Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Neurosci., 04 December 2024
Sec. Autonomic Neuroscience
This article is part of the Research Topic Interaction between the Vagus Nerve and Social Communication View all 3 articles

Teacher stress in social interactions in the light of polyvagal theory. An ambulatory assessment approach to teachers’ heart rate and heart rate variability

  • 1Department of Research and Development, University of Teacher Education Bern, Bern, Switzerland
  • 2Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, Department of Psychology, University of Bern, Bern, Switzerland
  • 3Psychosomatic Medicine, Department of Neurology, Inselspital, Bern University Hospital, Bern, Switzerland
  • 4Clinica Holistica Engiadina, Centre for Stress-Related Disorders, Susch, Switzerland
  • 5Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, Department of Psychology, University of Zurich, Zürich, Switzerland

Background: Teaching is a genuinely social and highly demanding task. Drawing on Porges’ phylogenetic polyvagal theory with three evolved systems and three associated behavioral responses (social engagement, mobilization, and immobilization), we investigated teachers’ heart rate and heart rate variability in social interactions using ambulatory assessments.

Methods: We continuously measured heart rate and heart rate variability of 42 apparently healthy teachers on a work and leisure day with ambulatory electrocardiogram. We videotaped four consecutive, same-day lectures of each teacher. We trained observers to code student aggression and frontal teaching behaviors in an event sampling procedure with the behavior observation system for analyzing aggressive behavior in school settings. Additionally, perceived teacher-student relationship, social support from other teachers / school management, occupational complaints, and vital exhaustion were assessed by teacher self-reports.

Results: Teachers showed an increased heart rate and a decreased heart rate variability on a workday compared to a leisure day, anticipatory stress before classes, as well as insufficient recovery during lunchtime. Observed student aggression and high proportion of frontal teaching were associated with lower heart rate variability, while better perceived teacher-student relationship was correlated with higher heart rate variability. Differently, teachers’ psychological strain and heart rate variability were unrelated to each other.

Conclusion: Corresponding to polyvagal theory, results suggest that successful social interactions are fundamental for teachers’ favorable cardiological reactions.

1 Introduction

Humans are genuinely social beings and fundamentally desire to form and maintain meaningful social connections with others (Baumeister and Leary, 1995). We live in a highly specialized society that relies on cooperation and social interaction in multiple relationships. The social brain hypothesis (Dunbar, 2009) posits that relationships were a driving force behind the evolution of large brains. Accordingly, our nervous system is geared not only toward pure survival but also toward communication and interaction with other people. Whereas diverse social interactions represent powerful resources, they can also be challenging and cause stress (Umberson and Karas Montez, 2010). The present study focuses on teaching as a genuinely social profession. Teaching is characterized by high social demands, which entail a variety of stressors (Soltau and Mienert, 2010; Rothland, 2013; Nieskens, 2016). Student aggression is a strong interpersonal stressor that harms humans’ close social bonds (Baumeister and Leary, 1995; Epel et al., 2018). However, positive teacher-student relationships or support from other school professionals can also be social resources (Spilt et al., 2011; Ju et al., 2015; Aldrup et al., 2018). Drawing on the polyvagal theory, this study examines teachers’ cardiac responses in social interactions.

Teachers face multiple and complex demands (Soltau and Mienert, 2010; Rothland, 2013; Nieskens, 2016). They must adapt the curriculum to children’s individual needs, maintain discipline, monitor the class, and sanction rules (Helmke, 2009; Rothland, 2013). Above and beyond these direct classroom challenges, teachers must continuously shape interactions with colleagues, school administrators, other involved professionals, and parents (Rothland, 2013). These interactions are characterized by a high social density, simultaneity, immediacy, unpredictability, informality, and publicity (Doyle, 1986; Lortie, 2002; Herzog, 2006) and occur both verbally and nonverbally (Schilbach, 2015). Taken together, the teaching profession is characterized by high levels of stress (Aloe et al., 2014; Eurofound and EU-OSHA, 2014), with 20 to 30% of teachers reporting that their job is stressful or very stressful (Kyriacou, 2015). It can be assumed that risk factors may increase stress, while resources may reduce stress.

One of the most challenging problems for teachers in the classroom and a primary source of teachers’ stress is student aggression (Boyle et al., 1995; Byrne, 1999; Evers et al., 2004; Lehr, 2004; Chang and Davis, 2009; McCormick and Barnett, 2011; Dicke et al., 2014; La Marca et al., 2023; Wettstein et al., 2023a, 2023b). Generally, aggression is a strong social interpersonal stressor that harms humans’ close social bonds (Baumeister and Leary, 1995; Epel et al., 2018). Student aggression refers to any behavior students perform with the intent of causing physical or mental harm to another person who is motivated to avoid the harm (e.g., Anderson and Bushman, 2002; Wettstein, 2008). Within educational settings, student aggression can manifest in diverse ways: verbal or physical, direct (e.g., insulting, hitting), or indirect (e.g., hiding objects, spreading rumors) (Björkqvist et al., 1992).

Levels of teacher stress also vary by methodical-didactical settings employed to promote student learning. Frontal teaching is particularly demanding because teachers are confronted with various demands at the same time while keeping an eye on all students simultaneously (Seitz, 2007; Wettstein, 2008). Students observe their teacher from a largely inactive stance and sometimes use this to become” invisible” to the teacher and/or to progressively test his or her boundaries (Wettstein, 2008). By that, frontal teaching is more likely to be a source of stress than cooperative forms of learning or individual work.

A good teacher-student relationship has shown to be a key resource for teacher health (Spilt et al., 2011; Aldrup et al., 2018), as it can prevent physiological stress in teachers. La Marca et al. (2023) found that a good teacher-student relationship is associated with lower levels of hair cortisol in teachers. It also reduces disruptions such as student aggression and encourages prosocial behavior (Obsuth et al., 2017) and is positively related to students’ motivation to learn (Wentzel, 2010), academic performance (Pianta et al., 2012), as well as psychosocial development (Davis, 2003; Pianta, 2006; Wentzel, 2010; Obsuth et al., 2017).

To successfully master challenging social interactions in everyday school life, teachers also need social support from other teachers, school management, or administrators. Thus, support from the school team is vital and can protect against stress (Ju et al., 2015). For example, Wettstein et al. (2023a) have found evidence that support from other teachers or school administration has a protective effect on teachers’ allostatic load. In addition, social support reduces the impact of stress on blood pressure (Steptoe, 2000) and on heart rate variability (Schwerdtfeger and Schlagert, 2011; Santarpia et al., 2023).

In sum teaching is characterized by complex and potentially challenging psychosocial relationships (Doyle, 1986; Lortie, 2002; Herzog, 2006). In the classroom, it requires a good awareness of the social events and the ability to respond sensitively and appropriately to the situation. Accordingly, previous research has shown that stress severely impairs these capabilities (Bodenmann et al., 1996; Porges, 1997).

When resources to cope with environmental demands are insufficient, and the situation is perceived as threatening (Lazarus and Folkman, 1984; Friedman-Krauss et al., 2014; McCarthy et al., 2016), psychological, physiological, and behavioral stress responses may result (Lazarus, 1991; Mauss and Robinson, 2009). The polyvagal theory (Porges, 1995, 2021) is a phylogenetic model that specifies the association between the evolution of the autonomic nervous system (ANS) and social communication (affective experiences, emotional expression, vocal communication, facial gestures, and social behavior), which is central for the teaching profession. The ANS comprises the sympathetic nervous system (SNS, usually associated with activation and energy mobilization) and the parasympathetic nervous system (PNS, responsible for vegetative and restorative functions) (Ehlert et al., 2013). The polyvagal theory proposes that the PNS is additionally divided into two distinct branches: the ventral and the dorsal vagus (Porges, 1995). The theory also states that the ANS has developed during evolution (Porges, 1995), and the different phylogenetically old structures are supposedly associated with distinct behavioral strategies [i.e., social engagement, mobilization (fight or flight), and immobilization; Porges (1997, 2001)] (Figure 1).

Figure 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. The polyvagal theory and its behavioral responses, modified from reference (Porges, 2003).

1.1 Social engagement

A person confronted with social challenges usually tries to overcome them through social interaction (Porges, 2001). The ventral vagus, being the phylogenetically youngest part of the ANS, is considered responsible for this. It is only found in mammals such as humans and forms the basis for social communication (Porges, 1995, 1997). The functional neural organization is called the social engagement system (Porges, 2001, 2003). The ventral vagus is myelinated, which enables rapid transmission of nerve signals and, thus, highly dynamic adaptation to the environment (Porges, 1995). It inhibits sympathetically controlled mobilization (i.e., fight or flight-behavior), regulates the heart rate, and acts as a vagal brake – preventing the sympathetic nervous system from influencing the heart (Porges, 1997). Central to activating the social engagement system is the perception of safety about the internal or external situation (Porges, 2003, 2022).

1.2 Mobilization (fight or flight)

When a challenge is not resolved through social interaction or when a situation is subjectively perceived as physically or socially threatening, the sympathetic nervous system is assumed to be activated (Porges, 2003). The sympathetic nervous system is the second oldest part of the ANS (Porges, 1997) and prepares the organism for the mobilization response (Porges, 1997). Physiologically, its activity is associated with the body’s energy-and oxygen provision needed to fuel a rapid response to danger (Porges, 1997). However, this mode reduces effective social communication and interaction skills (Porges, 1997). In service of facilitating fight (emotion of anger) or flight (emotion of fear) with a quickening of thoughts and scanning attention for potential sources of threat (Porges, 1997).

1.3 Immobilization

If mobilization is insufficient to deal with the stressor, the dorsal vagal complex is assumed to come into play (Porges, 1995), which manifests in immobilization behavior (e.g., decreasing heart rate, decline of bloody oxygen, sense of dread, and solidification of the body) and may perceived as “playing dead.”

Assessing the level of safety in the environment is critical for the individual to determine which behavioral strategies (social engagement, mobilization, immobilization) will be activated (Porges, 2003). The polyvagal theory proposes neuroception as a subconscious neural process that assesses the environment’s safety, danger, or life-threatening nature (Porges, 2003). The ANS is also assumed to be organized hierarchically, i.e., the social engagement system is mobilized first when safety is perceived (Porges, 1995). If this is insufficient or the situation is assessed as dangerous or even life-threatening, phylogenetically older parts of the ANS are activated (Porges, 2003). In a healthy organism, the SNS and PNS activity is in dynamic relative balance (Shaffer et al., 2014).

1.4 Stress and ANS

Widely used measures of ANS activity are heart rate (HR) and heart rate variability (HRV), as cardiac activity is largely under the control of ANS processes (Task Force of the European Society of Cardiology the North American Society of Pacing and Electrophysiology, 1996; Thayer et al., 2009; Shaffer and Ginsberg, 2017). The heart is under tonic inhibitory control of the vagus nerve, which functions as a vagal brake (Levy, 1990; Uijtdehaage and Thayer, 2000).

HR corresponds to the number of contractions (beats) per minute (Pinel, 2010). HRV refers to the beat-to-beat alterations between successive heartbeats and reflects the balance of the ANS (Task Force of the European Society of Cardiology the North American Society of Pacing and Electrophysiology, 1996). There are many different HRV parameters. RMSSD (root mean square of successive differences between normal heartbeats) and SDNN (Standard Deviation of the NN Interval) are particularly often used in research (Shaffer et al., 2014). RMSSD corresponds to the beat-to-beat variance in heart rate and is considered the standard measure of parasympathetic (vagally) mediated cardiac regulation (Shaffer et al., 2014). SDNN expresses the overall variability of the temporal fluctuations of the heartbeat (Shaffer et al., 2014). The SDNN indicates the ANS’s regulatory capacity and the cardiovascular system’s adaptability (Shaffer et al., 2014).

Under stress, there is usually an increase in HR and a decrease in vagally-related HRV indices (Task Force of the European Society of Cardiology the North American Society of Pacing and Electrophysiology, 1996). Elevated HRV is associated with a better ability to regulate stress and arousal (Thayer and Lane, 2000, 2009) and indicates a good self-regulatory capacity (emotion regulation) and adaptability (McGee et al., 2023). More work stress is usually associated with decreased HRV – an indicator of reduced vagal activity (Chandola et al., 2010).

The relationship between stress and cardiac parameters is well established (e.g., Allen et al., 2014), but there have been few studies examining these in the context of concrete stressful situations in teachers’ everyday lives (Beuchel and Cramer, 2023). A recent review indicates generally inconsistent results on the relationship between stress and cardiovascular parameters in teachers (McGee et al., 2023). Specific studies found that teachers have a higher HR (Schwerdtfeger et al., 2008) or lower HRV (Wettstein et al., 2020) on workdays compared to leisure days. In addition, a higher HR among teachers was also observed in teaching compared to organizational work (Scheuch and Knothe, 1997). A recent ambulatory assessment study found no significant correlations between perceived job demands or resources and HRV (Schmid and Thomas, 2020).

1.5 Anticipatory stress and recovery

Pre-lesson phases are particularly stressful for teachers. Scheuch and Knothe (1997) found a strong effect of anticipatory stress in teachers with particularly high heart rates before classes. Pieper et al. (2007) have found elevated HR and decreased HRV in teachers during worry episodes and stressful events compared to neutral periods. Anticipatory stress in the form of work-related worries had a particularly strong effect on HR and HRV in teachers.

Recovery. Some studies have investigated the physiological recovery of teachers after lessons. Steptoe (2000) has shown a lower HR in the evening compared to working hours. In another study Steptoe et al. (1999) found a reduction in cardiac parameters during leisure time compared to working time only in teachers who reported low levels of work stress. Teachers who reported high work stress failed to recover sufficiently from work stress in their leisure time (Steptoe et al., 1999). In another study, physiological relaxation in teachers, as measured by high-frequency power (HRV frequency domain parameter), tended to occur only during holidays but not weekends (Ritvanen et al., 2004).

1.6 Acute and chronic stress

Although in the teaching profession, the connection between stress and cardiovascular parameters has been increasingly demonstrated, some studies have failed to find an association between stressful events and changes in HR or HRV in teachers (e.g., Pieper et al., 2010).

The acute stress response is regarded as an adaptive adjustment of the organism to internal or external challenges (McEwen, 2007). Repeated or prolonged exposure to stress can harm the body (Porges, 2003), and in the long term, chronic stress can contribute to the development of psychological, psychosomatic, and physiological illnesses (e.g., depression, concentration problems, sleep disorders, and altered ANS activity) (Kokkinos, 2007; McEwen, 2007).

One stress-related outcome is psychological strain. Psychological strain can be operationalized by vital exhaustion (Appels and Mulder, 1989) or occupational problems (Hagemann and Geuenich, 2009). Vital exhaustion is a psychosomatic state characterized by excessive fatigue, lack of energy, increased irritability, and feelings of demoralization (Appels and Mulder, 1989). Occupational problems include negative feelings related to work (Hagemann and Geuenich, 2009).

Chronic stress has been shown to jeopardize health and well-being, reduce teaching quality, and harm student motivation and performance (Klusmann et al., 2016). In the long run, stress can impair teachers’ health, potentially leading to early dropouts from the profession (Ingersoll, 2001; Friedman, 2006) and may be associated with high subsequent health costs (Künzi and Oesch, 2016).

1.7 The present study

Research on teacher stress is mainly based on questionnaires for measuring subjective stress experiences, ignoring essential data on real-time physiological stress reactions (Krause et al., 2013; Wettstein et al., 2020). In self-reports, teachers have almost unanimously identified challenges in social interactions as the main stress factor (Scherzinger and Wettstein, 2019). However, it is still unclear under which conditions challenging interactions trigger acute psychological and physiological stress reactions in teachers (Wettstein et al., 2020). Only a few studies have investigated cardiovascular parameters in everyday lives of teachers (Pieper et al., 2007, 2010; Schmid and Thomas, 2020; Beuchel and Cramer, 2023). To the best of our knowledge, no studies have associated cardiac parameters in teachers with video-taped, observer-rated student aggression. Going beyond previous studies (Pieper et al., 2007, 2010; Schmid and Thomas, 2020; Beuchel and Cramer, 2023), we also examine the association between perceived teacher-student relationship or the methodical-didactical setting and cardiac parameters. Also, teachers’ diurnal courses of HR and HRV on workdays and leisure days have been insufficiently investigated to date. In the present study, we aim to deepen our knowledge about teachers’ cardiac responses to potentially stressful interactions and set out to identify potential risk factors and resources of the teaching profession. We will base our research on polyvagal theory and analyze teacher stress in the context of social interactions.

The present study set out to investigate differences in HR and HRV between workdays and leisure days and to examine whether there are anticipatory stress reactions on a workday. Additionally, we aimed to examine how potential risk factors (student aggression, methodical-didactical setting) and resources (perceived teacher-student relationship, perceived social support) are associated with teachers’ HR and HRV in everyday working life. We are also interested in the relationship between cardiac parameters and psychological strain as a stress-related consequence.

Going beyond previous studies on teacher stress, we included self-reports and used ambulatory assessment methods in the field, including observational and physiological measures, to research the participants in their natural habitats (Trull and Ebner-Priemer, 2013). This enables an ecologically valid assessment of teacher stress in everyday work and helps us better understand the interplay between psychological, physiological, and social factors.

We tested the following hypotheses:

H1: Compared to leisure days HR will be higher and HRV will be lower on workdays (hypothesis 1) (Chandola et al., 2010; Steptoe and Kivimäki, 2012; Järvelin-Pasanen et al., 2018).

H2: HR will be higher and HRV lower before classes than during classes (hypothesis 2) (Porges, 1997; Pieper et al., 2007).

H3: Assumed risk factors (observed student aggression and frontal teaching) will be related negatively and resources (perceived positive teacher-student relationship and perceived support within the school team) positively with teachers’ HRV (hypothesis 3). Inverse associations are expected for HR (Porges, 2003; Aloe et al., 2014; Dicke et al., 2014).

H4: Psychological strain variables (vital exhaustion and occupational problems) will be positively associated with HR and negatively associated with HRV (hypothesis 4) (Thayer and Lane, 2009).

2 Materials and methods

2.1 Participants

Teachers were recruited via flyers and circular emails sent to school administrations in the canton of Bern (Switzerland). The final sample consisted of 42 apparently healthy teachers (28 females; Mage = 39.66, SD = 11.99, range = 23–63) teaching at least 16 weekly lessons.

Participants came from 39 schools within the canton of Bern and taught grade levels ranging from kindergarten and elementary school (1st to 6th grade; n = 27) to secondary school (7th, 8th, and 9th grade; n = 12) and to high school and vocational school (10th, 11th, and 12th grade; n = 3). On average, teachers had 13.35 years of teaching experience (SD = 11.07, range = 1–40). We included different school types and levels to capture a comprehensive picture of the stress factors and resources in the entire education sector.

Exclusion criteria besides working outside of the canton of Bern were smoking more than 10 cigarettes per day, pregnancy, drug or substance abuse, use of cardiovascular drugs or other medication in the past 2 months (besides herbal medicine), consumption of psychoactive substances in the last 4 weeks, more than two standard alcoholic drinks per day, long-haul flights in the previous 2 weeks, acute infections, and cardiovascular or other chronic diseases. The school principals were informed, and all participating teachers signed informed consent.

2.2 Design and procedure

The present study is part of a larger, longitudinal ambulatory assessment project examining psychobiological stress in teachers (Wettstein et al., 2021, 2023a, 2023b; La Marca et al., 2023). As part of a multimodal approach, it comprises self-report, observational, and physiological measures to increase the ecological validity and to reduce potential common method bias.

A brief telephone interview was conducted to ensure that teachers met the inclusion and exclusion criteria. Teachers’ physiological stress responses were collected continuously through electrocardiogram (ECG) from awaking until 7:00 pm throughout one workday and one leisure day (the order of the days was counterbalanced). The teachers also kept diaries to assess their activities on each measurement day.

The study was approved by the Ethics Committee of the Canton of Bern and the Internal Review Board (IRB) of the University of Bern. It was conducted strictly in accord with current national data protection laws and with the declaration of Helsinki.

2.3 Measures

In advance of the study, teachers provided information on demographic variables (sex, age, years of teaching experience, and number of lessons taught in the observed class). In addition, they completed paper-pencil questionnaires on stress and resources in the teaching profession (i.e., perceived teacher-student relationship, social support from other teachers and the school management, teachers’ occupational problems, and vital exhaustion). The corresponding questionnaires were sent to them.

At an initial appointment at the PHBern, weight and height were recorded to determine the BMI. BMI was calculated by dividing the teachers’ weight (kg) (Seca 813; Reinach, Switzerland) by the square of their height (m) (Seca 213; Reinach, Switzerland). The procedure and the use of the ECG sensor were explained and practiced.

2.3.1 Behavioral observation

We equipped each classroom with a GoPro camera with an integrated wide-angle function and a dictaphone to record the methodological-didactical setting and student aggression. The camera was fixed in the center of the classroom on the back wall at a height of 180 cm. We obtained informed consent from the parents of children in the classrooms. Filming took place during four consecutive lessons on one workday. We synchronized the classroom videos with the ECG sensor. Two independent observers coded the video recordings using an event sampling procedure (Faßnacht, 1995) based on the behavior observation system for analyzing aggressive behavior in school settings BASYS (Wettstein, 2008). The focus was on the methodological-didactic setting and student aggression. In the present study, we focused on frontal teaching. An additional observer coded 10% of the video material to assess observer agreement. This resulted in an observer agreement of 0.80 for student aggression and 0.92 for classroom settings (weighted Cohen’s Kappa, correcting for the possibility of the agreement occurring by chance). Video coding was performed using MAXQDA Analytics Pro 2020 version 20.4.1 (VERBI Software, 2019). The mean value for the number of student aggressions and the proportion of frontal teaching was calculated for each teacher.

2.3.2 Psychometric measures

The perceived teacher-student relationship was measured with a subscale of the Classroom Questionnaire (Wettstein et al., 2016). Six items were rated on Likert scales ranging from 1 (not true) to 4 (true). A sample item is “I have a good relationship with my students.” Reliability was Cronbach’s α = 0.73.

Two items assessed perceived social support from other teachers (“If I need advice, other teachers are there for me.”) and the school management (“If I feel overwhelmed, I am supported by the school management.”), using 5-point Likert-scales ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree). The correlation between the two items was moderate, with a Spearman-Brown coefficient of rtt = 0.54. The two items were, therefore, analyzed as individual items.

Teachers’ occupational problems were assessed with a subscale of the Burnout Screening Scales (BOSS I; Hagemann and Geuenich, 2009). Ten items were rated on Likert scales ranging from 1 (does not apply) to 6 (applies strongly). A sample item is “I have lost the joy of my work.” Reliability was Cronbach’s α = 0.91.

Vital exhaustion was assessed using the German translation (cf. Rau et al., 2010) of the Maastricht Vital Exhaustion Questionnaire (MQ; Appels et al., 1987). The scale consists of 21 items assessing fatigue, difficulties falling asleep, apathy, irritability, energy loss, depression, and waking up exhausted. A sample item is: “Do you sometimes feel as if your body is like a battery that is losing its power?.” The 21 items could be rated on a three-point scale, from “statement is not true” (1) to “undecided” (2) or “true” (3). The scores of each item were added to calculate the total score, with higher scores indicating increased vital exhaustion. The reliability was Cronbach’s α = 0.88.

2.3.3 Electrophysiological measures

In order to take methodological aspects in HRV measures sufficiently into account (Laborde et al., 2017), we followed the guidelines of the Task Force of the European Society of Cardiology the North American Society of Pacing and Electrophysiology (1996). We recorded the ECG with the EcgMove4 sensor from Movisens Version 1.9.31.0 (movisens GmbH, Karlsruhe, Germany) with adhesive electrodes that are easy to apply. It records a single-channel ECG at a sampling rate of 1,024 Hz. Upon waking, the teachers applied the sensor using adhesive electrodes, and ECG data were recorded continuously until 7:00 pm.

The processing and analysis of the ECG data were conducted using Kubios HRV Premium version 3.5.0 (University of Eastern Finland, Kuopio/Finland; Tarvainen et al., 2014). Since artifacts significantly affect the reliability of HRV measurements (Tarvainen et al., 2014), raw data was edited manually in Kubios to review peak detection and ensure proper removal of artifacts (e.g., missing, extra, or misaligned beats) and ectopic beats. We followed Tarvainen et al. (2014) guidelines regarding artifact correction. To prevent a significant bias in the analysis results, the number of artifacts, even if corrected, did not exceed 5%.

To determine differences in cardiac parameters between the workday and the leisure day (H1), HR and HRV values were averaged on both days from waking until 7:00 pm. In addition, on workdays, we compared the cardiac parameters in the hour before the lesson begins (the time the teacher spends alone in the classroom before the lesson starts) with the value during the following lesson (i.e., 7–8 with 8–9) to check whether there were anticipatory stress reactions (H2). The averaged HR and HRV values on the work and leisure days were also used to test hypotheses 3 and 4.

After artifact correction (Catalano, 2002), HR, RMSSD, and SDNN were exported into SPSS and analyzed using a within-subject design (Laborde et al., 2017).

2.4 Data analysis

The Shapiro–Wilk test was used for each variable to test normal distribution. Due to non-normal distribution, the following variables were log-transformed before use for calculations: RMSSD and SDNN on work and leisure days, observed direct and indirect student aggression, frontal teaching, perceived teacher-student relationship, support from other teachers and the school management, occupational problems, vital exhaustion, BMI, and age. When correlated significantly (partial correlations), control variables (age, sex, and BMI) were considered. In addition, correlations between ECG and questionnaire variables were controlled for time differences between the two measurements (delay). We have calculated whether there are differential effects at the different school levels. No effects were found. Additionally, RMSSD on work and leisure days was controlled for age and BMI. SDNN on a workday was controlled for age, and, on a leisure day, for BMI. HR at work and leisure days were controlled for age and sex.

All analyses were conducted using IBM SPSS Statistics (Version 29). To compare the work with the leisure day (H1), we conducted a one-way repeated measures MANOVA with day as a factor (workday and leisure day), HR, RMSSD, and SDNN as dependent variables, and sex, age, and BMI as covariates. Regarding concerns about the non-normal distribution of RMSSD and SDNN, we used the log-transformed scales, and the Box test for equality of covariance matrices was not significant. For pairwise comparisons between times (H2), no repeated measures ANOVAs (HR) or Friedman tests (RMSSD and SDNN) were calculated, as there were missing values and only specific comparisons were of interest. Instead, single paired t-tests (HR) and Wilcoxon tests (RMSSD and SDNN) were calculated. The resulting p-values were adjusted with the false discovery rate control (Glickman et al., 2014). Descriptive statistics and bivariate correlations were computed for all variables to investigate hypotheses H3 and H4.

3 Results

3.1 Workday and leisure day

A one-way repeated measures MANOVA resulted in a significant effect of day [F(28,3) = 3.33, p = 0.34, partial η2 = 0.26, Wilks-Lambda = 0.74, n = 34] when controlling for sex, age, and BMI. The partial η2 of 0.26 indicates a large effect (Cohen, 1988). Bonferroni corrected pairwise comparisons revealed that HR was significantly (p = 0.034) higher on the workday (M = 78.81, SD = 7.11) than on the leisure day (M = 76.16, SD = 9.25). RMSSD values were significantly lower (p < 0.001) on the workday (M = 34.08, SD = 15.17) compared to the leisure day (M = 38.84, SD = 20.90). SDNN was also significantly (p = 0.028) lower on the workday (M = 80.49, SD = 46.97) than on the leisure day (M = 98.28, SD = 59.68).

Particularly, RMSSD was significantly lower on the workday compared to the leisure day at 1 pm (Mdnwork = 22.68, Mdnleisure = 33.59, z = −3.03, padj = 0.027, n = 32, r = 0.54, strong effect) and 2 pm (Mdnwork = 27.01, Mdnleisure = 29.41, z = −2.84, padj = 0.027, n = 32, r = 0.50, strong effect). For HR and SDNN, no significant difference was found between the same time on the workday and leisure day.

3.2 Anticipatory stress and diurnal course

Regarding anticipatory stress, HR was higher, and RMSSD and SDNN were lower before than during classes (Table 1). The course of cardiac parameters throughout a workday was investigated further in an exploratory manner. Dependent sample t-tests showed significant differences in mean HR on workdays between 6 and 7 am, 7 and 8 am, 8 and 9 am, 11 and 12 am, and 12 am and 1 pm. Wilcoxon tests showed that RMSSD values significantly differed on workdays between 6 and 7 am, 7 and 8 am, 8 and 9 am, 11 and 12 am, 12 am and 1 pm, and 2 and 3 pm. SDNN values significantly differed on workdays between 6 and 7 am, 8 and 9 am, 12 am and 1 pm, and 2 pm and 3 pm (Table 1). For the direction of the effects (higher-lower), consult the means and medians in Table 1 and the diurnal courses in Figures 24. HR was higher, and RMSSD was lower at 1 pm than 12 am. There were no significant differences on the leisure day between any two times for HR, RMSSD, and SDNN (all p > 0.05).

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Significant differences between the times within the workday for HR, RMSSD and SDNN.

Figure 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. HR during the workday and leisure day between 6 am and 7 pm. n workday = 28–38, n leisure day = 5–38.

Figure 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. RMSSD during the workday and leisure day between 6 am and 7 pm. n workday = 28–38, n leisure day = 5–38, RMSSD, root mean square of successive differences in milliseconds, *indicating significant (padj < 0.05) differences between work and leisure day.

Figure 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. SDNN during the workday and leisure day between 6 am and 7 pm. n workday = 28–38, n leisure day = 5–38, SDNN, standard deviation of the NN interval in milliseconds.

3.3 Risk factors and resources

Table 2 present means, standard deviations, and bivariate Pearson correlations between HR, RMSSD, and SDNN at work and leisure days, observed direct and indirect student aggression, frontal teaching, perceived teacher-student relationship, support from other teachers and the school management, and control variables.

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Descriptive statistics and intercorrelations.

RMSSD and SDNN (workday: r = 0.47, p = 0.002; leisure day: r = 0.37, p = 0.015) as well as RMSSD and HR were weakly to moderately associated (workday: r = −0.23, p = 0.090; leisure day: r = 0.47, p = 0.003). SDNN and HR were not significantly associated (workday: r = −0.02, p = 0.454; leisure day: r = −0.03, p = 0.442).

On workdays higher levels of direct student aggression were associated with higher HR and lower RMSSD, and higher levels of observed indirect student aggression were positively associated with HR. Teachers practicing more frontal teaching in their classes had lower RMSSD on leisure days, lower SDNN on workdays and higher HR on leisure days. While high RMSSD on workdays and high SDNN on leisure days were related to a perceived good teacher-student relationship, support from other teachers and the school management was not related to HR, RMSSD and SDNN on workdays.

3.4 Psychological strain

There were hardly any associations between psychological strain variables and cardiac measures. Occupational problems were not associated with teachers’ HR and HRV (Table 2). Surprisingly, there was a positive association between vital exhaustion and SDNN on both work and leisure days.

4 Discussion

The present study aimed to investigate the course of cardiac responses (HR and HRV) over a teacher’s workday and to clarify whether these differ in comparison to a leisure day using ambulatory assessment methods. We focused on risk factors and resources potentially increasing or reducing stress in the teaching profession, on occupational problems, and on vital exhaustion.

Teachers had higher HR and lower HRV levels on workdays than on leisure days. On workdays, teachers showed anticipatory stress (higher HR and lower HRV) before classes and an insufficient recovery at lunchtime. Also on workdays, both higher HR and lower HRV were associated with observer-rated direct student aggression. While a higher amount of frontal teaching was associated with lower HRV, a good perceived teacher-student relationship was associated with more favorable cardiac responses, i.e., higher levels of HRV. Other teachers’ and the school management’s support was unrelated to HR and HRV on workdays, and there was hardly any association between psychological strain variables and cardiac measures. Surprisingly, high levels of SDNN were associated with vital exhaustion on both work and leisure days.

4.1 Work and leisure day

As hypothesized, teachers showed an elevated HR and a reduced HRV on workdays compared to leisure days (hypothesis 1). This result aligns with previous studies showing that more work stress is associated with increased HR (Steptoe and Kivimäki, 2012) and reduced HRV (Chandola et al., 2010; Järvelin-Pasanen et al., 2018). Considering polyvagal theory (Porges, 1995, 1997, 2001), higher HR and lower HRV on workdays compared to leisure days speak for a state of increased physiological arousal during teaching. This can be explained using two different approaches. On the one hand, it may reflect a transitory removal of the vagal brake (reduced vagal tone), enabling a rapid increase in cardiac output without activating the sympathetic-adrenal system (Porges, 1997). Elevated HR and low HRV, on the other hand, may also indicate an activation of the sympathetic nervous system, which prepares the organism for mobilization through augmented energy levels and optimized cognitive readiness. Increased sympathetic nervous system activation is found particularly in prolonged stressful situations, when the environment is not perceived as safe, or when a situation cannot be resolved through social interaction (Porges, 1997, 2003; Kreibig, 2010). These conditions are usually found in the classroom, which is particularly demanding due to the simultaneity of the many and complex challenges such as classroom management, student interactions, and administrative work (Soltau and Mienert, 2010; Rothland, 2013; Nieskens, 2016).

Our results are partially consistent with previous findings demonstrating higher HR (Schwerdtfeger et al., 2008) or lower HRV (Wettstein et al., 2020) during work compared to leisure time in teachers. In contrast to Schwerdtfeger et al. (2008), Wettstein et al. (2020) found no differences in HR when comparing a workday and a leisure day of the same individual. As the latter study is a pilot study, it included a relatively small sample, which may not have had sufficient statistical power to detect smaller differences.

Reduced HR and increased HRV on leisure days likely indicate that teachers’ organisms perceive these days as low-threat environments, allowing for downregulating stress responses and promoting health, growth, and restoration (Porges, 1995). This is achieved by increasing the influence of the vagal brake on the cardiac pacemaker, which slows down the heart and inhibits the mobilization response of the sympathetic nervous system (Porges, 1997). The abovementioned finding aligns with the theory’s premise that the ANS is finely attuned to safety and danger cues within the social context (Porges, 2003).

Overall, the teachers showed a lower HR and a higher HRV on the leisure days than on the working days. However, when analyzing the teachers at the individual level, some of the teachers had slightly lower RMSSD and SDNN values on their leisure days than on their working days. It can be assumed that stressors also occur in private life, which can influence physiological values. Different physical activity levels on working and leisure days (e.g., housework like cleaning) or moderate exercise (cycling, walking) could also have influenced the cardiac parameters. However, teachers were instructed not to engage in excessive sport the day before and on the examination day.

4.2 Anticipatory stress and diurnal course

As expected in hypothesis 2, teachers showed a heightened physiological arousal (higher HR and lower HRV) before class. This finding aligns with an earlier study by Monat et al. (1972) finding that teachers’ anticipation of classroom interactions triggers stress responses. The elevation of HR may signify a shift toward a mobilized state, potentially linked to the body’s preparation for anticipated socially demanding situations (Porges, 1997). In light of the transactional stress model (Lazarus and Folkman, 1984), the anticipatory stress before lessons can be viewed as a product of primary and secondary cognitive appraisals, by which teachers evaluate potential demands of upcoming social interactions and their ability to cope with them.

In line with theory, Pieper et al. (2007) found that anticipatory stress-related worry episodes were associated with increased HR and decreased HRV in teachers, with particularly strong effects observed for work-related concerns. Similarly, Scheuch and Knothe (1997) observed particularly high HR values before lessons as an anticipatory stress reaction.

Changes in cardiac parameters throughout a workday were investigated in an exploratory manner. There were higher HR and lower HRV at midday on workdays compared to the lesson before, which may indicate that lunchtime is particularly stressful for teachers and does not provide sufficient recovery. Instead of relaxing, teachers may use lunchtime on workdays as efficiently as possible by completing organizational tasks (e.g., copying teaching materials and answering emails) or preparing lessons. Teacher colleagues commonly interact during lunchtime, discussing various school-related topics such as organizational matters and individual student behavior. The higher HR and lower HRV values may also indicate anticipatory stress effects when teachers are cognitively dealing with the upcoming challenges of teaching.

4.3 Risk factors and resources

We expected that risk factors (student aggression, frontal teaching) are positively associated with HR and negatively with HRV in teachers, whereas resources (perceived teacher-student relationship and perceived support from the school team) show the opposite effect. Hypothesis 3 was only partially confirmed. Observed student aggression is related to higher HR and lower HRV (RMSSD). Teachers practicing more frontal teaching showed lower HRV (SDNN) values. Teachers who perceived a better relationship with their students showed higher HRV values (RMSSD) but no change in the HR values. Contrary to expectations, there was no significant association between self-rated perceived social support and HR or HRV.

Aggressive student behavior represents a major source of teacher stress (Aloe et al., 2014; Dicke et al., 2014). It elicits physiological stress reactions within teachers, characterized by heightened HR and reduced HRV. Physiologically, these responses manifest as an augmentation in sympathetic arousal and a diminished parasympathetic influence. Based on earlier observational studies on the detection rate of student aggression by teachers (Wettstein, 2008), it must be assumed that teachers are not consciously aware of a considerable proportion of aggression due to the pressure to act in class and the complexity of the social situation. Considering the polyvagal theory, neuroception as a subconscious neural process (Porges, 2003) might be responsible for the fact that teachers still react physiologically to these incidents.

Neurophysiological states promoting mobilization behavior tend to be incompatible with social engagement behaviors (Porges, 2001). As a result, teachers seem to be less able to deal flexibly with challenging student behavior via communication in these moments. Moreover in stressful situations, their repertoire of coping strategies is likely restricted and rigidified, limiting their ability to respond adaptively to student aggression (Porges, 1995; Thayer and Lane, 2000). Teachers with a lower HRV may also be less successful in regulating their emotions and are, therefore, less likely to show cooperative behavior (Lischke et al., 2018). Consequently, there is a risk that challenging classroom situations will persist or exacerbate.

A perceived positive teacher-student relationship can have a protective effect and is likely associated with enhanced vagal tone and, consequently, with higher HRV (Porges, 2001, 2003). Through the subconscious process of neuroception, a perceived positive teacher-student relationship presumably activates a feeling of safety in teachers, promoting social engagement behavior (Porges, 2001, 2003). This may enable teachers to respond more sensitively and flexibly to challenges such as student aggression. A good teacher-student relationship also likely prevents student misbehavior and may foster prosocial student behavior (Pianta, 2006; Obsuth et al., 2017), which in turn can reduce stress levels among teachers (Aldrup et al., 2018). The perceived better teacher-student relationship may additionally act as a form of social support, contributing to the observed physiological relaxation in teachers.

Reduced HRV can result from elevated stress levels. However, individuals with reduced HRV might also interpret their environment differently. For example, Thayer and Brosschot (2005) demonstrated that people with a lower HRV react to neutral, harmless stimuli as if they were threatening. In contrast, those with higher HRV were better able to adapt their responses to environmental conditions. Consequently, it is plausible that teachers with a lower HRV might view the classroom environment as more hostile even in the absence of threat, increasing the likelihood of engaging in mobilization behavior.

The inverse correlation between HRV and frontal teaching emphasizes the potential stress-inducing nature of this didactic instructional method. Compared to other forms of teaching, frontal teaching is particularly challenging for teachers as they are highly exposed (Wettstein, 2008). They have to impart knowledge to the students and react immediately and adaptively to what is happening in the classroom. Meanwhile, students are relatively inactive and may attempt to go unnoticed or push boundaries (Wettstein, 2008). The reduced HRV observed during frontal teaching may reflect increased vigilance or arousal during these demanding pedagogical practices.

Our study revealed a notable correlation between frontal teaching or perceived teacher-student relationship and HRV, while no significant association with HR was observed. Student aggression was associated with both HRV and HR. It can be assumed that student aggression is related to an activation of the sympathetic nervous system (i.e., increased HR, decreased HRV), as aggression harms social relationships, which is particularly stressful for teachers (Baumeister and Leary, 1995; Epel et al., 2018). In contrast, frontal teaching appears to be a comparatively moderate stressor. This suggests that frontal teaching is related to a release of the vagal brake (i.e., reduced activation of the parasympathetic nervous system) (Porges, 1997).

The different findings regarding the association between RMSSD or SDNN and the risk factors or resources may have occurred since RMSSD and SDNN capture different aspects of HRV, specifically reflecting different branches of the ANS (Shaffer et al., 2014). RMSSD estimates vagally mediated (i.e., parasympathetic) changes in HRV (Shaffer et al., 2014). SDNN is more a measure of the general adaptability of the ANS. It reflects the interplay between the sympathetic and parasympathetic nervous systems (Shaffer et al., 2014), which could influence the different correlations.

4.4 Psychological strain

Contrary to our expectations, there were hardly any associations between psychological strain variables and cardiac measures (hypothesis 4). Occupational problems were not associated with teachers’ HR and HRV, and, unexpectedly, there were slightly positive associations between HRV (SDNN) and vital exhaustion.

Generally, findings on the association between chronic stress and HRV have been inconsistent, with hypoactive vagal tone (i.e., reduced HRV) often reported (Keltikangas-Järvinen and Heponiemi, 2004; Thayer and Lane, 2009; Collins and Karasek, 2010). However, increased HRV has been occasionally found with chronic stress (Wekenborg et al., 2019; Matuz et al., 2021; Jiryis et al., 2022). When exhaustion increases, the parasympathetic nervous system can be activated to achieve physiological relaxation. An elevated HRV may help to deal with stress-inducing situations by avoiding them more efficiently (Jiryis et al., 2022). In addition, elevated HRV seems to be associated with more withdrawal from subjectively unmanageable situations (Wekenborg et al., 2019) and with heightened task disengagement (Matuz et al., 2021).

In summary, the physiological response, as indicated by HRV, does not necessarily align with subjective reports of vital exhaustion or occupational stress. Also, other factors, possibly related to the specific dynamics of social interactions in the teaching environment or individual coping mechanisms, may contribute to the observed dissociation. A study by Lupien et al. (2022) suggests that the experience of psychological stress rather reflects poor emotion regulation capacities than physiological stress reactions. It is also conceivable that the relationship between subjective and physiological stress markers is not linear (Lupien et al., 2022).

Thus, physiological stress markers and psychological strain appear to be dissociated. Although commitment and relationship orientation may protect against physiological stress, they come at a price on a psychological level. Relationship work is exhausting.

4.5 Limitations and strengths

While our study on teacher stress in the context of social interactions and in light of polyvagal theory provides valuable insights, we must acknowledge certain limitations of our findings. The sample size and the participants’ specific characteristics may constrain our findings’ generalizability. Our study focused on apparently healthy and medication-free teachers, so the results may not be generalizable to all teachers. As the data was collected on one workday and one leisure day for each participant, the teachers’ daily form may also have influenced the results. Given the predominantly cross-sectional nature of the data collection, causality cannot be established. The ambulatory assessment approach employed in our study also has limitations. The reliance on wearable devices for continuous monitoring may introduce measurement errors or technological challenges that could affect the accuracy and reliability of the recorded HR and HRV data. In real-world environments, various factors impact physiological states, such as posture, respiratory depth, physical activity, food and beverage consumption, circadian rhythms, speech, and social situations (Houtveen and de Geus, 2009). Cardiac activity, in particular, is also affected by non-stress-related factors, such as physical activity (Zanstra and Johnston, 2011). The teachers’ different activity levels may also have influenced the cardiac parameters. The absence of experimental control over those factors poses a noteworthy risk of confounding variables. We believe laboratory and field approaches are complementary rather than conflicting strategies; they provide data and perspectives from distinct angles (Wilhelm and Grossman, 2010).

However, since most studies on teacher stress are based on questionnaire surveys or laboratory experiments, the ambulatory assessment approach is also one of the main strengths of our study, as it combines self-report, behavioral observations, and electrophysiological measures of teachers’ everyday working lives. This methodology provides a real-world insight into teachers’ daily experiences (Fahrenberg et al., 2007; Wilhelm and Grossman, 2010; Kockler et al., 2017) and thus allows for a more realistic view of daily stressors and resources. It enables a more thorough understanding of the multifaceted teaching profession. By capturing data in teachers’ naturalistic environments rather than in a controlled laboratory setting, we enhance the ecological validity of our findings. The application of polyvagal theory in the teaching profession is particularly exciting, as this profession involves many complex challenges in social interactions and requires a high degree of flexibility on the part of teachers. By examining HR and HRV in parallel, we gain a comprehensive understanding of ANS dynamics.

4.6 Implications

This study emphasizes the great complexity of the teaching profession and contributes to existing literature on the impact of classroom dynamics on teacher well-being. Our study offers various starting points for stress prevention and management for teachers, considering both work and non-work factors. One approach could be strengthening the teacher-student relationship, as this prevents teacher stress. Training teachers in strategies to foster positive connections with students may contribute to students’ academic success (Pianta et al., 2012) and enhance teacher well-being. The findings underscore the importance of pedagogical training addressing student behavior and reevaluating instructional methods to create a supportive and less stressful teaching environment. Reducing teachers’ overall stress levels is crucial to prevent stress from becoming chronic. Equally important is providing support for teachers in developing effective stress management strategies. The implications extend beyond individual classrooms, urging educators, administrators, and policymakers to promote positive school climates and equip teachers with the tools to navigate challenging situations effectively.

5 Conclusion

In conclusion, the article’s exploration of teacher stress through the lens of polyvagal theory, using ambulatory assessment of HR and HRV, provides valuable insights into the complex dynamics within educational settings and contributes to our understanding of the factors influencing teacher well-being and health. Positive social relationships are a fundamental human need (Baumeister and Leary, 1995), and the high relevance of social interactions is particularly evident in the teaching profession. Whereas teacher-student interactions can cause stress reactions in teachers (e.g., student aggression), a perceived positive relationship can also contribute to physiological relaxation. It should not be forgotten that building and maintaining positive relationships can be exhausting and come at a psychological cost. Overall, social interactions seem to be crucial for teacher health (Spilt et al., 2011; Aldrup et al., 2018).

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by the Swiss Ethics Committee Bern on research involving humans. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

FK: Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. AW: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. GJ: Formal analysis, Writing – original draft. IS: Writing – review & editing. Mg: Supervision, Writing – review & editing. RL: Conceptualization, Supervision, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare that financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. This research was funded by the Swiss National Science Foundation, grant number 100019_185484, and the University of Teacher Education Bern, grant number 16 w 0008 02. The APC was funded by the Swiss National Science Foundation.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Aldrup, K., Klusmann, U., Lüdtke, O., Göllner, R., and Trautwein, U. (2018). Student misbehavior and teacher well-being: testing the mediating role of the teacher-student relationship. Learn. Instr. 58, 126–136. doi: 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2018.05.006

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Allen, A. P., Kennedy, P. J., Cryan, J. F., Dinan, T. G., and Clarke, G. (2014). Biological and psychological markers of stress in humans: focus on the Trier social stress test. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 38, 94–124. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.11.005

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Aloe, A. M., Amo, L. C., and Shanahan, M. E. (2014). Classroom management self-efficacy and burnout: A multivariate meta-analysis. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 26, 101–126. doi: 10.1007/s10648-013-9244-0

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Anderson, C. A., and Bushman, B. J. (2002). Human aggression. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 53, 27–51. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135231

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Appels, A. P. W. M., Höppener, P., and Mulder, P. (1987). A questionnaire to assess premonitory symptoms of myocardial infarction. Int. J. Cardiol. 17, 15–24. doi: 10.1016/0167-5273(87)90029-5

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Appels, A., and Mulder, P. (1989). Fatigue and heart disease. The association between ‘vital exhaustion’ and past, present and future coronary heart disease. J. Psychosom. Res. 33, 727–738. doi: 10.1016/0022-3999(89)90088-3

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Baumeister, R. F., and Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to belong: desire for interpersonal attachments as a fundamental human motivation. Psychol. Bull. 117, 497–529. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.117.3.497

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Beuchel, P., and Cramer, C. (2023). Heart rate variability and perceived stress in teacher training: facing the reality shock with mindfulness? Glob. Adv. Integr. Med. Health 12, 1–16. doi: 10.1177/27536130231176538

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Björkqvist, K., Lagerspetz, K. M. J., and Kaukiainen, A. (1992). Do girls manipulate and boys fight? Developmental trends in regard to direct and indirect aggression. Aggress. Behav. 18, 117–127. doi: 10.1002/1098-2337(1992)18:2<117::AID-AB2480180205>3.0.CO;2-3

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bodenmann, G., Perrez, M., and Gottman, J. M. (1996). Die Bedeutung des intrapsychischen Copings für die dyadische Interaktion unter Stress. Z. Für Klin. Psychol. 25, 1–13.

Google Scholar

Boyle, G. J., Borg, M. G., Falzon, J. M., and Baglioni, A. J. Jr. (1995). A structural model of the dimensions of teacher stress. Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 65, 49–67. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8279.1995.tb01130.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Byrne, B. M. (1999). “The nomological network of teacher burnout: A literature review and empirically validated model” in Understanding and preventing teacher burnout: A sourcebook of international research and practice. eds. R. Vandenberghe and A. M. Huberman (Oxford, England: Cambridge University Press), 15–37.

Google Scholar

Catalano, J. T. (2002). Guide to ECG analysis. Philadelphia: Lippincott: Williams & Wilkins.

Google Scholar

Chandola, T., Heraclides, A., and Kumari, M. (2010). Psychophysiological biomarkers of workplace stressors. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 35, 51–57. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.11.005

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chang, M.-L., and Davis, H. A. (2009). “Understanding the role of teacher appraisals in shaping the dynamics of their relationships with students: deconstructing teachers’ judgments of disruptive behavior/students” in Advances in teacher emotion research. eds. P. A. Schutz and M. Zembylas (Boston: Springer US), 95–127.

Google Scholar

Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. 2nd Edn. New York: Routledge.

Google Scholar

Collins, S., and Karasek, R. (2010). Reduced vagal cardiac control variance in exhausted and high strain job subjects. Int. J. Occup. Med. Environ. Health 23, 267–278. doi: 10.2478/v10001-010-0023-6

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Davis, H. A. (2003). Conceptualizing the role and influence of student-teacher relationships on children’s social and cognitive development. Educ. Psychol. 38, 207–234. doi: 10.1207/S15326985EP3804_2

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dicke, T., Parker, P. D., Marsh, H. W., Kunter, M., Schmeck, A., and Leutner, D. (2014). Self-efficacy in classroom management, classroom disturbances, and emotional exhaustion: A moderated mediation analysis of teacher candidates. J. Educ. Psychol. 106, 569–583. doi: 10.1037/a0035504

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Doyle, W. (1986). “Classroom organization and management” in Handbook of research on teaching. ed. M. C. Wittrock , (New York: Macmillan) 392–431.

Google Scholar

Dunbar, R. I. M. (2009). The social brain hypothesis and its implications for social evolution. Ann. Hum. Biol. 36, 562–572. doi: 10.1080/03014460902960289

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ehlert, U., Marca, R. L., Abbruzzese, E., and Kübler, U. (2013). Biopsychologie. Stuttgart: Kohlhammer Verlag.

Google Scholar

Epel, E. S., Crosswell, A. D., Mayer, S. E., Prather, A. A., Slavich, G. M., Puterman, E., et al. (2018). More than a feeling: A unified view of stress measurement for population science. Front. Neuroendocrinol. 49, 146–169. doi: 10.1016/j.yfrne.2018.03.001

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Eurofound and EU-OSHA (2014). Psychosocial risks in Europe: Prevalence and strategies for prevention. Luxembourg: Publications Office of the European Union.

Google Scholar

Evers, W. J. G., Tomic, W., and Brouwers, A. (2004). Burnout among teachers: students’ and teachers’ perceptions compared. Sch. Psychol. Int. 25, 131–148. doi: 10.1177/0143034304043670

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fahrenberg, J., Myrtek, M., Pawlik, K., and Perrez, M. (2007). Ambulatory assessment - monitoring behavior in daily life settings. Eur. J. Psychol. Assess. 23, 206–213. doi: 10.1027/1015-5759.23.4.206

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Faßnacht, G. (1995). Systematische Verhaltensbeobachtung. München: Reinhardt.

Google Scholar

Friedman, I. A. (2006). “Classroom management and teacher stress and burnout” in Handbook of classroom management: Research, practice, and contemporary issues. eds. C. M. Evertson and C. S. Weinstein (Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates), 925–944.

Google Scholar

Friedman-Krauss, A. H., Raver, C. C., Morris, P. A., and Jones, S. M. (2014). The role of classroom-level child behavior problems in predicting preschool teacher stress and classroom emotional climate. Early Educ. Dev. 25, 530–552. doi: 10.1080/10409289.2013.817030

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Glickman, M. E., Rao, S. R., and Schultz, M. R. (2014). False discovery rate control is a recommended alternative to Bonferroni-type adjustments in health studies. J. Clin. Epidemiol. 67, 850–857. doi: 10.1016/j.jclinepi.2014.03.012

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hagemann, W., and Geuenich, K. (2009). Burnout-screening-Skalen: BOSS. Göttingen: Hogrefe.

Google Scholar

Helmke, A. (2009). Unterrichtsqualität und Lehrerprofessionalität. Diagnose, Evaluation und Verbesserung des Unterrichts. Seelze-Velber: Klett-Kallmeyer.

Google Scholar

Herzog, W. (2006). Zeitgemässe Erziehung: die Konstruktion pädagogischer Wirklichkeit. Velbrück: Wissenschaft.

Google Scholar

Houtveen, J. H., and de Geus, E. J. C. (2009). Noninvasive psychophysiological ambulatory recordings: study design and data analysis strategies. Eur. Psychol. 14, 132–141. doi: 10.1027/1016-9040.14.2.132

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ingersoll, R. M. (2001). Teacher turnover and teacher shortages: an organizational analysis. Am. Educ. Res. J. 38, 499–534. doi: 10.3102/00028312038003499

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Järvelin-Pasanen, S., Sinikallio, S., and Tarvainen, M. P. (2018). Heart rate variability and occupational stress—systematic review. Ind. Health 56, 500–511. doi: 10.2486/indhealth.2017-0190

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Jiryis, T., Magal, N., Fructher, E., Hertz, U., and Admon, R. (2022). Resting-state heart rate variability (HRV) mediates the association between perceived chronic stress and ambiguity avoidance. Sci. Rep. 12:17645. doi: 10.1038/s41598-022-22584-4

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ju, C., Lan, J., Li, Y., Feng, W., and You, X. (2015). The mediating role of workplace social support on the relationship between trait emotional intelligence and teacher burnout. Teach. Teach. Educ. 51, 58–67. doi: 10.1016/j.tate.2015.06.001

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keltikangas-Järvinen, L., and Heponiemi, T. (2004). Vital exhaustion, temperament, and cardiac reactivity in task-induced stress. Biol. Psychol. 65, 121–135. doi: 10.1016/S0301-0511(03)00112-1

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Klusmann, U., Richter, D., and Lüdtke, O. (2016). Teachers’ emotional exhaustion is negatively related to students’ achievement: evidence from a large-scale assessment study. J. Educ. Psychol. 108, 1193–1203. doi: 10.1037/edu0000125

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kockler, T. D., Tschacher, W., Santangelo, P. S., Limberger, M. F., and Ebner-Priemer, U. W. (2017). Specificity of emotion sequences in borderline personality disorder compared to posttraumatic stress disorder, bulimia nervosa, and healthy controls: an e-diary study. Borderline Personal. Disord. Emot. Dysregulation 4:26. doi: 10.1186/s40479-017-0077-1

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kokkinos, C. M. (2007). Job stressors, personality and burnout in primary school teachers. Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 77, 229–243. doi: 10.1348/000709905X90344

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Krause, A., Dorsemagen, C., and Meder, L. (2013). “Messung psychischer Belastungen im Unterricht mit RHIA-Unterricht” in Belastung und Beanspruchung im Lehrerberuf. ed. M. Rothland (Wiesbaden: Springer), 99–116.

Google Scholar

Kreibig, S. D. (2010). Autonomic nervous system activity in emotion: A review. Biol. Psychol. 84, 394–421. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2010.03.010

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Künzi, K., and Oesch, T. (2016). Berufsbedingte Krankheitskosten der Lehrpersonen. Teilprojekt des LCH im Rahmen des Projekts Gesundheit der Lehrpersonen. Schlussbericht. Available at: https://www.buerobass.ch/fileadmin/Files/2016/LCH_2016_KrankheitskostenLehrpersonen.pdf (Accessed March 28, 2022).

Google Scholar

Kyriacou, C. (2015). “Teacher stress and burnout: methodological perspectives” in International encyclopedia of the social and behavioral sciences. ed. J. D. Wright (Amsterdam: Elsevier), 72–74.

Google Scholar

La Marca, R., Schneider, S., Jenni, G., Kühne, F., Holtforth, M. G., and Wettstein, A. (2023). Associations between stress, resources, and hair cortisol concentration in teachers. Psychoneuroendocrinology 154:106291. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2023.106291

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Laborde, S., Mosley, E., and Thayer, J. F. (2017). Heart rate variability and cardiac vagal tone in psychophysiological research – recommendations for experiment planning, data analysis, and data reporting. Front. Psychol. 8, 1–18.doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00213

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lazarus, R. S. (1991). Emotion and adaptation. New York: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Lazarus, R. S., and Folkman, S. (1984). Stress, appraisal, and coping New York: Springer Publishing Company.

Google Scholar

Lehr, D. (2004). “Psychosomatisch erkrankte und ‘gesunde’ Lehrkräfte: Auf der Suche nach den entscheidenden Unterschieden” in Psychosomatische Erkrankungen bei Lehrerinnen und Lehrern: Ursachen, Folgen, Lösungen. eds. A. Hillert and E. Schmitz (Stuttgart: Schattauer), 120–140.

Google Scholar

Levy, M. N. (1990). Autonomic interactions in cardiac control. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 601, 209–221. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1990.tb37302.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lischke, A., Mau-Moeller, A., Jacksteit, R., Pahnke, R., Hamm, A. O., and Weippert, M. (2018). Heart rate variability is associated with social value orientation in males but not females. Sci. Rep. 8:7336. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-25739-4

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lortie, D. C. (2002). Schoolteacher: A sociological study. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

Google Scholar

Lupien, S. J., Leclaire, S., Majeur, D., Raymond, C., Jean Baptiste, F., and Giguère, C.-E. (2022). Doctor, I am so stressed out!’ A descriptive study of biological, psychological, and socioemotional markers of stress in individuals who self-identify as being ‘very stressed out’ or ‘zen. Neurobiol. Stress 18:100454. doi: 10.1016/j.ynstr.2022.100454

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Matuz, A., van der Linden, D., Kisander, Z., Hernádi, I., Karádi, K., and Csathó, Á. (2021). Enhanced cardiac vagal tone in mental fatigue: analysis of heart rate variability in time-on-task, recovery, and reactivity. PLoS One 16:e0238670. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0238670

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mauss, M. D., and Robinson, I. B. (2009). Measures of emotion: A review. Cogn. Emot. 23, 209–237. doi: 10.1080/02699930802204677

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

McCarthy, C. J., Lambert, R. G., Lineback, S., Fitchett, P., and Baddouh, P. G. (2016). Assessing teacher appraisals and stress in the classroom: review of the classroom appraisal of resources and demands. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 28, 577–603. doi: 10.1007/s10648-015-9322-6

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

McCormick, J., and Barnett, K. (2011). Teachers’ attributions for stress and their relationships with burnout. Int. J. Educ. Manag. 25, 278–293. doi: 10.1108/09513541111120114

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

McEwen, B. S. (2007). Physiology and neurobiology of stress and adaptation: central role of the brain. Physiol. Rev. 87, 873–904. doi: 10.1152/physrev.00041.2006

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

McGee, A. B., Scott, S. R., Manczak, E. M., and Watamura, S. E. (2023). Associations between the psychophysiological impacts of teacher occupational stress and stress biomarkers: A systematic review. Psychoneuroimmunol. J. 4, 1–16. doi: 10.32371/pnij/246145

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Monat, A., Averill, J. R., and Lazarus, R. S. (1972). Anticipatory stress and coping reactions under various conditions of uncertainty. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 24, 237–253. doi: 10.1037/h0033297

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Nieskens, B. (2016). “Der Arbeitsplatz Schule” in Beruf Lehrer/Lehrerin. ed. M. Rothland (Münster: Waxmann), 33–48. doi: 10.1007/978-3-531-18990-1_2

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Obsuth, I., Murray, A. L., Malti, T., Sulger, P., Ribeaud, D., and Eisner, M. (2017). A non-bipartite propensity score analysis of the effects of teacher–student relationships on adolescent problem and prosocial behavior. J. Youth Adolesc. 46, 1661–1687. doi: 10.1007/s10964-016-0534-y

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pianta, R. C. (2006). “Classroom Management and Relationships Between Children and Teachers: Implications for Research and Practice” in Handbook of classroom management: Research, practice, and contemporary issues. eds. C. M. Evertson and C. S. Weinstein (Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates), 685–710.

Google Scholar

Pianta, R. C., Hamre, B., and Stuhlman, M. (2012). “Relationships between teachers and children” in Handbook of psychology, Educational Psychology. eds. W. M. Reynolds and G. E. Miller (Hoboken New Jersey: John Wiley & Sons), 199–234.

Google Scholar

Pieper, S., Brosschot, J. F., van der Leeden, R., and Thayer, J. F. (2007). Cardiac effects of momentary assessed worry episodes and stressful events. Psychosom. Med. 69, 901–909. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e31815a9230

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pieper, S., Brosschot, J. F., van der Leeden, R., and Thayer, J. F. (2010). Prolonged cardiac effects of momentary assessed stressful events and worry episodes. Psychosom. Med. 72, 570–577. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e3181dbc0e9

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pinel, J. P. J. (2010). Basics of biopsychology. London: Pearson College Division.

Google Scholar

Porges, S. W. (1995). Orienting in a defensive world: mammalian modifications of our evolutionary heritage. A polyvagal theory. Psychophysiology 32, 301–318. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1995.tb01213.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Porges, S. W. (1997). Emotion: an evolutionary by-product of the neural regulation of the autonomic nervous system. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 807, 62–77. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1997.tb51913.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Porges, S. W. (2001). The polyvagal theory: phylogenetic substrates of a social nervous system. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 42, 123–146. doi: 10.1016/S0167-8760(01)00162-3

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Porges, S. W. (2003). Social engagement and attachment: A phylogenetic perspective. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1008, 31–47. doi: 10.1196/annals.1301.004

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Porges, S. W. (2021). Polyvagal theory: A biobehavioral journey to sociality. Compr. Psychoneuroendocrinol. 7:100069. doi: 10.1016/j.cpnec.2021.100069

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Porges, S. W. (2022). Polyvagal theory: A science of safety. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 16:871227. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2022.871227

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rau, R., Gebele, N., and Morling, K. (2010) in Untersuchung arbeitsbedingter Ursachen für das Auftreten von depressiven Störungen: Forschung Projekt F 1865. ed. Bundesanstalt für Arbeitsschutz und Arbeitsmedizin (Dortmund: BAuA).

Google Scholar

Ritvanen, T., Laitinen, T., and Hänninen, O. (2004). Relief of work stress after weekend and holiday season in high school teachers. J. Occup. Health 46, 213–215. doi: 10.1539/joh.46.213

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rothland, M. (2013). “Beruf: Lehrer/Lehrerin – Arbeitsplatz: Schule Charakteristika der Arbeitstätigkeit und Bedingungen der Berufssituation” in Belastung und Beanspruchung im Lehrerberuf: Modelle, Befunde, Interventionen. ed. M. Rothland (Wiesbaden: Springer Fachmedien), 21–39.

Google Scholar

Santarpia, F. P., Filosa, L., Ottaviani, C., Borgogni, L., and Alessandri, G. (2023). Supporting each other’s heart at work: an ambulatory field study. Int. J. Stress. Manag. 30, 262–273. doi: 10.1037/str0000287

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Scherzinger, M., and Wettstein, A. (2019). Classroom disruptions, the teacher–student relationship and classroom management from the perspective of teachers, students and external observers: A multimethod approach. Learn. Environ. Res. 22, 101–116. doi: 10.1007/s10984-018-9269-x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Scheuch, K., and Knothe, M. (1997). “Psychophysische Beanspruchung von Lehrern in der Unterrichtstätigkeit” in Jahrbuch für Lehrerforschung. eds. R. Tippelt and B. Schmidt (Weinheim: Juventa), 285–299.

Google Scholar

Schilbach, L. (2015). Eye to eye, face to face and brain to brain: novel approaches to study the behavioral dynamics and neural mechanisms of social interactions. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 3, 130–135. doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2015.03.006

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmid, R. F., and Thomas, J. (2020). Teachers’ ambulatory heart rate variability as an outcome and moderating variable in the job demands-resources model. Anxiety Stress Coping 33, 387–402. doi: 10.1080/10615806.2020.1746286

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Schwerdtfeger, A., Konermann, L., and Schönhofen, K. (2008). Self-efficacy as a health-protective resource in teachers? A biopsychological approach. Health Psychol. 27, 358–368. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.27.3.358

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Schwerdtfeger, A. R., and Schlagert, H. (2011). The conjoined effect of naturalistic perceived available support and enacted support on cardiovascular reactivity during a laboratory stressor. Ann. Behav. Med. 42, 64–78. doi: 10.1007/s12160-011-9272-2

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Seitz, S. (2007). “Die Bedeutung der Lehrerpersönlichkeit für das Gelingen von Erziehung und Unterricht” in Schwierige Schüler – was tun? Ein Ratgeber für die Unterrichtspraxis. ed. H. Göldner (München: Oldenburg), 13–40.

Google Scholar

Shaffer, F., and Ginsberg, J. P. (2017). An overview of heart rate variability metrics and norms. Front. Public Health 5:258. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2017.00258

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Shaffer, F., McCraty, R., and Zerr, C. L. (2014). A healthy heart is not a metronome: an integrative review of the heart’s anatomy and heart rate variability. Front. Psychol. 5:1040. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01040

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Soltau, A., and Mienert, M. (2010). Unsicherheit im Lehrerberuf als Ursache mangelnder Lehrerkooperation? Eine Systematisierung des aktuellen Forschungsstandes auf Basis des transaktionalen Stressmodells. Z. Für Pädagog. 5, 761–778. doi: 10.25656/01:7169

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Spilt, J. L., Koomen, H. M. Y., and Thijs, J. T. (2011). Teacher wellbeing: the importance of teacher–student relationships. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 23, 457–477. doi: 10.1007/s10648-011-9170-y

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Steptoe, A. (2000). Stress, social support and cardiovascular activity over the working day. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 37, 299–308. doi: 10.1016/S0167-8760(00)00109-4

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Steptoe, A., Cropley, M., and Joekes, K. (1999). Job strain, blood pressure and response to uncontrollable stress. J. Hypertens. 17, 193–200. doi: 10.1097/00004872-199917020-00003

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Steptoe, A., and Kivimäki, M. (2012). Stress and cardiovascular disease. Nat. Rev. Cardiol. 9, 360–370. doi: 10.1038/nrcardio.2012.45

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tarvainen, M. P., Niskanen, J.-P., Lipponen, J. A., Ranta-Aho, P. O., and Karjalainen, P. A. (2014). Kubios HRV – heart rate variability analysis software. Comput. Methods Prog. Biomed. 113, 210–220. doi: 10.1016/j.cmpb.2013.07.024

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Task Force of the European Society of Cardiology the North American Society of Pacing and Electrophysiology (1996). Heart rate variability: standards of measurement. Physiol. Interpret. Clin. Use 17, 354–381. doi: 10.1161/01.CIR.93.5.1043

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Thayer, J. F., and Brosschot, J. F. (2005). Psychosomatics and psychopathology: looking up and down from the brain. Psychoneuroendocrinology 30, 1050–1058. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2005.04.014

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Thayer, J. F., Hansen, A. L., Saus-Rose, E., and Johnsen, B. H. (2009). Heart rate variability, prefrontal neural function, and cognitive performance: the neurovisceral integration perspective on self-regulation, adaptation, and health. Ann. Behav. Med. 37, 141–153. doi: 10.1007/s12160-009-9101-z

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Thayer, J. F., and Lane, R. D. (2000). A model of neurovisceral integration in emotion regulation and dysregulation. J. Affect. Disord. 61, 201–216. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0327(00)00338-4

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Thayer, J. F., and Lane, R. D. (2009). Claude Bernard and the heart–brain connection: further elaboration of a model of neurovisceral integration. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 33, 81–88. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008.08.004

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Trull, T. J., and Ebner-Priemer, U. (2013). Ambulatory assessment. Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 9, 151–176. doi: 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-050212-185510

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Uijtdehaage, S. H. J., and Thayer, J. F. (2000). Accentuated antagonism in the control of human heart rate. Clin. Auton. Res. 10, 107–110. doi: 10.1007/BF02278013

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Umberson, D., and Karas Montez, J. (2010). Social relationships and health: A flashpoint for health policy. J. Health Soc. Behav. 51 Suppl, S54–S66. doi: 10.1177/0022146510383501

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

VERBI Software (2019). MAXQDA 2020 (20.4.1) [computer software]. Berlin, Germany: VERBI Software.

Google Scholar

Wekenborg, M. K., von Dawans, B., Hill, L. K., Thayer, J. F., Penz, M., and Kirschbaum, C. (2019). Examining reactivity patterns in burnout and other indicators of chronic stress. Psychoneuroendocrinology 106, 195–205. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2019.04.002

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wentzel, K. R. (2010). “Students’ relationships with teachers” in Handbook of research on schools, schooling and human development. eds. J. L. Meece and J. S. Eccles (New York: Routledge), 75–91.

Google Scholar

Wettstein, A. (2008). Beobachtungssystem zur Analyse aggressiven Verhaltens in schulischen Settings (BASYS). Bern: Hans Huber.

Google Scholar

Wettstein, A., Jenni, G., Schneider, I., Kühne, F. G., Holtforth, M., and La Marca, R. (2023a). Predictors of psychological strain and allostatic load in teachers: examining the long-term effects of biopsychosocial risk and protective factors using a LASSO regression approach. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 20:5760. doi: 10.3390/ijerph20105760

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wettstein, A., Jenni, G., Schneider, S., Kühne, F. G., Holtforth, M., and La Marca, R. (2023b). Teachers’ perception of aggressive student behavior through the lens of chronic worry and resignation, and its association with psychophysiological stress: an observational study. Soc. Psychol. Educ. 26, 1181–1200. doi: 10.1007/s11218-023-09782-2

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wettstein, A., Kühne, F., Tschacher, W., and La Marca, R. (2020). Ambulatory assessment of psychological and physiological stress on workdays and free days among teachers. A preliminary study. Front. Neurosci. 14:112. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2020.00112

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wettstein, A., Scherzinger, M., and Wyler, S. (2016). Fragebogen zur Erfassung von Störungen im Unterricht: die Faktorenstruktur der Schülerinnen-und Schülerversion. Empirische Sonderpädag 8, 189–202. doi: 10.25656/01:12304

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wettstein, A., Schneider, S., grosse Holtforth, M., and la Marca, R. (2021). Teacher stress: A psychobiological approach to stressful interactions in the classroom. Front. Educ. 6:1258. doi: 10.3389/feduc.2021.681258

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wilhelm, F. H., and Grossman, P. (2010). Emotions beyond the laboratory: theoretical fundaments, study design, and analytic strategies for advanced ambulatory assessment. Biol. Psychol. 84, 552–569. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2010.01.017

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zanstra, Y. J., and Johnston, D. W. (2011). Cardiovascular reactivity in real life settings: measurement, mechanisms and meaning. Biol. Psychol. 86, 98–105. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2010.05.002

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: heart rate, heart rate variability, polyvagal theory, student aggression, teacher stress, teacher-student relationship

Citation: Kühne F, Wettstein A, Jenni G, Schneider I, grosse Holtforth M and La Marca R (2024) Teacher stress in social interactions in the light of polyvagal theory. An ambulatory assessment approach to teachers’ heart rate and heart rate variability. Front. Neurosci. 18:1499229. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2024.1499229

Received: 20 September 2024; Accepted: 14 November 2024;
Published: 04 December 2024.

Edited by:

Luiz Carlos Marques Vanderlei, São Paulo State University, Brazil

Reviewed by:

Carmen Russoniello, East Carolina University, United States
Monika H. Donker, Utrecht University, Netherlands

Copyright © 2024 Kühne, Wettstein, Jenni, Schneider, grosse Holtforth and La Marca. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Fabienne Kühne, ZmFiaWVubmUua3VlaG5lQGFnLmNo

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.