Skip to main content

PERSPECTIVE article

Front. Pediatr., 18 April 2018
Sec. Children and Health

Executive Function Deficits in Seriously Ill Children—Emerging Challenges and Possibilities for Clinical Care

  • 1Cognitive Neurophysiology, Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Faculty of Medicine, Technische Universität Dresden, Dresden, Germany
  • 2Abteilung Neuropädiatrie, Medizinische Fakultät Carl Gustav Carus, Technische Universität Dresden, Dresden, Germany
  • 3Experimental Neurobiology, National Institute of Mental Health, Prague, Czechia
  • 4Institute of Psychology, Technische Universität Dresden, Dresden, Germany

The past years have seen an incredible increase in the quality and success rates of treatments in pediatric medicine. One of the resulting major challenges refers to the management of primary or secondary residual executive function deficits in affected children. These deficits lead to problems in the ability to acquire, understand, and apply abstract and complex knowledge and to plan, direct, and control actions. Executive functions deficits are important to consider because they are highly predictive of functioning in social and academic aspects of daily life. We argue that current clinical practice does not sufficiently account for the complex cognitive processes in this population. This is because widely applied pharmacological interventions only rarely account for the complexity of the underlying neuronal mechanisms and do not fit well into possibly powerful “individualized medicine” approaches. Novel treatment approaches targeting deficits in executive functions in seriously ill children could focus on neuronal oscillations, as these have some specific relations to different aspects of executive function. Importantly, such treatment approaches can be individually tailored to the individuals’ deficits and can be transferred into home-treatment or e-health solutions. These approaches are easy-to-use, can be easily integrated into daily life, and are becoming increasingly cost-effective.

Introduction

In recent years, advances in pediatric medicine have led to significantly higher survival rates of children with serious acute or chronic life-limiting conditions (14). In particular, the option of an individual adjustment of treatment to the particular characteristics of a particular disease in a particular patient (5) has opened new avenues for treating clinicians. These treatment advances have, however, resulted in an increasing group of children and (young) adult patients with residua or sequelae of (often rare) childhood diseases. This introduces new problem areas, since increased survival is resulting in new morbidities that may significantly affect psychosocial outcomes (1, 68). Concerning this issue, questions relating to general intellectual abilities in these children and the development of various cognitive functions are moving more and more into the focus (9). To name just a few, problems in cognitive functioning that are frequently associated with various (neuro)psychiatric/developmental disorder have been reported to occur as direct primary effects or as secondary results of a number of devastating diseases. These include, but are not limited to, cancer (10, 11), congenital, and chronic developmental disorders affecting the nervous system like epilepsy (12), sickle cell disease (13), spina bifida (14), phenylketonuria (15), and neurofibromatosis type 1 (16) as well as incidents significantly interfering with development like preterm birth (17) or traumatic brain injury (18, 19) (see Figure 1). We suggest that, just like it is the case for the underlying condition, such problems should be addressed through therapeutic strategies which strongly take the individual characteristics of deficits of the affected patients into account (see Table 1 and Figure 1).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. A large number of diseases can result in executive function deficits, which in turn significantly influence a variety of outcome variables. Various approaches can be applied to address such deficits. “+” denotes advantages of these intervention approaches, whereas “−” denotes disadvantages.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Comparison of cognitive (executive function) training, pharmacological interventions and oscillation-based interventions on different dimensions currently defining individualized/personalized medicine [adapted from Ref. (5)].

Central Cognitive Processes and Assessment of Executive (Dys)functions

In children with previous and ongoing severe acute or chronic illnesses, deficits in cognition can arise due to a number of factors. On the one hand, such diseases can directly affect the central nervous system and lead to structural or functional alterations which then impair cognitive functions and behavioral control. On the other hand, the structure and function of the central nervous system in these children can also be negatively affected by treatment, i.e., as side effects of pharmacological interventions, radio- and chemotherapy, and prolonged periods of hospitalization (20, 21). Depending on disease characteristics, developmental aspects (esp. neuronal plasticity) and treatment modalities, such impairments can be temporary or permanent. A large variety of cognitive functions, including attention, language, memory, and executive functions can be affected in children who have survived a serious illness. Based on their overarching significance for academic, vocational, and social functioning (22, 23), this paper will focus on executive function deficits occurring as a result of severe somatic illnesses in childhood (24).

Although a variety of (in some aspects even contrasting) models exist concerning executive functions, there is a general consensus that executive functions describe the ability to acquire, understand, and apply abstract and complex knowledge and to plan, direct, and control actions (24, 25). Poor executive functions have been associated with higher rates of violent crimes (26), risky financial behaviors (27), low social autonomy (28), and a generally lower quality of life (29). Executive functions can be examined using behavioral observations and questionnaire ratings [e.g., the behavior rating inventory of executive function or the learning, executive, and attention functioning scale (30)] and through specific neuropsychological tests like the behavioral assessment of the dysexecutive syndrome in children (31).

Cognitive Training Approaches—Current Practice and Caveats

Once executive dysfunctions have been identified in the individual child, specific neurocognitive training programs can be used to ameliorate such deficits (22) from preschool age onwards. Diamond and Lee (32) review important influencing factors and crucial general characteristics of potential interventions in childhood in great detail. Such programs focus on (i) the restoration of existing impairments using specific training exercises, (ii) the acquisition of specific compensatory techniques related to the organization and planning of behavior, and (iii) the modification of environmental factors (23). The few empirical studies that have examined the effects of cognitive interventions in children with severe acute or chronic disorders show mixed results (33, 34). Despite some promising reports of improvements in trained domains such as attention and memory, inhibitory control as a central element of executive function was not improved after any of the examined interventions (33). In addition, and although such interventions often have a high ecological validity, the transfer to other, even closely related (untrained) cognitive/executive functions is limited (34). Also, the quality of the available studies is rather low (33). Most importantly, the pathophysiological and neurobiological pathways leading to the deficits or to any positive treatment outcomes are only taken into account to a limited extent (33) (see Figure 1). Such a more neurobiological, mechanistic viewpoint on the underlying processes may significantly aid the development of interventions that are targeted and individually adjustable [i.e., conform with approaches of individualized medicine (5)] (see Table 1).

Dopamine-Based Mechanisms Underlying Executive Functions

Concerning neurobiological mechanisms, executive functions are mainly modulated by the catecholaminergic and dopaminergic system affecting fronto-striatal circuits (35, 36), even though serotonergic and glutamatergic mechanisms are also very important. Broadly speaking, “optimal” performance in numerous aspects of executive functions is reached when intermediate levels of dopamine are available (37, 38). In the case of “optimal” intermediate dopamine concentrations, there is an intricate balance between striatal dopamine D2-receptor related neural transmission (relevant for cognitive flexibility) and prefrontal dopamine D1-receptor related neural transmission (relevant for cognitive stability) (39). This pattern leads to an efficient balance between cognitive processes characterized by flexibility (e.g., readily allowing the processing of new sensory information driven by bottom-up influences) and stability (e.g., reducing distractibility and being driven by task goals in a top–down manner). Overall, cognitive flexibility and stability and the balance between them are crucial for executive functions (40).

Pharmacological Interventions—Current Practice and Caveats

Consequently, pharmacological interventions modulating these dopaminergic abnormalities are frequently used in the treatment of several psychiatric symptoms or disorders. Selective dopamine D2 antagonists (neuroleptics) like Risperidone (41, 42), for example, reduce the flexibility of mental representations upon administration. On the other hand, methylphenidate (MPH), a dopamine reuptake inhibitor commonly used to treat children with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder, prolongs the availability of dopamine in the synaptic cleft and leads to stronger activation of dopamine D1 receptors. This leads to more stable mental representations, and thus provides a balance for the D2-mediated cognitive flexibility. MPH has previously been applied within the context of disorders like cancer (4345), sickle cell disease (46), and neurofibromatosis type 1 (47).

While these approaches are characterized by good ecological validity, are readily accessible and carry low costs for families and health care providers, they are not without problems (see Figure 1). For one, studies have not consistently shown significant and beneficial effects in these cases. MPH-related increases of general IQ levels were reported in children with neurofibromatosis type 1 (47). Moreover, positive effects of low doses (0.3 mg/kg) of MPH on attention and social functioning in survivors of brain tumors and leukemia in childhood (44) were found, but do hardly generalize to academic performance and success (45, 48). Similarly, children with sickle cell disease showed isolated MPH-related improvements in inhibitory control, while other executive functions and behavior resembling symptoms of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) were not improved (46). Such mixed effects and the limited generalization may be explained by the fact that pharmacological interventions do not take the disease-specific cognitive profile and pathophysiological background into account, but function in a rather general fashion.

Thus, pharmacological interventions aiming to address cognitive (executive function) deficits in survivors of severe illnesses are connected to a trial-and-error approach and to post hoc hypotheses about why something has or has not been effective. This can lead to side effects concerning other cognitive functions and may interfere with pharmacological strategies addressing the somatic manifestations of the underlying disease. For example, pharmacological interventions targeting executive function deficits can induce cardio-metabolic changes and lowered arousal in the case of neuroleptics (49), and can lead to sleep problems, weight loss, and mood changes in the case of MPH (50). Therefore, medication may not always represent a practicable solution in clinical practice, also because it can, at least currently, hardly be tailored to the individual patients (see Table 1).

Neuronal Oscillations and Their Relevance for Executive Functions

As outlined above, both neuropsychological training approaches and pharmacological interventions are unable to address the individually different pattern of executive dysfunctions and the underlying pathophysiological background to a sufficient extent (see Table 1). In this regard, the use of treatment strategies targeting neuronal oscillations represents a possible and highly attractive option. The term “neuronal oscillations” refers to the synchronized and rhythmic electrical brain activity that can be measured on the head using EEG electrodes (51). Although there still are significant gaps in scientific knowledge concerning their functional relevance, some neuronal oscillations have already been linked to some specific cognitive functions [e.g., See Ref. (52)]. The theta frequency band (4–7 Hz), for example, seems to play one of the most important roles, as it has been shown to be closely linked to the level of cognitive control (5355). Specifically, theta oscillations originating from medial frontal regions (e.g., the anterior cingulate cortex) have been associated with attentional and cognitive resource allocation (53). Johnson et al. (56) have demonstrated that theta oscillations are an essential element of bidirectional connections between prefrontal and parietal regions and are highly relevant for working memory processes. Thus, the suggested connection to executive functions is at least partially mediated via long-range network connectivity, for which synchronized theta oscillations play a central role (57, 58). Conversely, executive functions are functionally largely based on precisely such long-range networks (54, 59, 60). In addition to frontal-midline theta power, beta oscillations (15–20 Hz) are also a relevant element as they have been suggested to be reflective of the maintenance of the current cognitive state and thus likely also represent elements of alertness and attention (61). Further, alpha oscillations should also be considered, since they have been suggested to represent the controlled access to information stored in memory (56, 62) and thus are also relevant for executive functions. Importantly, in all cases, a variety of oscillation characteristics apart from the frequency per se, such as power/amplitude, cross-frequency coupling, and phase locking, need to be considered in this context (51, 63, 64). Due to space limitations in the current paper, the interested reader is referred to a number of excellent reviews concerning neuronal oscillations in general and their relevance for executive functions in particular (53, 63, 6568). We propose that—if interpreted carefully and handled critically—this available and constantly expanding knowledge could be beneficial when attempting to ameliorate executive function deficits in children and (young) adults with severe acute or chronic conditions.

Oscillation-Based Interventions for Executive Dysfunctions: Neurofeedback

Such attempts have already been successfully made in the treatment of various pediatric (and adult) neuropsychiatric conditions in which neuronal oscillations have been shown to be altered [e.g., see Ref. (69, 70)]. One possibility of influencing these neuronal oscillations (and thus hopefully also the corresponding cognitive functions and behavior) is the application of frequency band neurofeedback training (7173). Frequency band neurofeedback uses single EEG electrodes to record neuronal oscillations from the patient’s head. Via specifically designed software, these recordings are converted into animations or simple computer games (e.g., a car driving on a race course). The patient is able to control these games by regulating the relevant parameters of the EEG (i.e., the target oscillation). Neurofeedback is usually conducted over a period of 2–3 months with 1–2 weekly sessions. Originally, frequency band neurofeedback has been applied in the treatment of patients with ADHD, where a downregulation of theta activity and the complementary upregulation of beta activity is usually conducted (72, 74) and has shown encouraging effects on the level of symptoms (73, 75) and executive functions (71). Yet, it is likely that the efficacy of these protocols can be enhanced by training patients to upregulate theta power (76).

Importantly, depending on the individual profile of executive function problems, a similar approach may be applicable to patients, in whom the deficits stem from severe underlying somatic disorders or their treatment (see Figure 1). In fact, beneficial effects of frequency-based neurofeedback on self-reported cognitive measures have already been demonstrated in a sample of breast cancer survivors who suffered from cognitive impairments following treatment (77). Also, successful applications in patients with epilepsy (78) and chronic fatigue (79) have been reported. In contrast, one study has reported no significant differences between neurofeedback and placebo feedback training in pediatric brain tumor survivors (80). In this context, however, only beta oscillations or sensorimotor rhythms (12–15 Hz) were targeted, not taking theta frequencies, which play a crucial role in executive functioning, into account.

Oscillation-Based Interventions for Executive Dysfunctions: Transcranial Stimulation Techniques

A less established approach may relate to the application of transcranial alternating current stimulation (tACS). Through tACS, a weak electrical current (<2 mA) is repeatedly applied to the head of the patient at the desired current frequency (e.g., 6 Hz). The assumption is that, after repetitive “training,” the brain will adapt to and “take over” these externally delivered oscillations (81). tACS theta frequency stimulation over parietal regions has been shown to result in significantly increased working memory capacity in healthy adults (82). So far, a small number of tACS-intervention studies have been conducted in patients with neurological disorders like epilepsy (83) and Parkinson’s disease (84), but studies reporting long-term effects or the applicability in pediatric populations have not been published so far. Overall, tACS has been described as a safe, specific, and direct possibility to influence pathological changes in neuronal oscillations (81). Yet, to be efficient, the targeted executive function deficit needs to be directly relatable to the characteristics (amplitude, frequency, phase, or regional coherence) of a particular neuronal oscillation (i.e., the stimulation protocol needs to be hypothesis-driven) and the tACS-induced changes must be present after stimulation offset (81). For these reasons and since the functional relevance of the oscillations in question remains to be investigated further, tACS should be classified as a rather experimental, but promising approach at this stage. Interestingly, another neuromodulation approach (transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS)) has recently been used in conjunction with a short behavioral training, with this specific combination of interventions leading to (i) significant working memory improvements and (ii) changes in theta/alpha oscillations in a small group of healthy controls (85). Although its effects on neuronal oscillations may be of a more indirect or supportive nature, tDCS could thus also constitute a promising and safe (86) approach in this regard.

Challenges, Arising Possibilities, and Future Perspectives for Oscillation-Based Treatment Approaches

Approaches like frequency band neurofeedback or tACS/tDCS could thus potentially be used to up-/downregulate patient’s oscillatory activity in order to achieve changes in cognition and thus also in behavior. However, there are also a number of challenges associated with this approach. For example, it may be argued that norm values for the power of the different frequency bands are only very difficult to obtain (74). This is the case, since such values vary considerably inter- and intraindividually and also strongly depend on the conditions under which they are measured. Moreover, basing intervention protocols for patients on oscillatory power measured in healthy individuals could lead to the use of training protocols which may not be as efficient as presumed (76), since the neurophysiological characteristics of a patient are probably different due to the mere presence of a disease. These individual differences need to be addressed using individually adapted intervention approaches. Decisions about which frequency bands should be up-/downregulated have to be based on the individual profile of executive (dys)function and on the (expanding) knowledge about the oscillatory correlates of these functions (see Figure 1).

Overall, we envisage that a promising and effective clinical approach to executive function deficits occurring in survivors of severe acute or chronic illnesses involves the development and application of specific, individualized treatment possibilities. Precisely, this could take place using oscillation-based intervention approaches. Currently, however, high initial costs (mainly for the equipment) and the resulting limited accessibility represent significant challenges for implementation. This is especially the case in more rural regions with a less developed medical infrastructure. Yet, a very effective way of addressing both of these challenges at once are “e-health” and “home-treatment approaches,” which would be particularly feasible in the case of neurofeedback training. To achieve this, EEG amplifiers are being developed that are portable, robust, and cost-effective due to only a few inbuilt electrode channels necessary for neurofeedback. In the future, such home-treatment approaches could be used to apply the feedback using the patients’ own mobile devices or TV/computer screens in their home environment. Such neurofeedback systems could be used to either supplement treatment sessions taking place in clinical settings or within the framework of telehealth initiatives, in which the entire course of treatment takes place in the home environment of the patient. Either way, to adhere to best practice and ethical standards (87) and to avoid or address side effects should they occur (88), such approaches would need to involve the close support and monitoring by clinicians experienced in the field. This is necessary, since such novel treatment approaches are only suitable for motivated patients in whom the current psychosocial situation is stable and who do not have any immediately threatening health concerns. To establish such boundary conditions, it may in some cases be helpful to initially attempt to target major cognitive deficits in attention or impulse control using pharmacological strategies described above. Such a procedure has been used successfully in patients with ADHD (71).

Furthermore, elements from cognitive-behavioral therapy (and in fact also the aforementioned cognitive training approaches) are an essential part of neuronal oscillation-based treatment approaches (72, 85). In addition, for home-treatment approaches, families need to be provided with a detailed introduction to the technical aspects of the treatment (e.g., how to set up the system, connect electrodes, conduct troubleshooting). To account for all these factors, at least one initial direct contact between the clinician and the patient family as well as regular follow-up contacts (potentially via phone or secure online platforms) are necessary. In addition, there also are ethical boundaries to consider in the case of home-treatment approaches that need to be monitored closely (e.g., financial constraints, involvement of health care provider and insurances, treatment offered by non-healthcare proficient sources). Finally, it remains to be clarified how any treatment outcomes could be effectively measured and quantified. This could potentially be achieved through the use of standardized cognitive test batteries which carry a high ecological validity.

So far, only one study has reported some outcomes of a partial neurofeedback home-treatment (89). Unfortunately, no control treatment was used and reported statistical analyses are limited to descriptions of individual symptom trajectories. This study, however, incorporates a detailed methodological description, and discusses a number of important considerations as well as possible pitfalls. More recently, alpha-frequency neurofeedback using a portable device has been shown to improve memory in a group of healthy adults (90). In addition, some home-treatment approaches have been reported concerning other biological parameters like electromyographic measures (91) and gait (92). Although these studies only report preliminary results, they nevertheless encourage further developments of these technologies. Through such developments, oscillations-based treatment options would not only become less costly, more widely available, and easy-to-use, but would also significantly enhance transferability of training success to daily life.

Summary

In sum, we postulate that survivors of severe pediatric illnesses could greatly benefit from oscillation-based intervention approaches as they have the potential to ameliorate executive dysfunctions, thus leading to significant improvements in daily life. To benefit from the effects of neuronal plasticity, such approaches should be considered as early on in development as possible. Based on their high functional relevance for executive functions and the experience gained from interventions in areas like ADHD, targeting theta oscillations via neurofeedback could be a good starting point. Such approaches would provide new opportunities for the treating clinicians as they would close existing gaps between (pediatric) psychology, (neuro)pediatrics, and basic research, consequently leading to better clinical care for the affected children, (young) adults, and their families.

Author Contributions

All authors conceived the paper, wrote and edited the manuscript and figure, and have approved the final version of this paper.

Conflict of Interest Statement

AB, MH, and BN declare no competing or potential conflicts of interest. VR has received payment for consulting and writing activities from Lilly, Novartis, and Shire Pharmaceuticals, lecture honoraria from Lilly, Novartis, Shire Pharmaceuticals, and Medice Pharma, and support for research from Shire and Novartis. He has carried out (and is currently carrying out) clinical trials in cooperation with the Novartis, Shire, and Otsuka companies. CB has received payment for consulting from GlaxoSmithKline and Teva.

Acknowledgments

We thank Benjamin Teufert for his help in creating the figure.

Funding

This work was supported by the Else Kröner-Fresenius Stiftung (2016_A94) and the Friede Springer Stiftung (033/2017).

References

1. Warnes CA. Adult congenital heart disease: the challenges of a lifetime. Eur Heart J (2017) 38:2041–7. doi:10.1093/eurheartj/ehw529

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Simmonds NJ. Ageing in cystic fibrosis and long-term survival. Paediatr Respir Rev (2013) 14(Suppl 1):6–9. doi:10.1016/j.prrv.2013.01.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Overholser LS, Moss KM, Kilbourn K, Risendal B, Jones AF, Greffe BS, et al. Development of a primary care-based clinic to support adults with a history of childhood cancer: the tactic clinic. J Pediatr Nurs (2015) 30:724–31. doi:10.1016/j.pedn.2015.05.023

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Yang Q, Rasmussen SA, Friedman JM. Mortality associated with Down’s syndrome in the USA from 1983 to 1997: a population-based study. Lancet (2002) 359:1019–25. doi:10.1016/S0140-6736(02)08092-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Fierz W. Challenge of personalized health care: to what extent is medicine already individualized and what are the future trends? Med Sci Monit (2004) 10:RA111–23.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

6. Camfield P, Camfield C. Transition to adult care for children with chronic neurological disorders. Ann Neurol (2011) 69:437–44. doi:10.1002/ana.22393

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Schor NF. Life at the interface: adults with “pediatric” disorders of the nervous system. Ann Neurol (2013) 74:158–63. doi:10.1002/ana.23910

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Turkel S, Pao M. Late consequences of chronic pediatric illness. Psychiatr Clin North Am (2007) 30:819–35. doi:10.1016/j.psc.2007.07.009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Hopkins RO, Jackson JC. Long-term neurocognitive function after critical illness. Chest (2006) 130:869–78. doi:10.1378/chest.130.3.869

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. de Ruiter MA, Schouten-van Meeteren AY, van Vuurden DG, Maurice-Stam H, Gidding C, Beek LR. Psychosocial profile of pediatric brain tumor survivors with neurocognitive complaints. Qual Life Res (2016) 25(2):435–46. doi:10.1007/s11136-015-1091-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Wolfe KR, Madan-Swain A, Kana RK. Executive dysfunction in pediatric posterior fossa tumor survivors: a systematic literature review of neurocognitive deficits and interventions. Dev Neuropsychol (2012) 37:153–75. doi:10.1080/87565641.2011.632462

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. D’Agati E, Cerminara C, Casarelli L, Pitzianti M, Curatolo P. Attention and executive functions profile in childhood absence epilepsy. Brain Dev (2012) 34:812–7. doi:10.1016/j.braindev.2012.03.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Yarboi J, Compas BE, Brody GH, White D, Rees Patterson J, Ziara K, et al. Association of social-environmental factors with cognitive function in children with sickle cell disease. Child Neuropsychol (2017) 23:343–60. doi:10.1080/09297049.2015.1111318

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Rose BM, Holmbeck GN. Attention and executive functions in adolescents with spina bifida. J Pediatr Psychol (2007) 32:983–94. doi:10.1093/jpepsy/jsm042

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Christ SE, Huijbregts SCJ, de Sonneville LMJ, White DA. Executive function in early-treated phenylketonuria: profile and underlying mechanisms. Mol Genet Metab (2010) 99(Suppl 1):S22–32. doi:10.1016/j.ymgme.2009.10.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Acosta MT, Gioia GA, Silva AJ. Neurofibromatosis type 1: new insights into neurocognitive issues. Curr Neurol Neurosci Rep (2006) 6:136–43. doi:10.1007/s11910-996-0036-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Aylward GP. Neurodevelopmental outcomes of infants born prematurely. J Dev Behav Pediatr (2014) 35:394–407. doi:10.1097/01.DBP.0000452240.39511.d4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Azouvi P, Arnould A, Dromer E, Vallat-Azouvi C. Neuropsychology of traumatic brain injury: an expert overview. Rev Neurol (Paris) (2017) 173:461–72. doi:10.1016/j.neurol.2017.07.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Loher S, Fatzer ST, Roebers CM. Executive functions after pediatric mild traumatic brain injury: a prospective short-term longitudinal study. Appl Neuropsychol Child (2014) 3:103–14. doi:10.1080/21622965.2012.716752

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Duffner PK. Risk factors for cognitive decline in children treated for brain tumors. Eur J Paediatr Neurol (2010) 14:106–15. doi:10.1016/j.ejpn.2009.10.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Minisini A, Atalay G, Bottomley A, Puglisi F, Piccart M, Biganzoli L. What is the effect of systemic anticancer treatment on cognitive function? Lancet Oncol (2004) 5:273–82. doi:10.1016/S1470-2045(04)01465-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Butler RW, Mulhern RK. Neurocognitive interventions for children and adolescents surviving cancer. J Pediatr Psychol (2005) 30:65–78. doi:10.1093/jpepsy/jsi017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Catroppa C, Anderson V. Planning, problem-solving and organizational abilities in children following traumatic brain injury: intervention techniques. Pediatr Rehabil (2006) 9:89–97. doi:10.1080/13638490500155458

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Diamond A. Executive functions. Annu Rev Psychol (2013) 64:135–68. doi:10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Stuss DT, Alexander MP. Executive functions and the frontal lobes: a conceptual view. Psychol Res (2000) 63:289–98. doi:10.1007/s004269900007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Hancock M, Tapscott JL, Hoaken PNS. Role of executive dysfunction in predicting frequency and severity of violence. Aggress Behav (2010) 36:338–49. doi:10.1002/ab.20353

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Spinella M, Yang B, Lester D. Prefrontal system dysfunction and credit card debt. Int J Neurosci (2004) 114:1323–32. doi:10.1080/00207450490476011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Mazaux JM, Masson F, Levin HS, Alaoui P, Maurette P, Barat M. Long-term neuropsychological outcome and loss of social autonomy after traumatic brain injury. Arch Phys Med Rehabil (1997) 78:1316–20. doi:10.1016/S0003-9993(97)90303-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Schraegle WA, Titus JB. Executive function and health-related quality of life in pediatric epilepsy. Epilepsy Behav (2016) 62:20–6. doi:10.1016/j.yebeh.2016.06.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Castellanos I, Kronenberger WG, Pisoni DB. Questionnaire-based assessment of executive functioning: psychometrics. Appl Neuropsychol Child (2016) 7(2):93–109. doi:10.1080/21622965.2016.1248557

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Chan RCK, Shum D, Toulopoulou T, Chen EYH. Assessment of executive functions: review of instruments and identification of critical issues. Arch Clin Neuropsychol (2008) 23:201–16. doi:10.1016/j.acn.2007.08.010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Diamond A, Lee K. Interventions shown to aid executive function development in children 4 to 12 years old. Science (2011) 333:959–64. doi:10.1126/science.1204529

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Robinson KE, Kaizar E, Catroppa C, Godfrey C, Yeates KO. Systematic review and meta-analysis of cognitive interventions for children with central nervous system disorders and neurodevelopmental disorders. J Pediatr Psychol (2014) 39:846–65. doi:10.1093/jpepsy/jsu031

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Titz C, Karbach J. Working memory and executive functions: effects of training on academic achievement. Psychol Res (2014) 78:852–68. doi:10.1007/s00426-013-0537-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Chandler DJ, Waterhouse BD, Gao W-J. New perspectives on catecholaminergic regulation of executive circuits: evidence for independent modulation of prefrontal functions by midbrain dopaminergic and noradrenergic neurons. Front Neural Circuits (2014) 8:53. doi:10.3389/fncir.2014.00053

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Goldman-Rakic PS. The prefrontal landscape: implications of functional architecture for understanding human mentation and the central executive. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci (1996) 351:1445–53. doi:10.1098/rstb.1996.0129

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Cools R, D’Esposito M. Inverted-U shaped dopamine actions on human working memory and cognitive control. Biol Psychiatry (2011) 69:e113–25. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.03.028

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Durstewitz D, Seamans JK. The dual-state theory of prefrontal cortex dopamine function with relevance to catechol-o-methyltransferase genotypes and schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry (2008) 64:739–49. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.05.015

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Dreyer JK, Herrik KF, Berg RW, Hounsgaard JD. Influence of phasic and tonic dopamine release on receptor activation. J Neurosci (2010) 30:14273–83. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1894-10.2010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Goschke T, Bolte A. Emotional modulation of control dilemmas: the role of positive affect, reward, and dopamine in cognitive stability and flexibility. Neuropsychologia (2014) 62:403–23. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.07.015

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Brinkman TM, Zhang N, Ullrich NJ, Brouwers P, Green DM, Srivastava DK, et al. Psychoactive medication use and neurocognitive function in adult survivors of childhood cancer: a report from the childhood cancer survivor study. Pediatr Blood Cancer (2013) 60:486–93. doi:10.1002/pbc.24255

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Pangilinan PH, Giacoletti-Argento A, Shellhaas R, Hurvitz EA, Hornyak JE. Neuropharmacology in pediatric brain injury: a review. PM R (2010) 2:1127–40. doi:10.1016/j.pmrj.2010.07.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Mulhern RK, Khan RB, Kaplan S, Helton S, Christensen R, Bonner M, et al. Short-term efficacy of methylphenidate: a randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled trial among survivors of childhood cancer. J Clin Oncol (2004) 22:4795–803. doi:10.1200/JCO.2004.04.128

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Portela MA, Rubiales AS, Centeno C. The use of psychostimulants in cancer patients. Curr Opin Support Palliat Care (2011) 5:164–8. doi:10.1097/SPC.0b013e3283462ff3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Smithson EF, Phillips R, Harvey DW, Morrall MCHJ. The use of stimulant medication to improve neurocognitive and learning outcomes in children diagnosed with brain tumours: a systematic review. Eur J Cancer (2013) 49:3029–40. doi:10.1016/j.ejca.2013.05.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Daly B, Kral MC, Brown RT, Elkin D, Madan-Swain A, Mitchell M, et al. Ameliorating attention problems in children with sickle cell disease: a pilot study of methylphenidate. J Dev Behav Pediatr (2012) 33:244–51. doi:10.1097/DBP.0b013e31824ba1b5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Lidzba K, Granstroem S, Leark RA, Kraegeloh-Mann I, Mautner V-F. Pharmacotherapy of attention deficit in neurofibromatosis type 1: effects on cognition. Neuropediatrics (2014) 45:240–6. doi:10.1055/s-0034-1368117

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Conklin HM, Reddick WE, Ashford J, Ogg S, Howard SC, Morris EB, et al. Long-term efficacy of methylphenidate in enhancing attention regulation, social skills, and academic abilities of childhood cancer survivors. J Clin Oncol (2010) 28:4465–72. doi:10.1200/JCO.2010.28.4026

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Devlin AM, Panagiotopoulos C. Metabolic side effects and pharmacogenetics of second-generation antipsychotics in children. Pharmacogenomics (2015) 16:981–96. doi:10.2217/pgs.15.55

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Groenman AP, Schweren LJS, Dietrich A, Hoekstra PJ. An update on the safety of psychostimulants for the treatment of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Expert Opin Drug Saf (2017) 16:455–64. doi:10.1080/14740338.2017.1301928

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Buzsáki G, Logothetis N, Singer W. Scaling brain size, keeping timing: evolutionary preservation of brain rhythms. Neuron (2013) 80:751–64. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2013.10.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Düzel E, Penny WD, Burgess N. Brain oscillations and memory. Curr Opin Neurobiol (2010) 20:143–9. doi:10.1016/j.conb.2010.01.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Cavanagh JF, Frank MJ. Frontal theta as a mechanism for cognitive control. Trends Cogn Sci (2014) 18:414–21. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2014.04.012

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Sarnthein J, Petsche H, Rappelsberger P, Shaw GL, von Stein A. Synchronization between prefrontal and posterior association cortex during human working memory. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A (1998) 95:7092–6. doi:10.1073/pnas.95.12.7092

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. von Stein A, Sarnthein J. Different frequencies for different scales of cortical integration: from local gamma to long range alpha/theta synchronization. Int J Psychophysiol (2000) 38:301–13. doi:10.1016/S0167-8760(00)00172-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Johnson EL, Dewar CD, Solbakk A-K, Endestad T, Meling TR, Knight RT. Bidirectional frontoparietal oscillatory systems support working memory. Curr Biol (2017) 27:1829.e–35.e. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2017.05.046

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Fries P. A mechanism for cognitive dynamics: neuronal communication through neuronal coherence. Trends Cogn Sci (2005) 9:474–80. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2005.08.011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Varela F, Lachaux J-P, Rodriguez E, Martinerie J. The brainweb: phase synchronization and large-scale integration. Nat Rev Neurosci (2001) 2:229–39. doi:10.1038/35067550

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Engel AK, Fries P, Singer W. Dynamic predictions: oscillations and synchrony in top-down processing. Nat Rev Neurosci (2001) 2:704–16. doi:10.1038/35094565

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Mizuhara H, Wang L-Q, Kobayashi K, Yamaguchi Y. A long-range cortical network emerging with theta oscillation in a mental task. Neuroreport (2004) 15:1233–8. doi:10.1097/01.wnr.0000126755.09715.b3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Engel AK, Fries P. Beta-band oscillations – signalling the status quo? Curr Opin Neurobiol (2010) 20:156–65. doi:10.1016/j.conb.2010.02.015

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Klimesch W. α-band oscillations, attention, and controlled access to stored information. Trends Cogn Sci (2012) 16:606–17. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2012.10.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Freunberger R, Werkle-Bergner M, Griesmayr B, Lindenberger U, Klimesch W. Brain oscillatory correlates of working memory constraints. Brain Res (2011) 1375:93–102. doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2010.12.048

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Hwang K, Ghuman AS, Manoach DS, Jones SR, Luna B. Frontal preparatory neural oscillations associated with cognitive control: a developmental study comparing young adults and adolescents. Neuroimage (2016) 136:139–48. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.05.017

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Cohen MX. A neural microcircuit for cognitive conflict detection and signaling. Trends Neurosci (2014) 37:480–90. doi:10.1016/j.tins.2014.06.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Helfrich RF, Knight RT. Oscillatory dynamics of prefrontal cognitive control. Trends Cogn Sci (2016) 20:916–30. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2016.09.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Mathalon DH, Sohal VS. Neural oscillations and synchrony in brain dysfunction and neuropsychiatric disorders: it’s about time. JAMA Psychiatry (2015) 72:840–4. doi:10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2015.0483

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Singer W. Neuronal oscillations: unavoidable and useful? Eur J Neurosci (2017). doi:10.1111/ejn.13796

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Uhlhaas PJ, Singer W. Neuronal dynamics and neuropsychiatric disorders: toward a translational paradigm for dysfunctional large-scale networks. Neuron (2012) 75:963–80. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2012.09.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Yahata N, Kasai K, Kawato M. Computational neuroscience approach to biomarkers and treatments for mental disorders. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci (2017) 71:215–37. doi:10.1111/pcn.12502

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Bluschke A, Broschwitz F, Kohl S, Roessner V, Beste C. The neuronal mechanisms underlying improvement of impulsivity in ADHD by theta/beta neurofeedback. Sci Rep (2016) 6:31178. doi:10.1038/srep31178

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

72. Hammond DC. What is neurofeedback: an update. J Neurother (2011) 15:305–36. doi:10.1080/10874208.2011.623090

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

73. Micoulaud-Franchi J-A, Geoffroy PA, Fond G, Lopez R, Bioulac S, Philip P. EEG neurofeedback treatments in children with ADHD: an updated meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. Front Hum Neurosci (2014) 8:906. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2014.00906

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

74. Heinrich H, Busch K, Studer P, Erbe K, Moll GH, Kratz O. EEG spectral analysis of attention in ADHD: implications for neurofeedback training? Front Hum Neurosci (2014) 8:611. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2014.00611

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

75. Sonuga-Barke EJS, Brandeis D, Cortese S, Daley D, Ferrin M, Holtmann M, et al. Nonpharmacological interventions for ADHD: systematic review and meta-analyses of randomized controlled trials of dietary and psychological treatments. Am J Psychiatry (2013) 170:275–89. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2012.12070991

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

76. Bluschke A, Roessner V, Beste C. Editorial perspective: how to optimise frequency band neurofeedback for ADHD. J Child Psychol Psychiatry (2016) 57:457–61. doi:10.1111/jcpp.12521

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

77. Alvarez J, Meyer FL, Granoff DL, Lundy A. The effect of EEG biofeedback on reducing postcancer cognitive impairment. Integr Cancer Ther (2013) 12:475–87. doi:10.1177/1534735413477192

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

78. Sterman MB, Egner T. Foundation and practice of neurofeedback for the treatment of epilepsy. Appl Psychophysiol Biofeedback (2006) 31:21–35. doi:10.1007/s10484-006-9002-x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

79. Hammond DC. Treatment of chronic fatigue with neurofeedback and self-hypnosis. NeuroRehabilitation (2001) 16:295–300.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

80. de Ruiter MA, Oosterlaan J, Schouten-van Meeteren AYN, Maurice-Stam H, van Vuurden DG, Gidding C, et al. Neurofeedback ineffective in paediatric brain tumour survivors: results of a double-blind randomised placebo-controlled trial. Eur J Cancer (2016) 64:62–73. doi:10.1016/j.ejca.2016.04.020

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

81. Vosskuhl J, Strüber D, Herrmann CS. Transkranielle wechselstromstimulation. Nervenarzt (2015) 86:1516–22. doi:10.1007/s00115-015-4317-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

82. Jaušovec N, Jaušovec K. Increasing working memory capacity with theta transcranial alternating current stimulation (tACS). Biol Psychol (2014) 96:42–7. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2013.11.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

83. Fregni F, Otachi PTM, Do Valle A, Boggio PS, Thut G, Rigonatti SP, et al. A randomized clinical trial of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation in patients with refractory epilepsy. Ann Neurol (2006) 60:447–55. doi:10.1002/ana.20950

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

84. Brittain J-S, Probert-Smith P, Aziz TZ, Brown P. Tremor suppression by rhythmic transcranial current stimulation. Curr Biol (2013) 23:436–40. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2013.01.068

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

85. Jones KT, Peterson DJ, Blacker KJ, Berryhill ME. Frontoparietal neurostimulation modulates working memory training benefits and oscillatory synchronization. Brain Res (2017) 1667:28–40. doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2017.05.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

86. Krishnan C, Santos L, Peterson MD, Ehinger M. Safety of noninvasive brain stimulation in children and adolescents. Brain Stimulat (2015) 8:76–87. doi:10.1016/j.brs.2014.10.012

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

87. Striefel S. Ethics in neurofeedback practice. In: Budzynski TH, Budzynski HK, Evans JR, Abarbanel A, editors. Introduction to Quantitative EEG and Neurofeedback: Advanced Theory and Applications. Oxford: Academic Press (2009). p. 475–92.

Google Scholar

88. Hammond DC, Kirk L. First, do no harm: adverse effects and the need for practice standards in neurofeedback. J Neurother (2008) 12:79–88. doi:10.1080/10874200802219947

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

89. Rossiter TR. Patient-directed neurofeedback for AD/HD. J Neurother (1998) 2:54–64. doi:10.1300/J184v02n04_04

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

90. Wei T-Y, Chang D-W, Liu Y-D, Liu C-W, Young C-P, Liang S-F, et al. Portable wireless neurofeedback system of EEG alpha rhythm enhances memory. Biomed Eng Online (2017) 16:128. doi:10.1186/s12938-017-0418-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

91. Rogante M, Silvestri S, Grigioni M, Zampolini M. Electromyographic audio biofeedback for telerehabilitation in hospital. J Telemed Telecare (2010) 16:204–6. doi:10.1258/jtt.2010.004012

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

92. Espay AJ, Baram Y, Dwivedi AK, Shukla R, Gartner M, Gaines L, et al. At-home training with closed-loop augmented-reality cueing device for improving gait in patients with Parkinson disease. J Rehabil Res Dev (2010) 47:573–81. doi:10.1682/JRRD.2009.10.0165

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: cognitive dysfunctions, executive function, therapy, cognitive training, chronic disease

Citation: Bluschke A, von der Hagen M, Novotna B, Roessner V and Beste C (2018) Executive Function Deficits in Seriously Ill Children—Emerging Challenges and Possibilities for Clinical Care. Front. Pediatr. 6:92. doi: 10.3389/fped.2018.00092

Received: 11 January 2018; Accepted: 23 March 2018;
Published: 18 April 2018

Edited by:

Richard Eugene Frye, Phoenix Children’s Hospital, United States

Reviewed by:

Elizabeth L. Johnson, University of California, Berkeley, United States
Gerry Leisman, University of Haifa, Israel
Amedeo D’Angiulli, Carleton University, Canada

Copyright: © 2018 Bluschke, von der Hagen, Novotna, Roessner and Beste. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Annet Bluschke, annet.bluschke@uniklinikum-dresden.de

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.