Skip to main content

REVIEW article

Front. Neurosci., 29 August 2024
Sec. Sleep and Circadian Rhythms
This article is part of the Research Topic Sex Differences in Sleep and Circadian Rhythms View all 4 articles

On the basis of sex and sleep: the influence of the estrous cycle and sex on sleep-wake behavior

  • Medical Readiness Systems Biology Branch, Walter Reed Army Institute of Research, Silver Spring, MD, United States

The recurrent hormonal fluctuations within reproductive cycles impact sleep-wake behavior in women and in rats and mice used in preclinical models of sleep research. Strides have been made in sleep-related clinical trials to include equal numbers of women; however, the inclusion of female rodents in neuroscience and sleep research is lacking. Female animals are commonly omitted from studies over concerns of the effect of estrus cycle hormones on measured outcomes. This review highlights the estrous cycle’s broad effects on sleep-wake behavior: from changes in sleep macroarchitecture to regionally specific alterations in neural oscillations. These changes are largely driven by cycle-dependent ovarian hormonal fluctuations occurring during proestrus and estrus that modulate neural circuits regulating sleep-wake behavior. Removal of estrous cycle influence by ovariectomy ablates characteristic sleep changes. Further, sex differences in sleep are present between gonadally intact females and males. Removal of reproductive hormones via gonadectomy in both sexes mitigates some, but not all sex differences. We examine the extent to which reproductive hormones and sex chromosomes contribute to sex differences in sleep-wake behavior. Finally, this review addresses the limitations in our understanding of the estrous cycle’s impact on sleep-wake behavior, gaps in female sleep research that are well studied in males, and the implications that ignoring the estrous cycle has on studies of sleep-related processes.

The estrous cycle

The estrous cycle is a recurrent pattern of hormonal changes that mediate fertility in several species of mammals. The recurrent cyclical progression through the phases of the estrous cycle is regulated by hormonal release from the hypothalamic–pituitary-gonadal (HPG) axis (Figure 1). Gonadotropin-releasing hormone (GnRH) neurons in the hypothalamus serve as a central common pathway for reproductive regulation in both females and males (Christian and Moenter, 2010). In females, pulsatile release of GnRH stimulates the release of luteinizing hormone (LH) and follicle stimulating hormone (FSH) from the anterior pituitary (Marshall and Griffin, 1993). LH and FSH regulate ovarian follicle maturation and the production of ovarian steroids, estradiol and progesterone. These ovarian steroids feedback on kisspeptin neurons, the upstream regulators of GnRH neurons, modulating GnRH neuron activity and controlling GnRH release (Christian and Moenter, 2010).

Figure 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Schematic of the hypothalamic–pituitary-gonadal axis activity across the rat and the mouse estrous cycle over a four-day (96 h) period. Each day is composed of a 12-h light 12-h dark cycle with ZT0 representing lights on and ZT12 representing lights off. While cycle phases are displayed as equal in length occurring over a single 24-h (12-light, 12-dark) period, actual phase lengths vary (Paccola et al., 2013; Ajayi and Akhigbe, 2020). Further phase onset and offset is not locked to transitions between light and dark. During metestrus and diestrus, infrequent GnRH pulses cause FSH and low levels of LH release from the anterior pituitary. During proestrus, elevated estradiol increases GnRH pulse frequency, triggering a surge of LH release inducing ovulation and decreasing in estradiol. Elevated progesterone feedback to kisspeptin (Kiss) neurons which modulate GnRH pulses, decreasing their frequency. Relative hormone levels for rats taken from Scharfman and MacLusky (2006) and Smith et al. (1975) and for mice from McLean et al. (2012).

In female rats and mice the estrous cycle is approximately 4–5 days in length and is divided into four phases: metestrus, diestrus, proestrus, and estrus (Miller and Takahashi, 2014). During metestrus and diestrus, infrequent GnRH pulses produce FSH and low levels of LH. FSH increases ovarian follicle maturation resulting in increased release of estradiol from ovarian follicles. During proestrus, elevated estradiol increases GnRH pulse frequency, triggering a surge of LH release (Park and Ramirez, 1989; Sarkar et al., 1976). Notably, increases in GnRH pulse frequency occur during the later portion of the light phase of proestrus with the LH surge taking place within the hours adjacent to the transition to the dark phase (Chappell, 2005; Miller et al., 2004) (Figure 1). In the ovary, this LH surge induces ovulation, the release of the oocyte from the primary follicle, 10–12 h post-LH surge (Paccola et al., 2013) and is marked by a rapid decrease in estradiol. The remnants of the primary ovarian follicle become the corpus luteum, which releases progesterone and prepares the uterus for implantation should fertilization occur (Niswender, 2002; Niswender et al., 1994). Peak fertility occurs directly following ovulation and during estrus. If fertilization and pregnancy does not occur during estrus, the corpus luteum breaks down, progesterone levels decrease, and the cycle begins anew. One key difference between mice and rats is the release pattern of progesterone across the estrous cycle. While rats display an increase in progesterone following the LH surge, progesterone peaks earlier in mice during diestrus (Wood et al., 2007; McLean et al., 2012) (Figure 1).

While estrous cycle phases are commonly diagramed as being equal in length, phase length varies within and across species. Rat estrous cycle phase lengths are approximately: metestrus 6–21 h, diestrus 48–72 h, proestrus 12–14 h, and estrus 12–48 h (Paccola et al., 2013; Ajayi and Akhigbe, 2020). Meanwhile, mice estrous cycle lengths are approximately: metestrus 2–24 h, diestrus 48–72 h, proestrus less than 24 h, and estrus 12–48 h (Ajayi and Akhigbe, 2020). Common methods for determining estrous cycle in mice and rats are outlined in Byers et al. (2012) and Ajayi and Akhigbe (2020).

The estrous cycle alters sleep-wake behavior

Electroencephalographic recordings in the 1960’s by Sawyer and colleagues identified changes in sleep-wake behavior across the estrous cycle in female Sprague–Dawley rats (Colvin et al., 1968; Yokoyama et al., 1965). During the dark/active phase of proestrus, female rats displayed reduced amounts of non-rapid eye movement (NREM) sleep and rapid eye movement (REM) sleep. Decreased sleep was accompanied by prolonged periods of wakefulness with enhanced arousal behavior and locomotor activity compared to analogous periods in other estrous phases. These sleep-wake changes coincided with estrous phases where increases in LH and FSH occurred—suggesting that reproductive hormones modulate sleep-wake rhythms (Colvin et al., 1968). Over 50 years later, the suppression of NREM sleep and REM sleep during proestrus has been replicated in several rat strains (Fang and Fishbein, 1996; Schwierin et al., 1998; Zhang et al., 1995; Hadjimarkou et al., 2008; Swift et al., 2019; Tóth et al., 2024; Kostin et al., 2020). During the dark phase of proestrus, the length and frequency of REM sleep bouts is decreased, and REM sleep is disproportionately suppressed compared to NREM sleep. Following proestrus, some studies have identified a rebound in either NREM sleep, REM sleep, or both during the subsequent light phase of estrus (Yokoyama et al., 1965; Schwierin et al., 1998; Swift et al., 2019). This rebound sleep is more consolidated with longer bouts of NREM and REM sleep and decreased NREM bout frequency (Swift et al., 2019). Sleep changes have also been reported during metestrus and diestrus, but broadly, proestrus and estrus have the largest impact on sleep in rats (Figure 2).

Figure 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Summary figure outlining broad changes in sleep-wake behavior, sleep architecture, and sleep neural oscillations across the estrous cycle in mice and rats. The estrous cycle is displayed across a four-day period with each day encapsulating a different phase. Subsequent panels represent the effect of ovariectomy (OVX) and OVX with hormone supplementation over the course of day. Each phase has a light and a dark phase. Changes displayed in light or dark designate when they took place. Changes measured across a 24-h period rather than solely a light or a dark phase are labeled as such. E2 designated estradiol; P4 designates progesterone. *Strain specific effects in mice (Koehl et al., 2003).

The impact of the estrous cycle on sleep in mice is less defined. Early research established that sleep in mice is largely influenced by genetic background (Valatax et al., 1972). Similarly, a mouse’s genetic background determines the extent to which sleep is affected by the estrous cycle. A seminal study by Koehl et al. (2003) compared sleep in Balb/cJ, C57BL/6J, and C3H/HeJ mice across the estrous cycle. Similar to rats, C57BL/6 J mice had decreased REM sleep during the dark phase of proestrus compared to diestrus but did not show a sleep rebound during estrus. C3H/HeJ mice displayed a rebound in NREM sleep during the light phase of estrus, but also had decreased REM sleep during the light phase of diestrus. Sleep-wake behavior in Balb/cJ mice was widely unaffected by the estrous cycle. A study of orexin knock-out C57BL/6 J used to study cataplexy found that the estrous cycle had no impact on sleep in female mice (Arthaud et al., 2022). Altogether, the estrous cycle has mixed effects on sleep in mice and these effects are highly dependent on strain (Figure 2).

Ovarian hormones produce cycle-specific changes in sleep

Early work hypothesized that ovarian release of estradiol and progesterone is necessary for the phase-dependent sleep changes during proestrus and estrus. Removing hormonal influence via ovariectomy (OVX) largely ablates the characteristic NREM and REM sleep changes seen within intact cycling females (Kleinlogel, 1983; Choi et al., 2021). However, OVX produces mixed effects on sleep-waking behavior. An initial study found that OVX rats had less REM sleep than intact female rats during estrus and diestrus (Yokoyama et al., 1965). In contrast, subsequent studies found that OVX rats had increased REM sleep during the dark phase compared to intact females (Fang and Fishbein, 1996; Kleinlogel, 1983; Li and Satinoff, 1996). One study found that OVX increased the total sleep time over a 24-h period—specifically by increasing dark phase NREM and REM sleep while decreasing waking (Colvin et al., 1969). A separate study found that OVX rats had increased NREM sleep in the light phase and less NREM in the dark phase alongside increased REM sleep during both light and dark phases (Li and Satinoff, 1996). Studies of OVX mice display decrease waking and increased NREM sleep across 24 h (Choi et al., 2021; Paul et al., 2006).

Reintroducing estradiol and progesterone into OVX animals produce sleep changes similar to those seen in proestrus and estrus (Figure 2). Further, the changes in sleep resulting from the reintroduction of ovarian hormones are more consistent than the changes following OVX. Administering estradiol to OVX rats and mice reduces REM sleep, NREM sleep, and increases waking (Choi et al., 2021; Colvin et al., 1969; Matsushima and Takeichi, 1990; Cusmano et al., 2014; Deurveilher et al., 2009, 2011; Paul et al., 2009; Schwartz and Mong, 2013; Schwartz and Mong, 2011; Pawlyk et al., 2008a,b). Moreover, these REM sleep and waking changes were most notable during the dark phase (Schwartz and Mong, 2013; Schwartz and Mong, 2011; Pawlyk et al., 2008b; Matsushima and Takeichi, 1990). Reintroducing progesterone also suppresses REM sleep during the dark phase (Deurveilher et al., 2009). The administration of estradiol and progesterone together produces an equal or synergistically greater suppression of REM and NREM sleep than estradiol alone (Deurveilher et al., 2009; Branchey et al., 1971). Taken together this suggests that elevated levels of estradiol and progesterone during proestrus are largely responsible for the suppression of sleep that normally occurs during this time.

Reproductive hormones also contribute to the development of sleep-wake circuitry in both males and females. Administering estradiol and progesterone to adult male rats that were castrated as neonates suppresses dark phase NREM and REM sleep similar to females (Branchey et al., 1973). This sleep suppression by administering ovarian hormones does not occur in males castrated as adults suggesting that the development of sleep-wake circuitry is influenced by perinatal reproductive hormones. Another study exposed neonatal females to masculinizing levels of testosterone and examined their sleep as adults (Cusmano et al., 2014). Masculinized adult females displayed reduced expression of neural activity marker c-fos in sleep-active neurons in ventral lateral preoptic area (VLPO) compared to control females (Cusmano et al., 2014). Administering ovarian hormones to these masculinized females also did not suppress sleep. Moreover, administering estradiol to females that were ovariectomized just prior to puberty or post-puberty as adults yields similar effects on sleep—suggesting that the effect of estradiol on sleep circuitry organization is specific to early development rather than puberty (Cusmano et al., 2014). As such, reproductive hormones help orchestrate the organization of sleep-wake circuitry during early development and are necessary for sleep’s modulation by reproductive hormones during puberty and adulthood.

Mechanisms of sleep modulation by ovarian hormones

Current evidence points to several different mechanisms by which ovarian hormones regulate sleep-waking behavior. First, elevated levels of estradiol promote wakefulness by suppressing activity in sleep-promoting brain regions. Work by Mong and colleagues has demonstrated that estradiol decreases activity in sleep-active neural population in the VLPO and median preoptic nucleus (MnPN) (Mong et al., 2011; Mong and Cusmano, 2016). Administration of estradiol in OVX rats decreases c-fos expression in VLPO neurons (Hadjimarkou et al., 2008; Deurveilher et al., 2008). Additionally, estradiol suppresses sleep-promoting prostaglandin D2. This suppression is driven by estradiol decreasing both prostaglandin D2 mRNA and the enzyme (lipcaline-type prostaglandin D synthase) responsible for its synthesis in VLPO neurons (Hadjimarkou et al., 2008; Mong et al., 2003; Urade and Hayaishi, 2011; Ueno et al., 1982; Mong et al., 2003). The suppression of REM sleep specifically by estradiol is partially due to its action in the MnPN. Blocking estrogen receptors in the MnPN prevents the estradiol-induced suppression of REM sleep (Mong and Cusmano, 2016; Cusmano et al., 2011). Estradiol also promotes wakefulness by enhancing activity in arousal centers. Administering estradiol increases c-fos expression in the tuberomammillary nucleus (TMN) arousal center (Hadjimarkou et al., 2008). As the VLPO and the TMN have reciprocal inhibitory connections (Saper et al., 2011), an increase in TMN neuron activity inhibits sleep-promoting VLPO neurons. Thus, estradiol promotes arousal by inhibiting sleep-promoting neurons and activating neurons in wake-promoting regions (Hadjimarkou et al., 2008).

The mechanisms by which progesterone modulate sleep are less clear. Progesterone has sleep inducing effects in rats and humans (Lancel et al., 1997). These effects are caused by progesterone’s neuroactive metabolites, allopregnanolone and pregnenolone, acting as positive allosteric modulators of the GABAA receptor, rather than progesterone acting on its endogenous receptor (Lancel et al., 1996; Lancel, 1999). This action is similar to that of benzodiazepines and is hypothesized to be the mechanism by which progesterone promotes sleep. Administering progesterone, allopregnanolone, or pregnanolone in male rats decreases NREM sleep latency (Lancel et al., 1996, 1997; Edgar et al., 1997). In contrast, administering progesterone to female rats, particularly with or following daily doses of estradiol, promotes wakefulness and suppresses sleep (Deurveilher et al., 2009; Branchey et al., 1971). The impact of progesterone’s influence on sleep when administered with estradiol is confounding, but perhaps necessary. Progesterone receptor expression is dependent on estrogen receptor gene expression (Quadros and Wagner, 2008); i.e., the absence of estradiol decreases progesterone receptor expression. This complicates studies that reintroduce solely progesterone in OVX animals and see no effect on sleep as progesterone receptor density is likely diminished without estradiol. Another consideration is the release pattern of progesterone between mice and rats. In rats, estradiol and progesterone release is largely concurrent during proestrus, whereas mice display maximal progesterone release earlier during diestrus (McLean et al., 2012; Scharfman and MacLusky, 2006). This separate release of estradiol and progesterone in mice allows a more granular evaluation of the impact of ovarian hormones and sleep-wake activity. However, it may also contribute to more variation on how sleep changes across the estrous cycle in mice.

A separate hypothesis suggests that progesterone suppresses sleep by increasing body temperature (Boivin and Shechter, 2010). Progesterone decreases neural activity in preoptic heat-sensitive neurons and increases activity in preoptic cold-sensing neurons, resulting in a net upward shift in the thermoregulatory set point and an overall increase in body temperature (Nakayama et al., 1975). Elevated levels of endogenous progesterone during the estrous cycle and the menstrual cycle increase body temperature (Marrone et al., 1976; Driver et al., 1996). Administration of exogenous progesterone elicits similar effects (Marrone et al., 1976; Driver et al., 1996; Stachenfeld et al., 2000). Increases in body temperature are associated with sleep disruption, REM sleep suppression, and insomnia (Lack et al., 2008). Women report the highest incidence of sleep disturbances during the luteal phase preceding menstruation when progesterone levels peak and core body temperature is elevated (Manber and Bootzin, 1997). However, the effect of progesterone on human sleep is also mixed. Administration of progesterone decreases sleep disturbances in postmenopausal women and increases NREM sleep in men (Caufriez et al., 2011; Friess et al., 1997; Schüssler et al., 2008; Wiedemann et al., 2017). However, progesterone receptor agonist megestrol acetate decreases REM sleep in men in an inverted U-shaped manner, i.e., high and low doses suppress REM to a lesser magnitude than medium doses (Wiedemann et al., 1998). Future work is necessary to better understand the mechanisms by which progesterone alters sleep.

Finally, ovarian hormones may also influence sleep by altering neuromodulatory output from arousal centers. The cholinergic basal forebrain, dopaminergic ventral tegmental area, noradrenergic locus coeruleus, serotonergic dorsal raphe, and the orexinergic lateral hypothalamus all modulate sleep-wake behavior. Moreover, optogenetic activation of these brain regions promotes wakefulness and prevents sleep (Carter et al., 2010; Zant et al., 2016; Eban-Rothschild et al., 2016; Moriya et al., 2021; De Luca et al., 2022).

Progesterone and estradiol receptors are expressed within these neuromodulator centers (Mong and Cusmano, 2016; Shughrue et al., 1997; Curran-Rauhut and Petersen, 2002) and their receptor density can fluctuate across the estrous cycle (Haywood et al., 1999). These fluctuations in receptor density, and ovarian hormones, likely impact neuromodulator tone. Furthermore, these neuromodulator centers also regulate mating behavior and sexual receptivity (Uphouse, 2014; Melis and Argiolas, 1995; Clemens et al., 1989; Muschamp et al., 2007; Etgen and Morales, 2002). Although the interplay of neuromodulators is more complex in mating behavior, maximal sexual receptivity in females occurs following ovulation during the night of proestrus where longer bouts of wake are common (Powers, 1970). It is likely that elevated neuromodulatory tone promotes both the increased wakefulness and sexual receptivity—potentially to maximize mating opportunities. However, there is a paucity of studies recording electrical activity or calcium transients from these arousal centers throughout the estrous cycle and further studies are necessary to determine how they are influenced by ovarian hormones.

The effect of prolactin on sleep

Prolactin is a peptide hormone released from the anterior pituitary which is responsible for regulating lactation in mammals (Bachelot and Binart, 2007). Prolactin release changes across the estrous cycle with levels remaining low through metestrus and diestrus, but increase dramatically during proestrus and remain elevated through estrus, particularly in mice (McLean et al., 2012; Freeman et al., 2000). In mice, prolactin release displays a brief pre-ovulatory increase before reaching its highest levels during estrus (McLean et al., 2012). In rats, peak prolactin release coincides with the LH surge during proestrus. Rats prolactin levels also display a diurnal rhythm with increased release during sleep (Sassin et al., 1972; Bethea and Neili, 1979). Moreover, the increased release of prolactin during sleep is dependent on sleep state rather than the time of day (Sassin et al., 1973). Prolactin levels are highest during NREM sleep, and decrease during REM sleep (Parker et al., 1974) due to modulation by hypothalamic dopamine (Freeman et al., 2000). Low dopamine during NREM sleep allows prolactin release while increased dopamine during REM sleep inhibits release (Spiegel et al., 1994). Despite prolactin’s release pattern during sleep and across the estrous cycle, little is known about the interplay between prolactin, sleep, and the estrous cycle phase. In rats, prolactin levels are higher during the dark phase than the light phase (Dunn et al., 1976; Laakso et al., 1988; Roky et al., 1995; Wong et al., 1983). Injections of prolactin during the light phase increase the amount of REM sleep, whereas administration during the dark phase increases waking and suppresses REM sleep—specifically by decreasing the number of REM sleep bouts (Roky et al., 1993, 1994). This REM sleep suppression is notable as it suggests that elevated prolactin may contribute to the decrease in REM sleep during dark phase of proestrus. However, to our knowledge, there is no study examining the effect of exogenous prolactin on sleep in either in gonadally-intact female rats or mice across the estrous cycle or in OVX animals.

Estrous cycle & sleep neural oscillations

Delta waves or slow waves are high amplitude 0.5–4 Hz oscillatory activity which occur within NREM sleep as the result of the simultaneous activation and subsequent inactivation of large neural populations. Slow waves are functionally important for sleep-dependent memory consolidation, cerebrospinal fluid circulation, and metabolite clearance in the brain during sleep (Xie et al., 2013; Huber et al., 2004; Fattinger et al., 2017; Boespflug and Iliff, 2018). Although studies in women have found no difference in NREM slow wave power or slow wave activity (SWA) across the menstrual cycle (Driver et al., 1996; Baker and Lee, 2018; Driver et al., 2008), NREM SWA does change across the estrous cycle in rats. A study of female rats found SWA was decreased during proestrus compared to other estrous phases (Schwierin et al., 1998), with SWA activity reaching the lowest level during the transitions period from the light to the dark phase. In contrast, another study found that SWA activity was increased in multiple brain regions during the dark phase of proestrus compared to all other estrous phases (Swift et al., 2019). However, both studies identified increased NREM delta power during the light phase of estrus. In mice there was no effect of estrous cycle phase or mouse strain (Balb/cJ, C57BL/6 J, C3H/HeJ) on NREM SWA (Koehl et al., 2003).

Hippocampal ripples are transient (~100 msec) periods of 100–250 Hz high frequency oscillatory activity that result from the activation of hippocampal pyramidal neurons primarily during NREM sleep and waking inactivity (Wilson and Mcnaughton, 1994; Roux et al., 2017; Jadhav et al., 2012). During sleep, ripples are important for the replay and stabilization of hippocampal memories encoded during waking (Girardeau et al., 2009; Gridchyn et al., 2020). Our understanding of hippocampal ripples in humans comes from studies of seizure patients implanted with focal electrodes (Wixted et al., 2014, 2018; Staresina et al., 2015); however, none of these studies have compared differences in ripple density or frequency between men and women. Numerous studies have recorded ripples from rodent hippocampi, but to our knowledge, no study exists comparing ripple characteristics in rats or mice between sexes or within females across the estrous cycle. One factor that could impact ripples is adenosine; activation of adenosine receptors decreases ripple incidence (Ortiz and Gutiérrez, 2015; Jarosch et al., 2015). Female rats display increased CA1 adenosine concentrations during proestrus compared to other cycle phases and males (Borgus et al., 2020), which could cause cycle-dependent variations in ripple incidence. Additionally, there is substantial evidence that ovarian hormones alter memory function and hippocampal cell morphology. Changes in estradiol and progesterone levels across the estrous cycle alter CA1 synaptic function and spine density (Woolley and McEwen, 1993; Woods and McEwen, 1992) and between sexes, estradiol acts differentially to potentiate synapses in males versus females (Oberlander and Woolley, 2017; Jain et al., 2019). Hippocampal memory function also changes across the estrous cycle with elevated levels of estradiol associated with enhanced memory performance (Luine, 2014). As ovarian hormones have broad and differing effects on hippocampal processes, future work should consider ripples and memory replay when examining sex differences in hippocampal function.

Sleep spindles are transient (0.5–2 s) of 10–15 Hz neural oscillatory activity that wax and wane in amplitude. Sleep spindles are generated during NREM sleep by reciprocal inter- and intra-circuit connections between thalamic and cortical neural circuits (Steriade et al., 1993; Clawson et al., 2016). In humans, rodents, and other mammals, spindles have been implicated in the consolidation of memories (Iotchev et al., 2017; Swift et al., 2018; Fogel and Smith, 2006; Mednick et al., 2013); either directly or via the coupling of spindles with NREM slow waves and hippocampal ripples (Siapas and Wilson, 1998; Wierzynski et al., 2009; Maingret et al., 2016). Women have a higher sleep spindle density (spindles/min) than men, with a greater percentage of spindles occurring in the left frontal cortex (Gaillard and Blois, 1981; Huupponen et al., 2002). Additionally, spindle density, length, frequency of spindle oscillations, and their spectral power change across the menstrual cycle (Driver et al., 1996; Ishizuka et al., 1994; De Zambotti et al., 2015). In female rats, spindle density increases during the light phase of proestrus compared to diestrus and estrus and the frequency of spindle oscillations peak during the light phase of proestrus and metestrus (Swift et al., 2019). Changes in spindles across the estrous cycle may be partially attributed to fluctuations in progesterone. Administration of progesterone and allopregnanolone in rats increases NREM sleep spindle power and the amount of intermediate or pre-REM sleep—a spindle-rich state that often precedes REM sleep in rats (Lancel et al., 1996, 1997). However, future work is necessary to confirm the potential impact of reproductive hormones on sleep spindles.

The beta (16–30 Hz) and gamma (30–100 Hz) bands are less commonly studied during sleep as they are often associated with waking or insomnia (Perlis et al., 2001). Menstrual cycle phase has a mild effect on NREM sleep beta with higher beta power during the luteal phase compared to the follicular phase (Driver et al., 1996). This study did not examine frequencies greater than 25 Hz, so it is unclear whether the menstrual cycle alters sleeping gamma power. However, NREM beta and gamma power are affected by the estrous cycle (Schwierin et al., 1998; Swift et al., 2019) with increases in both frequency bands during the dark phase of proestrus in rats. In mice, REM sleep gamma (30–120 Hz) is increased during estrus compared to diestrus (Barth et al., 2014). The increase in higher frequency bands (>16 Hz) during sleep may be partially attributed to elevated levels of progesterone. Administration of progesterone and allopregnanolone in rats increase 16–40 Hz spectral power during NREM and REM sleep (Lancel et al., 1996, 1997). Further, progesterone increases NREM beta power in men (Friess et al., 1997). The functional significance of increases in the beta and gamma bands during the dark phase of proestrus remains unknown but may represented increased waking pressure, potentially to maximize reproductive opportunities following ovulation.

REM sleep theta (5–9 Hz) emanates from the hippocampus and is generated by cholinergic and parvalbumin interneuron projections from the medial septum to the hippocampus (Ognjanovski et al., 2017; Simon et al., 2006). Frequency power within the REM sleep theta band varies across the estrous cycle phase (Schwierin et al., 1998). However, changes in REM theta were of small magnitude, regionally-specific, and occurred at isolated times within the cycle (Swift et al., 2019). The impact of OVX and hormone replacement on REM sleep theta is mixed. One study found that neither ovariectomy nor administering estradiol, progesterone, or both to OVX rats affected REM sleep theta power (Deurveilher et al., 2011). In contrast, four days of estradiol treatment reduced REM sleep theta power in OVX rats during the dark phase (Pawlyk et al., 2008a). Another study found that administering progesterone to male rats reduced 7–8 Hz theta in a dose dependent manner (Lancel et al., 1996). Together these studies demonstrate that while ovarian hormones may affect REM sleep theta further work is necessary to understand their relationship.

Sleep homeostasis and the estrous cycle

The homeostatic rebound in sleep amount and SWA following extended periods of wake is well documented in rodents and humans (Choi et al., 2021; Borbely and Achermann, 1999). SWA rebound following sleep deprivation is increased in women compared to men (Armitage and Hoffmann, 2001; Hajali et al., 2019). To our knowledge, no study has examined SWA rebound following sleep deprivation during specific menstrual phases; although different phases of the menstrual cycle are more prone to sleep disturbances (Manber and Bootzin, 1997). In mice, females show a greater rebound in NREM sleep following sleep deprivation compared to males (Paul et al., 2006). However, gonadectomy (GDX) attenuates the increased NREM sleep rebound when comparing GDX males and GDX females. Four days of REM sleep deprivation produces REM rebound in male and in female rats regardless of estrous phase. However, females entering proestrus following REM deprivation were the slowest to recover as demonstrated by prolonged period of REM sleep rebound (Andersen et al., 2008). It is likely that rats undergoing REM recovery during proestrus had prolonged recovery due to competing mechanisms: REM rebound following deprivation and REM suppression commonly seen during proestrus. In contrast, the SWA response to sleep deprivation is largely unaffected by the estrous cycle. Sleep deprivation during the light phase of proestrus or estrus produces a similar SWA response (Schwierin et al., 1998). However, ovarian hormones themselves can alter the response to sleep deprivation. Treating OVX females with estradiol, progesterone or a combination attenuates the SWA response to sleep deprivation (Deurveilher et al., 2009). All treatment groups displayed normal NREM and REM sleep rebound, but OVX rats treated with solely estradiol had a greater relative increase in REM sleep recovery.

Circadian influences on the estrous cycle

Light–dark (LD) cycles serve as cues which entrain molecular biological pacemakers. While all tissues contain pacemaker activity, the suprachiasmatic nuclei (SCN) in the hypothalamus is broadly the master pacemaker (Miller and Takahashi, 2014; Mohawk et al., 2012). Within the estrous cycle, both the release of ovarian hormones and proper SCN circadian signaling are necessary to elicit GnRH surges triggering ovulation. Lesioning the SCN abolishes circadian release of GnRH and produces aovulatory cycles (Wiegand and Terasawa, 1982). Ablation of ipsilateral SCN inputs to kisspeptin expressing neurons, the upstream regulators of GnRH neuron activity, decreases c-fos expression in GnRH neurons (Smarr et al., 2012). Further, bursts of GnRH neural activity which trigger the LH surge are under circadian regulation. In gonadally-intact females, LH surges are largely confined to the end of the light phase of proestrus (Miller and Takahashi, 2014; Chappell, 2005). Treating OVX female rats chronically with estradiol produces LH surges on multiple days, but notably, the surges are restricted to the end of the light phase (Legan and Karsch, 1975). Even altering the LD cycle does not greatly change the time within a day when LH surges occur. Animals housed under different LD cycles (e.g., L:D 12:12, 16:8) display LH surges at similar times in relation to the increased activity occurring at dark phase onset—regardless of the length of the LD cycle (Christian and Moenter, 2010; Moline et al., 1981). Moreover, ovulation occurs at similar times within the day regardless of whether animals are kept on a LD cycle, constant light, or constant dark (Mccormack and Sridaran, 1977).

Circadian rhythms also play a role in fertility and pregnancy. Circadian disruption can detrimentally affect reproductive health, particularly in women where it can cause irregular menstrual cycle and decreased fertility (Lawson et al., 2011; Wang et al., 2016; Lawson et al., 2015; Mínguez-Alarcón et al., 2017). Circadian disruption also affects the estrous cycle in rats and mice (Miller and Takahashi, 2014). Rotating LD cycles every 3, 6, or 12 days to model shift work increases the estrous cycle length in mice (Yoshinaka et al., 2017). Further, the magnitude of cycle disruption and the proportion of mice with disrupted cycles is dependent on the frequency of LD reversals. Mutations to the Clock gene, a key component of the molecular pacemaker, can also be used as a model of circadian disruption. Clock mutant females display dampened LH surges (<1.8 ng compared to >10 ng in controls), and loss of cyclicity of the estrous cycle compared to controls (Miller et al., 2004). When pregnant, Clock mutants have a higher rate of fetal resorption and failure to deliver the pups at full term (Miller et al., 2004). These pregnancy complications occur in the presence of no change in breeding behavior between mutants and controls. A similar relationship between circadian disruption and pregnancy complications in women has also been reported. A study of over 20,000 women found that night work the week prior increases the risk of miscarriage in a dose dependent manner (Begtrup et al., 2019)—suggesting that the detrimental effects of circadian disruption are well conserved across species.

Sex differences in sleep: sex chromosomes vs. gonadal function

Sleep in gonadally-intact males and females is most notably different during proestrus and estrus when females have less NREM and REM sleep and more waking compared to males (Fang and Fishbein, 1996; Swift et al., 2019; Kostin et al., 2020). Proestrus also marks sex differences in sleep microarchitecture, particularly in REM sleep. Even sex differences studies that do not account for the estrous cycle still identify differences between male and female sleep (Choi et al., 2021; Paul et al., 2006). These sex differences are largely dependent on gonadal function and the majority of differences disappear between sexes in GDX rats and GDX mice (Paul et al., 2006; Cusmano et al., 2014). Hormone replacement via testosterone treatment to GDX males and estradiol to GDX females largely restore sex differences in sleep and waking (Choi et al., 2021; Paul et al., 2009).

Despite this, not all sex differences in sleep disappear between male and female GDX rats and mice (Paul et al., 2006; Cusmano et al., 2014). GDX male mice have increased REM sleep recovery and decreased NREM delta power following sleep deprivation compared to GDX female mice (Paul et al., 2006). Another study found that GDX males have increase waking, and decreased NREM and REM sleep during the dark phase compared to GDX females (Choi et al., 2021). Interestingly, replacing endogenous hormones in GDX rats does not have equal effects across GDX males and female rats. Administering estradiol to GDX females suppresses sleep, particularly REM sleep, whereas infusion of testosterone into GDX males has little effect on sleep (Cusmano et al., 2014). Together these results suggest that sex differences in sleep are caused by factors outside of gonadal hormones, such as sex chromosomes.

The four-core genotype model is a tool that tests the influence of sex chromosomes or sex-linked traits independent of gonadal development and function. Within the model the SRY gene [whose absence or presence determines gonadal development regardless of sex chromosome (Berta et al., 1990; Sekido and Lovell-Badge, 2009)] is added to XX-females causing the development of male gonads. Similarly, the SRY gene is removed from XY-males to produce female gonads (Ehlen et al., 2013). The result is four genotypes: XY-male (+SRY, testes, male), XY-female (-SRY, ovaries, female), XX-male (+SRY, testes, male), and XX-female (-SRY, ovaries, female). One study used the model to determine whether sex chromosomes account for sex differences in sleep which persist after GDX (Ehlen et al., 2013). The study found that gonadally intact XX-females and XY-females have less NREM sleep during the dark phase compared to XY-males and XX-males. Following gonadectomy, GDX XX-males had more sleep than GDX XX-females during the dark phase. There was no differences between GDX XY-males or GDX XY-females (Ehlen et al., 2013). Finally, following 6hours of sleep deprivation, GDX XY-females had increased amounts NREM sleep and enhanced NREM SWA in comparison to GDX XX-females, suggesting Y-linked traits are responsible for increased homeostatic response to sleep deprivation. This study provides the first evidence that both hormones and sex chromosomes contribute to overall sex differences in sleep.

The four-core genotype model is currently only available in mice although a rat model is in preliminary development (Arnold et al., 2023). One consideration for the four-core genotype study (Ehlen et al., 2013) is that it utilized C57BL/6 J mice which show limited changes in sleep across the estrous cycle, but the authors dismissed these changes and did not control for the estrous phase (Paul et al., 2006). As estrous phase has a greater impact on sleep in rats, performing the same experiments in rats using a four-core genotype approach as well as controlling for the estrous phase may provide further insight. Preliminary results from the rat four-core model suggest that XY-female and XX-female rats both display normal estrous cycles with high fertility so replication of previous work in rats may yield novel results (Arnold et al., 2023).

Considerations and future directions

Sleep and memory consolidation

Extensive work supports the relationship between sleep and memory formation (Rasch and Born, 2013; Poe, 2017), as well as between ovarian hormones and memory formation (Luine, 2014). However, the intersection between hormonally driven sleep-wake changes and their influence on sleep-dependent memory consolidation remain unexplored. Previous studies have shown that memory formation and learning strategies are affected by estrous phase (Korol et al., 2004; Kirry et al., 2018). Memory improvements, particularly during proestrus have been attributed to increased estradiol promoting synaptic plasticity in hippocampal circuits, but the impact of sleep is uncertain. Increased spindle density during the light phase of proestrus and a suppression of REM during the dark phase represent physiologically relevant periods where variations in synaptic plasticity may alter memory consolidation (Fang and Fishbein, 1996; Schwierin et al., 1998; Swift et al., 2019). The suppression of sleep during the dark phase of proestrus, and the subsequent sleep rebound during the morning of estrus present unique periods to examine how physiological changes in sleep may impact sleep-dependent memory processes. For instance, females in the dark phase of proestrus may perform poorly on REM sleep-dependent tasks as REM sleep is almost completely suppressed during that time. The enhanced spindle density in the mPFC during proestrus may also serve to improve memory function—potentially by improving hippocampal connectivity via enhanced spindle-ripple coupling. Future work in gonadally-intact and OVX females should examine the impact estrous phase and ovarian hormones on sleep oscillations and how they impact sleep dependent memory consolidation.

Sleep and post-traumatic stress

Post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) and other anxiety disorders are more common in women compared to men (Kessler et al., 2017). Among other factors, this difference in prevalence has been partially attributed to changes in ovarian hormone levels promoting resilience or susceptibly to psychological stressors. Low hormone levels in the early follicular phase are purported to cause enhanced fear extinction in women compared to those in the late-follicular phase when hormone levels are elevated (Milad et al., 2006). In rats, females display increased levels of fear extinction retention during proestrus when estradiol and progesterone are elevated (Milad et al., 2009). Moreover, administering estradiol and/or progesterone to female rats in metestrus significantly improved fear extinction recall (Milad et al., 2009). In parallel, a separate body of work suggests that sleep disruption also contributes to mental health issues. Rats with disrupted sleep display enhanced anxiety-like behaviors and insomnia is bidirectionally related to anxiety in humans (Alvaro et al., 2013; Grubac et al., 2019; Krause et al., 2017). Despite both hormonal milieu and sleep impacting mental health, little work has examined the intersection of how sleep changes due to ovarian hormone fluctuations contribute trauma-related mental health disorders. Sleep disturbances preceding or following a trauma increase the likelihood of developing mental health disorders, particularly PTSD (Bryant et al., 2010; Koren et al., 2002). Sleep disturbances are commonly reported during the late luteal phase and menstruation in women (Baker and Driver, 2007). Further, sleep disturbances are also common during the transition to menopause when ovarian hormones decrease. These periods of disturbed sleep where ovarian hormone levels decrease may be potential windows of increased vulnerability to trauma, and as such, may contribute to the increased incidence of PTSD in women. However, few studies have evaluated this intersect. Only a single study in male rats (Vanderheyden et al., 2015) examined sleep in models of PTSD and no study has examined how estrus-related sleep changes impact behavior in PTSD models.

Combatting a male bias

Despite mandates for the inclusion of both sexes in future studies, the discrepancy between sexes persists today—particularly in neuroscience (Shansky, 2019; McCarthy et al., 2017; Shansky and Woolley, 2016). A reoccurring motif throughout this review is the absence of citable studies using female rodents. Indeed, even within the field of sleep research several fundamental physiological processes (e.g., sleep neural oscillatory activity) have not been studied in female rodents, much less female rodents across the estrous cycle. These fundamental physiological processes are often assumed to be 1) equivalent to those occurring in males or 2) unaffected by the estrous cycle. This assumption does a disservice to the field by promoting a male bias, and more perniciously, in some instances may be entirely false. Further, as these fundamental physiological processes (e.g., hippocampal ripples) impact higher order processes (e.g., memory consolidation) it becomes evident that a significant majority of our understanding of the neuroscience of sleep is solely derived from the male brain.

The reluctance to include female animals in studies partly stems from concern that females and estrus cycle hormones will increase variability on a measured outcomes (Shansky, 2019). However, previous studies have shown that including females does not inherently increase variability. Rocks et al. (2022) demonstrated that when not controlling for estrous cycle phase, certain behaviors are not significantly different between males and females. In the same merit, controlling for estrous phase may identify a previously masked sex difference or further elucidate an existing sex difference that is apparent when the cycle is not controlled for. While the inclusion of female animals and systematic estrous cycle tracking presents some challenges their inclusion in study design enhances relevancy and the scientific merit of the results.

Conclusion

The estrous cycle and reproductive hormones modulate sleep-wake behavior and play a vital role in the development of sleep neural circuitry. From sex chromosomes to gonads, reproductive processes influence female sleep creating a sex-specific milieu that is often quite distinct from males. Despite this, our understanding of sleep-wake processes is categorically biased towards males. While significant strides have been made towards including female animals in research, we have just begun to scratch the surface of studying foundational neurological processes in female animals. Researchers must view the inclusion of female animals in preclinical studies not as a burden, but as an opportunity and a necessity to those who their research informs. Doing so will holistically improve our understanding of sleep physiology alongside the more important goal of better understanding and improving women’s health.

Author contributions

KS: Conceptualization, Supervision, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. PU: Writing – review & editing. NG: Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare that no financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank Dr. Sue Moenter (University of Michigan) for her insights and input on estrous cycle physiology. Additionally, the authors thank Staff Sergeant Lucas Wentlandt for his feedback on the manuscript.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Author disclaimer

Material has been reviewed by the Walter Reed Army Institute of Research. There is no objection to its presentation and/publication. The opinions or assertions contained herein are the private views of the authors and are not to be construed as official or reflecting the view of the Department of the Army or the Department of Defense.

References

Ajayi, A. F., and Akhigbe, R. E. (2020). Staging of the estrous cycle and induction of estrus in experimental rodents: an update. Fertil. Res. Pract. 6, 5–15. doi: 10.1186/s40738-020-00074-3

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Alvaro, P. K., Roberts, R. M., and Harris, J. K. (2013). A systematic review assessing bidirectionality between sleep disturbances, anxiety, and depression. Sleep 36, 1059–1068. doi: 10.5665/sleep.2810

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Andersen, M. L., Antunes, I. B., Silva, A., Alvarenga, T. A. F., Baracat, E. C., and Tufik, S. (2008). Effects of sleep loss on sleep architecture in Wistar rats: gender-specific rebound sleep. Prog. Neuro.-Psychopharmacology. Biol. Psychiatry 32, 975–983. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2008.01.007

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Armitage, R., and Hoffmann, R. F. (2001). Sleep EEG, depression and gender. Sleep Med. Rev. 5, 237–246. doi: 10.1053/smrv.2000.0144

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Arnold, A. P., Chen, X., Grzybowski, M. N., Ryan, J. M., Sengelaub, D. R., Mohanroy, T., et al. (2023). A “four Core genotypes” rat model to distinguish mechanisms underlying sex-biased phenotypes and diseases. bioR 10:2023.02.09.527738. doi: 10.1101/2023.02.09.527738

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Arthaud, S. D. S., Villalba, M., Blondet, C., Morel, A. L., and Peyron, C. (2022). Effects of sex and estrous cycle on sleep and cataplexy in narcoleptic mice. Sleep 45, 1–12. doi: 10.1093/sleep/zsac089

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bachelot, A., and Binart, N. (2007). Reproductive role of prolactin. Reproduction 133, 361–369. doi: 10.1530/REP-06-0299

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Baker, F. C., and Driver, H. S. (2007). Circadian rhythms, sleep, and the menstrual cycle. Sleep Med. 8, 613–622. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2006.09.011

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Baker, F. C., and Lee, K. A. (2018). Menstrual cycle effects on sleep. Sleep Med. Clin. 13, 283–294. doi: 10.1016/j.jsmc.2018.04.002

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Barth, A. M. I., Ferando, I., and Mody, I. (2014). Ovarian cycle-linked plasticity of d-GABAA receptor subunits in hippocampal interneurons affects gamma oscillations in vivo. Front. Cell. Neurosci. 8:222. doi: 10.3389/fncel.2014.00222

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Begtrup, L. M., Specht, I. O., Hammer, P. E. C., Flachs, E. M., Garde, A. H., Hansen, J., et al. (2019). Night work and miscarriage: a Danish nationwide register-based cohort study. Occup. Environ. Med. 76, 302–308. doi: 10.1136/oemed-2018-105592

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Berta, P., Hawkins, J. R., Sinclair, A. H., Taylor, A., Griffiths, B. L., Goodfellow, P. N., et al. (1990). Genetic evidence equating SRY and testis-determining factor. Nature 348, 448–450. doi: 10.1038/348448a0

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bethea, C. L., and Neili, J. D. (1979). Prolactin secretion after cervical stimulation of rats maintained in constant dark or constant light. Endocrinology 104, 870–876. doi: 10.1210/endo-104-4-870

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Boespflug, E. L., and Iliff, J. J. (2018). The emerging relationship between interstitial fluid-cerebrospinal fluid exchange, amyloid-β, and sleep. Biol. Psychiatry 83, 328–336. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2017.11.031

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Boivin, D. B., and Shechter, A. (2010). Sleep, hormones, and circadian rhythms throughout the menstrual cycle in healthy women and women with premenstrual dysphoric disorder. Int. J. Endocrinol. 2010:259345. doi: 10.1155/2010/259345

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Borbely, A. A., and Achermann, P. (1999). Sleep homeostasis and models of sleep regulation. J. Biol. Rhythm. 14, 559–570. doi: 10.1177/074873099129000894

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Borgus, J. R., Puthongkham, P., and Venton, B. J. (2020). Complex sex and estrous cycle differences in spontaneous transient adenosine. J. Neurochem. 153, 216–229. doi: 10.1111/jnc.14981

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Branchey, M., Branchey, L., and Nadler, R. D. (1971). Effects of estrogen and progesterone on sleep patterns of female rats. Physiol. Behav. 6, 743–746. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(71)90267-8

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Branchey, L., Branchey, M., and Nadler, R. D. (1973). Effects of sex hormones on sleep patterns of male rats gonadectomized in adulthood and in the neonatal period. Physiol. Behav. 11, 609–611. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(73)90244-8

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bryant, R. A., Creamer, M., O’Donnell, M., Silove, D., and McFarlane, A. C. (2010). Sleep disturbance immediately prior to trauma predicts subsequent psychiatric disorder. Sleep 33, 69–74. doi: 10.1093/sleep/33.1.69

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Byers, S. L., Wiles, M. V., Dunn, S. L., and Taft, R. A. (2012). Mouse estrous cycle identification tool and images. PLoS One 7, 2–6. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0035538

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Carter, M. E., Yizhar, O., Chikahisa, S., Nguyen, H., Adamantidis, A., Nishino, S., et al. (2010). Tuning arousal with optogenetic modulation of locus coeruleus neurons. Nat. Neurosci. 13, 1526–1533. doi: 10.1038/nn.2682

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Caufriez, A., Leproult, R., L’Hermite-Balériaux, M., Kerkhofs, M., and Copinschi, G. (2011). Progesterone prevents sleep disturbances and modulates GH, TSH, and melatonin secretion in postmenopausal women. J. Clin. Endocrinol. Metab. 96, E614–E623. doi: 10.1210/jc.2010-2558

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chappell, P. E. (2005). Clocks and the block box: circadian influences on gonadotropin-releasing hormone secretion. J. Neuroendocrinol. 17, 119–130. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2826.2005.01270.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Choi, J., Kim, S. J., Fujiyama, T., Miyoshi, C., Park, M., Suzuki-Abe, H., et al. (2021). The role of reproductive hormones in sex differences in sleep homeostasis and arousal response in mice. Front. Neurosci. 15, 1–14. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2021.739236

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Christian, C. A., and Moenter, S. M. (2010). The neurobiology of preovulatory and estradiol-induced gonadotropin- releasing hormone surges. Endocr. Rev. 31, 544–577. doi: 10.1210/er.2009-0023

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Clawson, B. C., Durkin, J., and Aton, S. J. (2016). Form and function of sleep spindles across the lifespan. Neural Plast. 2016, 1–16. doi: 10.1155/2016/6936381

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Clemens, L., Barr, P., and Dohanich, G. (1989). Cholinergic regulation of female sexual behavior in rats demonstrated by manipulation of endogenous acetylcholine. Physiol. Behav. 45, 437–442. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(89)90152-2

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Colvin, G., Whitmoyer, D., Lisk, R., Walter, D., and Sawyer, C. (1968). Changes in sleep-wakefulness in female rates during circadian and estrous cycles. Brain Res. 7, 173–181. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(68)90095-4

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Colvin, G. B., Whitmoyer, D. I., and Sawyer, C. H. (1969). Circadian sleep-wakefulness patterns in rats after ovariectomy and treatment with estrogen. Exp. Neurol. 25, 616–625. doi: 10.1016/0014-4886(69)90104-6

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Curran-Rauhut, M. A., and Petersen, S. L. (2002). The distribution of progestin receptor mRNA in rat brainstem. Brain Res. Gene Expr. Patterns 1, 151–157. doi: 10.1016/S1567-133X(02)00011-X

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cusmano, D. M., Hadjimarkou, M. M., and Mong, J. A. (2014). Gonadal steroid modulation of sleep and wakefulness in male and female rats is sexually differentiated and neonatally organized by steroid exposure. Endocrinology 155, 204–214. doi: 10.1210/en.2013-1624

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cusmano, D. M., Viechweg, S., and Mong, J. A. (2011). Blockade of estrogenic action in the MNPN disrupts sleep/wake behavior in the female rat. Sleep 36:A0020.

Google Scholar

De Luca, R., Nardone, S., Grace, K. P., Venner, A., Cristofolini, M., Bandaru, S. S., et al. (2022). Orexin neurons inhibit sleep to promote arousal. Nat. Commun. 13:4163. doi: 10.1038/s41467-022-31591-y

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

De Zambotti, M., Willoughby, A. R., Sassoon, S. A., Colrain, I. M., and Baker, F. C. (2015). Menstrual cycle-related variation in physiological sleep in women in the early menopausal transition. J. Clin. Endocrinol. Metab. 100, 2918–2926. doi: 10.1210/jc.2015-1844

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Deurveilher, S., Cumyn, E. M., Rusak, B., Semba, K., and Peers, T. (2008). Estradiol replacement enhances sleep deprivation-induced c-Fos immunoreactivity in forebrain arousal regions of ovariectomized rats. Am. J. Physiol. Integr. Comp. Physiol. 295, R1328–R1340. doi: 10.1152/ajpregu.90576.2008

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Deurveilher, S., Rusak, B., and Semba, K. (2009). Estradiol and progesterone modulate spontaneous sleep patterns and recovery from sleep deprivation in ovariectomized rats. Sleep 32, 865–877

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Deurveilher, S., Rusak, B., and Semba, K. (2011). Female reproductive hormones alter sleep architecture in ovariectomized rats. Sleep 34, 519–530. doi: 10.1093/sleep/34.4.519

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Driver, H. S., Dijk, D. J., Werth, E., Biedermann, K., and Borbély, A. A. (1996). Sleep and the sleep electroencephalogram across the menstrual cycle in young healthy women. J. Clin. Endocrinol. Metab. 81, 728–735. doi: 10.1210/jcem.81.2.8636295

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Driver, H. S., Werth, E., Dijk, D. J., and Borbély, A. A. (2008). Menstrual cycle effects on sleep. Sleep Med. Clin. 3, 1–11. doi: 10.1016/j.jsmc.2007.10.003

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dunn, J. D., Hess, M., and Johnson, D. C. (1976). Effect of thyroidectomy on rhythmic gonadotropin release. Proc. Soc. Exp. Biol. Med. 151, 22–27. doi: 10.3181/00379727-151-39135

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Eban-Rothschild, A., Rothschild, G., Giardino, W. J., Jones, J. R., and de Lecea, L. (2016). VTA dopaminergic neurons regulate ethologically relevant sleep-wake behaviors. Nat. Neurosci. 19, 1356–1366. doi: 10.1038/nn.4377

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Edgar, D. M., Seidel, W. F., Gee, K. W., Lan, N. C., Field, G., Xia, H., et al. (1997). CCD-3693: an orally bioavailable analog of the endogenous neuroactive steroid, pregnanolone, demonstrates potent sedative hypnotic actions in the rat. J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther. 282, 420–429

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Ehlen, J. C., Hesse, S., Pinckney, L., and Paul, K. N. (2013). Sex chromosomes regulate nighttime sleep propensity during recovery from sleep loss in mice. PLoS One 8, e62205–e62219. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0062205

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Etgen, A. M., and Morales, J. C. (2002). Somatosensory stimuli evoke norepinephrine release in the anterior ventromedial hypothalamus of sexually receptive female rats. J. Neuroendocrinol. 14, 213–218. doi: 10.1046/j.0007-1331.2001.00764.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fang, J., and Fishbein, W. (1996). Sex differences in paradoxical sleep: influences of estrus cycle and ovariectomy. Brain Res. 734, 275–285. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(96)00652-X

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fattinger, S., De Beukelaar, T. T., Ruddy, K. L., Volk, C., Heyse, N. C., Herbst, J. A., et al. (2017). Deep sleep maintains learning efficiency of the human brain. Nat. Commun. 8:15405. doi: 10.1038/ncomms15405

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fogel, S. M., and Smith, C. T. (2006). Learning-dependent changes in sleep spindles and stage 2 sleep. J. Sleep Res. 15, 250–255. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2869.2006.00522.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Freeman, M. E., Kanyicska, B., Lerant, A., and Nagy, G. (2000). Prolactin: structure, function, and regulation of secretion. Physiol. Rev. 1, 1526–1631. doi: 10.1016/B978-012515400-0/50037-3

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Friess, E., Tagaya, H., Trachsel, L., Holsboer, F., and Rupprecht, R. (1997). Progesterone-induced changes in sleep in male subjects. Am. J. Phys. 272, E885–E891. doi: 10.1152/ajpendo.1997.272.5.E885

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gaillard, J. M., and Blois, R. (1981). Spindle density in sleep of normal subjects. Sleep 4, 385–391. doi: 10.1093/sleep/4.4.385

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Girardeau, G., Benchenane, K., Wiener, S. I., Buzsáki, G., and Zugaro, M. B. (2009). Selective suppression of hippocampal ripples impairs spatial memory. Nat. Neurosci. 12, 1222–1223. doi: 10.1038/nn.2384

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gridchyn, I., Schoenenberger, P., Neill, J. O., and Csicsvari, J. (2020). Assembly-specific disruption of hippocampal replay leads to selective memory deficit article assembly-specific disruption of hippocampal replay leads to selective memory deficit. Neuron 106, 291–300.e6. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2020.01.021

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Grubac, Z., Sutulovic, N., Ademovic, A., Velimirovic, M., Rasic-Markovic, A., Macut, D., et al. (2019). Short-term sleep fragmentation enhances anxiety-related behavior: the role of hormonal alterations. PLoS One 14, e0218920–e0218916. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0218920

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hadjimarkou, M. M., Benham, R., Schwarz, J. M., Holder, M. K., and Mong, J. A. (2008). Estradiol suppresses rapid eye movement sleep and activation of sleep-active neurons in the ventrolateral preoptic area. Eur. J. Neurosci. 27, 1780–1792. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2008.06142.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hajali, V., Andersen, M. L., Negah, S. S., and Sheibani, V. (2019). Sex differences in sleep and sleep loss-induced cognitive deficits: the influence of gonadal hormones. Horm. Behav. 108, 50–61. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2018.12.013

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Haywood, S. A., Simonian, S. X., Van der Beek, E. M., Bicknell, R. J., and Herbison, A. E. (1999). Fluctuating estrogen and progesterone receptor expression in brainstem norepinephrine neurons through the rat estrous cycle. Endocrinology 140, 3255–3263. doi: 10.1210/endo.140.7.6869

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Huber, R., Ghilardi, M. F., Massimini, M., and Tononi, G. (2004). Local sleep and learning. Nature 430, 78–81. doi: 10.1038/nature02663

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Huupponen, E., Himanen, S.-L., Värri, A., Hasan, J., Lehtokangas, M., and Saarinen, J. (2002). A study on gender and age differences in sleep spindles. Neuropsychobiology 45, 99–105. doi: 10.1159/000048684

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Iotchev, I. B., Kis, A., Bódizs, R., Van Luijtelaar, G., and Kubinyi, E. (2017). EEG transients in the sigma range during non-REM sleep predict learning in dogs. Sci. Rep. 7, 1–11. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-13278-3

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ishizuka, Y., Pollak, C. P., Shirakawa, S., Kakuma, T., Azumi, K., Usui, A., et al. (1994). Sleep spindle frequency changes during the menstrual cycle. J. Sleep Res. 3, 26–29. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2869.1994.tb00100.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Jadhav, S. P., Kemere, C., German, P. W., and Frank, L. M. (2012). Awake hippocampal sharp-wave ripples support spatial memory. Science 336, 1454–1458. doi: 10.1126/science.1217230

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Jain, X. A., Huang, G. Z., and Woolley, X. C. S. (2019). Latent sex differences in molecular signaling that underlies excitatory synaptic potentiation in the Hippocampus. Neuroscience 39, 1552–1565. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1897-18.2018

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Jarosch, M. S., Gebhardt, C., Fano, S., Huchzermeyer, C., ul Haq, R., Behrens, C. J., et al. (2015). Early adenosine release contributes to hypoxia-induced disruption of stimulus-induced sharp wave-ripple complexes in rat hippocampal area CA3. Eur. J. Neurosci. 42, 1808–1817. doi: 10.1111/ejn.12941

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kessler, R. C., Aguilar-Gaxiola, S., Alonso, J., Benjet, C., Bromet, E. J., Cardoso, G., et al. (2017). Trauma and PTSD in the WHO world mental health surveys. Eur. J. Psychotraumatol. 8:1353383. doi: 10.1080/20008198.2017.1353383

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kirry, A. J., Durigan, D. J., Twining, R. C., and Gilmartin, M. R. (2018). Estrous cycle stage gates sex differences in prefrontal muscarinic control of fear memory formation. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 161, 26–36. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2019.03.001

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kleinlogel, H. (1983). The female rat’s sleep during oestrous cycle. Neuropsychobiology 10, 228–237. doi: 10.1159/000118016

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Koehl, M., Battle, S. E., and Turek, F. W. (2003). Sleep in female mice: a strain comparison across the estrous cycle. Sleep 26, 267–272. doi: 10.1093/sleep/26.3.267

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Koren, D., Arnon, I., Lavie, P., and Klein, E. (2002). Sleep complaints as early predictors of posttraumatic stress disorder: a 1-year prospective study of injured survivors of motor vehicle accidents. Am. J. Psychiatry 159, 855–857. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.159.5.855

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Korol, D. L., Malin, E. L., Borden, K. A., Busby, R. A., and Couper-Leo, J. (2004). Shifts in preferred learning strategy across the estrous cycle in female rats. Horm. Behav. 45, 330–338. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2004.01.005

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kostin, A., Alam, A., Siegel, J., McGinty, D., and Alam, N. (2020). Sex- and age-dependent differences in sleep-wake characteristics of Fisher-344 rats. Neuroscience 427, 29–42. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2019.11.046

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Krause, A. J., Ben, S. E., Mander, B. A., Greer, S. M., Saletin, J. M., Goldstein-Piekarski, A. N., et al. (2017). The sleep-deprived human brain. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 18, 404–418. doi: 10.1038/nrn.2017.55

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Laakso, M.-L., Porkka-Heiskanen, T., Alila, A., Peder, M., and Johansson, G. (1988). Twenty-four-hour patterns of pineal melatonin and pituitary and plasma prolactin in male rats under ‘natural’ and artificial lighting conditions. Neuroendocrinology 48, 308–313. doi: 10.1159/000125027

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lack, L. C., Gradisar, M., Van Someren, E. J. W., Wright, H. R., and Lushington, K. (2008). The relationship between insomnia and body temperatures. Sleep Med. Rev. 12, 307–317. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2008.02.003

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lancel, M. (1999). Role of GABA(a) receptors in the regulation of sleep: initial sleep responses to peripherally administered modulators and agonists. Sleep 22, 33–42. doi: 10.1093/sleep/22.1.33

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lancel, M., Faulhaber, J., Holsboer, F., and Rupprecht, R. (1996). Progesterone induces changes in sleep comparable to those of agonistic GABA(a) receptor modulators. Am. J. Physiol. – Endocrinol. Metab. 271, E763–E772. doi: 10.1152/ajpendo.1996.271.4.e763

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lancel, M., Faulhaber, J., Schiffelholz, T., Romeo, E., di Michele, F., Holsboer, F., et al. (1997). Allopregnanolone affects sleep in a benzodiazepine-like fashion. J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther. 282, 1213–1218

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Lawson, C. C., Johnson, C. Y., Chavarro, J. E., Lividoti Hibert, E. N., Whelan, E. A., Rocheleau, C. M., et al. (2015). Work schedule and physically demanding work in relation to menstrual function: the nurses’ health study 3. Scand. J. Work Environ. Health 41, 194–203. doi: 10.5271/sjweh.3482

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lawson, C. C., Whelan, E. A., Hibert, E. N., Spiegelinan, D., and Rich-Edwards, J. W. (2011). Rotating shift work and menstrual cycle characteristics. Epidemiology 22, 305–312. doi: 10.1097/EDE.0b013e3182130016

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Legan, S. J., and Karsch, F. J. (1975). A daily signal for the LH surge in the rat. Endocrinology 96, 57–62. doi: 10.1210/endo-96-1-57

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, H., and Satinoff, E. (1996). Body temperature and sleep in intact and ovariectomized female rats. Am. J. Physiol. Integr. Comp. Physiol. 271, R1753–R1758. doi: 10.1152/ajpregu.1996.271.6.r1753

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Luine, V. N. (2014). Estradiol and cognitive function: past, present and future. Horm. Behav. 66, 602–618. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2014.08.011

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Maingret, N., Girardeau, G., Todorova, R., Goutierre, M., and Zugaro, M. (2016). Hippocampo-cortical coupling mediates memory consolidation during sleep. Nat. Neurosci. 19, 959–964. doi: 10.1038/nn.4304

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Manber, R., and Bootzin, R. R. (1997). Sleep and the menstrual cycle. Health Psychol. 16, 209–214. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.16.3.209

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Marrone, B. L., Thomas Gentry, R., and Wade, G. N. (1976). Gonadal hormones and body temperature in rats: effects of estrous cycles, castration and steroid replacement. Physiol. Behav. 17, 419–425. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(76)90101-3

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Marshall, G., and Griffin, M. (1993). The role of changing pulse frequency in the regulation of ovulation. Hum. Re. 8, 57–61. doi: 10.1093/humrep/8.suppl_2.57

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Matsushima, M., and Takeichi, M. (1990). Effects of intraventricular implantation of crystalline estradiol benzoate on the sleep-wakefulness Orcadian rhythm of Ovariectomized rats. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 44, 111–121. doi: 10.1111/j.1440-1819.1990.tb00448.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

McCarthy, M. M., Woolley, C. S., and Arnold, A. P. (2017). Incorporating sex as a biological variable in neuroscience: what do we gain? Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 18, 707–708. doi: 10.1038/nrn.2017.137

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mccormack, C. E., and Sridaran, R. (1977). Timing of ovulation in rats during exposure to continuous light: evidence for a circadian rhythm of luteinizing hormone secretion. Endocrinology 76, 135–144. doi: 10.1677/joe.0.0760135

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

McLean, A. C., Valenzuela, N., Fai, S., and Bennett, S. A. L. (2012). Performing vaginal lavage, crystal violet staining, and vaginal cytological evaluation for mouse estrous cycle staging identification. J. Vis. Exp. 67, 4–9. doi: 10.3791/4389

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mednick, S. C., Mcdevitt, E. A., Walsh, J. K., Wamsley, E., Paulus, M., Kanady, J. C., et al. (2013). The critical role of sleep spindles in hippocampal-dependent memory: a pharmacology study. J. Neurosci. 33, 4494–4504. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3127-12.2013

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Melis, M., and Argiolas, A. (1995). Dopamine and sexual behavior. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 19, 19–38. doi: 10.1016/0149-7634(94)00020-2

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Milad, M. R., Igoe, S. A., Lebron-Milad, K., and Novales, J. E. (2009). Estrous cycle phase and gonadal hormones influence conditioned fear extinction. Neuroscience 164, 887–895. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2009.09.011

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Milad, M. R., Orr, S. P., Klibanski, A., Wedig, M. M., Klibanski, A., Pitman, R. K., et al. (2006). Fear conditioning and extinction: influence of sex and menstrual cycle in healthy humans. Behav. Neurosci. 120, 1196–1203. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.120.5.1196

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Miller, B. H., Losee-Olson, S., Turek, F. W., Levine, J. E., Horton, T., and Takahashi, J. S. (2004). Circadian clock mutation disrupts estrous Cyclicity and maintenance of pregnancy. Curr. Biol. 14, 1367–1373. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2004.07.055

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Miller, B. H., and Takahashi, J. S. (2014). Central circadian control of female reproductive function. Front. Endocrinol. (Lausanne) 4:195. doi: 10.3389/fendo.2013.00195

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mínguez-Alarcón, L., Souter, I., Williams, P. L., Ford, J. B., Hauser, R., Chavarro, J. E., et al. (2017). Occupational factors and markers of ovarian reserve and response among women at a fertility center. Occup. Environ. Med. 74, 426–431. doi: 10.1136/oemed-2016-103953

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mohawk, J. A., Green, C. B., and Takahashi, J. S. (2012). Central and peripheral circadian clocks in mammals. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 35, 445–462. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-060909-153128

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Moline, M. L., Albers, H. E., Todd, R. B., and Moore-Ede, M. C. (1981). Light-dark entrainment of proestrous LH surges and circadian locomotor activity in female hamsters. Horm. Behav. 15, 451–458. doi: 10.1016/0018-506X(81)90009-X

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mong, J. A., Baker, F. C., Mahoney, M. M., Paul, K. N., Schwartz, M. D., Semba, K., et al. (2011). Sleep, rhythms, and the endocrine brain: influence of sex and gonadal hormones. J. Neurosci. 31, 16107–16116. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4175-11.2011

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mong, J. A., and Cusmano, D. M. (2016). Sex differences in sleep: impact of biological sex and sex steroids. Philos. Trans. R Soc. B Biol. Sci. 371:20150110. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2015.0110

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mong, J. A., Devidze, N., Frail, D. E., O'Connor, L. T., Samuel, M., Choleris, E., et al. (2003). Estradiol differentially regulates lipocalin-type prostaglandin D synthase transcript levels in the rodent brain: evidence from high-density oligonucleotide arrays and in situ hybridization. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 100, 318–323. doi: 10.1073/pnas.262663799

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mong, J. A., Devidze, N., Goodwillie, A., and Pfaff, D. W. (2003). Reduction of lipocalin-type prostaglandin D synthase in the preoptic area of female mice mimics estradiol effects on arousal and sex behavior. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 100, 15206–15211. doi: 10.1073/pnas.2436540100

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Moriya, R., Kanamaru, M., Okuma, N., Yoshikawa, A., Tanaka, K. F., Hokari, S., et al. (2021). Optogenetic activation of DRN 5-HT neurons induced active wakefulness, not quiet wakefulness. Brain Res. Bull. 177, 129–142. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2021.09.019

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Muschamp, J. W., Dominguez, J. M., Sato, S. M., Shen, R.-Y., and Hull, E. M. (2007). A role for Hypocretin (orexin) in male sexual behavior. Neuroscience 27, 2837–2845. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.4121-06.2007

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Nakayama, T., Suzuki, M., and Ishizuka, N. (1975). Action of progesterone on preoptic thermosensitive neurones. Nature 258:80. doi: 10.1038/258080a0

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Niswender, G. D. (2002). Review molecular control of luteal secretion of progesterone *. Reproduction 123, 333–339. doi: 10.1530/rep.0.1230333

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Niswender, G. D., Juengel, J. L., Mcguire, W. J., Belfiore, C. J., and Wiltbank, M. C. (1994). Luteal function: the estrous cycle and early pregnancy. Biol. Reprod. 50, 239–247. doi: 10.1095/biolreprod50.2.239

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Oberlander, X. J. G., and Woolley, C. S. (2017). 17B-estradiol acutely potentiates glutamatergic synaptic transmission in the Hippocampus through distinct mechanisms in males and females. Neuroscience 37, 12314–12327. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3011-17.2017

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ognjanovski, N., Schaeffer, S., Wu, J., Mofakham, S., Maruyama, D., Zochowski, M., et al. (2017). Parvalbumin-expressing interneurons coordinate hippocampal network dynamics required for memory consolidation. Nat. Commun. 8, 1–13. doi: 10.1038/ncomms15039

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ortiz, F., and Gutiérrez, R. (2015). Entorhinal cortex lesions result in adenosine-sensitive high frequency oscillations in the hippocampus. Exp. Neurol. 271, 319–328. doi: 10.1016/j.expneurol.2015.06.009

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Paccola, C. C., Resende, C. G., Stumpp, T., Miraglia, S. M., and Cipriano, I. (2013). The rat estrous cycle revisited: a quantitative and qualitative analysis. Anim. Reprod. 10, 677–683.

Google Scholar

Park, O., and Ramirez, V. (1989). Spontaneous changes in LHRH release during the rat estrous cycle, as measured with repetitive push-pull perfusions of the pituitary gland in the same female rats. Neuroendocrinology 50, 66–72. doi: 10.1159/000125203

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Parker, D. C., Rossman, L. G., and Vanderlaan, E. F. (1974). Relation of sleep-entrained human prolactin release to REM-NonREM cycles. J. Clin. Endocrinol. Metab. 38, 646–651. doi: 10.1210/jcem-38-4-646

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Paul, K. N., Dugovic, C., Turek, F. W., and Laposky, A. D. (2006). Diurnal sex differences in the sleep-wake cycle of mice are dependent on gonadal function. Sleep 29, 1211–1223. doi: 10.1093/sleep/29.9.1211

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Paul, K. N., Laposky, A. D., and Turek, F. W. (2009). Reproductive hormone replacement alters sleep in mice. Neurosci. Lett. 463, 239–243. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2009.07.081

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pawlyk, A. C., Alfinito, P. D., and Deecher, D. C. (2008a). Effect of 17α-ethinyl estradiol on active phase rapid eye movement sleep microarchitecture. Eur. J. Pharmacol. 591, 315–318. doi: 10.1016/j.ejphar.2008.06.084

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pawlyk, A. C., Alfinito, P. D., Johnston, G. H., and Deecher, D. C. (2008b). Subchronic 17α-ethinyl estradiol differentially affects subtypes of sleep and wakefulness in ovariectomized rats. Horm. Behav. 53, 217–224. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2007.09.018

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Perlis, M. L., Smith, M. T., Andrews, P. J., Orff, H., and Giles, D. E. (2001). Beta/gamma EEG activity in patients with primary and secondary insomnia and good sleeper controls. Sleep 24, 110–117. doi: 10.1093/sleep/24.1.110

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Poe, G. R. (2017). Sleep Is for Forgetting. J. Neurosci. 37, 464–473. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0820-16.2017

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Powers, J. B. (1970). Hormonal control of sexual receptivity during the estrous cycle of the rat. Physiol. Behav. 5, 831–835. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(70)90167-8

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Quadros, P. S., and Wagner, C. K. (2008). Regulation of progesterone receptor expression by estradiol is dependent on age, sex and region in the rat brain. Endocrinology 149, 3054–3061. doi: 10.1210/en.2007-1133

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rasch, B., and Born, J. (2013). About Sleep’s role in memory. Physiol. Rev. 93, 681–766. doi: 10.1152/physrev.00032.2012

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rocks, D., Cham, H., and Kundakovic, M. (2022). Why the estrous cycle matters for neuroscience. Biol. Sex Differ. 13, 62–14. doi: 10.1186/s13293-022-00466-8

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Roky, R., Obál, F., Valatx, J. L., Bredow, S., Fang, J., Pagano, L. P., et al. (1995). Prolactin and rapid eye movement sleep regulation. Sleep 18, 536–542

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Roky, R., Valatax, J., and Jouvet, M. (1993). Effect of prolactin on the sleep-wake cycle in rat. Neurosci. Lett. 156, 117–120. doi: 10.1016/0304-3940(93)90453-R

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Roky, R., Valatx, J. L., Paut-Pagano, L., and Jouvet, M. (1994). Hypothalamic injection of prolactin or its antibody alters the rat sleep-wake cycle. Physiol. Behav. 55, 1015–1019. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(94)90382-4

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Roux, L., Hu, B., Eichler, R., Stark, E., and Buzsáki, G. (2017). Sharp wave ripples during learning stabilize the hippocampal spatial map. Nat. Neurosci. 20, 845–853. doi: 10.1038/nn.4543

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Saper, C. B., Fuller, P. M., Pedersen, N. P., Lu, J., and Scammell, T. E. (2011). Sleep state switching. Neuron 68, 1023–1042. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.11.032.Sleep

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sarkar, D. K., Chiappa, S. A., Fink, G., and Sherwood, N. M. (1976). Gonadotropin-releasing hormone surge in pro-oestrous rats. Nature 264, 461–463. doi: 10.1038/264461a0

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sassin, J. F., Frantz, A. G., Kapen, S., and Weitzman, E. D. (1973). The nocturnal rise of human prolactin is dependent on sleep. J. Clin. Endocrinol. Metab. 37, 436–440. doi: 10.1210/jcem-37-3-436

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sassin, J. F., Frantz, A. G., Weitzman, E. D., and Kapen, S. (1972). Human prolactin: 24-hour pattern with increased release during sleep. Science 177, 1205–1207. doi: 10.1126/science.177.4055.1205

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Scharfman, H. E., and MacLusky, N. J. (2006). The influence of gonadal hormones on neuronal excitability, seizures, and epilepsy in the female. Epilepsia 47, 1423–1440. doi: 10.1111/j.1528-1167.2006.00672.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Schüssler, P., Kluge, M., Yassouridis, A., Dresler, M., Held, K., Zihl, J., et al. (2008). Progesterone reduces wakefulness in sleep EEG and has no effect on cognition in healthy postmenopausal women. Psychoneuroendocrinology 33, 1124–1131. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2008.05.013

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Schwartz, M. D., and Mong, J. A. (2011). Estradiol suppresses recovery of REM sleep following sleep deprivation in ovariectomized female rats. Physiol. Behav. 104, 962–971. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2011.06.016

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Schwartz, M. D., and Mong, J. A. (2013). Estradiol modulates recovery of REM sleep in a time-of-day-dependent manner. Am. J. Physiol. Regul. Integr. Comp. Physiol. 305, R271–R280. doi: 10.1152/ajpregu.00474.2012

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Schwierin, B., Borbély, A. A., and Tobler, I. (1998). Sleep homeostasis in the female rat during the estrous cycle. Brain Res. 811, 96–104. doi: 10.1016/S0006-8993(98)00991-3

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sekido, R., and Lovell-Badge, R. (2009). Sex determination and SRY: down to a wink and a nudge? Trends Genet. 25, 19–29. doi: 10.1016/j.tig.2008.10.008

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Shansky, R. M. (2019). Are hormones a “female problem” for animal research? Science 364, 825–826. doi: 10.1126/science.aaw7570

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Shansky, R. M., and Woolley, C. S. (2016). Considering sex as a biological variable will be valuable for neuroscience research. J. Neurosci. 36, 11817–11822. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1390-16.2016

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Shughrue, P. J., Lane, M. V., and Merchenthaler, I. (1997). Comparative distribution of estrogen receptor-alpha and -beta mRNA in the rat central nervous system. J. Comp. Neurol. 388, 507–525. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1096-9861(19971201)388:4<507::aid-cne1>3.0.co;2-6

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Siapas, A. G., and Wilson, M. A. (1998). Coordinated interactions between hippocampal ripples and cortical spindles during slow-wave sleep. Neuron 21, 1123–1128. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(00)80629-7

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Simon, A. P., Poindessous-Jazat, F., Dutar, P., Epelbaum, J., and Bassant, M.-H. (2006). Firing properties of anatomically identified neurons in the medial septum of anesthetized and Unanesthetized restrained rats. Neuroscience 26, 9038–9046. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1401-06.2006

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Smarr, B. L., Morris, E., and De La Iglesia, H. O. (2012). The dorsomedial suprachiasmatic nucleus times circadian expression of Kiss1 and the luteinizing hormone surge. Endocrinology 153, 2839–2850. doi: 10.1210/en.2011-1857

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, M. S., Freeman, M. E., and Neill, J. D. (1975). The control of progesterone secretion during the estrous cycle and early pseudopregnancy in the rat: prolactin, gonadotropin and steroid levels associated with rescue of the corpus luteum of pseudopregnancy. Endocrinology 96, 219–226. doi: 10.1210/endo-96-1-219

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Spiegel, K., Follenius, M., Simon, C., Saini, J., Ehrhart, J., and Brandenberger, G. (1994). Prolactin secretion and sleep. Sleep 17, 20–27. doi: 10.1093/sleep/17.1.20

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Stachenfeld, N. S., Silva, C., and Keefe, D. L. (2000). Estrogen modifies the temperature effects of progesterone. J. Appl. Physiol. 88, 1643–1649. doi: 10.1152/jappl.2000.88.5.1643

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Staresina, B. P., Bergmann, T. O., Bonnefond, M., van der Meij, R., Jensen, O., Deuker, L., et al. (2015). Hierarchical nesting of slow oscillations, spindles and ripples in the human hippocampus during sleep. Nat. Neurosci. 18, 1679–1686. doi: 10.1038/nn.4119

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Steriade, M., McCormick, D., and Sejnowski, T. (1993). Thalamocortical oscillations in the sleeping and aroused brain. Science 262, 679–685. doi: 10.1126/science.8235588

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Swift, K. M., Gross, B. A., Frazer, M. A., Bauer, D. S., Clark, K. J. D., Vazey, E. M., et al. (2018). Abnormal locus Coeruleus sleep activity alters sleep signatures of memory consolidation and impairs place cell stability and spatial memory. Curr. Biol. 28, 3599–3609.e4. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2018.09.054

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Swift, K. M., Keus, K., Echeverria, C. G., Cabrera, Y., Jimenez, J., Holloway, J., et al. (2019). Sex differences within sleep in gonadally intact rats. Sleep 43:zsz289. doi: 10.1093/sleep/zsz289

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tóth, A., Traub, M., Bencsik, N., Détári, L., Hajnik, T., and Dobolyi, A. (2024). Sleep- and sleep deprivation-related changes of vertex auditory evoked potentials during the estrus cycle in female rats. Sci. Rep. 14, 1–18. doi: 10.1038/s41598-024-56392-9

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ueno, R., Ishikawa, Y., Nakayama, T., and Hayaishi, O. (1982). Prostaglandin D2 induces sleep when microinjected into the preoptic area of conscious rats. Biochem. Biophys. Res. Commun. 109, 576–582. doi: 10.1016/0006-291X(82)91760-0

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Uphouse, L. (2014). Pharmacology of serotonin and female sexual. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 121, 31–42. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2013.11.008

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Urade, Y., and Hayaishi, O. (2011). Prostaglandin D 2 and sleep / wake regulation. Sleep Med. Rev. 15, 411–418. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2011.08.003

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Valatax, J., Bugat, R., and Jouvet, M. (1972). Genetic studies of sleep in mice. Nature 238, 226–227. doi: 10.1038/238226a0

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Vanderheyden, W. M., George, S. A., Urpa, L., Kehoe, M., Liberzon, I., and Poe, G. R. (2015). Sleep alterations following exposure to stress predict fear-associated memory impairments in a rodent model of PTSD. Exp. Brain Res. 233, 2335–2346. doi: 10.1007/s00221-015-4302-0

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, Y., Gu, F., Deng, M., Guo, L., Lu, C., Zhou, C., et al. (2016). Rotating shift work and menstrual characteristics in a cohort of Chinese nurses. BMC Womens Health 16, 24–29. doi: 10.1186/s12905-016-0301-y

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wiedemann, K., Hirschmann, M., Knaudt, K., Rupprecht, R., Seier, F. E., and Holsboer, F. (1998). Sleep endocrine effects of megestrol acetate in healthy men. J. Neuroendocrinol. 10, 719–727. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2826.1998.00259.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wiedemann, K., Lauer, C. J., Hirschmann, M., Knaudt, K., and Holsboer, F. (2017). Sleep-endocrine effects of mifepristone and megestrol acetate in healthy men. Am. J. Physiol. Metab. 274, E139–E145. doi: 10.1152/ajpendo.1998.274.1.e139

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wiegand, S., and Terasawa, E. (1982). Discrete lesions reveal functional heterogeneity of Suprachiasmatic structures in regulation of gonadotropin secretion in the female rat. Neuroendocrinology 34, 395–404. doi: 10.1159/000123335

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wierzynski, C. M., Lubenov, E. V., Gu, M., and Siapas, A. G. (2009). State-dependent spike-timing relationships between hippocampal and prefrontal circuits during sleep. Neuron 61, 587–596. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.01.011

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wilson, M. A., and Mcnaughton, B. L. (1994). Reactivation of hippocampal ensemble memories during sleep. Science 265, 676–679. doi: 10.1126/science.8036517

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wixted, J. T., Goldinger, S. D., Squire, L. R., Kuhn, J. R., Papesh, M. H., Smith, K. A., et al. (2018). Coding of episodic memory in the human hippocampus. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 115, 1093–1098. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1716443115

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wixted, J. T., Squire, L. R., Jang, Y., Papesh, M. H., Goldinger, S. D., Kuhn, J. R., et al. (2014). Sparse and distributed coding of episodic memory in neurons of the human hippocampus. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 111, 9621–9626. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1408365111

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wong, C.-C., Dohler, K.-D., Geerlings, H., and von Zur, M. A. (1983). Influence of age, strain and season on Orcadian periodicity of pituitary, gonadal and adrenal hormones in the serum of male laboratory rats. Horm. Res. 17, 202–215. doi: 10.1159/000179699

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wood, G. A., Fata, J. E., Watson, K. L. M., and Khokha, R. (2007). Circulating hormones and estrous stage predict cellular and stromal remodeling in murine uterus. Reproduction 133, 1035–1044. doi: 10.1530/REP-06-0302

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Woods, C. S., and McEwen, B. S. (1992). Estradiol mediates fluctuation in hippocampal synapse density during the estrous cycle in the adult rat. Neuroscience 12, 2549–2554. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.12-07-02549.1992

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Woolley, C. S., and McEwen, B. S. (1993). Roles of estradiol and progesterone in regulation of hippocampal dendritic spine density during the estrous cycle in the rat. J. Comp. Neurol. 336, 293–306. doi: 10.1002/cne.903360210

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Xie, L., Kang, H., Xu, Q., Chen, M. J., Liao, Y., Thiyagarajan, M., et al. (2013). Sleep drives metabolite clearance from adult brain. Science 342, 373–377. doi: 10.1126/science.1241224.Sleep

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Yokoyama, A., Domingo Ramirez, V., and Sawyer, C. H. (1965). Sleep and wakefulness in female rats under various hormonal and physiological conditions. Gen. Comp. Endocrinol. 7, 10–17. doi: 10.1016/0016-6480(66)90082-7

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Yoshinaka, K., Yamaguchi, A., Matsumura, R., Node, K., Tokuda, I., and Akashi, M. (2017). Effect of different light–dark schedules on estrous cycle in mice, and implications for mitigating the adverse impact of night work. Genes Cells 22, 876–884. doi: 10.1111/gtc.12522

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zant, J. C., Kim, T., Prokai, L., Szarka, S., McNally, J., McKenna, J. T., et al. (2016). Cholinergic neurons in the basal forebrain promote wakefulness by actions on neighboring non-cholinergic neurons: an opto-dialysis study. J. Neurosci. 36, 2057–2067. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3318-15.2016

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, S. Q., Kimura, M., and Inoué, S. (1995). Sleep patterns in cyclic and pseudopregnant rats. Neurosci. Lett. 193, 125–128. doi: 10.1016/0304-3940(95)11685-P

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: estrous cycle, sleep, sex differences, female rodents, proestrus, estrus

Citation: Swift KM, Gary NC and Urbanczyk PJ (2024) On the basis of sex and sleep: the influence of the estrous cycle and sex on sleep-wake behavior. Front. Neurosci. 18:1426189. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2024.1426189

Received: 30 April 2024; Accepted: 12 August 2024;
Published: 29 August 2024.

Edited by:

Maria Hadjimarkou, University of Sussex, United Kingdom

Reviewed by:

Michael Paul Murphy, University of Kentucky, United States
Phillip R. Kramer, Texas A and M University, United States
Jessica A. Loweth, Rowan University School of Osteopathic Medicine, United States

Copyright © 2024 Swift, Gary and Urbanczyk. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Kevin M. Swift, kevin.m.swift2.ctr@health.mil

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.