Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Pain Res., 10 May 2022
Sec. Pain Research Methods
This article is part of the Research Topic Women in Science: Pain Research Methods View all 6 articles

Understanding Pain and Trauma Symptoms in Veterans From Resting-State Connectivity: Unsupervised Modeling

  • 1Emotion and Pain Laboratory, San Francisco Veterans Affairs Health Care Center, San Francisco, CA, United States
  • 2Department of Psychiatry, University of California, San Francisco, San Francisco, CA, United States
  • 3Stress and Neuroimaging Laboratory, San Diego Veterans Affairs Health Care Center, San Francisco, CA, United States
  • 4Center of Excellence in Stress and Mental Health, San Diego Veterans Affairs Health Care Center, San Diego, CA, United States
  • 5Department of Psychiatry, University of California, San Diego, San Diego, CA, United States

Trauma and posttraumatic stress are highly comorbid with chronic pain and are often antecedents to developing chronic pain conditions. Pain and trauma are associated with greater utilization of medical services, greater use of psychiatric medication, and increased total cost of treatment. Despite the high overlap in the clinic, the neural mechanisms of pain and trauma are often studied separately. In this study, resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging (rs-fMRI) scans were completed among a diagnostically heterogeneous sample of veterans with a range of back pain and trauma symptoms. Using Group Iterative Multiple Model Estimation (GIMME), an effective functional connectivity analysis, we explored an unsupervised model deriving subgroups based on path similarity in a priori defined regions of interest (ROIs) from brain regions implicated in the experience of pain and trauma. Three subgroups were identified by patterns in functional connection and differed significantly on several psychological measures despite similar demographic and diagnostic characteristics. The first subgroup was highly connected overall, was characterized by functional connectivity from the nucleus accumbens (NAc), the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), and the posterior cingulate cortex (PCC) to the insula and scored low on pain and trauma symptoms. The second subgroup did not significantly differ from the first subgroup on pain and trauma measures but was characterized by functional connectivity from the ACC and NAc to the thalamus and from ACC to PCC. The third subgroup was characterized by functional connectivity from the thalamus and PCC to NAc and scored high on pain and trauma symptoms. Our results suggest that, despite demographic and diagnostic similarities, there may be neurobiologically dissociable biotypes with different mechanisms for managing pain and trauma. These findings may have implications for the determination of appropriate biotype-specific interventions that target these neurological systems.

Introduction

Pain and trauma are highly comorbid conditions (16), especially among the military and veteran population (7, 8), and both are associated with dramatic changes in the brain structure and function (9, 10). Using experimental pain and task-based functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), we and others have repeatedly shown that men and women with trauma demonstrate dysregulated behavioral (11, 12) and brain response to pain, depicted in the increased pain avoidance and dysregulated modulatory response within interoceptive, reward, and frontal modulatory networks (1317).

Resting-state fMRI (rs-fMRI) allows for the identification of resting-state networks, which represent spatial distributions of synchronized fluctuations in blood oxygen level-dependent (BOLD) fMRI responses over time (18). Several neuroimaging studies have shown that chronic low back pain is associated with alterations in resting-state brain activity and dynamics (1923). Changes within default mode network (DMN), salience network (SN), and cortico-striatal connectivity have all been reported (24, 25), and reduced resting-state insula connectivity in chronic back pain resolved following successful spinal surgery or zygapophysial (facet) joint block (26). Similarly, growing research demonstrates that chronic trauma is associated with alterations in resting-state connectivity such as DMN, SN, and central executive network (CEN, also referred to as the frontoparietal control network) (27, 28). Substantial overlap between changes in resting-state functional connectivity in both chronic pain and trauma is consistent with clinical observations of comorbidity between these conditions, and theoretical models wasproposed to underlie pain–trauma overlap (6). Nevertheless, to the best of our knowledge, no study has examined resting-state networks in individuals with comorbid pain and trauma symptoms. Understanding shared vs. unique underlying neural networks related to pain and/or trauma may provide the basis for novel, targeted interventions.

To examine the degree to which pain and/or trauma symptoms alter resting-state functional connectivity, we recruited a diagnostically heterogeneous sample of veterans with varying degrees of back pain and trauma symptoms and employed a model-free analysis approach with the goal of identifying mechanistically meaningful subgroups. Such diagnosis-free approach supports the National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH) Research Domain Criteria (RDoC) goals to understand mechanisms of psychiatric disorders and their comorbidities in a transdiagnostic manner (29). Data-driven approaches that characterize resting-state functional connectivity patterns show great promise in elucidating neurobiological mechanisms underlying clinical conditions and heterogeneous symptoms (30, 31). Especially, employing effective connectivity models, rather than Pearson's correlation and distance measures, can lead to an improved understanding of directional and causal relationships between networks rather than finding simple correlational associations (32, 33). A novel data-driven strategy called group iterative multiple model estimation (GIMME) has been shown to be particularly effective in deriving directed pathways and is successful in separating clinically heterogeneous population (34, 35). GIMME is a version of unified structural equation modeling (uSEM) that uses time-series analysis within an SEM framework to estimate the prediction strength of directed paths between variables controlling for lagged and contemporaneous effects of all the variables entered into the model (36). Simulation and experimental evidence suggest that GIMME is robust against problems associated with signal-to-noise ratio, hemodynamic response variability, and low sample sizes (35, 37). A modification of GIMME, called subgrouping within GIMME (or “S-GIMME”), can identify neural subtypes in small sample sizes and outperforms other clustering methods (34).

The current study sought to utilize S-GIMME with resting-state functional connectivity to identify whether pain and trauma symptoms are associated with differential functional connectivity patterns at rest in a diagnostically heterogeneous sample of veterans within brain regions implicated in the experience of pain and trauma [i.e., insula, anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), thalamus, posterior cingulate cortex (PCC), and nucleus accumbens (NAc)]. We hypothesized that a model-free approach would identify specific connectivity patterns for increased pain, trauma, and their overlap.

Materials and Methods

Fifty-seven veterans (9 women, mean ± SD age: 35.5 ± 3.75, range 28–44) gave written informed consent to participate in this study, which was approved by the University of California San Francisco Human Research Protection Program and Veterans Affairs San Francisco Healthcare System Research and Development Committee. Participants were recruited from the VA San Francisco Healthcare System (VASFHS) via advertisement materials (e.g., flyers posted in the hospital public areas) or through referrals from other ongoing research projects at this site and through mining VA electronic health records. The sample included individuals with a range of back pain and/or trauma symptoms. Study inclusions were (1) veterans between the ages of 18 and 55; (2) veterans experiencing back pain and/or history of life-threatening trauma during combat and experiencing distress, such as excessive anxiety, jumpiness, or nightmares, related to trauma; (3) veterans that were able to undergo MRI (e.g., have no contraindications for MR); and (4) veterans currently not taking narcotic medication. Subjects were excluded from the study if they: (1) had a current or lifetime history of bipolar disorder, psychosis, eating disorders, and obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD); (2) had a history of alcohol and/or drug dependence within 3 months of study participation; (3) showed a neurological disorder that might be associated with cognitive dysfunction (including head trauma associated with fracture, pre-deployment traumatic brain injury (TBI) resulting in loss or alteration of consciousness, cerebrovascular accident, or intracranial surgery, aneurysm, and seizures); (4) had prior neurosurgery; (5) had irremovable ferromagnetic material; (6) were pregnant; (7) were claustrophobic; (8) had TBI with loss of consciousness (LOC) > 10 min; and (9) had vision problems uncorrectable with lenses. Three subjects were excluded from the analyses due to extensive motion in the MRI scanner.

Clinical Measures

All subjects underwent a Structural Clinical Interview administered by trained interviewers according to the Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (38) to establish current and past psychiatric diagnoses. In addition, each subject completed a Research Electronic Data Capture (REDCap) battery of questionnaires assessing specific pain, trauma, and comorbid symptoms. The battery included pain severity and interference from the Brief Pain Inventory (BPI) (39) and post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) checklist for DSM5 (PTSD Checklist version 5, PCL-5) (40) to assess pain and trauma symptoms, respectively. In addition, subjects completed Beck Depression Inventory-2 (BDI-2) (41) to assess the severity of depressive symptoms, the Pain Catastrophizing Scale (PCS) (42), and a short form of the Pain Anxiety Symptoms Scale (PASS-20) (43) to assess pain-related cognitions, and the Combat Exposure Scale (CES) (44) to access wartime stressors experienced by combatants. Note that we used the symptoms spectrum approach in the current study; nevertheless, each subject was also given a tentative group assignment based on meeting/not-meeting trauma criterion A, and/or chronic pain diagnosis, as recommended (45, 46).

MRI Acquisition and Preprocessing

Brain imaging data were acquired on 3-T Siemens Skyra MRI scanner. All subjects underwent (1) a high-resolution T1-weighted structural scan (320 slices; repetition time, TR = 2,400 ms; echo time, TE = 2.24 ms; flip angle, FA = 8°; field of view, FOV = 166.4 mm × 240 mm; matrix size = 208 × 300; voxel size = 0.8 mm × 0.8 mm × 0.8 mm); and (2) rs-fMRI scan (66 slices in interleaved ascending order; TR = 1,000 ms; TE = 35 ms; FA = 62°; FOV = 208 mm × 208 mm; matrix size = 104 × 104; voxel size = 2 mm × 2 mm × 2 mm; MB factor = 6) while laying comfortably in the MRI scanner. The rs-fMRI run was 8:22 min in length, during which 502 functional volumes were acquired. Data were preprocessed using fMRIPrep 1.5.7 (47), which is based on Nipype 1.4.0 (48, 49) and followed the recommended pipeline. Briefly, data were (1) de-spiked by removal of regional statistical outliers with interpolated regional means, (2) temporarily corrected to slice acquisition, (3) spatially corrected for six direction motion parameters (x, y, z, roll, pitch, and yaw) and their derivatives, (4) aligned to anatomical T1 and normalized brain space, and (5) scaled for percent signal change. For more details of the pipeline, see the section corresponding to workflows in fMRIPrep's documentation (47). Data were smoothed with a Gaussian kernel (full-width half-maximum, FWHM = 5 mm) and band-pass filtered (range [0.008, 0.09] Hz).

Resting-State Networks

We extracted the mean BOLD time series from the average activity across voxels using five bilateral regions of interest (ROIs) averaged across both sites: thalamus, insula, ACC (SN and interoception), PCC (DMN), and NAc (reward/relief) using Hammers' atlas (50, 51) (see inset on the right in Figure 1). The mask was chosen based on the fact that these regions are part of the networks that are heavily implicated in both pain and trauma (5255). Considering that our sample was heterogeneous, which is typical for the veteran population, identifying symptom-specific vs. comorbid and diagnostic vs. symptomatic changes in functional connectivity between these regions at rest can provide valuable insight into the biological underpinnings of pain and/or trauma symptoms, diagnoses, and potential treatment targets. These five ROIs were then entered into connectivity classification analyses with unsupervised S-GIMME (see below) to derive subgroups based on path similarity.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Unsupervised GIMME (USG) identified common and unique effective connectivity for the three subgroups based on effective connectivity paths. Subgroup 1 [USG1, yellow, n = 16, 7 connections] was characterized by functional connectivity to the insula from NAc, ACC, and PCC, and from the thalamus to ACC and bidirectional interconnections between the thalamus and PCC. Subgroup 2 [USG2, indigo, n = 23, 4 connections] was characterized by functional connectivity from the ACC and NAc to the thalamus and from ACC to PCC. Subgroup 3 [USG3, sky blue, n = 15, 4 connections] was characterized by functional connectivity from the thalamus and PCC to NAc. Both USG1 and USG3 had overlapping functional connections from PCC to ACC (garnet), whereas USG2 and USG3 were characterized by overlapping functional connectivity from the insula to ACC (cyan). No connections were shared between USG1 and USG2. (Right) The box on the right shows pictorial of the five regions of interest (ROIs) extents of voxels included in signal averages [Hammers_mith atlas n30r83 (51)]. We extracted the mean BOLD time series from the average activity across voxels using five bilateral regions averaged across both sites; only one side is displayed for clarity. IC, insular cortex; ACC, anterior cingulate cortex; Thal, thalamus; PCC, posterior cingulate cortex; NAc, nucleus accumbens; BOLD, blood oxygen level-dependent. c.f. text for details.

Unsupervised S-GIMME

An unsupervised subgroup classification was performed using unsupervised subgrouping-GIMME, or S-GIMME, in R (R Core Team) in order to identify whether clinically relevant subgroups of subjects can be derived based upon resting-state effective connectivity patterns (34). Unsupervised S-GIMME can be used to not only identify common pathways across all subjects but also importantly classify subjects within subgroups based upon unique connectivity profiles and estimate subject-level connectivity (34). First, unsupervised S-GIMME searches across individuals to derive a connectivity profile of lagged (lag = 1 TR) and contemporaneous directed pathways that are common for the majority of subjects (group-level) and prunes the remaining paths. Furthermore, unsupervised S-GIMME uses unsupervised community detection [Walktrap; (56, 57)] on the similarity matrix derived from the individual-level estimates of the group-level connections to identify an optimal number of subgroups who have similar connectivity profiles. Once subgroups are identified, unsupervised S-GIMME searches for unique connectivity paths for each data-driven subgroup. Finally, unsupervised S-GIMME identifies individual-level connections using the group- and subgroup-derived temporal patterns as priority. After identification of subgroups derived from unsupervised S-GIMME, we compared subgroups on clinical measures to examine clinical relevance of the identified subgroups. Like previous reports (58, 59), we only report on contemporaneous pathways.

Subgroup Characterization Analysis

Statistical analysis of clinical and psychological variables was conducted in R and JASP (60). JASP (Version 0.14.1), ANCOVAs, t-tests, and chi-square tests were used to compare S-GIMME subgroups on clinical and demographic variables (i.e., age and education). For pain and trauma symptoms, results were uncorrected for multiple comparisons due to a priori assumption. Other results were corrected for multiple comparisons using Dunn's method. Post-hoc analyses also explored associations of unique connectivity by subgroup with clinical variables using Spearman's correlations.

Results

Unsupervised S-GIMME

Unsupervised S-GIMME produced a three-group solution (Figure 1), sorting 16 subjects into subgroup 1 (USG1, yellow), 23 subjects into subgroup 2 (USG2, indigo), and 15 subjects into subgroup 3 (USG3, sky blue). The connectivity and directionality of connections between ROIs within each subgroup are illustrated in Figure 1 (left). Overall, USG1 had 7 connections between 5 ROIs, whereas USG2 and USG3 had 4 connections. USG1 was characterized by functional connectivity to the insula from NAc, ACC, and PCC and from the thalamus to ACC and bidirectional interconnections between the thalamus and PCC. USG2 was characterized by functional connectivity from the ACC and NAc to the thalamus and from ACC to PCC. USG3 was characterized by functional connectivity from thalamus and PCC to NAc. Several connections were overlapping between the subgroups. Both USG2 and USG3 were characterized by overlapping functional connectivity from the insula to ACC, whereas USG1 and USG3 had overlapping functional connections from PCC to ACC. No connections were shared between USG1 and USG2. We have displayed (Figure 1, right) the extent of voxels included in signal averages for each of the five ROIs in Figure 1 [Hammers_mith atlas n30r83 (51)].

Demographic, Clinical, and Psychological Variables

Unsupervised S-GIMME subgroups did not differ significantly on age [F(2,1.50) = 1.41, p = 0.254], sex (χ2 = 1.972, p = 0.373), race/ethnicity (χ2 = 8.295, p = 0.405), education level [F(2,1.48) = 0.766, p = 0.471], marital (χ2 = 8.255, p = 0.409), or employment (χ2 = 0.609, p = 0.996) status (see Table 1). Similarly, there was no significant difference in the reported mild TBI (χ2 = 0.414, p = 0.813) or the proportion of veterans meeting/not-meeting trauma and/or chronic pain criteria (χ2 = 6.208, p = 0.400, Figure 3). Similarly, no significant differences between subgroup analyses in combat exposure were observed [see Table 1, F(2,1.50) = 0.347, p = 0.708].

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Subjects' characteristics.

Conversely, unsupervised S-GIMME subgroups differed significantly on several clinical symptoms and psychological measures (see Table 1). This is graphically demonstrated by the radial plot in Figure 2.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Graphic representation of clinical and psychological variables among the three subgroups identified by unsupervised S-GIMME based on effective connectivity paths at rest. Subgroup 1 [USG1, yellow, n = 16], subgroup 2 [USG2, indigo, n = 23], and subgroup 3 [USG3, sky blue, n = 15]. The radar plot is bounded by the minimum and maximum scores in the current sample for each scale (these averages for each subgroup are displayed for reference). c.f. Table 1 for statistical details. Severity, pain severity; Interference, pain interference; both from Brief Pain Inventory (BPI); PCS, Pain Catastrophizing Scale; PCL-5, PTSD Checklist version 5; BDI-2, Beck Depression Inventory-2; PASS-20, Pain Anxiety Symptoms Scale; c.f. text for details.

As can be seen in Figure 2, USG1 was characterized by low scores of pain and psychological symptoms, USG3 was characterized by high scores of pain and psychological symptoms with USG2 being in the middle. GIMME subgroup differences for pain and trauma symptoms are shown in Figure 3A. As determined by one-way ANCOVA with GIMME subgroup as a fixed factor and sex as a covariate, PCL-5 scores were significantly different [F(2,1.50) = 4.107, p = 0.022]. Dunn's post-hoc comparisons of the GIMME subgroups indicated that the mean in the USG3 group was significantly higher than that in the USG1 and USG2 (p < 0.05) groups, and there was no significant difference between USG1 and USG2 (p = 0.469). Both pain severity and pain interference showed similar differences, yet ANCOVA only showed a tendency for significant GIMME subgroup effect while co-varying for sex [pain severity: F(2,1.50) = 2.220, p = 0.1; pain interference: F(2,1.50) = 2.161, p = 0.1]. Since we hypothesized a priori that GIMME subgroups would differ in pain and/or trauma symptoms, we performed Dunn's post-hoc comparisons of the GIMME subgroups. Post-hoc analysis indicated that the mean in the USG3 group was significantly higher than that in the USG2 for pain intensity and pain interference (p < 0.05). Relative to USG1, USG3 was significantly higher on pain interference (p < 0.05) and approached significance on pain intensity (p = 0.055). There were no significant differences in either pain intensity or pain interference between USG1 and USG2 (p-values >0.5). When PCS, BDI-2, and PASS-20 scores were examined, the one-way ANCOVA with GIMME subgroup as a fixed factor and sex as a covariate showed that PCS scores, as well as the PASS-20 scores [F(2, 1.50) = 4.421, p = 0.017], were significantly different [F(2,1.50) = 3.963, p = 0.025], whereas BDI-2 scores approached significance [F(2,1.50) = 2.815, p = 0.069] to be different between the three GIMME subgroups.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Clinical (A) and diagnostic (B) characteristics of GIMME subgroups identified by unsupervised S-GIMME based on effective connectivity paths at rest. (A) Box plots of trauma and pain symptoms (marginal mean ± SE) for each of the subgroups. (top) PCL scores were significantly different [F (2,1.50) = 4.107, p < 0.05], ANCOVA (GIMME subgroup as a fixed factor and sex as covariate); Dunn's post-hoc comparisons indicated that the mean in the USG3 group was significantly higher than that in the USG1 and USG2 (p-values < 0.05), and there was no significant difference between USG1 and USG2 (p = 0.469). (middle) Pain severity (from BPI) showed a tendency for a significant GIMME subgroup effect while covarying for sex [F(2,1.50) = 2.220, p = 0.1]; Dunn's post-hoc comparisons indicated that the mean in the USG3 group was significantly higher than that in the USG2 (p < 0.05) and approached significance when compared with USG1 (p = 0.055); there was no significant difference between USG1 and USG2 (p-values > 0.5) (bottom) Pain interference (from BPI) showed a tendency for significant GIMME subgroup effect while covarying for sex [F(2,1.50) = 2.161, p = 0.1]. Dunn's post-hoc comparisons indicated that the mean in the USG3 group was significantly higher than that in the USG2 and USG1 (p < 0.05). There was no significant difference between USG1 and USG2 (p-values > 0.5). (B) Treemap charts show a hierarchical view of tentative diagnostic groupings within each of the GIMME subgroups. The symptoms-based approach was utilized in the current work; nevertheless, each subject was also given a tentative group assignment based on meeting/not-meeting trauma criterion A, and/or chronic pain diagnosis, as recommended (45, 46). NONE—no pain/no trauma, PAIN—pain/no trauma, TRAUMA—trauma/no pain, and BOTH—pain/trauma. The proportion of each diagnostic group within each GIMME subgroup is shown. There was no significant differences between subgroup analyses in the proportion of veterans meeting/not-meeting trauma and/or chronic pain criteria (χ2 = 6.208, p = 0.400); * <0.05, c.f. Table 1 and text for details.

Exploratory Correlational Analysis

To examine which connections were indicative of a vulnerability or resiliency, GIMME subgroup-specific correlations were performed between pain and trauma scores and individual path estimates for subgroup-unique connections. For the USG1, or the group with the minimal levels of psychiatric and pain symptoms, we found that the NAc connection to the insula, which was unique to this group, was inversely correlated with pain interference (ρ = -0.631, p = 0.004) and PCS (ρ = −0.722, p < 0.001) scores (Figures 4A,B). No other subgroup-specific connections showed a significant relationship with the psychological and/or pain measures.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Exploratory correlations: (A) Insula—Accumbens Connectivity and Pain Catastrophizing Association in USG1. NAc connection to the insula, which was unique to subgroup USG1, or the group with the minimal levels of psychological and pain symptoms, was inversely and significantly correlated with pain catastrophizing scores (PCS) (ρ = −0.722, p < 0.001). The Scatter plot (middle) shows Pearson's correlations for a visual demonstration. Density plots for NAc to insula connectivity (left) and PCS scores (right) within USG1 are shown. (B) Insula—Accumbens and Pain Interference Association in USG1. NAc connection to the insula, which was unique to subgroup USG1, was inversely and significantly correlated with pain interference scores (ρ = -0.631, p = 0.004). The Scatter plot (middle) shows Pearson's correlations for a visual demonstration. Density plots for NAc to insula connectivity (left) and interference scores (right) are shown. No other subgroup-specific connections showed a significant relationship with the psychological and/or pain measures. ** <0.01, *** <0.001; PCS, Pain Catastrophizing Scale; Interference, pain interference measure from Brief Pain Inventory (BPI).

Discussion

The present study reports new findings of unique functional connectivity patterns associated with pain and trauma symptoms during rest in a sample of veterans with a spectrum of pain and trauma symptoms. Using an unsupervised modeling approach, we identified unique connectivity patterns in these veterans at rest. Specifically, we found three separate subgroups showing different functional connectivity between the chosen ROIs that play an important role in pain, reward, and trauma (5255), despite having similar demographic and clinical diagnostic characteristics. This study suggests that even though they may have similar demographic and diagnostic characteristics, there may be neurobiologically dissociable biotypes with different mechanisms for handling pain and trauma. This may have implications for the determination of appropriate biotype-specific interventions that target these neurological systems.

We found that the subgroups, defined by unsupervised S-GIMME as showing differential functional connectivity patterns at rest, were clearly separable in their clinical symptoms of trauma and pain. Specifically, we found that the group characterized by the lowest pain and trauma scores (USG1), or the least clinically impaired subgroup was highly connected overall, i.e., compared to the other two groups, USG1 showed more subgroup-unique connections, and overlapping functional connections with the other subgroups. This so-called “low symptom group” was characterized by functional connectivity from NAc, ACC, and PCC to the insula, and also from the thalamus to ACC and bidirectional interconnections between the thalamus and PCC. The insula plays a pivotal role in emotional and interoceptive processing (52, 61, 62). Besides its proposed role as an interoceptive sensory cortex (63, 64), substantial evidence supports the notion that the insula represents a hub for dynamic switching between different networks, i.e., based on the emotional and interoceptive needs of an organism, the insula helps to access attention, working memory, and other higher-order cognitive processes (62, 65). Not surprisingly, many pain and psychiatric disorders are thought to be related to faulty insula connectivity (6671). Such interpretation is consistent with our findings of the insula being a hub for connectivity at rest in the “low symptom,” i.e., the least clinically impaired, subgroup. Interestingly, this “low symptom” subgroup is the only group that showed unique connectivity from NAc to the insula, which also was inversely correlated with pain interference and PCS scores in this subgroup. In other words, those with the lowest pain catastrophizing and pain interference scores had the highest functional connectivity from NAc to the insula, potentially suggesting the protective role of such connections. The NAc is at the core of the reward system, as it integrates emotional information to modulate motivated behavior (72, 73). In addition, a meta-analysis of healthy subjects has shown that spontaneous activity in the NAc predicts activity in the insula, suggesting consistent co-activation among these regions (74). Our study shows that co-activation of these regions plays an important role in discriminating between anticipating more painful vs. anticipating less painful experimental pain stimuli (75). This is consistent with the reported activation of NAc during pain predictive cues, the anticipation of pain relief, and the reported dopaminergic inputs to NAc during both reward and punishment (7680). Furthermore, the motivation-decision model of pain states that actions are influenced by decisions about whether to approach something pleasant (a reward) or avoid something unpleasant (pain/loss) (53). We believe that NAc–insula connection provides a balance between pain and pain relief, and functional connection between these regions at rest thus underlies a healthy (or resilient) way to respond to stressful events such as trauma and/or pain.

With regard to the thalamus to ACC connectivity that was also unique in the “low symptom” subgroup, both the ACC and the thalamus are implicated in the affective/emotional aspects of pain and are thought to play important roles in pain modulation. More specifically, evidence suggests that, in response to pain, the ACC engages lower parts of the descending pain control system (i.e., PAG, hypothalamus, and rostral ventromedial medulla), which in turn exert an opioid-dependent inhibitory influence on spinal nociceptive processing, reducing nociceptive input to thalamic and cortical regions, and ultimately leading to a reduced pain experience (81). As such, it is possible that our observation of greater connectivity between the ACC and the thalamus in the “low symptom” subgroup could reflect the proper functioning of the descending analgesic pathway that is ready to engage in the setting of painful/traumatic experiences.

We found that the GIMME subgroup that demonstrated the highest pain and trauma symptoms, or the comorbid “high symptom” subgroup, reported the highest pain catastrophizing, pain anxiety, and depressive symptom severity scores and was characterized by functional connectivity from the thalamus to NAc and from PCC to NAc. Considering the above fact, the lack of insula connectivity with NAc and with other regions in this subgroup can potentially underlie the highest clinical impairment in this subgroup. The fact that NAc appears to be a hub for connectivity at rest in the “high symptom” subgroup may not be surprising, considering the role that NAc plays in chronification of pain in both human and animal models (25, 8285). Furthermore, chronic pain patients suffer from anhedonia, decreased motivation, and exhibit impaired value-based decision-making—all properties of the cortico-striatal system (8688). Not surprisingly, decreased motivation and impaired valence-based processing are typical for PTSD (17), and NAc circuitry is implicated in post-trauma active avoidance (89). The thalamus is a central hub of homeostatic information (90), whereas the PCC is implicated in regulating the focus of attention (e.g., internal vs. external) (91). Greater NAc connectivity between these regions in this “high symptom” subgroup may work jointly to influence pain catastrophizing and pain anxiety via the increased projection of internal pain states and the increased demand to direct internal attention away from such states.

We also found that “low symptom” and “high symptom” subgroups had overlapping functional connections from PCC to ACC. We speculated that increased connectivity here underlies coping with stress, yet in one group, it is an adaptive flexible response, such as mind wandering and cognitive control, whereas in another, it is maladaptive, conveying learned helplessness (92).

Our model-free approach identified another subgroup that was characterized by low pain and trauma symptoms, similar to the “low symptom” subgroup, but was functionally separated from the “low symptom” and “high symptoms” subgroups. This, so-called, “medium symptom” subgroup showed unique connections from ACC to PCC. PCC is a key part of DMN). More DMN connectivity in this group can likely be understood in the context of DMN regulatory response to pain, depression, and trauma. Evidence within the broader chronic pain literature suggests that stronger DMN connectivity may reflect an enhanced ability to engage in mind wandering (i.e., attention fluctuating away from the present sensory environment) (93, 94), thereby decreasing the salience of pain (95, 96). Therefore, it is possible that our analysis revealed a subgroup that is different from the “low symptom” subgroup by being able to disengage from pain and/or trauma through avoidance and/or dissociation, a process often associated with pain, trauma, and depression (13, 15, 16, 97). This would result in an increased ability to wander the mind away from painful and traumatic experiences and thus decrease their immediate salience. Avoidant and suppressive responses may alleviate distress in the short term but, in the long term, may become maladaptive and in fact exacerbate emotional behaviors and interfere with one's ability to extinguish fear responses (98), potentially creating neural vulnerability for pain and trauma chronification. Of note, it has also been suggested that within DMN and to PCC connectivity in the context of pain could reflect pain rumination (i.e., perseverative negative thinking about pain) (99), yet this interpretation is less likely since this group did not demonstrate heightened pain or trauma symptoms. We also found that this “medium symptom” subgroup had overlapping functional connections with the “high symptom” subgroup, i.e., the group with the highest clinical impairment. These overlapping functional connections were from insula to ACC. With regard to insula–ACC connectivity, mechanistically, increased connections between limbic sensory cortex (insula) and limbic motor cortex (cingulate) convey stronger affective experience (62, 100) and increased motivation to modulate feeling of pain, and we have previously shown that weaker connections during experimental pain task are associated with increased subjective pain and avoidant brain response in those with combat trauma and PTSD (16). Thus, it is likely, that the increased insula–ACC connectivity in these subgroups underlies avoidant response to pain and trauma, yet in the “medium symptom” subgroup, the avoidant response is effective in reducing subjective experience.

We hypothesized that the model-free approach would identify specific connectivity patterns for increased pain, increased trauma, and their overlap. We did not, however, observe pain-specific vs. trauma-specific circuits in our heterogeneous sample, but the degree of symptoms was driving our subgroup differences. Note that the incidence of meeting the criteria for pain and/or trauma in our sample was not different among the subgroups. This supports the RDoC transdiagnostic notion (29). It is also of note that patients and veterans, in particular, rarely present with a single disorder but often with multiple comorbidities and that treatments tested in uncomplicated patients often fail in clinical settings. This potentially more generalizable approach may direct choices for various non-opioid interventions and need to be tested in the future.

Limitations of the current study include the use of a cross-sectional design, which restricted our ability to draw conclusions as to whether the brain findings represent a vulnerability to, or impact of, chronic pain and/or trauma. It is also important to note that our sample had relatively high levels of education, and although this demographic factor did not differ between groups, its limited range may reduce the generalizability of these findings. Similarly, the proportion of women in this study was low; thus, current findings may not generalize to the female population. In addition, our overall sample size was relatively small but is comparable with other recent studies using rs-fMRI (28). Furthermore, GIMME has been shown to be robust with small sample sizes, which suggests that this is an ideal method for investigations with challenging-to-recruit patient populations. Nonetheless, future work with larger samples will be required to determine if larger samples will also find similar subtypes as those observed in our study and to probe the unique within-subgroup connectivity profiles. In addition, we used S-GIMME, which is a version of uSEM that uses time-series analysis with an SEM framework (36). Although it offers an improvement over simple functional connectivity models (e.g., Pearson's correlation and distance measures) by providing directional and causal relationships between networks (32, 33, 101), it also has several limitations. These include statistical dependence of multiple features, reduced equivalency with other methods, and a lower maximum of allowable features in the model (due to the increased computational load <20 lagged and contemporaneous features are allowed into the model). Nevertheless, this approach, when combined with the RDoC approach, provides a unique opportunity to understand unique functional brain connectivity as it maps to pain and trauma symptoms in heterogeneous samples.

Conclusions

We report novel evidence of unique functional connectivity patterns associated with pain and trauma symptoms in a sample of veterans. Using an unsupervised approach rather than predefined diagnostic grouping, we identified a unique and overlapping pattern of functional connectivity at rest that represented subjects with differential clinical symptoms subtypes. Our “low symptom” subtype that was characterized by integrated connectivity patterns between the networks, both in the hub (i.e., insula) and pain modulatory networks, may be consistent with the resilient biotype following trauma. Conversely, the “high symptom” subtype was the most interesting candidate as a risk-related biotype. We believe that specific connections or whole-brain patterns are important for determining vulnerability or resilience subtypes and may be used to classify future samples.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by University of California San Francisco Human Research Protection Program and Veterans Affairs San Francisco Healthcare System Research and Development Committee. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

IS, ADS, and ANS were all integral in the acquisition of data, provided input for the analysis and interpretation of the data, and gave final approval of the version to be published. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This work was supported in part by the United States Department of Veterans Affairs I01-CX-000816, I01-CX- 001652, I01-CX001542, and I01-CX-001762, the National Institute of Arthritis and Musculoskeletal and Skin Diseases of the National Institutes of Health under Award Number U19AR076737, and the Painless Research Foundation.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

1. Lew HL, Otis JD, Tun C, Kerns RD, Clark ME, Cifu DX. Prevalence of chronic pain, posttraumatic stress disorder, and persistent postconcussive symptoms in OIF/OEF veterans: polytrauma clinical triad. J Rehabil Res Dev. (2009) 46:697–702. doi: 10.1682/JRRD.2009.01.0006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Fishbain DA, Pulikal A, Lewis JE, Gao J. Chronic pain types differ in their reported prevalence of Post -Traumatic Stress Disorder (PTSD) and there is consistent evidence that chronic pain is associated with ptsd: an evidence-based structured systematic review. Pain Med. (2017) 18:711–35. doi: 10.1093/pm/pnw065

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Webb EK, Huggins AA, Belleau EL, Taubitz LE, Hanson JL, deRoon-Cassini TA, et al. Acute posttrauma resting-state functional connectivity of periaqueductal gray prospectively predicts posttraumatic stress disorder symptoms. Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging. (2020) 5:891–900 doi: 10.1016/j.bpsc.2020.03.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Linnemorken LTB, Granan LP, Reme SE. Prevalence of posttraumatic stress symptoms and associated characteristics among patients with chronic pain conditions in a Norwegian University Hospital outpatient pain clinic. Front Psychol. (2020) 11:749. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00749

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Otis JD, Keane TM, Kerns RD. An examination of the relationship between chronic pain and post-traumatic stress disorder. J Rehabil Res Dev. (2003) 40:397–405. doi: 10.1682/JRRD.2003.09.0397

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Asmundson GJ, Katz J. Understanding the co-occurrence of anxiety disorders and chronic pain: state-of-the-art. Depress Anxiety. (2009) 26:888–901. doi: 10.1002/da.20600

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Higgins DM, Kerns RD, Brandt CA, Haskell SG, Bathulapalli H, Gilliam W, et al. Persistent pain and comorbidity among operation enduring freedom/operation iraqi freedom/operation new dawn veterans. Pain Med. (2014) 15:782-90. doi: 10.1111/pme.12388

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Seal KH, Shi Y, Cohen G, Cohen BE, Maguen S, Krebs EE, et al. Association of mental health disorders with prescription opioids and high-risk opioid use in US veterans of Iraq and Afghanistan. JAMA. (2012) 307:940–7. doi: 10.1001/jama.2012.234

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Ng SK, Urquhart DM, Fitzgerald PB, Cicuttini FM, Hussain SM, Fitzgibbon BM. The relationship between structural and functional brain changes and altered emotion and cognition in chronic low back pain brain changes: a systematic review of MRI and fMRI Studies. Clin J Pain. (2018) 34:237–61. doi: 10.1097/AJP.0000000000000534

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Nutt DJ, Malizia AL. Structural and functional brain changes in posttraumatic stress disorder. J Clin Psychiatry. (2004) 65:11−7.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

11. Moeller-Bertram T, Strigo IA, Simmons AN, Schilling JM, Patel P, Baker DG. Evidence for acute central sensitization to prolonged experimental pain in posttraumatic stress disorder. Pain Med. (2014) 15:762–71. doi: 10.1111/pme.12424

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Tesarz J, Baumeister D, Andersen TE, Vaegter HB. Pain perception and processing in individuals with posttraumatic stress disorder: a systematic review with meta-analysis. Pain Rep. (2020) 5:e849. doi: 10.1097/PR9.0000000000000849

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Mickleborough MJ, Daniels JK, Coupland NJ, Kao R, Williamson PC, Lanius UF, et al. Effects of trauma-related cues on pain processing in posttraumatic stress disorder: an fMRI investigation. J Psychiatry Neurosci. (2011) 36:6–14. doi: 10.1503/jpn.080188

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Geuze E, Westenberg HG, Jochims A, de Kloet CS, Bohus M, Vermetten E, et al. Altered pain processing in veterans with posttraumatic stress disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry. (2007) 64:76–85. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.64.1.76

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Strigo IA, Simmons AN, Matthews SC, Grimes EM, Allard CB, Reinhardt LE, et al. Neural correlates of altered pain response in women with posttraumatic stress disorder from intimate partner violence. Biol Psychiatry. (2010) 68:442–50. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.03.034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Strigo IA, Spadoni AD, Inslicht SS, Simmons AN. Repeated exposure to experimental pain differentiates combat traumatic brain injury with and without post-traumatic stress disorder. J Neurotrauma. (2018) 35:297–307. doi: 10.1089/neu.2017.5061

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Elman I, Upadhyay J, Langleben DD, Albanese M, Becerra L, Borsook D. Reward and aversion processing in patients with post-traumatic stress disorder: functional neuroimaging with visual and thermal stimuli. Transl Psychiatry. (2018) 8:240. doi: 10.1038/s41398-018-0292-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Fox MD, Greicius M. Clinical applications of resting state functional connectivity. Front Syst Neurosci. (2010) 4:19. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2010.00019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Apkarian AV, Baliki MN, Geha PY. Towards a theory of chronic pain. Prog Neurobiol. (2009) 87:81–97. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2008.09.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Baliki MN, Baria AT, Apkarian AV. The cortical rhythms of chronic back pain. J Neurosci. (2011) 31:13981–90. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1984-11.2011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Wasan AD, Loggia ML, Chen LQ, Napadow V, Kong J, Gollub RL. Neural correlates of chronic low back pain measured by arterial spin labeling. Anesthesiology. (2011) 115:364–74 10.1097/ALN.0b013e318220e880.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

22. Baliki MN, Chialvo DR, Geha PY, Levy RM, Harden RN, Parrish TB, et al. Chronic pain and the emotional brain: specific brain activity associated with spontaneous fluctuations of intensity of chronic back pain. J Neurosci. (2006) 26:12165–73. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3576-06.2006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Tagliazucchi E, Balenzuela P, Fraiman D, Chialvo DR. Brain resting state is disrupted in chronic back pain patients. Neurosci Lett. (2010) 485:26–31. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2010.08.053

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Hashmi JA, Baliki MN, Huang L, Baria AT, Torbey S, Hermann KM, et al. Shape shifting pain: chronification of back pain shifts brain representation from nociceptive to emotional circuits. Brain. (2013) 136:2751–68. doi: 10.1093/brain/awt211

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Baliki MN, Petre B, Torbey S, Herrmann KM, Huang L, Schnitzer TJ, et al. Corticostriatal functional connectivity predicts transition to chronic back pain. Nat Neurosci. (2012) 15:1117–9. doi: 10.1038/nn.3153

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Ceko M, Shir Y, Ouellet JA, Ware MA, Stone LS, Seminowicz DA. Partial recovery of abnormal insula and dorsolateral prefrontal connectivity to cognitive networks in chronic low back pain after treatment. Hum Brain Mapp. (2015) 36:2075–92. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22757

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Akiki TJ, Averill CL, Abdallah CG. A network-based neurobiological model of PTSD: evidence from structural and functional neuroimaging studies. Curr Psychiatry Rep. (2017) 19:81. doi: 10.1007/s11920-017-0840-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Stout DM, Harlé KM, Norman SB, Simmons AN, Spadoni AD. Resting-state connectivity subtype of comorbid PTSD and alcohol use disorder moderates improvement from integrated prolonged exposure therapy in Veterans. Psychol Med. (2021) 30:1-10. doi: 10.1017/S0033291721001513

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Braun U, Schaefer A, Betzel RF, Tost H, Meyer-Lindenberg A, Bassett DS. A Meyer-Lindenberg, and DS Bassett From Maps to Multi-dimensional Network Mechanisms of Mental Disorders. Neuron. (2018) 97:14–31. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2017.11.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Drysdale AT, Grosenick L, Downar J, Dunlop K, Mansouri F, Meng Y, et al. Resting-state connectivity biomarkers define neurophysiological subtypes of depression. Nat Med. (2017) 23:28–38. doi: 10.1038/nm.4246

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Zhang X, Braun U, Tost H, Bassett DS. Data-driven approaches to neuroimaging analysis to enhance psychiatric diagnosis and therapy. Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging. (2020) 5:780–90. doi: 10.1016/j.bpsc.2019.12.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Friston KJ. Functional and effective connectivity: a review. Brain Connect. (2011) 1:13–36. doi: 10.1089/brain.2011.0008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Reid AT, Headley DB, Mill RD, Sanchez-Romero R, Uddin LQ, Marinazzo D, et al. Advancing functional connectivity research from association to causation. Nat Neurosci. (2019) 22:1751–60. doi: 10.1038/s41593-019-0510-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Gates KM, Lane ST, Varangis E, Giovanello K, Guskiewicz K. Unsupervised classification during time-series model building. Multivariate Behav Res. (2017) 52:129–48. doi: 10.1080/00273171.2016.1256187

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Gates KM, Molenaar PC. Group search algorithm recovers effective connectivity maps for individuals in homogeneous and heterogeneous samples. Neuroimage. (2012) 63:310–9. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.06.026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Gates KM, Molenaar PC, Hillary FG, Ram N, Rovine MJ. Automatic search for fMRI connectivity mapping: an alternative to Granger causality testing using formal equivalences among SEM path modeling, VAR, and unified SEM. Neuroimage. (2010) 50:1118–25. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.12.117

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Mumford JA, Ramsey JD. Bayesian networks for fMRI: a primer. Neuroimage. (2014) 86:573–82. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.10.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Sheehan D, Janavs J, Baker R, Harnett-Sheehan K, Knapp E, Sheehan M. Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview. Tampa: University of South Florida (1994).

Google Scholar

39. Cleeland CS, Ryan K. The brief pain inventory. Pain Res Group. (1991) 28:143–7.

Google Scholar

40. Weathers F. B., Litz T., Keane P., Palmieri B., Marx P., Schnurr. The PTSD Checklist for DSM-5 (PCL-5). Scale Available National Center PTSD. (2013) 10

Google Scholar

41. Beck AT, Steer RA, Brown GK. Manual for the Beck Depression Inventory-II. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation (1996). p. 1–82.

Google Scholar

42. Sullivan M, Bishop S, Pivik J. The pain catastrophizing scale: develompent and validation. Psychol Assess. (1995) 7:524–32. doi: 10.1037/1040-3590.7.4.524

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. McCracken LM, Dhingra L. A short version of the Pain Anxiety Symptoms Scale (PASS-20): preliminary development and validity. Pain Res Manag. (2002) 7:45–50. doi: 10.1155/2002/517163

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. McCarthy DM, Pedersen SL, Thompsen DM, Leuty ME. Clinical evaluation of a measure to assess combat exposure. Psychol Assess J Consult Clin Psychol. (1989) 1:53–5. doi: 10.1037/1040-3590.1.1.53

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Protocol ATI. Trauma-Informed Care in Behavioral Health Services. Rockville, USA: Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration (2014)

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

46. Treede RD, Rief W, Barke A, Aziz Q, Bennett MI, Benoliel R, et al. A classification of chronic pain for ICD-11. Pain. (2015) 156:1003–7. doi: 10.1097/j.pain.0000000000000160

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Esteban O, Markiewicz CJ, Blair RW, Moodie CA, Isik AI, Erramuzpe A, et al. fMRIPrep: a robust preprocessing pipeline for functional MRI. Nat Meth. (2019) 16:111–6. doi: 10.1038/s41592-018-0235-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Gorgolewski KJ, Esteban O, Markiewicz CJ, Ziegler E, Ellis DG, Notter MP, et al. Nipype. Software, Zenodo (2018).

Google Scholar

49. Gorgolewski K, Burns CD, Madison C, Clark D, Halchenko YO, Waskom ML, et al. Nipype: a flexible, lightweight and extensible neuroimaging data processing framework in python. Front Neuroinform. (2011) 5:13. doi: 10.3389/fninf.2011.00013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Faillenot I, Heckemann RA, Frot M, Hammers A. Macroanatomy and 3D probabilistic atlas of the human insula. Neuroimage. (2017) 150:88–98. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.01.073

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Hammers A, Allom R, Koepp MJ, Free SL, Myers R, Lemieux L, et al. Three-dimensional maximum probability atlas of the human brain, with particular reference to the temporal lobe. Hum Brain Mapp. (2003) 19:224–47. doi: 10.1002/hbm.10123

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Craig AD. How do you feel? Interoception: the sense of the physiological condition of the body. NatRevNeurosci. (2002) 3:655–66. doi: 10.1038/nrn894

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Fields HL. How expectations influence pain. Pain. (2018) 159: S3–10. doi: 10.1097/j.pain.0000000000001272

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Baliki MN, Mansour AR, Baria AT, Apkarian AV. Functional reorganization of the default mode network across chronic pain conditions. PLoS ONE. (2014) 9:e106133. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0106133

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Stevens JS, Harnett NG, Lebois LAM, van Rooij SJH, Ely TD, Roeckner A, et al. Brain-based biotypes of psychiatric vulnerability in the acute aftermath of trauma. Am J Psychiatry. (2021) 178:1037–49. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2021.20101526

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Gates KM, Henry T, Steinley D, Fair DA. A Monte Carlo evaluation of weighted community detection algorithms. Front Neuroinform. (2016) 10:45. doi: 10.3389/fninf.2016.00045

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Orman GK, Labatut V. A comparison of community detection algorithms on artificial networks. In: International Conference on Discovery Science. Berlin: Springer (2009).

Google Scholar

58. Price RB, Gates K, Kraynak TE, Thase ME, Siegle GJ. Data-driven subgroups in depression derived from directed functional connectivity paths at rest. Neuropsychopharmacology. (2017) 42:2623–32. doi: 10.1038/npp.2017.9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Price RB, Lane S, Gates K, Kraynak TE, Horner MS, Thase ME, et al. Parsing heterogeneity in the brain connectivity of depressed and healthy adults during positive mood. Biol Psychiatry. (2017) 81:347–57. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2016.06.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. JASP Team,. JASP (Version 0.16.2). (2022). Available online at: https://jasp-stats.org/faq/how-do-i-cite-jasp/

Google Scholar

61. Craig AD. How do you feel [mdash] now? The anterior insula and human awareness. Nat Rev Neurosci. (2009) 10:59–70. doi: 10.1038/nrn2555

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Craig AD. How do You Feel?: An Interoceptive Moment With Your Neurobiological Self. USA: Princeton University Press (2015). doi: 10.1515/9781400852727

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Craig AD. Referee Report For: The dorsal posterior insula is not an island in pain but subserves a fundamental role - Response to: “Evidence against pain specificity in the dorsal posterior insula” by Davis et al. F1000Res. (2015) 4:1207. doi: 10.12688/f1000research.7287.1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Segerdahl AR, Mezue M, Okell TW, Farrar JT, Tracey I. The dorsal posterior insula subserves a fundamental role in human pain. Nat Neurosci. (2015) 18:499–500. doi: 10.1038/nn.3969

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Menon V, Uddin LQ. Saliency, switching, attention and control: a network model of insula function. Brain Struct Funct. (2010) 214:655–67. doi: 10.1007/s00429-010-0262-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Simmons AN, Norman SB, Spadoni AD, Strigo IA. Neurosubstrates of remission following prolonged exposure therapy in veterans with posttraumatic stress disorder. Psychother Psychosom. (2013) 82:382–9. doi: 10.1159/000348867

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Strigo IA, Matthews SC, Simmons AN. Decreased frontal regulation during pain anticipation in unmedicated subjects with major depressive disorder. Transl Psychiatry. (2013) 3:e239. doi: 10.1038/tp.2013.15

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Simmons A, Strigo IA, Matthews SC, Paulus MP, Stein MB. Initial evidence of a failure to activate right anterior insula during affective set shifting in posttraumatic stress disorder. Psychosom Med. (2009) 71:373–7. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e3181a56ed8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Strigo IA, Matthews SC, Simmons AN, Oberndorfer T, Klabunde M, Reinhardt LE, et al. Altered insula activation during pain anticipation in individuals recovered from anorexia nervosa: Evidence of interoceptive dysregulation. Int J Eat Disord. (2013) 46:23–33. doi: 10.1002/eat.22045

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Oberndorfer T, Simmons A, McCurdy D, Strigo I, Matthews S, Yang T, et al. Greater anterior insula activation during anticipation of food images in women recovered from anorexia nervosa versus controls. Psychiatry Res Neuroimag. (2013) 214:132–41. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2013.06.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Strigo IA, Matthews SC, Simmons AN. Right anterior insula hypoactivity during anticipation of homeostatic shifts in major depressive disorder. Psychosom Med. (2010) 72:316–23. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e3181d07873

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

72. Mogenson GJ, Jones DL, Yim CY. From motivation to action: functional interface between the limbic system and the motor system. Prog Neurobiol. (1980) 14:69–97. doi: 10.1016/0301-0082(80)90018-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

73. Roesch MR, Singh T, Brown PL, Mullins SE, Schoenbaum G. Ventral striatal neurons encode the value of the chosen action in rats deciding between differently delayed or sized rewards. J Neurosci. (2009) 29:13365–76 doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2572-09.2009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

74. Cauda F, Cavanna AE, D'agata F, Sacco K, Duca S, Geminiani GC. Functional connectivity and coactivation of the nucleus accumbens: a combined functional connectivity and structure-based meta-analysis. J Cogn Neurosci. (2011) 23:2864–77. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2011.21624

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

75. Kadlec M, Tosun D, Strigo I. BOLD decoding of individual pain anticipation biases during uncertainty. bioRxiv. (2019) 675645. doi: 10.1101/675645

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

76. Baliki MN, Geha PY, Fields HL, Apkarian AV. Predicting value of pain and analgesia: nucleus accumbens response to noxious stimuli changes in the presence of chronic pain. Neuron. (2010) 66:149–60. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.03.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

77. Schwartz N, Miller C, Fields HL. Cortico-accumbens regulation of approach-avoidance behavior is modified by experience and chronic pain. Cell Rep. (2017) 19:1522–31. doi: 10.1016/j.celrep.2017.04.073

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

78. Ungless MA, Magill PJ, Bolam JP. Uniform inhibition of dopamine neurons in the ventral tegmental area by aversive stimuli. Science. (2004) 303:2040–2. doi: 10.1126/science.1093360

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

79. Seymour B, O'Doherty JP, Dayan P, Koltzenburg M, Jones AK, Dolan RJ, et al. Temporal difference models describe higher-order learning in humans. Nature. (2004) 429:664–7. doi: 10.1038/nature02581

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

80. Zubieta JK, Bueller JA, Jackson LR, Scott DJ, Xu Y, Koeppe RA, et al. Placebo effects mediated by endogenous opioid activity on mu-opioid receptors. J Neurosci. (2005) 25:7754–62. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0439-05.2005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

81. Eippert F, Bingel U, Schoell ED, Yacubian J, Klinger R, Lorenz J, et al. Activation of the opioidergic descending pain control system underlies placebo analgesia. Neuron. (2009) 63:533–43. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.07.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

82. Baliki MN, Schnitzer TJ, Bauer WR, Apkarian AV. Brain morphological signatures for chronic pain. PLoS ONE. (2011) 6:e26010. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0026010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

83. Vachon-Presseau E, Tétreault P, Petre B, Huang L, Berger SE, Torbey S, et al. Corticolimbic anatomical characteristics predetermine risk for chronic pain. Brain. (2016) 139:1958–70. doi: 10.1093/brain/aww100

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

84. Makary MM, Polosecki P, Cecchi GA, DeAraujo IE, Barron DS, Constable TR, et al. Loss of nucleus accumbens low-frequency fluctuations is a signature of chronic pain. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. (2020) 117:10015–23. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1918682117

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

85. Benarroch EE. Involvement of the nucleus accumbens and dopamine system in chronic pain. Neurology. (2016) 87:1720–6. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000003243

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

86. Apkarian AV, Sosa Y, Krauss BR, Thomas PS, Fredrickson BE, Levy RE, et al. Chronic pain patients are impaired on an emotional decision-making task. Pain. (2004) 108:129–36. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2003.12.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

87. Geha P, deAraujo I, Green B, Small DM. Decreased food pleasure and disrupted satiety signals in chronic low back pain. Pain. (2014) 155:712–22. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2013.12.027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

88. Schwartz N, Temkin P, Jurado S, Lim BK, Heifets BD, Polepalli JS, et al. Chronic pain Decreased motivation during chronic pain requires long-term depression in the nucleus accumbens. Science. (2014) 345:535–42. doi: 10.1126/science.1253994

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

89. Ramirez F, Moscarello JM, LeDoux JE, Sears RM. Active avoidance requires a serial basal amygdala to nucleus accumbens shell circuit. J Neurosci. (2015) 35:3470–7. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1331-14.2015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

90. De Groote A, de Kerchove d'Exaerde A. Thalamo-Nucleus Accumbens Projections in Motivated Behaviors and Addiction. Front Syst Neurosci. (2021) 15:711350. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2021.711350

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

91. Leech R, Sharp DJ. The role of the posterior cingulate cortex in cognition and disease. Brain. (2014) 137:12–32. doi: 10.1093/brain/awt162

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

92. Chase HW, Auerbach RP, Brent DA, Posner J, Weissman MM, Talati A. Dissociating default mode network resting state markers of suicide from familial risk factors for depression. Neuropsychopharmacology. (2021) 46:1830–8. doi: 10.1038/s41386-021-01022-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

93. Kucyi A, Salomons TV, Davis KD. Mind wandering away from pain dynamically engages antinociceptive and default mode brain networks. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. (2013) 110:18692–7. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1312902110

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

94. Mason MF, Norton MI, Van Horn JD, Wegner DM, Grafton ST, Macrae CN. Wandering minds: the default network and stimulus-independent thought. Science. (2007) 315:393–5. doi: 10.1126/science.1131295

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

95. Kucyi A, Davis KD. The dynamic pain connectome. Trends Neurosci. (2015) 38:86–95. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2014.11.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

96. Wiech K, Ploner M, Tracey I. Neurocognitive aspects of pain perception. Trends Cogn Sci. (2008) 12:306–13. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2008.05.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

97. Defrin R, Schreiber S, Ginzburg K. Paradoxical pain perception in posttraumatic stress disorder: the unique role of anxiety and dissociation. J Pain. (2015) 16:961–70. doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2015.06.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

98. Foa EB, Kozak MJ. Emotional processing of fear: exposure to corrective information. Psychol Bull. (1986) 99:20–35. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.99.1.20

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

99. Kucyi A, Moayedi M, Weissman-Fogel I, Goldberg MB, Freeman BV, Tenenbaum HC, et al. Enhanced medial prefrontal-default mode network functional connectivity in chronic pain and its association with pain rumination. J Neurosci. (2014) 34:3969–75. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5055-13.2014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

100. Touroutoglou A, Hollenbeck M, Dickerson BC, Feldman Barrett L. Dissociable large-scale networks anchored in the right anterior insula subserve affective experience and attention. Neuroimage. (2012) 60:1947–58. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.02.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

101. Stephan KE, Friston KJ. Analyzing effective connectivity with functional magnetic resonance imaging. Wiley Interdiscip Rev Cogn Sci. (2010)1:446–59. doi: 10.1002/wcs.58

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: insula, nucleus accumbens, effective connectivity, neuroimaging, veterans, catastrophizing, chronic back pain, PTSD

Citation: Strigo IA, Spadoni AD and Simmons AN (2022) Understanding Pain and Trauma Symptoms in Veterans From Resting-State Connectivity: Unsupervised Modeling. Front. Pain Res. 3:871961. doi: 10.3389/fpain.2022.871961

Received: 08 February 2022; Accepted: 07 April 2022;
Published: 10 May 2022.

Edited by:

Peter Sörös, University of Oldenburg, Germany

Reviewed by:

Pascal Tetreault, Université de Sherbrooke, Canada
Scott Holmes, Boston Children's Hospital, United States

Copyright © 2022 Strigo, Spadoni and Simmons. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Irina A. Strigo, irina.strigo@ucsf.edu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.