- 1Westlake Laboratory of Life Sciences and Biomedicine, Hangzhou, China
- 2School of Life Sciences, Westlake University, Hangzhou, China
- 3Institute of Biology, Westlake Institute for Advanced Study, Hangzhou, China
Adolescence is a pivotal stage during development when one’s personality, emotion, and behavioral traits are shaped to a great extent, and the underlying neural circuits undergo substantial developmental organizations. Dramatic and dynamic changes occur in sleep architecture throughout the postnatal developmental course. Insufficient sleep and disruption of sleep/wake coherence are prevalent among the adolescents worldwide, and even so in young patients with neuropsychiatric conditions. Although accumulating evidence has suggested a tight association between sleep disruption and depression/anxiety, the causal relationship remains largely unclear. More importantly, most of these studies focused on adult subjects, and little is known about the role of sleep during the development of mood and behavior. Here we review recent studies investigating the acute and chronic effects of adolescent sleep disruption on depression and anxiety both in humans and rodent models with focuses on the assessment methodology and age. By discussing the findings and unsolved problems, we hope to achieve a better understanding of the relationship between sleep and mental health in adolescents and provide insights for future research.
Introduction
Adolescence is the transition period between childhood and adulthood when individuals experience significant physiological and psychological transformations. It is believed to be a critical period for higher-order brain functions such as learning, decision-making, emotion, and behavior. It is also a “risk” stage for many neuropsychiatric disorders, including major depressive disorders, anxiety disorders, alcohol and other substance abuse, schizophrenia, and bipolar disorder (Davidson et al., 2015; Solmi et al., 2022). The neural underpinnings of this susceptibility remain largely unknown. It likely involves the delayed maturation, and thus prolonged vulnerability, of brain structures underlying these higher-order functions, such as the prefrontal cortex (PFC) (Kolk and Rakic, 2022). Following the rapid formation of massive synaptic connections in childhood, the adolescent brain undergoes substantial refinement and modification of neural connections, therefore exhibits a high level of plasticity and hence more vulnerable to environmental changes compared to the adult brain (Kolk and Rakic, 2022; Moyer and Zuo, 2018). This was mostly demonstrated in the sensory-motor cortices, but evidence suggesting other cortical regions, such as the PFC, as well as subcortical regions follow the similar developmental trajectory, although the time course may be different (Kolk and Rakic, 2022).
Sleep architecture evolves dramatically during postnatal development (Roffwarg et al., 1966). The total amount of sleep decreases progressively and dramatically from 14-16 h/day in newborn babies to 7–8 h/day in adults. Almost 50% of total sleep at birth (~ 8 h/day) is the rapid-eye-movement (REM) sleep, which is rapidly reduced in childhood, and by the onset of adolescence, it constitutes of 20% of total sleep (~2 h/day). On the other hand, non-REM (NREM) sleep first increases in infancy and remains at relatively high level (~9 h/day), before it demonstrates a substantial decrease during adolescence and eventually contributes to 80% of adult sleep (~6 h/day). In addition to the sleep duration changes, specific brain activity patterns also demonstrate dynamics over the adolescent developmental course. Compared to early adolescence, the EEG delta power in NREM sleep and theta power in REM sleep decrease by about 60% at the end of adolescence (Campbell and Feinberg, 2009). These changes occur about 1 year earlier in girls than in boys, likely due to the advanced puberty onset in females (Campbell et al., 2012). Sleep spindles also show decrease in amplitude but increase in frequency, density, and coherence during adolescence (Herrera and Tarokh, 2024). Recent surveys suggest that although sleep pattern varies cross-nationally, insufficient sleep is prevalent among adolescents world-wide especially on the school days, with the percentage of adolescents meeting sleep recommendations (8 h/day) (Hirshkowitz et al., 2015) ranging from 32 to 86% (Gariepy et al., 2020; Zhang et al., 2023). Transformations of lifestyle and physiology greatly shape sleep schedule during adolescence, and adolescents start to show a rapidly increasing risk of sleep problems. A natural shift in circadian rhythm when entering puberty leads to delayed bed time by ~2 h, while the sleep need is not reduced (Crowley et al., 2007). This may cause “delayed sleep phase syndrome” which is more common among adolescents than in adults (Gariepy et al., 2020; Wheaton and Claussen, 2021). As teenagers grow up, increasing social activities lead to “social jet lag” and irregular sleep schedule (Hasler et al., 2022). The relationship between sleep and mental health has long been postulated based on the high occurrence of sleep problems in psychiatric disorders (Ford and Kamerow, 1989). Recent findings further underscore a developmental link given the prevalence of sleep disturbances in neurodevelopmental disorders, such as autism and schizophrenia, and in neuropsychiatric disorders not typically considered a developmental disorder but having a high risk of disease onset during adolescence, such as depression and anxiety disorders; in many cases, sleep disturbances precede the progression of psychiatric illness and are thought to be a cause of mood dysregulation and behavioral abnormalities (Hertenstein et al., 2019).
Sleep disruptions in depression and anxiety in humans
Sleep disturbances have long been viewed as one of the core symptoms of depression (Ford and Kamerow, 1989; Jindal and Thase, 2004). In fact, doctors may not be confident to diagnose depression in the absence of sleep complaints. Common sleep disturbances linked to depression include insomnia (80%), hypersomnia (15%), and obstructive sleep apnea (20%) (Zhang et al., 2022). About 40% of insomnia patients are also clinically diagnosed with depression, suggesting a bidirectional association (Hertenstein et al., 2019; Zhang et al., 2022). Interestingly, patients with depression might oscillate between insomnia and hypersomnia within the same depressive episode, highlighting the complexity of this relationship. Like depression, patients with anxiety disorders frequently experience insomnia, including increased subjective sleep disturbance, reduced sleep continuity, and decreased total sleep time (Alfano et al., 2009).
In a study that sampled 87 adolescents (age 12–17) and 88 younger children (age 6–11) (Alfano et al., 2009), depressive symptoms were more strongly associated with sleep problems in adolescents, whereas anxiety appeared to be linked to sleep issues across all youth. The authors generated a single sleep problems factor that contained sleep-related items extracted from the Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL), Revised Child Anxiety and Depression Scales (RCADS) and Revised Children’s Manifest Anxiety Scale (RCMAS) and also consistent with other commonly used measures of childhood sleep (e.g., Children’s Sleep Habits Questionnaire), including overtiredness, less (or more) sleep than most children, trouble sleeping, and worries about going to bed at night. They found in both age groups, the sleep problems factor had a strong correlation with the anxiety measures, including generalized anxiety, panic and agoraphobia, and social anxiety in RCADS. However, for depression measures using the Child Depression Inventory (CDI), sleep problems showed a higher correlation coefficient in the adolescent group than in the children group. A large-sample survey in junior high/high school teenagers in Japan presented interesting sex differences in sleep habits and the associations with depression/anxiety that female adolescents generally sleep less than male adolescents (by ~20 min) and that their sleep duration with the lowest risk of depression/anxiety is also less (by 50–60 min) (Ojio et al., 2016). In addition, more insomnia, hypersomnia, or nightmares, shorter REM sleep latency and increased rapid eye movement activity were reported in suicidal psychiatric patients than the nonsuicidal ones, and suicide is perhaps the most devastating consequence of depression that significantly increases during adolescence (Liu and Buysse, 2006). Furthermore, longitudinal studies found that the association between sleep problems and depression/anxiety increased in adolescence as compared to that in early childhood and that a set of sleep variables at age 15, including total sleep time on school days, daytime sleepiness, and night waking, predicted anxiety and depression symptoms prospectively at later stages, either by clinic assessment or self-report questionnaires (Orchard et al., 2020; Gregory and O'Connor, 2002). Roberts and Duong showed that increased risk of major depression by reduced sleep may in turn increase the risk of further sleep decrease in a one-year follow-up study, indicating a reciprocal relationship (Roberts and Duong, 2014). In contrast, Wang et al. found that sleep problems in adolescence (age 14–17) can be retrospectively predicted by anxiety/depression symptoms in childhood (age 5), but not vice versa (Wang et al., 2016). These correlation studies demonstrate tight yet intricate associations between sleep disturbances and depression/anxiety in a developmental context (for a systematic review, see Zhang et al., 2022).
In experimental settings, acute or chronic sleep deprivation (SD) has been shown to cause immediate mood disruption, psychological distress and depressive- and anxiety-like symptoms both in adults (Talbot et al., 2010; Dinges et al., 1997; Babson et al., 2010) and in adolescents (Talbot et al., 2010; Dagys et al., 2012; Baum et al., 2014). In contrast, interestingly, SD was initially suggested as an alleviating method for depression by Johann Christian August Heinroth in 1818, who was considered the first university professor of psychiatry (Steinberg and Hegerl, 2014). This idea, named “SD therapy,” was supported by clinical reports and experimental trials subsequently at some level (Tse et al., 2024; Ioannou et al., 2021; Giedke and Schwarzler, 2002; Hemmeter et al., 2010). One-time total SD or repeated sleep restriction over days (a couple of hours per day) seemed effective either as add-ons to standard treatments for depression or as single-component therapy (Tse et al., 2024; Ioannou et al., 2021; Giedke and Schwarzler, 2002; Hemmeter et al., 2010). This discrepancy may be due to a difference in the subjects: the SD therapy was used in patients already diagnosed with depression or insomnia, or both, whereas SD with mood-disrupting consequences was mostly done in healthy subjects. However, the mechanisms underlying these opposite mood effects remain largely unknown and warrant more investigation. Recent mechanistic research in humans and mouse models added another layer of mystery to this paradoxical role of sleep in depression/anxiety (please see Sleep deprivation to alleviate depression/anxiety in the next section).
Sleep disruptions and depression- and anxiety-like behaviors in rodents
A role of sleep in regulating mood has emerged from studies in rodent models over the past decades. However, before we discuss the findings, special consideration must be given to the disruption methods, manipulation ages and potential confounding factors.
Disruption methods and duration
Up until now, multiple sleep disruption or deprivation methods have been developed. Perhaps one of the simplest methods is to expose animals to novel objects/environments or running wheels and utilize their voluntary exploration or exercise to keep them awake. This method is known to cause minimal stress (Kopp et al., 2006). However, mice can quickly become habituated to the stimulus, and thus the deprivation usually is no longer effective after the initial 1–2 h. Another manual deprivation method is by “gently touching” the animal when it displays signs of sleep. However, this protocol requires continuous monitoring of the mouse behavior and/or EEG, and the amount of “touches” required to keep wakefulness can increase substantially after the initial 1–2 h. Thus, this technique is labor-intensive and difficult to keep consistent across experimenters, making it challenging for chronic SD. Automated SD, on the other hand, has been widely adopted in long-term experiments. The setups typically involve forced motion or alertness to prolong wakefulness from hours to days, by using devices such as moving treadmills or rotating wheels (Colavito et al., 2013), small alternating platforms (Pierard et al., 2007), spinning disks (Rechtschaffen et al., 1983), or grid floors above water (Shinomiya et al., 2003). Recent studies used programmed shaking platform (Bian et al., 2022; Lord et al., 2022; Bian and De Lecea, 2023) or moving bars (Puech et al., 2023) showed effective deprivation for 4 h or more. Furthermore, ways to selectively deprive a sleep state (i.e., NREM or REM) are still limited. REM-specific deprivation (REM-D) has been reported, but the efficacy and specificity are not satisfactory. For example, REM-D using the platform-over-water method (Morden et al., 1967) in mice only resulted in ~65% reduction of REM sleep and a significant reduction of NREM sleep (Arthaud et al., 2015). NREM-specific disruption, on the other hand, is more challenging, as NREM reduction will normally be followed by REM depletion. NREM fragmentation without affecting REM sleep, however, has been achieved by optogenetic methods (Rolls et al., 2011).
Experimental age
In mice, adolescence roughly corresponds to between postnatal day (P) 23 and P60 (Brust et al., 2015). Sleep studies in developing mice has been facing the challenges of polysomnographic recording in young pups, due to their small and fragile skulls as well as the limited recovery time allowed between the surgery and desired recording age. Therefore, the amount of sleep deprived were often measured using behavioral criteria instead, which are less accurate and unable to identify sleep states or specific components. The rapid growth of developing skull also makes longitudinal recording difficult. Recently, researchers have managed to record EEG/EMG in post-weaning mice (Medina et al., 2022). However, aside from the tolerance issue, the isolation required by polysomnographic recording with cable transmission may cause potential problems, as juvenile social isolation has been found to cause significant changes in sociability (Yamamuro et al., 2020) and sleep patterns (Sotelo et al., 2024). Wireless polysomnography allowing sleep recording in the litter will potentially provide a solution to this dilemma, although further optimization toward lighter headset and longer recording time is needed. EEG/EMG in neonatal rats are much easier. Indeed, a lot of insights regarding sleep’s role in early development came from rat studies (Frank, 2020). Nonetheless, the isolation problem remains, and the lack of genetic tools has limited mechanistic interrogations in rats.
Stress as a confounding factor
SD has long been recognized to induce stress, which is signified by the elevated stress hormones in the blood (e.g., cortisol in humans, corticosterone in rodents) and can dramatically alter emotion, behavior, or sleep itself (Nollet et al., 2020). The stress brought about by SD comes from two major sources. One is the physical stimulation applied to keep the animal awake in the experimental apparatus, e.g., platform shaking or bar rotating, and can be controlled through proper experimental design. The other source is the loss of sleep per se. Prolonged sleep loss causes significant corticosterone elevation in rats and eventually leads to pre-mature death even when the physical stimulation was controlled (Rechtschaffen et al., 1983), suggesting that sleep alone plays an essential role in maintaining physiological integrity. In the context of development, if sleep disruption contributes to depression and anxiety, is this effect merely a consequence of compromised physiology, or mediated by distinct mechanisms? To answer this question, it is vital to dissect the contribution of sleep in a setting without systematic stress response, which is challenging and often omitted by previous studies. It should be noted that SD is not necessarily accompanied by a detectable stress response. Whether a SD protocol is stressful or not largely depends on the disruption method and duration as well as the manipulation age (Nollet et al., 2020). SD methods like exposing to novel objects (Kopp et al., 2006) and gentle handling (Vecsey et al., 2013; Hagewoud et al., 2010) caused no increase in plasma corticosterone level, or only a mild increase to which the animal can quickly habituate, and therefore are not considered significant stressors. Carefully designed setups using running wheels, shaking/rotating platforms, or closed-loop disruptions can also achieve sleep loss without inducing significant corticosterone increase (Bian et al., 2022; Lord et al., 2022; Bian and De Lecea, 2023; Nollet et al., 2020). In addition, setting up proper stress control [e.g., restrain stress (Bian et al., 2022) or direct injection of corticosterone (Yang et al., 2014)] and manipulating individual sleep components without changing the overall sleep amount also help to exclude the confounding effects from the behavioral results (Bian et al., 2022). Furthermore, the long-lasting feature of depression and anxiety as a mood suggests that the underlying neurobiological changes should be persistent. Therefore, after SD manipulation is removed but before behavioral assessment, allowing sleep to recover (or not) may be a good strategy to exclude the immediate stress influence and examine the long-term effects of SD. Notably, the magnitude of corticosterone increase induced by SD is more severe in neonatal rats than in adolescents despite a developmental increase in baseline corticosterone level, suggesting developmental endurance of SD stress (Hairston et al., 2001).
Depression- and anxiety-like behavior caused by adolescent SD
To assess anxiety-like behavior, open field (OFT), elevated plus maze (EPM), light/dark chamber (LD) tests are commonly used (Crawley, 2007). These tests compare the time that mice spent in the “low-danger” area (edges in OFT, closed arms in EPM, or dark chambers in LD) over the “high-danger” area (center area in OFT, open arms in EPM, or light chambers in LD) as an index of anxiety level. To assay different aspects of depression, multiple behavioral tests have been developed. Increased immobility time in the forced swimming test (FST) is considered a sign of despair in rodents, whereas decreased preference for sucrose over water in the sucrose preference drinking test (SPT) reflects anhedonia (Crawley, 2007). In addition, sucrose negative contrast test (SNCT) measures the response change to reward downshift, and the depressed animals may exhibit hypo- or hypersensitivity to the shifting stimuli (Matthews et al., 1996). Self-grooming in the sucrose splash test (SST) is considered to reflect self-care motivation, the impairment of which indicates depressive state (Lefter et al., 2020).
As summarized in Table 1, behavioral changes reflecting a depressive and anxious mood have been reported following SD at different developmental stages. Early adolescent SD in rats using rotating bars (6–8 h/day, P19–P32) were reported to increase anxiety, as measured by EPM and LD, and decrease motivation for self-care and increase despair as measured by SST and FST, respectively (Atrooz et al., 2019; Atrooz et al., 2022). These behavioral changes were likely long-lasting as some of them were observed at later timepoints approximately 1 or 2 months after SD. However, in the study the corticosterone level was not examined, therefore the confounding stress cannot be excluded. In another rat study, 18 h/day REM-D for a longer period in adolescence (P21–P42) caused similar reduction in open-arm time and entries in EPM at P49, without changing the reward response downshift in SNCT (da Silva et al., 2018), but the corticosterone level was also increased by REM-D. Deprivation of either total or REM sleep at an earlier childhood stage (P14–21) in mice, rats or prairie voles returned negative results on anxiety-like behavior (Lord et al., 2022; Feng and Ma, 2003; Jones et al., 2020), with the mouse and vole studies showing no significant stress, suggesting the anxiety-inducing effect may be specific to adolescent SD. In mice, long-term, mild SD from early neonatal stage all the way to mid/late-adolescence (3 h/day by gentle handling, P5–P42/52) did not cause an increase in corticosterone level, but decreased locomotion, which might be considered a sign of depression, although the anxiety level in EPM seemed unchanged (Sare et al., 2016; Sare et al., 2019). Depressive-like behaviors, on the other hand, were induced by only 8 days of SD of similar intensity by non-stressful gentle-handling method in mice during P42–49, including longer immobility time and shortened immobility latency in FST, although stress was not excluded in this study (Murack et al., 2021). These behavioral changes did not occur when the same SD protocol was performed in adult animals, indicating a development-specific impact (Murack et al., 2021). Notably, when rats were subjected to early adolescent SD (P19–32), more anxiety-like behavior were displayed immediately after SD (P33) and at young adult stage (P60), but not at the later stage (P90), whereas depression-like behavior was more dramatic at P90, but not at earlier stages (Atrooz et al., 2019; Atrooz et al., 2022). This temporal dissociation of anxiety- and depression-like states indicate in-parallel mechanisms caused by adolescent SD or differential susceptibilities of the symptoms. Alternatively, there may be a mechanistic link between the two sets of behavioral abnormalities that sustained anxiety, caused by SD, leads to depression.
Adolescent SD-induced social deficits and their relationship with depression and anxiety
Social withdraw and difficulties in social communication are a core symptom shared by many neuropsychiatric disorders, including depression, anxiety disorders, autism and schizophrenia. Interestingly in several studies above mentioned, there was a striking social phenotype. Feng and Ma showed that REM-D in pre-weaning male rats (P14–21) decreased their mounting latency when facing female conspecifics in adulthood, but intromission and ejaculation were not affected (Feng and Ma, 2003). However, maternal separation alone produced similar deficits in the same study, making it less clear how much sleep loss contributes to this defect. These REM-deprived rats also displayed decreased aggression in shock-induced fighting (Feng and Ma, 2003). In voles, early-life SD led to decreased social bonding (Jones et al., 2019). Saré et al. performed daily gentle handling SD (3 h/day) in mice during P5–42 and found that it led to slight but significant increase in sociability immediately after SD and in social novelty preference 4 weeks later (Sare et al., 2016; Sare et al., 2019). However, when SD was prolonged to P52 (Sare et al., 2019), or performed only during middle (Bian et al., 2022) or late adolescence (Lord et al., 2022), it seemed only to impair social novelty preference. Notably, this effect is not likely due to stress or juvenile social isolation as these confounding factors were carefully avoided or controlled in one of these studies (Bian et al., 2022). Interestingly, although loss of social interest is often viewed as a sign of depression in human patients, adolescent SD-induced lack of social novelty preference was not likely due to depression, based on the following observations (Bian et al., 2022). Firstly, the overall sociability was not changed in SD mice. Secondly, the level of rewarding dopaminergic activity, including the activation of dopaminergic neurons in the ventral tegmental area (VTA) and the dopamine release in the nucleus accumbens (NAc), was not reduced; it was the timing of these dopaminergic activities in relation to social interaction events that was impaired in SD mice. Thirdly, SD mice exhibited normal craving for food and sucrose, and their VTA response to these non-social rewards was not significantly changed, suggesting the absence of general anhedonia. Finally, EPM results suggest SD mice had normal anxiety level compared to the control group. Therefore, the impact of adolescent sleep on mood and behavior is multi-layered, and detailed dissection of intricate effects and mechanisms of SD is warranted.
Sleep deprivation to alleviate depression/anxiety
In contrast to the perspective that long-term sleep disruption leads to heightened risk of depression and anxiety, transient SD has been shown clinically a rapid and efficient way to alleviate depression with a success rate of 60–70%, better than any existing anti-depressant drugs (Tse et al., 2024; Boland et al., 2017). SD increases the homeostatic sleep drive and thus, supposedly leads to a more consolidated recovery sleep afterwards. However, this does not likely play a role here because the mood response occurs, if it does, within hours after SD treatment begins, and in fact, more than 80% of the responders show a relapse into depression after the recovery sleep (Hemmeter et al., 2010). Human fMRI revealed that one night of total SD enhanced connectivity between the amygdala and anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), which is associated with a better mood and impaired in depressed patients, mimicking anti-depressant effects (Chai et al., 2023). Research in mice showed that 12 h SD was sufficient to significantly reduce the immobility time in FST, as well as that in a tail suspension test (TST), another assay for depression, and that activation of astrocyte adenosine signaling mediated this SD effect (Hines et al., 2013). Enhanced level of mood-boosting neuromodulators, such as dopamine and serotonin, has been suggested to account for SD’s anti-depressant effect (Wu et al., 2024; Bjorvatn et al., 2002), but more complex changes are likely at the receptor level (Tuan et al., 2023; Vaseghi et al., 2023), making it still far from conclusive whether SD promotes or impairs dopaminergic and serotonergic neurotransmission. Interestingly, 6 h SD by gentle handling was shown to inhibit neuroinflammation and improving neuroplasticity in the ACC (Shi et al., 2023) and 12 h SD by a rotating bar enhanced dopamine signaling in the mesocorticolimbic but not nigrostriatal pathway (Wu et al., 2024) (but corticosterone level was not examined in this study), whereas a stressful 72-h SD protocol resulted in systematic dopaminergic maladaptation and enhanced neuroinflammation in the brain (Tuan et al., 2023). These findings offer mechanistic insights into the different impacts of acute versus chronic SD in the brain, especially in the structures heavily implicated in depression/anxiety, such as the limbic system. Yet the evidence so far is still limited, and many questions remain unanswered. How does the acute changes in the affected brain regions lead to more systematic deficits when SD is prolonged? How does the brain process the accumulating sleep pressure (and the stress that comes with it) and the mood boost? Can SD truly enhance mood, or is it merely a rapid state transition with no lasting benefits or even detrimental outcomes later? These unresolved questions necessitate further investigation, and the answers may in turn help understanding the long-term effects of SD in adolescence.
Conclusion and future directions
The prevalence of sleep disruptions among adolescents, particularly those with neuropsychiatric conditions, is a global concern. While we have begun to understand the acute and chronic effects of sleep disruptions on mood, emotion, and behavior, much remains unknown about the long-term impact of these disruptions on the developing brain, both under physiological and pathological conditions. Key questions remain unanswered regarding to what extent sleep disruption contributes to a specific symptom in a particular disorder, whether sleep disruption is more “disruptive” during development than in adulthood, whether the impact is long-lasting and/or reversible, and what sleep components, brain regions and cell types play major roles. Future research should aim to address these gaps in our understanding. SD experiments in adult rodents have revealed global changes in multiple neural or non-neural pathways that may serve as the underlying mechanisms. However, due to challenges in implementing circuit-specific measurement and manipulation methods in developing animals, our understanding of the intricate role of sleep in development is still in its early stages. Our recent work utilizing viral tracing, fiber photometry and optogenetic tools demonstrates that SD during a critical, mid-adolescent period impairs social behavior by overexciting the developing VTA dopaminergic circuits and disrupting their developmental wiring (Bian et al., 2022). We further identified NREM slow waves to be a major contributing sleep component (Bian et al., 2022). Recent development of non-invasive, ultrasound stimulation techniques further offers great potentials for circuit- or neuronal type-specific interrogations during adolescence (Murphy et al., 2024; Murphy et al., 2022). There is also a need for more comprehensive and systematic understanding based on the dissection of contributions of sleep components (SWA, spindles, etc.) and neural circuits in depression and anxiety, as well as novel tools to facilitate this work. The answers will help achieve a better understanding of the relationship between adolescent sleep and mental health, and ultimately, develop more effective intervention methods for adolescents suffering from sleep disruptions and neuropsychiatric disorders.
Author contributions
RC: Conceptualization, Writing – original draft. W-JB: Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Supervision, Writing – review & editing.
Funding
The author(s) declare that financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. This work was supported by the Westlake Laboratory of Life Sciences and Biomedicine and Westlake University startup funding (W.-J.B.) and Key R&D Program of Zhejiang (2024SSYS0031).
Conflict of interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Publisher’s note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
References
Alfano, C. A., Zakem, A. H., Costa, N. M., Taylor, L. K., and Weems, C. F. (2009). Sleep problems and their relation to cognitive factors, anxiety, and depressive symptoms in children and adolescents. Depress. Anxiety 26, 503–512. doi: 10.1002/da.20443
Arthaud, S., Varin, C., Gay, N., Libourel, P. A., Chauveau, F., Fort, P., et al. (2015). Paradoxical (REM) sleep deprivation in mice using the small-platforms-over-water method: polysomnographic analyses and melanin-concentrating hormone and hypocretin/orexin neuronal activation before, during and after deprivation. J. Sleep Res. 24, 309–319. doi: 10.1111/jsr.12269
Atrooz, F., Alrousan, G., Hassan, A., and Salim, S. (2022). Early-life sleep deprivation enhanced alcohol consumption in adolescent rats. Front. Neurosci. 16:856120. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2022.856120
Atrooz, F., Liu, H., Kochi, C., and Salim, S. (2019). Early life sleep deprivation: role of Oxido-inflammatory processes. Neuroscience 406, 22–37. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2019.02.021
Babson, K. A., Trainor, C. D., Feldner, M. T., and Blumenthal, H. (2010). A test of the effects of acute sleep deprivation on general and specific self-reported anxiety and depressive symptoms: an experimental extension. J. Behav. Ther. Exp. Psychiatry 41, 297–303. doi: 10.1016/j.jbtep.2010.02.008
Baum, K. T., Desai, A., Field, J., Miller, L. E., Rausch, J., and Beebe, D. W. (2014). Sleep restriction worsens mood and emotion regulation in adolescents. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 55, 180–190. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12125
Bian, W. J., Brewer, C. L., Kauer, J. A., and de Lecea, L. (2022). Adolescent sleep shapes social novelty preference in mice. Nat. Neurosci. 25, 912–923. doi: 10.1038/s41593-022-01076-8
Bian, W. J., and De Lecea, L. (2023). Automated sleep deprivation setup using a shaking platform in mice. Bio Protoc. 13:e4620. doi: 10.21769/BioProtoc.4620
Bjorvatn, B., Gronli, J., Hamre, F., Sorensen, E., Fiske, E., Bjorkum, A. A., et al. (2002). Effects of sleep deprivation on extracellular serotonin in hippocampus and frontal cortex of the rat. Neuroscience 113, 323–330. doi: 10.1016/S0306-4522(02)00181-1
Boland, E. M., Rao, H., Dinges, D. F., Smith, R. V., Goel, N., Detre, J. A., et al. (2017). Meta-analysis of the antidepressant effects of acute sleep deprivation. J. Clin. Psychiatry 78, e1020–e1034. doi: 10.4088/JCP.16r11332
Brust, V., Schindler, P. M., and Lewejohann, L. (2015). Lifetime development of behavioural phenotype in the house mouse (Mus musculus). Front. Zool. 12:17. doi: 10.1186/1742-9994-12-S1-S17
Campbell, I. G., and Feinberg, I. (2009). Longitudinal trajectories of non-rapid eye movement delta and theta EEG as indicators of adolescent brain maturation. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 106, 5177–5180. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0812947106
Campbell, I. G., Grimm, K. J., de Bie, E., and Feinberg, I. (2012). Sex, puberty, and the timing of sleep EEG measured adolescent brain maturation. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 109, 5740–5743. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1120860109
Chai, Y., Gehrman, P., Yu, M., Mao, T., Deng, Y., Rao, J., et al. (2023). Enhanced amygdala-cingulate connectivity associates with better mood in both healthy and depressive individuals after sleep deprivation. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 120:e2214505120. doi: 10.1073/pnas.2214505120
Colavito, V., Fabene, P. F., Grassi-Zucconi, G., Pifferi, F., Lamberty, Y., Bentivoglio, M., et al. (2013). Experimental sleep deprivation as a tool to test memory deficits in rodents. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 7:106. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2013.00106
Crawley, J. N. (2007). “Emotional behaviors: Animal models of psychiatric diseases” in What's wrong with my mouse?: Behavioral phenotyping of transgenic and knockout mice. 2nd ed (Hoboken, NJ, USA: John Wiley & Sons, Inc.), 226–265.
Crowley, S. J., Acebo, C., and Carskadon, M. A. (2007). Sleep, circadian rhythms, and delayed phase in adolescence. Sleep Med. 8, 602–612. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2006.12.002
da Silva, R.-L. J., Machado, R. B., and Suchecki, D. (2018). Chronic REM sleep restriction in juvenile male rats induces anxiety-like behavior and alters monoamine Systems in the Amygdala and Hippocampus. Mol. Neurobiol. 55, 2884–2896. doi: 10.1007/s12035-017-0541-3
Dagys, N., McGlinchey, E. L., Talbot, L. S., Kaplan, K. A., Dahl, R. E., and Harvey, A. G. (2012). Double trouble? The effects of sleep deprivation and chronotype on adolescent affect. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 53, 660–667. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2011.02502.x
Davidson, L. L., Grigorenko, E. L., Boivin, M. J., Rapa, E., and Stein, A. (2015). A focus on adolescence to reduce neurological, mental health and substance-use disability. Nature 527, S161–S166. doi: 10.1038/nature16030
Dinges, D. F., Pack, F., Williams, K., Gillen, K. A., Powell, J. W., Ott, G. E., et al. (1997). Cumulative sleepiness, mood disturbance, and psychomotor vigilance performance decrements during a week of sleep restricted to 4-5 hours per night. Sleep 20, 267–277
Feng, P., and Ma, Y. (2003). Instrumental REM sleep deprivation in neonates leads to adult depression-like behaviors in rats. Sleep 26, 990–996. doi: 10.1093/sleep/26.8.990
Ford, D. E., and Kamerow, D. B. (1989). Epidemiologic study of sleep disturbances and psychiatric disorders. An opportunity for prevention? JAMA 262, 1479–1484. doi: 10.1001/jama.1989.03430110069030
Frank, M. G. (2020). The ontogenesis of mammalian sleep: form and function. Curr. Sleep Med. Rep. 6, 267–279. doi: 10.1007/s40675-020-00190-y
Gariepy, G., Danna, S., Gobina, I., Rasmussen, M., Gaspar de Matos, M., Tynjala, J., et al. (2020). How are adolescents sleeping? Adolescent sleep patterns and sociodemographic differences in 24 European and north American countries. J. Adolesc. Health 66, S81–S88. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2020.03.013
Giedke, H., and Schwarzler, F. (2002). Therapeutic use of sleep deprivation in depression. Sleep Med. Rev. 6, 361–377. doi: 10.1053/smrv.2002.0235
Gregory, A. M., and O'Connor, T. G. (2002). Sleep problems in childhood: a longitudinal study of developmental change and association with behavioral problems. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 41, 964–971. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200208000-00015
Hagewoud, R., Havekes, R., Novati, A., Keijser, J. N., Van der Zee, E. A., and Meerlo, P. (2010). Sleep deprivation impairs spatial working memory and reduces hippocampal AMPA receptor phosphorylation. J. Sleep Res. 19, 280–288. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2869.2009.00799.x
Hairston, I. S., Ruby, N. F., Brooke, S., Peyron, C., Denning, D. P., Heller, H. C., et al. (2001). Sleep deprivation elevates plasma corticosterone levels in neonatal rats. Neurosci. Lett. 315, 29–32. doi: 10.1016/S0304-3940(01)02309-6
Hasler, B. P., Graves, J. L., Soehner, A. M., Wallace, M. L., and Clark, D. B. (2022). Preliminary evidence that circadian alignment predicts neural response to monetary reward in late adolescent drinkers. Front. Neurosci. 16:803349. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2022.803349
Hemmeter, U. M., Hemmeter-Spernal, J., and Krieg, J. C. (2010). Sleep deprivation in depression. Expert. Rev. Neurother. 10, 1101–1115. doi: 10.1586/ern.10.83
Herrera, C. G., and Tarokh, L. (2024). A Thalamocortical perspective on sleep spindle alterations in neurodevelopmental disorders. Curr. Sleep Med. Rep. 10, 103–118. doi: 10.1007/s40675-024-00284-x
Hertenstein, E., Feige, B., Gmeiner, T., Kienzler, C., Spiegelhalder, K., Johann, A., et al. (2019). Insomnia as a predictor of mental disorders: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Sleep Med. Rev. 43, 96–105. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2018.10.006
Hines, D. J., Schmitt, L. I., Hines, R. M., Moss, S. J., and Haydon, P. G. (2013). Antidepressant effects of sleep deprivation require astrocyte-dependent adenosine mediated signaling. Transl. Psychiatry 3:e212. doi: 10.1038/tp.2012.136
Hirshkowitz, M., Whiton, K., Albert, S. M., Alessi, C., Bruni, O., DonCarlos, L., et al. (2015). National Sleep Foundation's sleep time duration recommendations: methodology and results summary. Sleep Health 1, 40–43. doi: 10.1016/j.sleh.2014.12.010
Ioannou, M., Wartenberg, C., Greenbrook, J. T. V., Larson, T., Magnusson, K., Schmitz, L., et al. (2021). Sleep deprivation as treatment for depression: systematic review and meta-analysis. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 143, 22–35. doi: 10.1111/acps.13253
Jindal, R. D., and Thase, M. E. (2004). Treatment of insomnia associated with clinical depression. Sleep Med. Rev. 8, 19–30. doi: 10.1016/S1087-0792(03)00025-X
Jones, C. E., Opel, R. A., Kaiser, M. E., Chau, A. Q., Quintana, J. R., Nipper, M. A., et al. (2019). Early-life sleep disruption increases parvalbumin in primary somatosensory cortex and impairs social bonding in prairie voles. Sci. Adv. 5:eaav5188. doi: 10.1126/sciadv.aav5188
Jones, C. E., Wickham, P. T., and Lim, M. M. (2020). Early life sleep disruption is a risk factor for increased ethanol drinking after acute footshock stress in prairie voles. Behav. Neurosci. 134, 424–434. doi: 10.1037/bne0000410
Kolk, S. M., and Rakic, P. (2022). Development of prefrontal cortex. Neuropsychopharmacology 47, 41–57. doi: 10.1038/s41386-021-01137-9
Kopp, C., Longordo, F., Nicholson, J. R., and Luthi, A. (2006). Insufficient sleep reversibly alters bidirectional synaptic plasticity and NMDA receptor function. J. Neurosci. 26, 12456–12465. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2702-06.2006
Lefter, R., Ciobica, A., Timofte, D., Ababei, D., Dobrin, R., Luca, A., et al. (2020). A new biological approach in generating an irritable bowel syndrome rat model - focusing on depression in sucrose splash test and body weight change. Rom. Biotech. Lett. 25, 1554–1562. doi: 10.25083/rbl/25.3/1554.1562
Liu, X., and Buysse, D. J. (2006). Sleep and youth suicidal behavior: a neglected field. Curr. Opin. Psychiatry 19, 288–293. doi: 10.1097/01.yco.0000218600.40593.18
Lord, J. S., Gay, S. M., Harper, K. M., Nikolova, V. D., Smith, K. M., Moy, S. S., et al. (2022). Early life sleep disruption potentiates lasting sex-specific changes in behavior in genetically vulnerable Shank3 heterozygous autism model mice. Mol. Autism. 13:35. doi: 10.1186/s13229-022-00514-5
Matthews, K., Wilkinson, L. S., and Robbins, T. W. (1996). Repeated maternal separation of preweanling rats attenuates behavioral responses to primary and conditioned incentives in adulthood. Physiol. Behav. 59, 99–107. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(95)02069-1
Medina, E., Schoch, H., Ford, K., Wintler, T., Singletary, K. G., and Peixoto, L. (2022). Shank3 influences mammalian sleep development. J. Neurosci. Res. 100, 2174–2186. doi: 10.1002/jnr.25119
Morden, B., Mitchell, G., and Dement, W. (1967). Selective REM sleep deprivation and compensation phenomena in the rat. Brain Res. 5, 339–349. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(67)90042-X
Moyer, C. E., and Zuo, Y. (2018). Cortical dendritic spine development and plasticity: insights from in vivo imaging. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 53, 76–82. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2018.06.002
Murack, M., Chandrasegaram, R., Smith, K. B., Ah-Yen, E. G., Rheaume, E., Malette-Guyon, E., et al. (2021). Chronic sleep disruption induces depression-like behavior in adolescent male and female mice and sensitization of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis in adolescent female mice. Behav. Brain Res. 399:113001. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2020.113001
Murphy, K. R., Farrell, J. S., Bendig, J., Mitra, A., Luff, C., Stelzer, I. A., et al. (2024). Optimized ultrasound neuromodulation for non-invasive control of behavior and physiology. Neuron. 112, 3252–3266. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2024.07.002
Murphy, K. R., Farrell, J. S., Gomez, J. L., Stedman, Q. G., Li, N., Leung, S. A., et al. (2022). A tool for monitoring cell type-specific focused ultrasound neuromodulation and control of chronic epilepsy. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 119:e2206828119. doi: 10.1073/pnas.2206828119
Nollet, M., Wisden, W., and Franks, N. P. (2020). Sleep deprivation and stress: a reciprocal relationship. Interface Focus 10:20190092. doi: 10.1098/rsfs.2019.0092
Ojio, Y., Nishida, A., Shimodera, S., Togo, F., and Sasaki, T. (2016). Sleep duration associated with the lowest risk of depression/anxiety in adolescents. Sleep 39, 1555–1562. doi: 10.5665/sleep.6020
Orchard, F., Gregory, A. M., Gradisar, M., and Reynolds, S. (2020). Self-reported sleep patterns and quality amongst adolescents: cross-sectional and prospective associations with anxiety and depression. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 61, 1126–1137. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.13288
Pierard, C., Liscia, P., Philippin, J. N., Mons, N., Lafon, T., Chauveau, F., et al. (2007). Modafinil restores memory performance and neural activity impaired by sleep deprivation in mice. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 88, 55–63. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2007.07.006
Puech, C., Badran, M., Barrow, M. B., Runion, A. R., and Gozal, D. (2023). Solriamfetol improves chronic sleep fragmentation-induced increases in sleep propensity and ameliorates explicit memory in male mice. Sleep 46:zsad057. doi: 10.1093/sleep/zsad057
Rechtschaffen, A., Gilliland, M. A., Bergmann, B. M., and Winter, J. B. (1983). Physiological correlates of prolonged sleep deprivation in rats. Science 221, 182–184. doi: 10.1126/science.6857280
Roberts, R. E., and Duong, H. T. (2014). The prospective association between sleep deprivation and depression among adolescents. Sleep 37, 239–244. doi: 10.5665/sleep.3388
Roffwarg, H. P., Muzio, J. N., and Dement, W. C. (1966). Ontogenetic development of the human sleep-dream cycle. Science 152, 604–619. doi: 10.1126/science.152.3722.604
Rolls, A., Colas, D., Adamantidis, A., Carter, M., Lanre-Amos, T., Heller, H. C., et al. (2011). Optogenetic disruption of sleep continuity impairs memory consolidation. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 108, 13305–13310. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1015633108
Sare, R. M., Levine, M., Hildreth, C., Picchioni, D., and Smith, C. B. (2016). Chronic sleep restriction during development can lead to long-lasting behavioral effects. Physiol. Behav. 155, 208–217. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2015.12.019
Sare, R. M., Song, A., Levine, M., Lemons, A., Loutaev, I., Sheeler, C., et al. (2019). Chronic sleep restriction in developing male mice results in long lasting behavior impairments. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 13:90. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2019.00090
Shi, S., Zhang, M., Xie, W., Ju, P., Chen, N., Wang, F., et al. (2023). Sleep deprivation alleviates depression-like behaviors in mice via inhibiting immune and inflammatory pathways and improving neuroplasticity. J. Affect. Disord. 340, 100–112. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2023.07.119
Shinomiya, K., Shigemoto, Y., Okuma, C., Mio, M., and Kamei, C. (2003). Effects of short-acting hypnotics on sleep latency in rats placed on grid suspended over water. Eur. J. Pharmacol. 460, 139–144. doi: 10.1016/S0014-2999(02)02915-1
Solmi, M., Radua, J., Olivola, M., Croce, E., Soardo, L., Salazar de Pablo, G., et al. (2022). Age at onset of mental disorders worldwide: large-scale meta-analysis of 192 epidemiological studies. Mol. Psychiatry 27, 281–295. doi: 10.1038/s41380-021-01161-7
Sotelo, M. I., Markunas, C., Kudlak, T., Kohtz, C., Vyssotski, A. L., Rothschild, G., et al. (2024). Neurophysiological and behavioral synchronization in group-living and sleeping mice. Curr. Biol. 34, 132–146.e5. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2023.11.065
Steinberg, H., and Hegerl, U. (2014). Johann Christian august Heinroth on sleep deprivation as a therapeutic option for depressive disorders. Sleep Med. 15, 1159–1164. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2014.03.027
Talbot, L. S., McGlinchey, E. L., Kaplan, K. A., Dahl, R. E., and Harvey, A. G. (2010). Sleep deprivation in adolescents and adults: changes in affect. Emotion 10, 831–841. doi: 10.1037/a0020138
Tse, K. Y. K., Maurer, L. F., Espie, C. A., and Kyle, S. D. (2024). The effect of single-component sleep restriction therapy on depressive symptoms: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J. Sleep Res. :e14180. doi: 10.1111/jsr.14180
Tuan, L. H., Yeh, J. W., Lee, L. J. H., and Lee, L. J. (2023). Sleep deprivation induces dopamine system maladaptation and escalated corticotrophin-releasing factor signaling in adolescent mice. Mol. Neurobiol. 60, 3190–3209. doi: 10.1007/s12035-023-03258-2
Vaseghi, S., Mostafavijabbari, A., Alizadeh, M. S., Ghaffarzadegan, R., Kholghi, G., and Zarrindast, M. R. (2023). Intricate role of sleep deprivation in modulating depression: focusing on BDNF, VEGF, serotonin, cortisol, and TNF-α. Metab. Brain Dis. 38, 195–219. doi: 10.1007/s11011-022-01124-z
Vecsey, C. G., Wimmer, M. E., Havekes, R., Park, A. J., Perron, I. J., Meerlo, P., et al. (2013). Daily acclimation handling does not affect hippocampal long-term potentiation or cause chronic sleep deprivation in mice. Sleep 36, 601–607. doi: 10.5665/sleep.2556
Wang, B., Isensee, C., Becker, A., Wong, J., Eastwood, P. R., Huang, R. C., et al. (2016). Developmental trajectories of sleep problems from childhood to adolescence both predict and are predicted by emotional and behavioral problems. Front. Psychol. 7:1874. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01874
Wheaton, A. G., and Claussen, A. H. (2021). Short sleep duration among infants, children, and adolescents aged 4 Months-17 years - United States, 2016-2018. MMWR Morb. Mortal Wkly. Rep. 70, 1315–1321. doi: 10.15585/mmwr.mm7038a1
Wu, M., Zhang, X., Feng, S., Freda, S. N., Kumari, P., Dumrongprechachan, V., et al. (2024). Dopamine pathways mediating affective state transitions after sleep loss. Neuron 112, 141–154.e8. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2023.10.002
Yamamuro, K., Bicks, L. K., Leventhal, M. B., Kato, D., Im, S., Flanigan, M. E., et al. (2020). A prefrontal-paraventricular thalamus circuit requires juvenile social experience to regulate adult sociability in mice. Nat. Neurosci. 23, 1240–1252. doi: 10.1038/s41593-020-0695-6
Yang, G., Lai, C. S., Cichon, J., Ma, L., Li, W., and Gan, W. B. (2014). Sleep promotes branch-specific formation of dendritic spines after learning. Science 344, 1173–1178. doi: 10.1126/science.1249098
Zhang, Y., Huang, Z., Zhang, M., Li, C., Zhao, Z., Zhang, X., et al. (2023). Sleep status among children and adolescents aged 6-17 years - China, 2016-2017. China CDC Wkly 5, 11–16. doi: 10.46234/ccdcw2023.003
Keywords: adolescent sleep, sleep disruption, depression, anxiety, neural development
Citation: Chai R and Bian W-J (2024) Adolescent sleep and its disruption in depression and anxiety. Front. Neurosci. 18:1479420. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2024.1479420
Edited by:
Chelsea Vadnie, Ohio Wesleyan University, United StatesReviewed by:
Yanhua Huang, University of Pittsburgh, United StatesCopyright © 2024 Chai and Bian. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Wen-Jie Bian, YmlhbndlbmppZUB3ZXN0bGFrZS5lZHUuY24=