Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Pain Res., 07 September 2023
Sec. Pain Research Methods
This article is part of the Research Topic Investigations of neural processes underlying normal pain perception, its regulation, and alterations in aberrant pain conditions View all articles

Pain catastrophizing is associated with reduced neural response to monetary reward

\r\nMegan E. Cooke,,Megan E. Cooke1,2,3Robert R. Edwards,Robert R. Edwards3,4Grace L. Wheeler,Grace L. Wheeler1,2William A. Schmitt,William A. Schmitt1,2Lindsay V. NielsenLindsay V. Nielsen1Joanna M. Streck,,Joanna M. Streck1,3,5Randi M. Schuster,Randi M. Schuster1,3Kevin PotterKevin Potter1A. Eden Evins,A. Eden Evins1,3Jodi M. Gilman,,
\r\nJodi M. Gilman1,2,3*
  • 1Center for Addiction Medicine, Department of Psychiatry, Massachusetts General Hospital, Boston, MA, United States
  • 2Athinoula A. Martinos Center in Biomedical Imaging, Department of Radiology, Massachusetts General Hospital, Charlestown, MA, United States
  • 3Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, United States
  • 4Department of Anesthesiology, Perioperative, and Pain Medicine, Brigham & Women’s Hospital, Chestnut Hill, MA, United States
  • 5Tobacco Research and Treatment Center, Division of General Internal Medicine, Department of Medicine, Massachusetts General Hospital, Boston, MA, United States

Introduction: Pain catastrophizing, a measure of an individual's negative emotional and cognitive appraisals of pain, has been included as a key treatment target in many psychological interventions for pain. However, the neural correlates of pain catastrophizing have been understudied. Prior neuroimaging evidence suggests that adults with pain show altered reward processing throughout the mesocorticolimbic reward circuitry.

Methods: In this study, we tested the association between Pain Catastrophizing Scale (PCS) scores and neural activation to the Monetary Incentive Delay (MID) reward neuroimaging task in 94 adults reporting a range of pain, insomnia, and mood symptoms.

Results: Results indicated that PCS score but not pain intensity was significantly associated with blunted activation in the caudate and putamen in response to feedback of successful vs. unsuccessful trials on the MID task. Mediation analyses indicated that PCS score fully mediated the relationship between depression symptoms and reward activation.

Discussion: These findings provide evidence that pain catastrophizing is independently associated with altered striatal function apart from depression symptoms and pain intensity. Thus, in individuals experiencing pain and/or co- morbid conditions, reward dysfunction is directly related to pain catastrophizing.

1. Introduction

Millions of Americans suffer from pain, which is a defining characteristic of many medical and psychiatric conditions, and is associated with substantial morbidity, mortality and disability (13). Research suggests that fear appraisal (i.e., predictions or interpretations of sensory input as threatening) plays an important role in the experience of pain (49). For example, studies have demonstrated that appraisal of innocuous sensory input as painful leads to experiencing pain, which in turn leads to further appraisal of threat and subsequent further increased pain, resulting in a self-reinforcing feedback loop (1012). The fear-avoidance model (13) suggests that individuals who experience acute pain might develop fear and avoidance behaviors if they associate certain activities or movements with pain. This fear can lead to a cycle of increased pain, disability, and emotional distress.

One important type of fear appraisal is pain catastrophizing, which is broadly conceptualized as an exaggerated negative cognitive or affective response during an actual or anticipated pain experience (e.g., magnification of negative outcome expectancies) (1416). Pain catastrophizing is associated with a host of adverse clinical outcomes independent of pain intensity including amplified pain experiences (17), greater negative functional impact of pain (18), and poor treatment response in patients with chronic pain (1921). Importantly, pain catastrophizing has been listed as a risk factor for the development of chronic pain (22). As a result, pain catastrophizing has been included as a key treatment target in many cognitive-behavioral, acceptance- and mindfulness-based psychological treatments for pain (4, 2325).

The neurobiological mechanisms underlying pain catastrophizing are not fully understood, especially regarding reward processing. Existing neuroimaging evidence suggests that adults with chronic pain, particularly those with co-morbid depression, show altered reward processing throughout the mesocorticolimbic reward circuitry (2634), however this relationship has not been examined with pain catastrophizing. The relationship between pain catastrophizing and neural reward processes is critical for investigation because dysfunction in the mesocorticolimic system may indicate a mechanism through which pain catastrophizing exacerbates pain and poorer quality of life and thus could act as a neural target for treatment. Generally, pain catastrophizing is associated with heightened pain perception, emotional distress, and negative affect. This negative emotional state could potentially influence the brain's reward system and its response to rewarding stimuli. People who engage in pain catastrophizing might have difficulties experiencing rewards or positive emotions due to their heightened focus on pain and negative outcomes. Additionally, there is ongoing debate about whether pain catastrophizing is a distinct construct from depression given that both measure negative affect constructs (14). Some evidence suggests that pain catastrophizing is a distinct but related construct to depression and that pain catastrophizing mediates the relationship between depression and pain (14, 35, 36). However, no studies have examined whether depression and pain catastrophizing are associated with unique neural processes which is critical to begin parsing out these constructs as distinct vs. overlapping constructs in their contributions to pain.

Our group recently reported results of a clinical trial among a heterogeneous group of participants with pain, insomnia, and anxiety/depression symptoms who were randomized to medical cannabis or a waitlist control condition (37). This trial included a comprehensive functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) battery at baseline, in addition to self-reported pain measures. Here, we report a secondary analysis of baseline fMRI scans, in which we investigate neural mechanisms associated with pain catastrophizing. We aimed to (1) investigate the neural correlates associated with pain catastrophizing among adults using baseline neuroimaging data from the parent clinical trial and (2) to examine the extent to which pain catastrophizing and depression display similar or different patterns of brain activation. We hypothesized that higher pain catastrophizing would be associated with decreased activation to monetary reward in the striatum, beyond the influence of more general affective factors such as comorbid depression symptoms.

2. Methods

2.1. Participants

This present study represents a secondary analysis of a larger randomized clinical trial of medical cannabis, (NCT03224468) and non-cannabis using community members with self-reported symptoms of pain, insomnia, depression, or anxiety. Participants were recruited primarily through community advertising. Exclusionary criteria included current heavy cannabis use (daily or almost-daily in the past 3 months), any magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) contraindications (e.g., pregnancy, claustrophobia), history of serious medical disorders (e.g., diabetes, cardiovascular disease, HIV, hepatitis C, migraines, head injury), as well as any current substance use disorder. All participants were enrolled between July 17, 2017 and February 11, 2020 and provided written informed consent. Screening procedures took place at Massachusetts General Hospital Center for Addiction Medicine, and scans were conducted at the Athinoula A. Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging. This study was approved by the MassGeneral Brigham Human Research Committee.

2.2. Procedure

2.2.1. Trial design

Eligible participants attended a baseline visit where they completed a battery of medical and psychiatric questionnaires. Participants also completed a baseline MRI scan at a subsequent session. During the baseline session prior to the MRI scan, participants completed a medical history, psychiatric interview [Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview; MINI (38)], and questionnaires assessing pain intensity, pain catastrophizing, and mood symptoms.

2.2.2. Measures

2.2.2.1. Brief pain inventory

The Pain Severity subscale of the Brief Pain Inventory Short Form (39) (BPI-PS) was used to assess pain intensity on a 0–10 point scale (0 = no pain, 10 = worst pain imaginable).

2.2.2.2. Pain catastrophizing

The Pain Catastrophizing Scale (PCS) (40) consisted of 13 questions addressing feelings and thoughts related to the experience of pain (e.g., “There's nothing I can do to reduce the intensity of the pain.”). Each question was answered on a 5-point Likert scale with 0 being “not at all” and 4 being “all the time”, giving a total score ranging from 0 to 52. Higher scores indicated more catastrophizing.

2.2.2.3. Depression

The Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (HADS) (41) assessed depressive (HADS-Dep) symptoms through 7 questions. Each question was answered on a 4-point Likert scale from 0 to 3, giving a total score ranging from 0 to 21. A score of 11 or higher indicated a probable mood disorder (depression) while a score of 8–10 indicated a borderline case.

2.2.3. Neuroimaging

Participants were scanned using a 3 T Siemens Trio scanner with a 32-channel head coil. Structural and functional scans were acquired using parameters outlined by the Human Connectome Project (HCP) (42). A T1-weighted structural scan was acquired using the following parameters: TA = 7:38 min, voxel size = 0.7 mm3 × 0.7 mm3 × 0.7 mm3, GRAPPA acceleration factor 2, A-P phase encoding, 256 slices, 224 mm FoV read, slice thickness = 0.70 mm, TR = 2,400 ms, TE = 2.02 ms, TI = 1,000 ms, echo spacing 7.6 ms, bandwidth = 270 Hz/Px. Functional MRI data was acquired using the following parameters: TA = 8:47 min, voxel size = 2.0 mm3 × 2.0 mm3 × 2.0 mm3, slices = 69, phase encoding P-A, FoV read 220 mm, slice thickness 2.00 mm, TR = 1,530 ms, TE = 30.0 ms, Multi-slice mode = interleaved, slice acceleration factor PE = 2.

2.2.4. Behavioral reward task

During the MRI scan, participants completed the monetary incentive delay (MID) task to measure reward anticipation and reward delivery of monetary rewards (4345). There were three trial types: win money (small reward: $0.20 or large reward: $5.00), lose money (small punishment: $0.20 or large punishment: $5.00), or no incentive ($0). Participants saw a cue (pink circle, yellow square, or blue triangle) at the beginning of trials that indicated the valence (win/reward, loss/punishment, or no incentive) and the amount of money at stake ($0, $0.20, or $5.00). This cue presentation (2,000 ms) was followed by a jittered anticipatory delay (1,500–4,000 ms). A black target shape (matched shape as the previously presented cue) was then shown, and participants could gain money or avoid losing money by pressing a response button while the target shape was on the screen. The time the target was on the screen (150–500 ms) was dynamically manipulated to maintain a 60% success rate. After a short response window, feedback was provided (2,000 ms total). Participants received 20 reward trials, 20 loss trials, and 10 neutral trials. The adaptive algorithm resulted in 30 positive feedback trials (split between reward, loss, and neutral) and 20 negative feedback trials on average. Participants completed two runs of the task (approximately 5.5 min each). All subjects received practice on the task outside the MRI scanner and the mean reaction time (RT) from this practice was used to set the initial time the target was on the screen.

Performance was measured by calculating accuracy as well as reaction times. After the scan, participants filled out the post-MID questionnaire (PMQ) (43) which asks participants to rate the extent to which they felt vigorous, drowsy, energetic, excited, fearful, happy, quiet, restful, tense, tired, unhappy, or calm when they saw each trial type cue.

2.3. Analysis plan

2.3.1. Demographic and behavioral outcomes

All effects in demographic, behavioral, and imaging outcomes were tested among all participants with baseline data (n = 94) and a useable MRI scan, regardless of recruitment source (clinical trial participants or participants from the community) to maximize statistical power. We investigated the association of PCS with (1) RT, (2) accuracy, and (3) responses on the PMQ by running a linear mixed effects model in R using the lme4 package. Due to the high correlations between emotions assessed in the PMQ, these emotions were averaged within trial type to create 3 broad mood states: positive (vigorous, energetic, excited), negative [drowsy, tired, fearful, unhappy, tense, happy (reversed coded)], and neutral (quiet, calm, restful). In all models, magnitude of MID trial type (small, large, neutral), valence of MID trial type (reward, punishment, neutral), and PCS score were included as fixed effects and participant was included as a random effect.

2.3.2. fMRI data processing

Standard pre-processing procedures were performed in FSL (FMRIB's Software Library, www.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl); each subject's functional and structural scans were first co-registered using FLIRT (FSL's linear registration tool) with functional registration using the BBR cost function with a fieldmap generated from two opposing polarity topup acquisitions, and in a next step registered to a high-resolution image in standard space. Motion correction was performed using MCFLIRT and brain extraction was performed for non-brain removal using BET (FSL's Brain Extraction Tool). Motion outliers were also removed using FSL's motion outlier command. Spatial smoothing was applied using a Gaussian kernel with a full-width at half maximum (FHWM) of 5 mm. A high-pass temporal filter was applied to remove lower frequency noise. Preprocessed images were analyzed using FEAT (FMRI Expert Analysis Tool) in FSL. A first-level within-subject analysis was performed using a general linear model (GLM). For each participant, contrast images of brain activity were generated for higher-level analyses. In a second-level between-subject analysis, regions of significant activation across the brain were assessed using permutation tests (N = 5,000) at a cluster threshold of 2.3 and a significance threshold of p < 0.05, with a reward mask obtained from NeuroSynth (reward_uniformity-test_z_FDR_0.01.nii; see Supplementary Figure S1) (46). Our main interest was the association between PCS and brain activation during the anticipation and feedback stage of the MID task. To differentiate the unique effects of pain catastrophizing and negative affect on reward processing, for any contrast with a cluster associated with PCS, we separately tested the association of PCS, BPI-PS, or HADS-Dep with voxels within the mask. In addition to the voxelwise analyses, we conducted ROI analyses using anatomical striatal masks created in WFUPickAtlas (http://fmri.wfubmc.edu/software/pickatlas) and extracted the mean contrast of parameter estimates (COPEs), or beta weights. The purpose of these analyses was to visualize and further explore effects seen in the voxelwise analyses. Pearson's correlations were calculated to investigate the relationship between change in brain activation within the ROI (win vs. loss trials), and clinical symptoms (PCS, BPI and HADS-Dep scores). ROI analyses were performed in R.

As a specificity analysis, we ran a set of mediation models comparing the extent to which PCS mediated the relationship between HADS-Dep and striatal activation or HADS-Dep mediated the relationship between PCS and striatal activation. Mediation analyses were performed in R using the mediation package (47).

3. Results

3.1. Demographics

Ninety-four participants who had usable baseline scans were included in these analyses. One participant was dropped due to poor scan quality and one was excluded due to a diagnosis of cannabis use disorder at baseline (exclusionary criteria for the parent study). The sample was primarily white and female, with 78% either full time employed or a student, and half the sample met diagnostic criteria for lifetime major depressive or anxiety disorder (See Table 1 for baseline participant characteristics).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Sample demographics and descriptives.

3.2. Correlations

At baseline, pain catastrophizing score (PCS) correlated with depression (r = 0.53, p = 4.08 × 10−8) but was not significantly associated with pain severity (BPI-PS, r = 0.198, p = 0.06).

3.3. Behavioral responses to the MID task

Reaction times (RTs) were significantly shorter for reward (M = 259, SD = 19.9) vs. loss (M = 261, SD = 20.7) or neutral trials (M = 266, SD = 23.7, p = 0.04) and were significantly shorter for large (±$5, M = 258, SD = 19.1) vs. small (±$0.20, M = 262, SD = 21.2) or no incentive trials (M = 266, SD = 23.7, p = 1.74 × 10−7). There was no significant association between PCS and RT (p = 0.31). Accuracy was significantly higher for large reward or punishment trials (p < 2.0 × 10−16) but not for different valence types (p = 0.53). There was no association between PCS and accuracy (p = 0.88). There was no effect of depression (p's > 0.24) and still no effect of PCS (p's > 0.20) on RT or on accuracy when HADS-D was added to the model.

3.4. Pain catastrophizing and MID task mood ratings

Higher PCS scores were associated with lower positive mood across all trial types (Figure 1), with no significant interaction between PCS scores and magnitude (high, low) or valence (reward, loss, neutral) of MID trial type. PCS score was not significantly associated with negative or neutral mood (Table 2). When controlling for depression, the association between PCS score and positive mood was no longer significant (stnd beta = −0.12, t = −1.28, p = 0.20).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. The relationship between PCS and positive mood rating by MID task trial type. PCS scores are broken into 3 categories [None (PCS = 0), Low (1–9), and Med-High (10–41)], for visualization, but were analyzed continuously in the models.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Association of PCS with post-MID task mood ratings.

3.5. Neural responses to the MID task

In a voxel-wise analysis, both anticipation and feedback of reward and loss trials were associated with expected activation of the bilateral striatum, compared to no-incentive trials (Figures S1, S2). PCS was associated with two clusters within the right striatum (MNI coordinates 20, 4, 8, and 18, 22, −4) during the feedback phase (successful > unsuccessful trials) such that those with higher PCS scores had lower striatal activation (Figure 2). PCS was not significantly correlated with activation to either successful hit trials vs. neutral or unsuccessful miss trials vs. neutral, indicating that the difference between successful vs. unsuccessful trials, rather than the response to either alone, was driving this correlation (Figure 3). Neither BPI-PS nor HADS-Dep was associated with caudate or putamen activation in response to feedback for successful trials vs. unsuccessful trials. There were no clusters of brain activation associated with PCS that passed the significance threshold during the anticipation phase of the MID task.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Caudate activation associated with PCS. Brain activation z-score maps, averaged across all participants and thresholded at FDR = 0.05, for anticipation of reward vs neutral contrast, at a threshold of z = 2.31.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Caudate activation by trial type during the feedback phase. PCS scores are broken into 3 categories [None (PCS = 0), Low (1–9), and Med-High (10–41)], for visualization, but were analyzed continuously in the models. Error bars represent standard error of the mean.

Due to the high behavioral correlation between PCS and HADS-Dep seen in this sample and previous findings of an association between blunted activation to the MID task and depression (4850), we performed a mediation analysis to test the specificity of our finding to pain catastrophizing. First, in an ROI analysis with extracted COPEs from the right caudate, we found a negative association between activation to feedback of successful > unsuccessful trials and HADS-Dep scores (r = −0.22, CI = −0.02 to −0.41, p = 0.03). The relationship between HADS-Dep and caudate activation was not significant when PCS was added to the model (beta = −0.02, p = 0.39, CI = −0.06, 0.02, Figure 4A, path C') and PCS fully mediated the relationship between HADS-Dep and caudate activation (indirect effect = −0.02, p = 0.03, 95% CI: −0.04, 0.00, Figure 4A, path AB). HADS-Dep did not significantly mediate the relationship between PCS and right caudate activation (indirect effect = −0.004, p = 0.41, 95% CI: −0.01, 0.00). Therefore, PCS is uniquely associated with right caudate activation, independent from HADS-Dep.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Mediation models examining the mediating effects on caudate activation to successful vs unsuccessful trials. (A) This model shows PCS mediating the relationship between depressive symptoms and caudate activation. (B) This model shows depressive symptoms do not mediate the relationship between PCS and caudate activation. All betas are unstandardized.

We ran two sensitivity analyses. First, to further visualize the association between PCS and activation, we split the sample according to whether participants showed negative or positive activation to successful > unsuccessful trials. Those with positive scores (i.e., subjects with more activation of the right caudate on successful vs. unsuccessful trials) had lower PCS scores (M = 6.5) than the group with negative activation (M = 10.8). The groups did not differ on HADS-Dep (p > .05). BPI-PS did not correlate with activation in the right caudate (p > 0.05). Second, we ran a sensitivity analysis including the 10 participants in the sample who currently met criteria for major depressive disorder or a major depressive episode, and the 51 participants who have never had a major depressive episode. The individuals with current depression showed significantly greater PCS scores (M = 17.20) than those without depression (M = 5.80, t = −2.60, p = 0.03). There was no difference between the groups (MDD vs. no MDD) in activation to successful vs. unsuccessful trials in the right caudate (t = 0.61, p = 0.55). Thus, current depression status did not account for the association between PCS and caudate activation.

4. Discussion

In this sample of 94 individuals, who reported a range of conditions including pain, insomnia, and anxiety and depression symptoms, we found that pain catastrophizing, as assessed with the PCS, was associated with decreased activation in the right caudate on a task designed to elicit activation of the reward system. Mediation analyses indicated that this relationship between pain catastrophizing and caudate activation was distinct from an effect of depression on reward activation. Thus, pain catastrophizing appears to be an important variable to consider when assessing brain mechanisms of reward. Higher pain catastrophizing was also associated with decreased positive mood ratings, though not negative mood ratings, of the MID cues, consistent with prior studies suggesting that individuals high in pain catastrophizing experience and express dampened positive affective responses (51).

Individuals with pain often experience negative emotional states associated with chronic pain that increase the incidence of comorbidities like anxiety and anhedonia (52, 53), which is reflective of the well-established relationship between pain and negative affect that we also observed in this study. Clinical and preclinical data also show that relief of affective component pain is reflected by activation of reward circuits (27, 54, 55). In the current study, we found that while catastrophizing and depression were correlated, only catastrophizing was consistently associated with reduced activation in the right caudate to feedback of successful vs. unsuccessful trials. Thus, pain catastrophizing may be an important contributing factor to consider when studying affective components of pain that may affect brain circuitry.

Decreased striatal activation has been reported in studies of chronic pain patients (5658), almost exclusively during the period of anticipation to reward or loss (56, 58). These studies implicate dysregulation in the striatum and other reward regions, such as the prefrontal cortex (PFC) (58). We attempted to replicate associations between mPFC activation on the MID task and pain catastrophizing but found no significant associations in the anticipation or outcome phase. We note that these previous studies compared groups of individuals with and without chronic pain, and did not include catastrophizing as a main outcome. In our analyses, rather than comparing those with vs. without pain, we included participants with a full range of pain scores and analyzed the PCS continuously. Thus, it is possible that dampened anticipatory activation in the PFC to the MID is a characteristic of chronic pain patients, but may not directly relate to pain catastrophizing. It is also worth noting that these previous studies generally had small sample sizes, which could lead to inflated effect sizes, reiterating the need for continued work to replicate these findings with larger sample sizes (59).

4.1. Limitations

Although the sample size (N = 94) is larger than most previous neuroimaging studies examining pain catastrophizing, replication of these results in larger samples is needed. Additionally, the study was cross-sectional so we are unable to comment on the timing of the development of depressive symptoms, PCS, and reward activation. Relatedly, we were unable to fully disentangle the specific correlations due to pain catastrophizing vs. depression, as these two constructs were highly correlated in our sample. Finally, the associations reported here need to be replicated in other samples including chronic pain samples.

Overall, catastrophizing was consistently associated with reduced reward-related caudate activation during a monetary task (i.e., those higher in catastrophizing showed less caudate activation on feedback of successful trials), and its effects were somewhat diminished when controlling for depressive symptoms, suggesting a co-influence of these inter-related variables on reward processing. This finding may represent a shared association of pain catastrophizing and depression on the reward system. This work further underscores the importance of the reward system in our understanding of the processing of pain. Future pain research should consider the inclusion of pain catastrophizing measures to continue to parse the relationship between pain, catastrophizing, and depression, to better understand if these constructs confer unique or joint effects on caudate activation in response to rewards.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by Massachusetts General Brigham Human Subjects Committee. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

JG, RS, AE contributed to concept and design for the study. KP and MC contributed to the statistical analysis. MC and JG drafted the manuscript. RS, RE, JS, LN, and AE contributed to the critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content. GW and WS collected data and provided feedback on analyses, JG and AE obtained funding for the project. JG and MC had full access to all the data in the study and take responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis. KP conducted and is responsible for data analysis. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

MC is supported by the Louis V. Gerstner III Research Scholar Award from the Gerstner Family Foundation. JG is supported by R01DA042043 from the National Institute on Drug Abuse. RS is supported by K23DA042946 from the National Institute on Drug Abuse. AE is supported by K24DA030443 from the National Institute on Drug Abuse.

Conflict of interest

AE has received research grant support to her institution from Pfizer Inc, Forum Pharmaceuticals, and GK, consultation fees from Charles River Analytics, and honoraria for advisory work from Pfizer, and Kaurna Pharmaceuticals in the past 5 years for work unrelated to this project.

The remaining authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpain.2023.1129353/full#supplementary-material

References

1. Dahlhamer J. Prevalence of chronic pain and high-impact chronic pain among adults—United States, 2016. MMWR Morb Mortal Wkly Rep (2018) 67:1001–6. doi: 10.15585/mmwr.mm6736a2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Institute of Medicine. Relieving pain in America: A blueprint for transforming prevention, care, education, and research. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press (2011). doi: 10.17226/13172

3. Zajacova A, Grol-Prokopczyk H, Zimmer Z. Pain trends among American adults, 2002–2018: patterns, disparities, and correlates. Demography. (2021) 58:711–38. doi: 10.1215/00703370-8977691

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Ashar YK, Gordon A, Schubiner H, Uipi C, Knight K, Anderson Z, et al. Effect of pain reprocessing therapy vs placebo and usual care for patients with chronic back pain: a randomized clinical trial. JAMA Psychiatry. (2021) 79(1):3–23. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2021.2669

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Barrett LF. The theory of constructed emotion: an active inference account of interoception and categorization. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. (2017) 12:1833. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsx060

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Barrett LF, Simmons WK. Interoceptive predictions in the brain. Nat Rev Neurosci. (2015) 16:419–29. doi: 10.1038/nrn3950

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Corder G, Ahanonu B, Grewe BF, Wang D, Schnitzer MJ, Scherrer G. An amygdalar neural ensemble that encodes the unpleasantness of pain. Science. (2019) 363:276–81. doi: 10.1126/science.aap8586

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Sterling P. Allostasis: a model of predictive regulation. Physiol Behav (2012) 106:5–15. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2011.06.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Woolf CJ. Central sensitization: implications for the diagnosis and treatment of pain. PAIN. (2011) 152:S2. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2010.09.030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Baliki MN, Apkarian AV. Nociception, pain, negative moods, and behavior selection. Neuron. (2015) 87:474–91. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.06.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Büchel C, Geuter S, Sprenger C, Eippert F. Placebo analgesia: a predictive coding perspective. Neuron. (2014) 81:1223–39. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.02.042

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Seymour B. Pain: a precision signal for reinforcement learning and control. Neuron. (2019) 101:1029–41. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2019.01.055

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Vlaeyen JWS, Linton SJ. Fear-avoidance and its consequences in chronic musculoskeletal pain: a state of the art. Pain. (2000) 85:317–32. doi: 10.1016/S0304-3959(99)00242-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Leung L. Pain catastrophizing: an updated review. Indian J Psychol Med. (2012) 34:204–17. doi: 10.4103/0253-7176.106012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Petrini L, Arendt-Nielsen L. Understanding pain catastrophizing: putting pieces together. Front Psychol. (2020) 11:603420. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.603420

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Quartana PJ, Campbell CM, Edwards RR. Pain catastrophizing: a critical review. Expert Rev Neurother. (2009) 9:745–58. doi: 10.1586/ern.09.34

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Vervoort T, Goubert L, Eccleston C, Bijttebier P, Crombez G. Catastrophic thinking about pain is independently associated with pain severity, disability, and somatic complaints in school children and children with chronic pain. J Pediatr Psychol. (2006) 31:674–83. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/jsj059

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Edwards RR, Haythornthwaite JA, Smith MT, ScM BK, Katz JN. Catastrophizing and depressive symptoms as prospective predictors of outcomes following total knee replacement. Pain Research & Management. (2009) 14:6.

Google Scholar

19. Burns JW, Kubilus A, Bruehl S, Harden RN, Lofland K. Do changes in cognitive factors influence outcome following multidisciplinary treatment for chronic pain? A cross-lagged panel analysis. J Consult Clin Psychol. (2003) 71:81–91. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.71.1.81

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Edwards RR, Dworkin RH, Sullivan MD, Turk DC, Wasan AD. The role of psychosocial processes in the development and maintenance of chronic pain. J Pain. (2016) 17:T70–92. doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2016.01.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Edwards RR, Cahalan C, Calahan C, Mensing G, Smith M, Haythornthwaite JA. Pain, catastrophizing, and depression in the rheumatic diseases. Nat Rev Rheumatol. (2011) 7:216–24. doi: 10.1038/nrrheum.2011.2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. McGreevy K, Bottros MM, Raja SN. Preventing chronic pain following acute pain: risk factors, preventive strategies, and their efficacy. Eur J Pain Suppl. (2011) 5:365–72. doi: 10.1016/j.eujps.2011.08.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Cherkin DC, Sherman KJ, Balderson BH, Cook AJ, Anderson ML, Hawkes RJ, et al. Effect of mindfulness-based stress reduction vs cognitive behavioral therapy or usual care on back pain and functional limitations in adults with chronic low back pain: a randomized clinical trial. JAMA. (2016) 315:1240–9. doi: 10.1001/jama.2016.2323

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. McCracken LM, Vowles KE. Acceptance and commitment therapy and mindfulness for chronic pain: model, process, and progress. Am Psychol. (2014) 69:178–87. doi: 10.1037/a0035623

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Thorn BE, Eyer JC, Van Dyke BP, Torres CA, Burns JW, Kim M, et al. Literacy-adapted cognitive behavioral therapy versus education for chronic pain at low-income clinics. Ann Intern Med. (2018) 168:471–80. doi: 10.7326/M17-0972

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Baliki MN, Petre B, Torbey S, Herrmann KM, Huang L, Schnitzer TJ, et al. Corticostriatal functional connectivity predicts transition to chronic back pain. Nat Neurosci. (2012) 15:1117–9. doi: 10.1038/nn.3153

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Baliki MN, Geha PY, Fields HL, Apkarian AV. Predicting value of pain and analgesia: nucleus accumbens response to noxious stimuli changes in the presence of chronic pain. Neuron. (2010) 66:149–60. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.03.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Berger SE, Baria AT, Baliki MN, Mansour A, Herrmann KM, Torbey S, et al. Risky monetary behavior in chronic back pain is associated with altered modular connectivity of the nucleus accumbens. BMC Res Notes. (2014) 7:739. doi: 10.1186/1756-0500-7-739

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. de Heer EW, Gerrits MMJG, Beekman ATF, Dekker J, van Marwijk HWJ, de Waal MWM, et al. The association of depression and anxiety with pain: a study from NESDA. PLoS One. (2014) 9:e106907. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0106907

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Garland EL, Trøstheim M, Eikemo M, Ernst G, Leknes S. Anhedonia in chronic pain and prescription opioid misuse. Psychol Med. (2020) 50:1977–88. doi: 10.1017/S0033291719002010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Leknes S, Tracey I. A common neurobiology for pain and pleasure. Nat Rev Neurosci. (2008) 9:314–20. doi: 10.1038/nrn2333

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Loggia ML, Berna C, Kim J, Cahalan CM, Gollub RL, Wasan AD, et al. Disrupted brain circuitry for pain-related reward/punishment in fibromyalgia. Arthritis Rheumatol. (2014) 66:203–12. doi: 10.1002/art.38191

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Martikainen IK, Nuechterlein EB, Peciña M, Love TM, Cummiford CM, Green CR, et al. Chronic back pain is associated with alterations in dopamine neurotransmission in the ventral Striatum. J Neurosci. (2015) 35:9957–65. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4605-14.2015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Martikainen IK, Peciña M, Love TM, Nuechterlein EB, Cummiford CM, Green CR, et al. Alterations in endogenous opioid functional measures in chronic back pain. J Neurosci. (2013) 33:14729–37. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1400-13.2013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Keefe FJ, Lefebvre JC, Egert JR, Affleck G, Sullivan MJ, Caldwell DS. The relationship of gender to pain, pain behavior, and disability in osteoarthritis patients: the role of catastrophizing. Pain. (2000) 87:325–34. doi: 10.1016/S0304-3959(00)00296-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Michaelides A, Zis P. Depression, anxiety and acute pain: links and management challenges. Postgrad Med. (2019) 131:438–44. doi: 10.1080/00325481.2019.1663705

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Gilman JM, Schuster RM, Potter KW, Schmitt W, Wheeler G, Pachas GN, et al. Effect of medical marijuana card ownership on pain, insomnia, and affective disorder symptoms in adults: a randomized clinical trial. JAMA Netw Open. (2022) 5(3):e222106. doi: 10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2022.2106

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Sheehan DV, Lecrubier Y, Sheehan KH, Amorim P, Janavs J, Weiller E, et al. The mini-international neuropsychiatric interview (M.I.N.I.): the development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. J Clin Psychiatry. (1998) 59(Suppl 20):22–33; quiz 34–57.9881538

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

39. Cleeland CS, Ryan KM. Pain assessment: global use of the brief pain inventory. Ann Acad Med Singapore. (1994) 23:129–38.8080219

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

40. Sullivan MJ, Bishop SR, Pivik J. The pain catastrophizing scale: development and validation. Psychol Assess. (1995) 7:524. doi: 10.1037/1040-3590.7.4.524

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Bjelland I, Dahl AA, Haug TT, Neckelmann D. The validity of the hospital anxiety and depression scale: an updated literature review. J Psychosom Res. (2002) 52:69–77. doi: 10.1016/S0022-3999(01)00296-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Van Essen DC, Smith SM, Barch DM, Behrens TEJ, Yacoub E, Ugurbil K. The WU-minn human connectome project: an overview. NeuroImage. (2013) 80:62–79. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.05.041

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Casey BJ, Cannonier T, Conley MI, Cohen AO, Barch DM, Heitzeg MM, et al. The adolescent brain cognitive development (ABCD) study: imaging acquisition across 21 sites. Dev Cogn Neurosci. (2018) 32:43–54. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2018.03.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Knutson B, Westdorp A, Kaiser E, Hommer D. FMRI visualization of brain activity during a monetary incentive delay task. NeuroImage. (2000) 12:20–7. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2000.0593

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Yau W-YW, Zubieta J-K, Weiland BJ, Samudra PG, Zucker RA, Heitzeg MM. Nucleus accumbens response to incentive stimuli anticipation in children of alcoholics: relationships with precursive behavioral risk and lifetime alcohol use. J Neurosci. (2012) 32:2544–51. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1390-11.2012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Yarkoni T, Poldrack RA, Nichols TE, Van Essen DC, Wager TD. Large-scale automated synthesis of human functional neuroimaging data. Nat Methods. (2011) 8:665–70. doi: 10.1038/nmeth.1635

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Tingley D, Yamamoto T, Hirose K, Keele L, Imai K. mediation: r package for causal mediation analysis. J Stat Softw. (2014) 59:1–38. doi: 10.18637/jss.v059.i05

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Forbes EE, Hariri AR, Martin SL, Silk JS, Moyles DL, Fisher PM, et al. Altered striatal activation predicting real-world positive affect in adolescent Major depressive disorder. Am J Psychiatry. (2009) 166:64–73. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.07081336

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Pizzagalli DA, Holmes AJ, Dillon DG, Goetz EL, Birk JL, Bogdan R, et al. Reduced caudate and nucleus Accumbens response to rewards in unmedicated individuals with Major depressive disorder. Am J Psychiatry. (2009) 166:702–10. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.08081201

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Zhang W-N, Chang S-H, Guo L-Y, Zhang K-L, Wang J. The neural correlates of reward-related processing in major depressive disorder: a meta-analysis of functional magnetic resonance imaging studies. J Affect Disord. (2013) 151:531–9. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2013.06.039

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Pulvers K, Hood A. The role of positive traits and pain catastrophizing in pain perception. Curr Pain Headache Rep. (2013) 17:330. doi: 10.1007/s11916-013-0330-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Elman I, Borsook D, Volkow ND. Pain and suicidality: insights from reward and addiction neuroscience. Prog Neurobiol. (2013) 109:1–27. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2013.06.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. McWilliams LA, Cox BJ, Enns MW. Mood and anxiety disorders associated with chronic pain: an examination in a nationally representative sample. Pain. (2003) 106:127–33. doi: 10.1016/S0304-3959(03)00301-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Becerra L, Borsook D. Signal valence in the nucleus accumbens to pain onset and offset. Eur J Pain. (2008) 12:866–9. doi: 10.1016/j.ejpain.2007.12.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Navratilova E, Xie JY, Okun A, Qu C, Eyde N, Ci S, et al. Pain relief produces negative reinforcement through activation of mesolimbic reward–valuation circuitry. Proc Natl Acad Sci. (2012) 109:20709–13. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1214605109

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Kim M, Mawla I, Albrecht DS, Admon R, Torrado-Carvajal A, Bergan C, et al. Striatal hypofunction as a neural correlate of mood alterations in chronic pain patients. NeuroImage. (2020) 211:116656. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.116656

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Loggia ML, Berna C, Kim J, Cahalan CM, Martel M-O, Gollub RL, et al. The lateral prefrontal cortex mediates the hyperalgesic effects of negative cognitions in chronic pain patients. J Pain Off J Am Pain Soc. (2015) 16:692–9. doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2015.04.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Martucci KT, Borg N, MacNiven KH, Knutson B, Mackey SC. Altered prefrontal correlates of monetary anticipation and outcome in chronic pain. Pain. (2018) 159:1494–507. doi: 10.1097/j.pain.0000000000001232

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Marek S, Tervo-Clemmens B, Calabro FJ, Montez DF, Kay BP, Hatoum AS, et al. Reproducible brain-wide association studies require thousands of individuals. Nature. (2022) 603:654–60. doi: 10.1038/s41586-022-04492-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: monetary incentive delay, depression, pain, fMRI, caudate, putamen

Citation: Cooke ME, Edwards RR, Wheeler GL, Schmitt WA, Nielsen LV, Streck JM, Schuster RM, Potter K, Evins A.E and Gilman JM (2023) Pain catastrophizing is associated with reduced neural response to monetary reward. Front. Pain Res. 4:1129353. doi: 10.3389/fpain.2023.1129353

Received: 22 December 2022; Accepted: 25 August 2023;
Published: 7 September 2023.

Edited by:

Lejian Huang, Northwestern University, United States

Reviewed by:

Pascal Tetreault, Université de Sherbrooke, Canada
Laura Petrini, Aalborg University, Denmark

© 2023 Cooke, Edwards, Wheeler, Schmitt, Nielsen, Streck, Schuster, Potter, Evins and Gilman. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Jodi M. Gilman jgilman1@mgh.harvard.edu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.