimuli during early development, and infants are more familiar to the happy prosody. Another finding in this study was that IDM produced significantly shorter ERP latency in response to different emotional prosodies compared to the control, which was inconsistent with the above research. While in deRegnier et al. study (40), it also showed shorter latency of P2 in ERP of IDM when recognizing mother's voice from stranger's. In general, the latency reflects the speed at which the brain processes information and often correlates with the maturation of the myelination of the nerves (41). Though shorter latency of ERP was found in both studies, it doesn't mean more mature of the brain in IDMs. Because longer duration of information processing was observed in control both in our and deRegnier's studies, which means greater continuous processing capacity. Besides, there's no relationship between short latency and better neurodevelopmental outcome at 24 months old in our study. This finding needs to be replication, admitting more subjects, and sleep cycle should also be monitored and discussed synchronously, which may affect the result of ERP, either.

The current study (42) shows the role of ERP in prediction of neurological outcome in infants and children. deRegnier et al. study (40) has shown that IDM produce a different pattern of ERP compares to controls and the altered pattern may be associated with cognition outcome at 1 year of age (as evidenced by a decrease in mental development index). A prospective study in Singapore (43), which performed AERP in IDM at 6 months and 18 months, showed that left hemisphere MMR wave amplitude in the brain at 6 months of age was significantly associated with BSID-III cognitive scores, i.e., the higher the MMR wave amplitude, the higher the cognitive score. Our study used ASQ as a screening method and did not find any children with developmental abnormalities. It may be related to the fact that glycemic control was satisfied in the mothers of the IDM in this study during pregnancy and the newborns were born without complications such as hypoglycemia. The IDMs in this study showed a slightly decrease in ERP amplitude, it may indicate that the small effect of maternal hyperglycemia on the brain development. Therefore, further classification of IDM according to maternal glucose during pregnancy and serum iron levels would be beneficial to explore the effect of maternal glucose levels during pregnancy on IDM neurodevelopment.

Autism is a severe but not rare developmental mental disorder that may begin as early as infancy (44). The onset of autism is characterized by early onset, complex symptoms and a poor prognosis. Although the etiology of autism is not yet clear and there is no satisfactory treatment, early diagnosis and treatment have an important role on the prognosis of children with autism at pre-school age. The main screening scale commonly used in China at this stage is the M-CHAT. It has a high degree of authenticity, specificity and reliability (25). The screening in our study did not find any children with ASD, but analysis showed a negative correlation between ERP amplitude elicited by fearful prosody at F3/F4 and their scores in the early postnatal period. It is unclear if this means that the risk of developing ASD is higher when ERP amplitude is lower, but it is an extremely interesting phenomenon. Further follow-up and large sample size are needed to explore this.

Limitations

(1) There was no child with developmental retardation at 24 months old. This could be attribute to the small number of subjects included in this study and most mothers in this study had satisfactory glucose control. However, we still observed difference in ERP between the IDM and control group. Subsequent studies may admit more subjects with poor maternal glucose control, and the predictive role of ERP in IDM neurodevelopmental outcome will be discussed further. (2) Due to the COVID-19 epidemic, this study used ASQ and M-CHAT screening tests, which are easy for parents to complete at home, instead of diagnostic scales for infant neurodevelopment. This may be more convenience for parents, but not as objective as scales done by doctors. In addition, some emotional and behavioral problems, such as attention deficit hyperactivity disorder couldn't be diagnosed such early, and long term follow-up is needed for these children.

Conclusion

This study found that Infants of diabetic mothers had different procession in emotional prosodies, especially in fearful prosody during neonatal period, as evidenced by a shortened latency and decreased ERP amplitude. Though all children in this study passed neurodevelopment assessment at 24 months old. Infants with low ERP amplitude under fearful prosody at F3/F4 had high M-CHAT scores at 24 months, which should be alerted to the risk of ASD.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Ethics Committee for Clinical Research of Peking University First Hospital. Written informed consent to participate in this study was provided by the participants' legal guardian/next of kin.

Author Contributions

All authors listed have made a substantial, direct, and intellectual contribution to the work and approved it for publication.

Funding

This study was funded by Beijing Municipal Science & Technology (Z191100006619049 to XH), National Key Research and Development Program of China (2021YFC2700700 to XH), Capital's Funds for Health Improvement and Research (2022-3-40715 to XH), Beijing Municipal Science & Technology Commission (Z211100002921050 to LL), Young Scientists Fund of the NSFC (82101806 to LL), and Scientific Research Seed Fund of Peking University First Hospital (No. 2019SF12 to RZ).

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fped.2022.861432/full#supplementary-material

References

1. Aguiree, Brown F, Cho A, Ho N, Gisela D, Sheree D, et al. IDF Diabetes Atlas. 6th ed. Brussels: International Diabetes Federation (2013).

Google Scholar

2. Verner AM, Manderson J, Lappin TR, McCance DR, Halliday HL, Sweet DG. Influence of maternal diabetes mellitus on fetal iron status. Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed. (2007) 92:399–401. doi: 10.1136/adc.2006.097279

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Esposito K, Nappo F, Marfella R, Giugliano G, Giugliano F, Ciotola M, et al. Inflammatory cytokine concentrations are acutely increased by hyperglycemia in humans: role of oxidative stress. Circulation. (2002) 106:2067–72. doi: 10.1161/01.CIR.0000034509.14906.AE

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Xiang AH, Wang X, Martinez MP, Walthall JC, Curry ES, Page K. Darios Getahun. JAMA. (2015) 313:1425–34. doi: 10.1001/jama.2015.2707

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Landa RJ, Gross AL, Stuart EA, Faherty A. Developmental trajectories in children with and without autism spectrum disorders: the first 3 years. Child Dev. (2013) 84:429. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01870.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Landa R, Garrett-Mayer E. Development in infants with autism spectrum disorders: a prospective study. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. (2006) 47:629. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01531.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Chawarska K, Klin A, Paul R, Volkmar F. Autism spectrum disorder in the second year: stability and change in syndrome expression. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. (2007) 48:128. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01685.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Landa RJ, Holman KC Garrett-Mayer Social E. and communication development in toddlers with early and later diagnosis of autism spectrum disorders. Arch Gen Psychiatry. (2007) 64:853. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.64.7.853

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Mitchell S, Brian J, Zwaigenbaum L, Roberts W, Szatmari P, Smith I, et al. Early language and communication development of infants later diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder. J Dev Behav Pediatr. (2006) 27:S69. doi: 10.1097/00004703-200604002-00004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Palomo R, Belinchón M, Ozonoff Autism S. and family home movies: a comprehensive review. J Dev Behav Pediatr. (2006) 27:S59. doi: 10.1097/00004703-200604002-00003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Ozonoff S, Iosif A-M, Baguio F, Cook IC, Hill MM, Hutman T, et al. A prospective study of the emergence of early behavioral signs of autism. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. (2010) 49:256. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2009.11.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Ozonoff s, Iosif A-M, Young GS, Hepburn S, Thompson M, Colombi C, et al. Onset patterns in autism: correspondence between home video and parent report. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. (2011) 50:796. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2011.03.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Tuchman RF, Rapin I, Shinnar Autistic S. dysphasic children. I: clinical characteristics Pediatrics. (1991) 88:1211. doi: 10.1542/peds.88.6.1211

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Tuchman RF, Rapin I. Regression in pervasive developmental disorders: seizures and epileptiform electroencephalogram correlates. Pediatrics. (1997) 99:560. doi: 10.1542/peds.99.4.560

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Werner E, Dawson G. Validation of the phenomenon of autistic regression using home videotapes. Arch Gen Psychiatry. (2005) 62:889. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.62.8.889

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Turner LM, Stone WL, Pozdol SL, Coonrod EE. Follow-up of children with autism spectrum disorders from age 2 to age 9. Autism. (2006) 10:243. doi: 10.1177/1362361306063296

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Susan L, Hyman Susan E, Levy Scott M Myers Council Council on children with disabilities section section on developmental and behavioral pediatric identification evaluation and and management of children with autism spectrum disorder. Pediatrics. 2020:145:e20193447. doi: 10.1542/peds.2019-3447

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Levine MD, Carey WB, Crocker AC. Developmental-Behavioral Pediatrics. 3rd ed. Philadelphia, PA: WB Saunders (1999). p. 589.

Google Scholar

19. Cheng Y, Lee SY, Chen HY, Wang PY, Decety J. Voice and emotion processing in the human neonatal brain. J Cogn Neurosci. (2012) 24:1411–9. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00214

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Sun G, Xie H, Liu Y, Chen Y, Hou X, Zhang D. Impact of brain injury on processing of emotional prosodies in neonates. Front Pediatr. (2019) 7:192. doi: 10.3389/fped.2019.00192

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. DiStefano C, Senturk D, Spurling S. ERP evidence of semantic processing in children with ASD. Dev Cogn Neurosci. (2019) 36:100640. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2019.100640

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Berger H, Gagnon R, Sermer M. Guideline No. 393-Diabetes in Pregnancy. J Obstet Gynaecol Can. (2019) 41:1814–25. doi: 10.1016/j.jogc.2019.03.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Velikonja T, Edbrooke-Childs J, Calderon A, Sleed M, Brown A. Deighton J. The psychometric properties of the ages & stages questionnaires for ages 2–25: a systematic review. Child Care Health Dev. (2017) 43:1–17. doi: 10.1111/cch.12397

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Singh A, Yeh C J, Boone Blanchard Ages S. and Stages Questionnaire: a global screening scale. Boletín Médico del Hospital Infantil de México. (2017) 74:5–12. doi: 10.1016/j.bmhimx.2016.07.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Vanvuchelen M, Roeyers H, De Weerdt W. Do imitation problems reflect a core characteristic in autism? evidence from a literature review. Res Autism Spectr Disord. (2011) 5:89–95. doi: 10.1016/j.rasd.2010.07.010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Robins DL, Casagrande K, Barton M, Chen CM, Dumont-Mathieu T, Fein D. VaIidation of the modified checklist for Autism in toddlers, revised with follow-up (M-CHAT-R/F). Pediatrics. (2014) 133:37. doi: 10.1542/peds.2013-1813

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Lynch KM, Alves JM, Chow T, Clark KA, Luo S, Toga AW et al. Selective morphological and volumetric alterations in the hippocampus of children exposed in utero to gestational diabetes mellitus. Hum Brain Mapp. (2021) 42:2583–92. doi: 10.1002/hbm.25390

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Squire LR, Zola-Morgan S. The medial temporal lobe memory system. Science. (1991) 253:1380–6. doi: 10.1126/science.1896849

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Dahlquist G, Kallen B. Schoolmarks for Swedish children whose mothers had diabetes during pregnancy: a population-based study. Diabetologia. (2007) 50:1826–31. doi: 10.1007/s00125-007-0744-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Hintz SR, Barnes PD, Bulas D, Slovis TL, Finer NN, Wrage LA, et al. Neuroimaging and neurodevelopmental outcome in extremely preterm infants. Pediatrics. (2015) 135:e32–42. doi: 10.1542/peds.2014-0898

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Ment LR, Bada HS, Barnes P, Grant PE, Hirtz D, Papile LA et al. Practice parameter: neuroimaging of the neonate: report of the quality standards subcommittee of the american academy of neurology and the practice committee of the child neurology society. Neonatology. (2002) 58:1726–38. doi: 10.1212/WNL.58.12.1726

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Plaisier A, Govaert P. Lequin, MH, Dudink J. Optimal timing of cerebral MRI in preterm infants to predict long-term neurodevelopmental outcome: a systematic review. Am J Neuroradiol. (2014) 35:841–7. doi: 10.3174/ajnr.A3513

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Watanabe K, Hayakawa F, Okumura A. Neonatal EEG: a powerful tool in the assessment of brain damage in preterm infants. Brain Dev. (1999) 21:361–72. doi: 10.1016/S0387-7604(99)00034-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Hellström-Westas L, Rosén I. Electroencephalography and brain damage in preterm infants. Early Hum Dev. (2005) 81:255–61. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2005.01.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Majnemer A, Rosenblatt B. Evoked potentials as predictors of outcome in neonatal intensive care unit survivors: review of literature. Pediatr Neurol. (1996) 14:189–95. doi: 10.1016/0887-8994(96)00049-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Pierrat V, Eken P, de Vries LS. The predictive value of cranial ultrasound and somatosensory evoked potentials after nerve stimulation for adverse neurological outcome in preterm infants. Dev Med Child Neurol. (1997) 39:398–403. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8749.1997.tb07453.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Pike AA, Marlow N. The role of cortical evoked responses in predicting neuromotor outcome in very preterm infants. Early Hum Dev. (2000) 57:123–35. doi: 10.1016/S0378-3782(99)00061-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. DeRegnier R-A, Long JD, Georgieff MK, Nelson CA. Using event related potentials to study perinatal nutrition and brain development in infants of diabetic mothers. Dev Neuropsychol. (2007) 31:379–96. doi: 10.1080/87565640701229524

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Ingrid Cordón M, Georgieff MK, Nelson CA. Neural correlates of emotion processing in typically developing children and children of diabetic mothers. Dev Neuropsychol. (2009) 34:683–700. doi: 10.1080/87565640903265129

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Deregnier RA, Nelson CA, Thomas KM, Wewerka S, Georgieff MK. Neurophysiologic evaluation of auditory recognition memory in healthy newborn infants and infants of diabetic mothers. J Pediatr. (2000) 137:777–84. doi: 10.1067/mpd.2000.109149

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Eggermont JJ. On the rate of maturation of sensory evoked potentials. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. (1988) 70:293–305. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(88)90048-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Depoorter A, Früh J, Herrmann K, Zanchi D, Weber P. Predicting neurodevelopmental outcome in preterm born infants using auditory event-related potentials a systematic review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2018) 89:99–110. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.02.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Cai S, Qiu A, Broekman BF, Wong EQ, Gluckman PD, Godfrey KM, et al. The influence of gestational diabetes on neurodevelopment of children in the first two years of life: a prospective study. PLoS ONE. (2016) 11:e0162113. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0162113

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Courchesne E, Pramparo T, Gazestani VH, Lombardo MV, Pierce K, Lewis NE. The ASD living biology: from cell proliferation to clinical phenotype. Mol Psychiatry. (2019) 24:88–107. doi: 10.1038/s41380-018-0056-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: neonate, gestational diabetes mellitus, emotional prosody, neurodevelopment, event-related potential

Citation: Sun G, Liu Y, Zhang R, Peng C, Geng Y, Zhou F, Hou X and Liu L (2022) Emotional Prosodies Processing and Its Relationship With Neurodevelopment Outcome at 24 Months in Infants of Diabetic Mothers. Front. Pediatr. 10:861432. doi: 10.3389/fped.2022.861432

Received: 03 February 2022; Accepted: 19 April 2022;
Published: 18 May 2022.

Edited by:

Monica Gori, Italian Institute of Technology (IIT), Italy

Reviewed by:

Justyna Paprocka, Medical University of Silesia, Poland
Maddalena Brambilla, University of Parma, Italy

Copyright © 2022 Sun, Liu, Zhang, Peng, Geng, Zhou, Hou and Liu. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Lili Liu, sliuli-vs-junny@vip.sina.com

These authors have contributed equally to this work

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.