Skip to main content

MINI REVIEW article

Front. Neurosci., 09 August 2024
Sec. Perception Science
This article is part of the Research Topic Processing of Face and Other Animacy Cues in the Brain View all 4 articles

Face processing in animal models: implications for autism spectrum disorder

  • 1Center for Mind/Brain Sciences, University of Trento, Rovereto, Italy
  • 2Center for Behavioral Neuroscience and Communication (BNC), Vita-Salute San Raffaele University, Milan, Italy
  • 3Faculty of Psychology, Vita-Salute San Raffaele University, Milan, Italy

Processing facial features is crucial to identify social partners (prey, predators, or conspecifics) and recognize and accurately interpret emotional expressions. Numerous studies in both human and non-human primates provided evidence promoting the notion of inherent mechanisms for detecting facial features. These mechanisms support a representation of faces independent of prior experiences and are vital for subsequent development in social and language domains. Moreover, deficits in processing faces are a reliable biomarker of autism spectrum disorder, appearing early and correlating with symptom severity. Face processing, however, is not only a prerogative of humans: other species also show remarkable face detection abilities. In this review, we present an overview of the current literature on face detection in vertebrate models that could be relevant to the study of autism.

Introduction

Faces convey a great amount of socially relevant information related to emotional and mental states, identity, and intention. Processing facial information is crucial for social and cognitive development (Wagner et al., 2013), so much so that newborns within an hour of birth are already biased to orient to faces and prefer them over any other stimulus (Johnson et al., 1991; Valenza et al., 1996). This early preference is even observed towards schematic geometric (face-like) patterns resembling a face (three black squares organized as an upside-down triangle; Goren et al., 1975; Johnson et al., 1991) and might already be present during the last gestational trimester (Reid et al., 2017). Although still a matter of debate, evidence suggests the presence of a specialized cognitive system present since the first moments of life. Some authors have proposed a two-process model supporting face processing to explain newborns’ preference for real and schematic faces (Johnson et al., 1991; Morton and Johnson, 1991). According to this model, the inherent predisposition of newborns to visually attend to faces and schematic face-like stimuli is guided by a rudimental representation (CONSPEC; see Morton and Johnson, 1991), which can drive their preference for simple, geometrical characteristics of these stimuli (oval bounded area, top-heavy featural pattern, and positive contrast; see Johnson, 2005). Neuropsychological studies indicating residual face processing abilities in patients with visual cortical impairments and electrophysiological studies suggest the involvement of a visual system supporting fast and coarse (based on low spatial frequencies) facial detection localized in subcortical areas such as the superior colliculus, the pulvinar, and the amygdala (Johnson, 2005). As development proceeds, the predisposition is complemented by a second adaptive mechanism (termed CONLEARN; see Morton and Johnson, 1991) relying on preferential exposure to faces to refine and adapt the cortical representations of faces and to ascribe identity. Other authors have proposed alternative hypotheses to explain the early preference responses to face and face-like stimuli based on intrinsic visual properties of the stimuli, such as the stimulus energy (Kleiner, 1987) or the structural properties of the stimuli (for example the presence of a “top-heavy” configuration, Turati et al., 2002; Simion and Di Giorgio, 2015) that may drive the preference for faces in human newborns without being related to “facedness.” Moreover, a recent study investigating face processing in newborns challenges the hypothesis that the preference for face-like stimuli is mainly mediated by subcortical structures, demonstrating a clear activation of cortical structures upon exposure to upright face-like compared to inverted or scrambled stimuli (Buiatti et al., 2019).

Several studies indicate that the visual processing of facial information is significantly compromised in neurodevelopmental disorders (Collin et al., 2013). Schizophrenia patients show deficits in face detection (Chen et al., 2008; Romagnano et al., 2022), face identity (Russell et al., 2024), and facial emotion recognition (see for a review Bortolon et al., 2015). Moreover, disruptions in face processing are one of the earliest indicators of social deficits in autism spectrum disorders (ASD) and play a key role in the pathophysiology of these disorders (Johnson, 2014; Johnson et al., 2015; Pavlova et al., 2017). Infants at risk of ASD often exhibit impairments in processing face and eye-gaze direction, further emphasizing the significance of these social orienting mechanisms as early biomarkers for ASD.

However, processing facial information is not just a prerogative of humans; several other animal species possess remarkable face recognition abilities.

Like humans, infant non-human primates prefer faces (Pascalis and Kelly, 2009; Pascalis et al., 2021). Among mammals, sheep and other ungulate species show sophisticated face recognition abilities (Proops and McComb, 2012; Towler et al., 2019); dogs have been shown to respond specifically to their owners’ faces and recognize their facial emotional expressions (Barber et al., 2016; Cuaya et al., 2016). Several avian species possess remarkable face processing abilities (Brecht et al., 2017; Suwandschieff et al., 2023), including pigeons and domestic chicks, where neural correlates of face discrimination have been investigated (Clark et al., 2022; Kobylkov et al., 2024). Fish also rely on visual information in the head and face regions to drive their social affiliative responses (Karplus and Algom, 1981; Wang and Takeuchi, 2017; Nunes et al., 2020). Finally, individual recognition based on facial features has been described also in paper wasps and honeybees (Avarguès-Weber et al., 2018; Tibbetts et al., 2021).

Face processing comprises multiple functional components, including visual processing, facial identity analysis, and facial emotion expression recognition. We focus here on face detection, defined as the ability to detect the presence of faces in the visual scene based on first-order information (i.e., basic spatial properties of facial features). This form of visual discrimination is the simplest and earliest form of face processing, and it is more likely to be ubiquitous across species.

Moreover, the similarities in face processing observed in different species suggest a common origin that could be exploited to investigate the underlying neurobiological mechanisms and to elucidate the earliest expression of social deficits in neurodevelopmental disorders such as ASD. This review presents an overview of the current literature on face detection in vertebrate models potentially relevant to the study of ASD.

Evidence of face processing in newborn vertebrates

A significant contribution to the knowledge about face detection comes from the extensive literature on face processing development in non-human primates (see Pascalis et al., 2021 for a review). Infant primates, including humans, exhibit a strong preference for faces. Cross-fostering and restricted social experience experiments in rhesus (Macaca mulatta) and Japanese monkeys (Macaca fuscata) indicate differences in the ability of these two species to respond to the faces of other species. Japanese macaques raised without face experience for several months maintained their preferences for both monkey and human faces, showing an innate response to faces independent of the species (Sugita, 2008). After limiting their visual experience to human or monkey faces, their preference was tuned toward the predominant faces they were exposed to, demonstrating a strong influence of experience on the innate representation of faces (Sugita, 2008, 2009). Rhesus macaques, instead, express a strong preference for faces of their own species already at 3 months of age (Fujita, 1987, 1993). Subsequent studies on young infants (3 weeks old) rhesus macaques raised without face experience also supported the idea of an early coarse representation of faces that is quickly refined to the own species due to exposure (Simpson et al., 2017). This data supports the existence, also in monkeys, of hard-wired mechanisms providing an early representation of faces independent of experience. These mechanisms are then fine-tuned by experience according to the predominant face stimuli found in the environment.

In addition to Japanese macaque monkeys, domestic chicks (Rosa-Salva et al., 2010), and land tortoises (Versace et al., 2020) show innate face preference responses similar to those observed in human newborns.

Research involving domestic chicks (Gallus gallus domesticus) offers a convenient method for regulating the animals’ visual exposure prior to hatching by maintaining the eggs and the hatchlings in complete darkness. Unlike many mammalian species, dark rearing does not compromise the chicks’ visual system’s development. Moreover, domestic chicks are a precocial species: the hatchlings already possess a mature visual system, can immediately and efficiently explore the environment, and are strongly driven to social interaction. Thanks to these advantages, studies on domestic chicks have demonstrated the presence of innate representations of visual stimuli important for the animals’ survival, including faces and face-like schematic configurations (Rosa-Salva et al., 2010; Rosa Salva et al., 2012), biological motion (Vallortigara et al., 2005; Zanon et al., 2024), and animacy (Rosa Salva et al., 2015). As for face detection, a series of experiments showed that visually inexperienced newborn chicks are spontaneously driven to approach a schematic stimulus representing a face compared to several other stimuli sharing the same top-heavy configuration but lacking the “facedness” property (Rosa-Salva et al., 2010). Subsequent studies demonstrated the sensitivity of the chicks to the reversal of contrast polarity and a right hemisphere advantage for the detection of faces (Rosa Salva et al., 2012) similar to what was shown in human neonates (Farroni et al., 2005; Buiatti et al., 2019). Overall, domestic chicks seem to express orienting responses to visual stimuli equivalent to those observed in developmental human studies, advocating this flexible animal model to investigate the neurobehavioral bases of face detection.

Neural correlates of face detection

Visual object recognition in mammals is mediated by a series of recurrent, hierarchically, and topographically organized cortical circuits, often referred to as the ventral visual processing stream. Visual information is sent from the primary visual cortex (V1) to the extra-striate occipital visual areas and converges in the ventral part of the temporal cortex in high-level visual areas. The simplest aspect of face processing, which involves detecting the presence of faces in the visual scene, requires extracting basic features of the stimulus independent of contexts and viewpoints. Face detection has been hypothesized to involve a different hierarchical visual pathway than the recognition of other objects and to be supported by cortical face-selective areas (Kanwisher et al., 1997; McCarthy et al., 1997). Face-selective regions in the brain have been described in the ventrolateral aspects of the occipital and temporal cortex (see for review Duchaine and Yovel, 2015). These areas respond preferentially to faces, being active significantly more in response to faces than other non-face visual stimuli (objects, places, body parts, and letters). The occipital face area (OFA; Gauthier et al., 2000), an early visual area in the inferior occipital gyrus, and the face-selective region in the superior temporal sulcus (fSTS; Hoffman and Haxby, 2000) produce an initial representation of faces based on first-order elements, i.e., eyes, nose and mouth, but lack sensitivity to their spatial configuration (Pitcher et al., 2007; Liu et al., 2010). The most robust face-selective activation is observed in the fusiform gyrus of the temporal lobe, named the fusiform face area (FFA; Kanwisher et al., 1997; McCarthy et al., 1997). Numerous studies have indicated that FFA is activated in response to face stimuli rather than their visual features (Liu et al., 2010) and that it exhibits selective activation in response to different types of facial stimuli, including photographs, drawings, and depictions of animal faces (Tong et al., 2000). Moreover, the neural representation within the FFA is sensitive to the “inversion effect” (originally unveiled through the “Thatcher illusion” by Thompson, 1980), which causes a disproportionate drop in recognition of upside-down (inverted) faces compared to upright faces not observed for inverted objects (Yovel and Kanwisher, 2005).

A recent report found evidence of cortical involvement in face processing at birth, similar to the face-selective areas observed in the adult brain (Buiatti et al., 2019). Using high-density EEG and measuring frequency-tagged signals in newborns exposed to upright face-like, inverted, and scrambled face stimuli, the study found a stronger response to face-like stimuli in cortical structures along the occipitotemporal pathway, similar to those observed in adults (Buiatti et al., 2019).

In addition to the face-selective areas in the visual cortex, an extended network of additional face processing areas has been described as involved in identity recognition, facial expression and emotional processing (Haxby et al., 2000; Calder and Young, 2005). These include lateral prefrontal cortex regions that are involved in both featural and configural processing of faces and provide a top-down control to the temporal cortex (Heekeren et al., 2004; Renzi et al., 2013).

Non-human primate research has also been fundamental in investigating the neural bases of face perception (Tsao and Livingstone, 2008; Rossion and Taubert, 2019). Face-selective areas, called face patches, have been described in macaque monkeys (Tsao et al., 2008), vervet monkeys (Zangenehpour and Chaudhuri, 2005), marmosets (Hung et al., 2015), and chimpanzees’ (Parr et al., 2009) inferotemporal (IT) cortex.

In parallel to discovering face-selective areas in the temporal cortex of humans and non-human primates, several studies demonstrated that a subcortical visual pathway largely mediates face perception (Johnson et al., 2015; Almasi and Behrmann, 2021). It is widely recognized that newborns have the ability to orient themselves toward faces and stimuli that resemble faces. Considering the poor maturation of the cerebral cortex in the first months after birth, many authors have suggested that subcortical visual pathways may support the immature cortical structures during this time (Johnson, 2005). Evidence confirms this hypothesis, including recent studies showing a monocular advantage in infants’ and adults’ face processing (Almasi and Behrmann, 2021; Dalrymple et al., 2021) and suggesting the engagement of subcortical visual pathways. This evolutionarily conserved visual system includes part of the retinocollicular system, the amygdala, the lateral geniculate nucleus, the pulvinar, and the superior colliculus. In addition to studies in infants, face-selective activation has also been demonstrated in subcortical regions of the adult brain, independent of emotional expression, for example, in the amygdala and the superior colliculus (Mende-Siedlecki et al., 2013). Recent studies in monkeys have demonstrated that a specific population of neurons in the superior colliculus responded stronger and faster to the face-like than non-face patterns (Le et al., 2020), elucidating some of the subcortical neural correlates involved in face detection. To reconcile with recent reports of cortical areas in the occipitotemporal cortex participating in newborns’ face detection (Buiatti et al., 2019), studies have suggested a substantial link between the subcortical and the cortical circuits to support face processing in early development (Pessoa and Adolphs, 2010).

A recent report investigated face-selective neurons also in domestic chicks (Kobylkov et al., 2024). The authors examined the response of a group of neurons in the nidopallium, a region of the chick brain believed to be homologous to the human prefrontal cortex, of face-naïve young domestic chicks exposed to schematic face-like stimuli. Using single-cell recordings, Kobylkov et al. (2024) demonstrated the presence of face-selective neurons in the caudolateral nidopallium of domestic chicks that respond significantly stronger to upright face-like stimuli compared to other configurations (inverted, asymmetric, or frequency-filtered facial stimuli) or to face parts (Kobylkov et al., 2024). Moreover, the authors demonstrated that face-selectivity in this neuronal population emerges independently of previous visual experience, supporting the hypothesis of an innate system for face detection in domestic chicks (see for a review Kobylkov and Vallortigara, 2024). Interestingly, face selectivity has been described in the prefrontal cortex of both humans (Heekeren et al., 2004; Renzi et al., 2013) and non-human primates (Chan et al., 2016; Schaeffer et al., 2020) demonstrating, once more, the strong translational value of this animal model.

Face detection in neurodevelopmental disorders: an animal model perspective

Face processing deficit is the most important early impairment in ASD and one of the most reliable findings in the literature. Several studies have examined face processing abilities in ASD (see for a review Campatelli et al., 2013; Bi and Fang, 2017), documenting behavioral differences at different levels: facial expression recognition (Uljarevic and Hamilton, 2013; Lozier et al., 2014; Loth et al., 2018), individual identity recognition (Weigelt et al., 2012; Minio-Paluello et al., 2020), visual attention to faces (Klin et al., 2002; Pelphrey et al., 2002; Riby and Hancock, 2009; Chita-Tegmark, 2016; Reisinger et al., 2020), and face perception (Carver and Dawson, 2002; Klin et al., 2002). Deficits in visual attention and face detection seem to characterize ASD from very early in life: while face detection deficits in ASD have been reported more consistently in older children and adults (Webb et al., 2017; Bathelt et al., 2022), newborns with increased ASD risk also show reduced responses to face-like stimuli already at birth (Di Giorgio et al., 2016, 2021; Bradshaw et al., 2020). In addition, neurophysiological and neuroimaging studies have shown altered activation patterns in cortical brain areas associated with face detection (Shephard et al., 2020; Tye et al., 2022). Hypoactivation of FFA during tasks involving face perception appears to be the most consistently observed functional abnormality in ASD (Schultz et al., 2000, 2003; Schultz and Klin, 2002; Nickl-Jockschat et al., 2015; Wang et al., 2024). Other subcortical face processing networks exhibit functional alterations and modified connectivity in individuals with ASD (Kleinhans et al., 2011). For example, the amygdala displays atypical activation in ASD (Kliemann et al., 2012; Philip et al., 2012; Rutishauser et al., 2013; Wang and Li, 2023); aberrant activation in ASD was also described in the pulvinar and the superior colliculus (Kleinhans et al., 2011; Huang et al., 2022).

Despite the advancement in the understanding of brain connectivity and functional changes related to face processing abnormalities in ASD, the neurobiological bases of these dysfunctions are still not well understood. Given the high heterogeneity of the symptoms and neurobiological alterations associated with ASD (Lamanna and Meldolesi, 2024), as well as the adaptative changes occurring in the developing brain to compensate for the deficits, understanding the mechanisms behind face processing abilities in ASD requires developmental studies. So far, very few studies have addressed the developmental aspect of these deficits, i.e., what are the mechanisms behind the lack of preferential attention to faces in individuals with ASD from early developmental stages? Some authors (Dawson et al., 2005) have proposed that face processing deficits are mediated by underlying impairment in social motivation, resulting in the failure to attend to socially relevant stimuli and, therefore, limiting social development. Others (Johnson, 2005) have suggested that in individuals with ASD, the disruption of the subcortical face processing pathway is a primary cause of impaired social orienting mechanisms, which in turn compromises exposure to faces and, therefore, the development of typical social abilities.

Given human neonatal studies’ limits and ethical constraints, together with the pervasive nature of face processing in vertebrates, adopting a developmental approach to studying face detection mechanisms with a comparative perspective in animal models of ASD may represent a valuable strategy to investigate these deficits.

The development of highly efficient and precise genetic tools has significantly spurred research in modeling human disorders in non-human primates, including in the context of autism (Zhao et al., 2018). A recent study by Zhou et al. (2019) analyzed the effect of a mutation in the SH3 and ankyrin repeat domains 3 (SHANK3) gene, a highly penetrant, monogenic risk factor for ASD, on Cynomolgus macaque monkeys (Macaca fascicularis) visual attention to social and nonsocial stimuli. Using images of faces, objects, and faces with threatening or neutral expressions, the authors found a reduced fixation time and increased aversion to the images in the mutant monkey compared to the controls despite the low number of mutant monkeys analyzed (5 mutants, 1 female). Another study was recently conducted (Zhao et al., 2019) investigating attentive behavior towards faces in juvenile macaques (Macaca fascicularis) exposed to valproic acid (VPA), an anticonvulsant known to interfere with the development of the social brain and increase the risk of developing ASD in humans (Christensen et al., 2013). VPA’s mechanism of action involves its direct inhibition of histone deacetylases (HDACs), interfering with normal chromatin deacetylation and disrupting the transcription of multiple ASD-associated genes (Meng et al., 2022; Guerra et al., 2023; Krueger et al., 2024; Zarate-Lopez et al., 2024). Using eye-tracking analysis to measure the animals’ attention to faces or scenes containing conspecifics, the authors found that juvenile monkeys exposed to VPA attended to non-social stimuli significantly more than their control siblings (Zhao et al., 2019). Studies have also been carried out on models of ASD in marmoset monkeys (Callithrix jacchus) exposed to VPA. Nakagami et al. (2022) examined the visual attention of juvenile and adult marmosets exposed to VPA and found a reduction in the time spent gazing at other conspecifics already at 15–19 weeks.

Despite the limited reports of direct face detection testing in monkeys, the evidence accumulated so far is promising, and the potential for investigating the neural correlates and neurobiological mechanisms underlying the reported deficits in social preference is encouraging (Watson and Platt, 2012; Bauman and Schumann, 2018; Katsnelson, 2018; Zhao et al., 2018).

In addition to human newborns and monkeys, several other vertebrate species possess remarkable face detection abilities. In domestic chicks, younglings express striking orienting responses to a broad spectrum of visual representations of the typical appearance and features associated with living beings. Several studies have investigated the behavioral and neurobiological bases of these abilities in domestic chicks, including extensive investigation of their innate preferences for human faces and face-like stimuli (see for a review Di Giorgio et al., 2017). Thanks to their precocious and strong social response to visual cues, domestic chicks have also attracted attention as model organisms to investigate other behavioral and neurobiological mechanisms relevant to autism (Csillag et al., 2022; Matsushima et al., 2024). Exposure to the anticonvulsant VPA impairs the chicks’ ability to orient to the appearance of a stuffed hen (Sgadò et al., 2018) to motion cues typical of animate agents (Lorenzi et al., 2019) and, most interestingly, to face-like configurations similar to those that bias the attention of human neonates (Adiletta et al., 2021). Interestingly, neurobiological mechanisms potentially relevant for impairments in face processing are starting to be investigated (Adiletta et al., 2022).

Discussion

Despite a consensus on the significance of face processing in social development and ASD, the nature and origin of the face processing deficits and the underlying neurobiological mechanisms are still unclear. Thanks to the pervasive nature of face processing in vertebrates, employing a comparative approach to study the development of face detection mechanisms in animal models of ASD may shed light on these mechanisms.

Many studies investigating the neurobiological bases of ASD in animal models are focused on rodents, especially mice, thanks to their genetic and neuroanatomical homology to humans. However, given the prolonged postnatal development of the visual system and the difficulty in analyzing visually mediated early behavioral responses, research into face processing in rodents has been limited (Schnell et al., 2019; Watanabe et al., 2022). However, both rats and mice have remarkable visual perception abilities (Djurdjevic et al., 2018) that could be exploited to further investigate face processing deficits in rodent ASD models.

Prioritizing species with early visual perceptual skills that replicate many of the features of face processing in humans, readily testable at postnatal developmental stages, may be instrumental in providing the key to clarifying the nature and origin of these mechanisms and their role in atypical social development. Very few studies have explored this approach.

The recent development of highly efficient and precise genetic tools, such as CRISPR/Cas9 and TALEN (transcription activator-like (TAL) effector nucleases), makes exploring transgenic strategies in several species possible, including the domestic fowl. Since the first transgenic chick was generated (Salter et al., 1987), much effort has been devoted to developing strategies to induce genetic modifications and improve germline transmission in domestic chicks (see for a review Sid and Schusser, 2018). CRISPR/Cas9-mediated genome editing has been previously employed in domestic chicks to generate somatic mutations in early embryos used for developmental studies (Gandhi et al., 2017), but only recently have stable Cas9-expressing chicken lines been generated (Rieblinger et al., 2021), opening new perspectives to yield loss-of-function mutations in this species, including those to model ASD.

Author contributions

PS: Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. AP: Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. JL: Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. AA: Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing, Conceptualization.

Funding

The author(s) declare financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. The study was supported by intramural funds and by PRIN 2022 [DD 104 del 02/02/22 – PNRR – M4 – C2 – INV 1.1 – PRIN – Behavioral, molecular and neuroanatomical characterization of two models of autism in domestic chicks and zebrafish – 2022LJZRBY] CUP (E53D23008910006).

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Adiletta, A., Pedrana, S., Rosa-Salva, O., and Sgadò, P. (2021). Spontaneous visual preference for face-like stimuli is impaired in newly-hatched domestic chicks exposed to Valproic acid during embryogenesis. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 15:733140. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2021.733140

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Adiletta, A., Pross, A., Taricco, N., and Sgadò, P. (2022). Embryonic valproate exposure alters mesencephalic dopaminergic neurons distribution and septal dopaminergic gene expression in domestic chicks. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 16:804881. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2022.804881

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Almasi, R. C., and Behrmann, M. (2021). Subcortical regions of the human visual system do not process faces holistically. Brain Cogn. 151:105726. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2021.105726

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Avarguès-Weber, A., d’Amaro, D., Metzler, M., Finke, V., Baracchi, D., and Dyer, A. G. (2018). Does holistic processing require a large brain? Insights from honeybees and wasps in fine visual recognition tasks. Front. Psychol. 9:1313. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01313

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Barber, A. L. A., Randi, D., Müller, C. A., and Huber, L. (2016). The processing of human emotional faces by pet and lab dogs: evidence for lateralization and experience effects. PLoS One 11:e0152393. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0152393

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bathelt, J., Koolschijn, P. C. M., and Geurts, H. M. (2022). Atypically slow processing of faces and non-faces in older autistic adults. Autism 26, 1737–1751. doi: 10.1177/13623613211065297

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bauman, M. D., and Schumann, C. M. (2018). Advances in nonhuman primate models of autism: integrating neuroscience and behavior. Exp. Neurol. 299, 252–265. doi: 10.1016/j.expneurol.2017.07.021

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bi, T., and Fang, F. (2017). Impaired face perception in individuals with autism Spectrum disorder: insights on diagnosis and treatment. Neurosci. Bull. 33, 757–759. doi: 10.1007/s12264-017-0187-1

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bortolon, C., Capdevielle, D., and Raffard, S. (2015). Face recognition in schizophrenia disorder: a comprehensive review of behavioral, neuroimaging and neurophysiological studies. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 53, 79–107. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.03.006

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bradshaw, J., Klin, A., Evans, L., Klaiman, C., Saulnier, C., and McCracken, C. (2020). Development of attention from birth to 5 months in infants at risk for autism spectrum disorder. Dev. Psychopathol. 32, 491–501. doi: 10.1017/s0954579419000233

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Brecht, K. F., Wagener, L., Ostojić, L., Clayton, N. S., and Nieder, A. (2017). Comparing the face inversion effect in crows and humans. J. Comp. Physiol. A. 203, 1017–1027. doi: 10.1007/s00359-017-1211-7

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Buiatti, M., Giorgio, E. D., Piazza, M., Polloni, C., Menna, G., Taddei, F., et al. (2019). Cortical route for facelike pattern processing in human newborns. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 116, 4625–4630. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1812419116

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Calder, A. J., and Young, A. W. (2005). Understanding the recognition of facial identity and facial expression. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 6, 641–651. doi: 10.1038/nrn1724

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Campatelli, G., Federico, R. R., Apicella, F., Sicca, F., and Muratori, F. (2013). Face processing in children with ASD: literature review. Res. Autism Spectr. Disord. 7, 444–454. doi: 10.1016/j.rasd.2012.10.003

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Carver, L. J., and Dawson, G. (2002). Development and neural bases of face recognition in autism. Mol. Psychiatry 7, S18–S20. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4001168

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chan, A., Trefler, A., and Babajani-Feremi, A. (2016). Verifying face selectivity in the human prefrontal cortex: data from \textbackslashtextasciitilde500 participants. J. Vis. 16:718. doi: 10.1167/16.12.718

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, Y., Norton, D., Ongur, D., and Heckers, S. (2008). Inefficient face detection in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Bull. 34, 367–374. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbm071

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chita-Tegmark, M. (2016). Social attention in ASD: a review and meta-analysis of eye-tracking studies. Res. Dev. Disabil. 48, 79–93. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2015.10.011

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Christensen, J., Grønborg, T., Sørensen, M., Schendel, D., Parner, E., Pedersen, L., et al. (2013). Prenatal valproate exposure and risk of autism spectrum disorders and childhood autism. JAMA 309, 1696–1703. doi: 10.1001/jama.2013.2270

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Clark, W., Chilcott, M., Azizi, A., Pusch, R., Perry, K., and Colombo, M. (2022). Neurons in the pigeon visual network discriminate between faces, scrambled faces, and sine grating images. Sci. Rep. 12:589. doi: 10.1038/s41598-021-04559-z

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Collin, L., Bindra, J., Raju, M., Gillberg, C., and Minnis, H. (2013). Facial emotion recognition in child psychiatry: a systematic review. Res. Dev. Disabil. 34, 1505–1520. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2013.01.008

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Csillag, A., Ádám, Á., and Zachar, G. (2022). Avian models for brain mechanisms underlying altered social behavior in autism. Front. Physiol. 13:1032046. doi: 10.3389/fphys.2022.1032046

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cuaya, L. V., Hernández-Pérez, R., and Concha, L. (2016). Our faces in the Dog’s brain: functional imaging reveals temporal cortex activation during perception of human faces. PLoS One 11:e0149431. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0149431

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dalrymple, K. A., Khan, A. F., Duchaine, B., and Elison, J. T. (2021). Visual input to the left versus right eye yields differences in face preferences in 3-month-old infants. Dev. Sci. 24:e13029. doi: 10.1111/desc.13029

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dawson, G., Webb, S. J., and McPartland, J. (2005). Understanding the nature of face processing impairment in autism: insights from behavioral and electrophysiological studies. Dev. Neuropsychol. 27, 403–424. doi: 10.1207/s15326942dn2703_6

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Di Giorgio, E., Frasnelli, E., Salva, O. R., Luisa, S. M., Puopolo, M., Tosoni, D., et al. (2016). Difference in visual social predispositions between newborns at low-and high-risk for autism. Sci. Rep. 6:26395. doi: 10.1038/srep26395

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Di Giorgio, E., Loveland, J. L., Mayer, U., Rosa-Salva, O., Versace, E., and Vallortigara, G. (2017). Filial responses as predisposed and learned preferences: early attachment in chicks and babies. Behav. Brain Res. 325, 90–104. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2016.09.018

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Di Giorgio, E., Rosa-Salva, O., Frasnelli, E., Calcagnì, A., Lunghi, M., Scattoni, M. L., et al. (2021). Abnormal visual attention to simple social stimuli in 4-month-old infants at high risk for autism. Sci. Rep. 11:15785. doi: 10.1038/s41598-021-95418-4

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Djurdjevic, V., Ansuini, A., Bertolini, D., Macke, J., and Zoccolan, D. (2018). Accuracy of rats in discriminating visual objects is explained by the complexity of their perceptual strategy. Curr. Biol. 28, 1005–1015.e5. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2018.02.037

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Duchaine, B., and Yovel, G. (2015). A revised neural framework for face processing. Ann. Rev. Vision Sci. 1, 393–416. doi: 10.1146/annurev-vision-082114-035518

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Farroni, T., Johnson, M. H., Menon, E., Zulian, L., Faraguna, D., and Csibra, G. (2005). Newborns’ preference for face-relevant stimuli: effects of contrast polarity. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 102, 17245–17250. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0502205102

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fujita, K. (1987). Species recognition by five macaque monkeys. Primates 28, 353–366. doi: 10.1007/bf02381018

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fujita, K. (1993). Development of visual preference for closely related species by infant and juvenile macaques with restricted social experience. Primates 34, 141–150. doi: 10.1007/bf02381385

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gandhi, S., Piacentino, M., Vieceli, F., and Bronner, M. (2017). Optimization of CRISPR/Cas9 genome editing for loss-of-function in the early chick embryo. Dev. Biol. 432, 86–97. doi: 10.1016/j.ydbio.2017.08.036

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gauthier, I., Tarr, M. J., Moylan, J., Skudlarski, P., Gore, J. C., and Anderson, A. W. (2000). The fusiform face area is part of a network that processes faces at the individual level. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 12, 495–504. doi: 10.1162/089892900562165

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Goren, C., Sarty, M., and Wu, P. (1975). Visual following and pattern discrimination of face-like stimuli by newborn infants. Pediatrics 56, 544–549. doi: 10.1542/peds.56.4.544

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Guerra, M., Medici, V., Weatheritt, R., Corvino, V., Palacios, D., Geloso, M. C., et al. (2023). Fetal exposure to valproic acid dysregulates the expression of autism-linked genes in the developing cerebellum. Transl. Psychiatry 13:114. doi: 10.1038/s41398-023-02391-9

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Haxby, J. V., Hoffman, E. A., Gobbini, M. I., Haxby, J. V., Hoffman, E. A., and Gobbini, M. I. (2000). The distributed human neural system for face perception. Trends Cogn. Sci. 4, 223–233. doi: 10.1016/s1364-6613(00)01482-0

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Heekeren, H. R., Marrett, S., Bandettini, P. A., and Ungerleider, L. G. (2004). A general mechanism for perceptual decision-making in the human brain. Nature 431, 859–862. doi: 10.1038/nature02966

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoffman, E. A., and Haxby, J. V. (2000). Distinct representations of eye gaze and identity in the distributed human neural system for face perception. Nat. Neurosci. 3, 80–84. doi: 10.1038/71152

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Huang, Y., Vangel, M., Chen, H., Eshel, M., Cheng, M., Lu, T., et al. (2022). The impaired subcortical pathway from superior colliculus to the amygdala in boys with autism Spectrum disorder. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 16:666439. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2022.666439

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hung, C.-C., Yen, C. C., Ciuchta, J. L., Papoti, D., Bock, N. A., Leopold, D. A., et al. (2015). Functional mapping of face-selective regions in the extrastriate visual cortex of the marmoset. J. Neurosci. 35, 1160–1172. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.2659-14.2015

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnson, M. H. (2005). Subcortical face processing. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 6, 766–774. doi: 10.1038/nrn1766

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnson, M. H. (2014). Autism: demise of the innate social orienting hypothesis. Curr. Biol. 24, R30–R31. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2013.11.021

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnson, M., Dziurawiec, S., Ellis, H., and Morton, J. (1991). Newborns’ preferential tracking of face-like stimuli and its subsequent decline. Cognition 40, 1–19. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(91)90045-6

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnson, M. H., Senju, A., and Tomalski, P. (2015). The two-process theory of face processing: modifications based on two decades of data from infants and adults. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 50, 169–179. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.10.009

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kanwisher, N., McDermott, J., and Chun, M. M. (1997). The fusiform face area: a module in human extrastriate cortex specialized for face perception. J. Neurosci. Off. J. Soc. Neurosci. 17, 4302–4311. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.17-11-04302.1997

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Karplus, I., and Algom, D. (1981). Visual cues for predator face recognition by reef fishes. Z. Tierpsychol. 55, 343–364. doi: 10.1111/j.1439-0310.1981.tb01277.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Katsnelson, A. (2018). Modeling autism. Lab Anim. 47, 41–44. doi: 10.1038/laban.1394

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kleiner, K. A. (1987). Amplitude and phase spectra as indices of infants’ pattern preferences. Infant Behav. Dev. 10, 49–59. doi: 10.1016/0163-6383(87)90006-3

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kleinhans, N. M., Richards, T., Johnson, L. C., Weaver, K. E., Greenson, J., Dawson, G., et al. (2011). fMRI evidence of neural abnormalities in the subcortical face processing system in ASD. NeuroImage 54, 697–704. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.07.037

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kliemann, D., Dziobek, I., Hatri, A., Baudewig, J., and Heekeren, H. R. (2012). The role of the amygdala in atypical gaze on emotional faces in autism Spectrum disorders. J. Neurosci. 32, 9469–9476. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.5294-11.2012

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Klin, A., Jones, W., Schultz, R., Volkmar, F., and Cohen, D. (2002). Visual fixation patterns during viewing of naturalistic social situations as predictors of social competence in individuals with autism. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 59, 809–816. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.59.9.809

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kobylkov, D., Rosa-Salva, O., Zanon, M., and Vallortigara, G. (2024). Innate face detectors in the nidopallium of young domestic chicks. bioRxiv :2024.02.15.580445. doi: 10.1101/2024.02.15.580445

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kobylkov, D., and Vallortigara, G. (2024). Face detection mechanisms: Nature vs. nurture. Front. Neurosci. 18:1404174. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2024.1404174

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Krueger, B., Dorsey, S., Mocci, E., and Lane, M. (2024). Rapid effects of valproic acid on the fetal brain transcriptome: implications for brain development and autism. Research Square. [Preprint]. doi: 10.21203/rs.3.rs-3684653/v1

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lamanna, J., and Meldolesi, J. (2024). Autism Spectrum disorder: brain areas involved, neurobiological mechanisms, diagnoses and therapies. Int. J. Mol. Sci. 25:2423. doi: 10.3390/ijms25042423

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Le, Q. V., Le, Q. V., Nishimaru, H., Matsumoto, J., Takamura, Y., Hori, E., et al. (2020). A prototypical template for rapid face detection is embedded in the monkey superior colliculus. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 14:5. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2020.00005

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, J., Harris, A., and Kanwisher, N. (2010). Perception of face parts and face configurations: an fMRI study. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 22, 203–211. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21203

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lorenzi, E., Pross, A., Rosa-Salva, O., Versace, E., Sgadò, P., and Vallortigara, G. (2019). Embryonic exposure to Valproic acid affects social predispositions for dynamic cues of animate motion in newly-hatched chicks. Front. Physiol. 10:501. doi: 10.3389/fphys.2019.00501

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Loth, E., Garrido, L., Ahmad, J., Watson, E., Duff, A., and Duchaine, B. (2018). Facial expression recognition as a candidate marker for autism spectrum disorder: how frequent and severe are deficits? Mol. Autism 9:7. doi: 10.1186/s13229-018-0187-7

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lozier, L. M., Vanmeter, J. W., and Marsh, A. A. (2014). Impairments in facial affect recognition associated with autism spectrum disorders: a meta-analysis. Dev. Psychopathol. 26, 933–945. doi: 10.1017/s0954579414000479

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Matsushima, T., Izumi, T., and Vallortigara, G. (2024). The domestic chick as an animal model of autism spectrum disorder: building adaptive social perceptions through prenatally formed predispositions. Front. Neurosci. 18:1279947. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2024.1279947

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

McCarthy, G., Puce, A., Gore, J. C., and Allison, T. (1997). Face-specific processing in the human fusiform gyrus. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 9, 605–610. doi: 10.1162/jocn.1997.9.5.605

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mende-Siedlecki, P., Verosky, S. C., Turk-Browne, N. B., and Todorov, A. (2013). Robust selectivity for faces in the human amygdala in the absence of expressions. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 25, 2086–2106. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00469

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Meng, Q., Zhang, W., Wang, X., Jiao, C., Xu, S., Liu, C., et al. (2022). Human forebrain organoids reveal connections between valproic acid exposure and autism risk. Transl. Psychiatry 12:130. doi: 10.1038/s41398-022-01898-x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Minio-Paluello, I., Porciello, G., Pascual-Leone, A., and Baron-Cohen, S. (2020). Face individual identity recognition: a potential endophenotype in autism. Mol. Autism. 11:81. doi: 10.1186/s13229-020-00371-0

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Morton, J., and Johnson, M. H. (1991). CONSPEC and CONLERN: a two-process theory of infant face recognition. Psychol. Rev. 98, 164–181. doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.98.2.164

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Nakagami, A., Yasue, M., Nakagaki, K., Nakamura, M., Kawai, N., and Ichinohe, N. (2022). Reduced childhood social attention in autism model marmosets predicts impaired social skills and inflexible behavior in adulthood. Front. Psych. 13:885433. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2022.885433

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Nickl-Jockschat, T., Rottschy, C., Thommes, J., Schneider, F., Laird, A. R., Fox, P. T., et al. (2015). Neural networks related to dysfunctional face processing in autism spectrum disorder. Brain Struct. Funct. 220, 2355–2371. doi: 10.1007/s00429-014-0791-z

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Nunes, A. R., Carreira, L., Anbalagan, S., Blechman, J., Levkowitz, G., and Oliveira, R. F. (2020). Perceptual mechanisms of social affiliation in zebrafish. Sci. Rep. 10:3642. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-60154-8

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Parr, L. A., Hecht, E., Barks, S. K., Preuss, T. M., and Votaw, J. R. (2009). Face processing in the chimpanzee brain. Curr. Biol. 19, 50–53. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2008.11.048

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pascalis, O., Damon, F., Guo, K., and Méary, D. (2021). It takes one to know one: do human and nonhuman Primates share similar face processing? Singapore: Springer Singapore.

Google Scholar

Pascalis, O., and Kelly, D. J. (2009). The origins of face processing in humans: phylogeny and ontogeny. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 4, 200–209. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-6924.2009.01119.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pavlova, M. A., Guerreschi, M., Tagliavento, L., Gitti, F., Sokolov, A. N., Fallgatter, A. J., et al. (2017). Social cognition in autism: face tuning. Sci. Rep. 7:2734. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-02790-1

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pelphrey, K. A., Sasson, N. J., Reznick, J. S., Paul, G., Goldman, B. D., and Piven, J. (2002). Visual scanning of faces in autism. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 32, 249–261. doi: 10.1023/a:1016374617369

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pessoa, L., and Adolphs, R. (2010). Emotion processing and the amygdala: from a “low road” to “many roads” of evaluating biological significance. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 11, 773–782. doi: 10.1038/nrn2920

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Philip, R. C. M., Dauvermann, M. R., Whalley, H. C., Baynham, K., Lawrie, S. M., and Stanfield, A. C. (2012). A systematic review and meta-analysis of the fMRI investigation of autism spectrum disorders. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 36, 901–942. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.10.008

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pitcher, D., Walsh, V., Yovel, G., and Duchaine, B. (2007). TMS evidence for the involvement of the right occipital face area in early face processing. Curr. Biol. 17, 1568–1573. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2007.07.063

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Proops, L., and McComb, K. (2012). Cross-modal individual recognition in domestic horses (Equus caballus) extends to familiar humans. Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 279, 3131–3138. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2012.0626

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Reid, V., Dunn, K., Young, R., Amu, J., Donovan, T., and Reissland, N. (2017). The human fetus preferentially engages with face-like visual stimuli. Curr. Biol. 27, 1825–1828.e3. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2017.05.044

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Reisinger, D. L., Shaffer, R. C., Horn, P. S., Hong, M. P., Pedapati, E. V., Dominick, K. C., et al. (2020). Atypical social attention and emotional face processing in autism Spectrum disorder: insights from face scanning and Pupillometry. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 13:76. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2019.00076

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Renzi, C., Schiavi, S., Carbon, C.-C., Vecchi, T., Silvanto, J., and Cattaneo, Z. (2013). Processing of featural and configural aspects of faces is lateralized in dorsolateral prefrontal cortex: a TMS study. NeuroImage 74, 45–51. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.02.015

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Riby, D. M., and Hancock, P. J. B. (2009). Do faces capture the attention of individuals with Williams syndrome or autism? Evidence from tracking eye movements. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 39, 421–431. doi: 10.1007/s10803-008-0641-z

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rieblinger, B., Sid, H., Duda, D., Bozoglu, T., Klinger, R., Schlickenrieder, A., et al. (2021). Cas9-expressing chickens and pigs as resources for genome editing in livestock. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 118:e2022562118. doi: 10.1073/pnas.2022562118

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Romagnano, V., Sokolov, A. N., Steinwand, P., Fallgatter, A. J., and Pavlova, M. A. (2022). Face pareidolia in male schizophrenia. Schizophrenia 8:112. doi: 10.1038/s41537-022-00315-y

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rosa Salva, O., Mayer, U., and Vallortigara, G. (2015). Roots of a social brain: developmental models of emerging animacy-detection mechanisms. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 50, 150–168. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.12.015

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rosa Salva, O., Regolin, L., and Vallortigara, G. (2012). Inversion of contrast polarity abolishes spontaneous preferences for face-like stimuli in newborn chicks. Behav. Brain Res. 228, 133–143. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2011.11.025

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rosa-Salva, O., Regolin, L., and Vallortigara, G. (2010). Faces are special for newly hatched chicks: evidence for inborn domain-specific mechanisms underlying spontaneous preferences for face-like stimuli. Dev. Sci. 13, 565–577. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00914.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rossion, B., and Taubert, J. (2019). What can we learn about human individual face recognition from experimental studies in monkeys? Vis. Res. 157, 142–158. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2018.03.012

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Russell, M. T., Hajdúk, M., Springfield, C. R., Klein, H. S., Bass, E. L., Mittal, V. A., et al. (2024). Identity recognition from faces and bodies in schizophrenia spectrum disorders. Schizophrenia Res. Cognit. 36:100307. doi: 10.1016/j.scog.2024.100307

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rutishauser, U., Tudusciuc, O., Wang, S., Mamelak, A. N., Ross, I. B., and Adolphs, R. (2013). Single-neuron correlates of atypical face processing in autism. Neuron 80, 887–899. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.08.029

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Salter, D. W., Smith, E. J., Hughes, S. H., Wright, S. E., and Crittenden, L. B. (1987). Transgenic chickens: insertion of retroviral genes into the chicken germ line. Virology 157, 236–240. doi: 10.1016/0042-6822(87)90334-5

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Schaeffer, D. J., Selvanayagam, J., Johnston, K. D., Menon, R. S., Freiwald, W. A., and Everling, S. (2020). Face selective patches in marmoset frontal cortex. Nat. Commun. 11:4856. doi: 10.1038/s41467-020-18692-2

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Schnell, A., Van den Bergh, G., Vermaercke, B., Gijbels, K., Bossens, C., and de Beeck, H. (2019). Face categorization and behavioral templates in rats. J. Vis. 19:9. doi: 10.1167/19.14.9

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Schultz, R. T., Gauthier, I., Klin, A., Fulbright, R. K., Anderson, A. W., Volkmar, F., et al. (2000). Abnormal ventral temporal cortical activity during face discrimination among individuals with autism and Asperger syndrome. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 57, 331–340. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.57.4.331

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Schultz, R. T., Grelotti, D. J., Klin, A., Kleinman, J., der Gaag, C. V., Marois, R., et al. (2003). The role of the fusiform face area in social cognition: implications for the pathobiology of autism. Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B Biol. Sci. 358, 415–427. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2002.1208

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Schultz, R. T., and Klin, A. (2002). Genetics of childhood disorders: XLIII. Autism, part 2: neural foundations. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 41, 1259–1262. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200210000-00018

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sgadò, P., Rosa-Salva, O., Versace, E., and Vallortigara, G. (2018). Embryonic exposure to Valproic acid impairs social predispositions of newly-hatched chicks. Sci. Rep. 8:5919. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-24202-8

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Shephard, E., Milosavljevic, B., Mason, L., Elsabbagh, M., Tye, C., Gliga, T., et al. (2020). Neural and behavioural indices of face processing in siblings of children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD): a longitudinal study from infancy to mid-childhood. Cortex 127, 162–179. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2020.02.008

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sid, H., and Schusser, B. (2018). Applications of gene editing in chickens: a new era is on the horizon. Front. Genet. 9:456. doi: 10.3389/fgene.2018.00456

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Simion, F., and Di Giorgio, E. (2015). Face perception and processing in early infancy: inborn predispositions and developmental changes. Front. Psychol. 6:969. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00969

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Simpson, E. A., Jakobsen, K. V., Damon, F., Suomi, S. J., Ferrari, P. F., and Paukner, A. (2017). Face detection and the development of own-species Bias in infant macaques. Child Dev. 88, 103–113. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12565

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sugita, Y. (2008). Face perception in monkeys reared with no exposure to faces. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 105, 394–398. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0706079105

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sugita, Y. (2009). Innate face processing. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 19, 39–44. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2009.03.001

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Suwandschieff, E., Mundry, R., Kull, K., Kreuzer, L., and Schwing, R. (2023). ‘Do I know you?’ Categorizing individuals on the basis of familiarity in kea (Nestor notabilis). R. Soc. Open Sci. 10:230228. doi: 10.1098/rsos.230228

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Thompson, P. (1980). Margaret Thatcher: a new illusion. Perception 9, 483–484. doi: 10.1068/p090483

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tibbetts, E. A., Pardo-Sanchez, J., Ramirez-Matias, J., and Avarguès-Weber, A. (2021). Individual recognition is associated with holistic face processing in Polistes paper wasps in a species-specific way. Proc. R. Soc. B 288:20203010. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2020.3010

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tong, F., Nakayama, K., Moscovitch, M., Weinrib, O., and Kanwisher, N. (2000). Response properties of the human fusiform face area. Cogn. Neuropsychol. 17, 257–280. doi: 10.1080/026432900380607

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Towler, A., Kemp, R. I., Bruce, V., Burton, A. M., Dunn, J. D., and White, D. (2019). Are face recognition abilities in humans and sheep really ‘comparable’? R. Soc. Open Sci. 6:180772. doi: 10.1098/rsos.180772

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tsao, D. Y., and Livingstone, M. S. (2008). Mechanisms of face perception. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 31, 411–437. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.30.051606.094238

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tsao, D. Y., Moeller, S., and Freiwald, W. A. (2008). Comparing face patch systems in macaques and humans. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 105, 19514–19519. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0809662105

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Turati, C., Simion, F., Milani, I., and Umiltà, C. (2002). Newborns’ preference for faces: what is crucial? Dev. Psychol. 38, 875–882. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.38.6.875

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tye, C., Bussu, G., Gliga, T., Elsabbagh, M., Pasco, G., Johnsen, K., et al. (2022). Understanding the nature of face processing in early autism: a prospective study. J. Psychopathol. Clin. Sci. 131, 542–555. doi: 10.1037/abn0000648

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Uljarevic, M., and Hamilton, A. (2013). Recognition of emotions in autism: a formal Meta-analysis. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 43, 1517–1526. doi: 10.1007/s10803-012-1695-5

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Valenza, E., Simion, F., Cassia, V. M., and Umiltà, C. (1996). Face preference at birth. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 22, 892–903. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.22.4.892

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Vallortigara, G., Regolin, L., and Marconato, F. (2005). Visually inexperienced chicks exhibit spontaneous preference for biological motion patterns. PLoS Biol. 3:e208. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0030208

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Versace, E., Damini, S., and Stancher, G. (2020). Early preference for face-like stimuli in solitary species as revealed by tortoise hatchlings. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 117, 24047–24049. doi: 10.1073/pnas.2011453117

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wagner, J. B., Luyster, R. J., Yim, J. Y., Tager-Flusberg, H., and Nelson, C. A. (2013). The role of early visual attention in social development. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 37, 118–124. doi: 10.1177/0165025412468064

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, Y., Cao, R., Chakravarthula, P. N., Yu, H., and Wang, S. (2024). Atypical neural encoding of faces in individuals with autism spectrum disorder. Cereb. Cortex 34, 172–186. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhae060

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, S., and Li, X. (2023). A revisit of the amygdala theory of autism: twenty years after. Neuropsychologia 183:108519. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2023.108519

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, M.-Y., and Takeuchi, H. (2017). Individual recognition and the ‘face inversion effect’ in medaka fish (Oryzias latipes). eLife 6:e24728. doi: 10.7554/elife.24728

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Watanabe, S., Masuda, S., Shinozuka, K., and Borlongan, C. (2022). Preference and discrimination of facial expressions of humans, rats, and mice by C57 mice. Anim. Cogn. 25, 297–306. doi: 10.1007/s10071-021-01551-y

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Watson, K. K., and Platt, M. L. (2012). Of mice and monkeys: using non-human primate models to bridge mouse-and human-based investigations of autism spectrum disorders. J. Neurodev. Disord. 4:21. doi: 10.1186/1866-1955-4-21

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Webb, S., Neuhaus, E., and Faja, S. (2017). Face perception and learning in autism spectrum disorders. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 70, 970–986. doi: 10.1080/17470218.2016.1151059

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Weigelt, S., Koldewyn, K., and Kanwisher, N. (2012). Face identity recognition in autism spectrum disorders: a review of behavioral studies. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 36, 1060–1084. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.12.008

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Yovel, G., and Kanwisher, N. (2005). The neural basis of the behavioral face-inversion effect. Curr. Biol. 15, 2256–2262. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2005.10.072

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zangenehpour, S., and Chaudhuri, A. (2005). Patchy organization and asymmetric distribution of the neural correlates of face processing in monkey Inferotemporal cortex. Curr. Biol. 15, 993–1005. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2005.04.031

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zanon, M., Lemaire, B. S., Papeo, L., and Vallortigara, G. (2024). Innate sensitivity to face-to-face biological motion. iScience 27:108793. doi: 10.1016/j.isci.2024.108793

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zarate-Lopez, D., Torres-Chávez, A. L., Gálvez-Contreras, A. Y., and Gonzalez-Perez, O. (2024). Three decades of valproate: a current model for studying autism Spectrum disorder. Curr. Neuropharmacol. 22, 260–289. doi: 10.2174/1570159x22666231003121513

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhao, H., Jiang, Y., and Zhang, Y. Q. (2018). Modeling autism in non-human primates: opportunities and challenges. Autism Res. 11, 686–694. doi: 10.1002/aur.1945

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhao, H., Wang, Q., Yan, T., Zhang, Y., Xu, H., Yu, H., et al. (2019). Maternal valproic acid exposure leads to neurogenesis defects and autism-like behaviors in non-human primates. Transl. Psychiatry 9:267. doi: 10.1038/s41398-019-0608-1

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhou, Y., Sharma, J., Ke, Q., Landman, R., Yuan, J., Chen, H., et al. (2019). Atypical behaviour and connectivity in SHANK3-mutant macaques. Nature 570, 326–331. doi: 10.1038/s41586-019-1278-0

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: face detection, social orienting, domestic chick, neurodevelopmental disorders, valproic acid

Citation: Sgadò P, Pross A, Lamanna J and Adiletta A (2024) Face processing in animal models: implications for autism spectrum disorder. Front. Neurosci. 18:1462272. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2024.1462272

Received: 09 July 2024; Accepted: 29 July 2024;
Published: 09 August 2024.

Edited by:

Ichiro Fujita, Osaka University, Japan

Reviewed by:

Luca Tommasi, University of Studies G. d’Annunzio Chieti and Pescara, Italy

Copyright © 2024 Sgadò, Pross, Lamanna and Adiletta. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Paola Sgadò, paola.sgado@unitn.it; Alice Adiletta, alice.adiletta@unitn.it

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.