Skip to main content

EDITORIAL article

Front. Neurosci., 03 June 2024
Sec. Decision Neuroscience
This article is part of the Research Topic Neuropsychology of Human Social Decision-Making: The Role of Emotions View all 5 articles

Editorial: Neuropsychology of human social decision-making: the role of emotions

\r\nX. T. Wang
X. T. Wang1*Sarah HillSarah Hill2Hongjian CaoHongjian Cao3
  • 1Applied Psychology Division of the School of Humanities and Social Science, The Chinese University of Hong Kong, Shenzhen, China
  • 2Psychology Department, Texas Christian University, Fort Worth, TX, United States
  • 3Psychology Department, The University of Hong Kong, Pokfulam, Hong Kong SAR, China

As social beings, humans live in an increasingly complex social environment, particularly in the current time of the Internet and AI. Characterized by volatility, uncertainty, complexity, and ambiguity (VUCA), decision-making in such an environment has long been considered among the cores of human social behaviors. Notably, rapid advancements in neuroscientific techniques in the past few decades allowed a more comprehensive approach to understanding cognitive as well as emotional mechanisms underlying social decision-making (see Ernst and Paulus, 2005; Rilling and Sanfey, 2011; Ruff and Fehr, 2014; Suzuki and O'Doherty, 2020; Wallace and Hofmann, 2021).

Against this backdrop, this Research Topic represents a collective effort to follow such research leads. We gathered a group of scholarly works on the neuropsychology of human social decision-making. First, in their conceptual article, Crivelli and Balconi called for more attention to the emergence of shared representations, affective experience, and intentions shaping group relational dynamics. They proposed a multi-agent perspective on shared affects and interpersonal syntonization in group decision-making. In addition, three empirical studies were also selected. By instructing observers to voluntarily identify the orientation of a Necker cube while manipulating its ambiguity, Kuc et al. studied the perceptual bias in favor of the from-above Necker cube perspective in goal-directed behavior. In addition, they also analyzed electroencephalogram (EEG) signals to identify potential biomarkers that could explain the observed perceptual bias. Using the trust game as the decision-making paradigm, Zhu et al. conducted two experiments to examine trust asymmetry from the perspective of risk sources under descriptive vs. experiential decisions. Last but not least, in two studies using behavioral experimental procedures and EEG techniques, Vicente et al. investigated the neurophysiological correlates of interpersonal discrepancy and social adjustment in an interactive decision-making task in dyads.

While addressing different neural aspects of social decision-making, we hoped to sort out the roles of emotions in regulating social decisions. This still unfulfilled objective leaves us with more questions for future research. Over the years, the views of emotions in decision-making have undergone several transformations. The role of emotions in decision-making changed from a disturbing factor in utility theories of neoclassic economics (Elster, 1998) to a rich resource of information (Clore and Huntsinger, 2007) and the key to success (Goleman, 2006). While the Somatic Marker Hypothesis (Damasio, 1996) highlights the inevitability of emotions in decision-making, the Risk as Feelings Hypothesis (Loewenstein et al., 2001) emphasizes the unique role of anticipatory emotion (as opposed to reactive emotions) induced by episodic future thinking for guiding rational decisions (Wang et al., 2022). The articles in this Research Topic also provide insightful discussions on future directions for the neuroscience of emotions in social decision-making.

Author contributions

XW: Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. SH: Writing – review & editing. HC: Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare that financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. This research was supported, in part, by the National Natural Science Foundation of Shenzhen under grant number: JCYJ20220530143803009 and by the National Natural Science Foundation of China under grant number: NSFC 31971025 to XW.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Clore, G. L., and Huntsinger, J. R. (2007). How emotions inform judgment and regulate thought. Trends Cogn. Sci. 11, 393–399. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2007.08.005

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Damasio, A. R. (1996). The somatic marker hypothesis and the possible functions of the prefrontal cortex. Philos. Trans. Royal Soc. Lond. Ser. B 351, 1413–1420. doi: 10.1098/rstb.1996.0125

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Elster, J. (1998). Emotions and economic theory. J. Econ. Literat. 36, 47–74.

Google Scholar

Ernst, M., and Paulus, M. P. (2005). Neurobiology of decision making: a selective review from a neurocognitive and clinical perspective. Biol. Psychiatr. 58, 597–604. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.06.004

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Goleman, D. (2006). Emotional Intelligence. New York, NY: Bantam.

Google Scholar

Loewenstein, G. F., Weber, E. U., Hsee, C. K., and Welch, N. (2001). Risk as feelings. Psychol. Bullet. 127, 267–286. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.127.2.267

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rilling, J. K., and Sanfey, A. G. (2011). The neuroscience of social decision-making. Ann. Rev. Psychol. 62, 23–48. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.121208.131647

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ruff, C. C., and Fehr, E. (2014). The neurobiology of rewards and values in social decision making. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 15, 549–562. doi: 10.1038/nrn3776

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Suzuki, S., and O'Doherty, J. P. (2020). Breaking human social decision making into multiple components and then putting them together again. Cortex 127, 221–230. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2020.02.014

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wallace, K. J., and Hofmann, H. A. (2021). Decision-making in a social world: integrating cognitive ecology and social neuroscience. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 68, 152–158. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2021.03.009

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, X. T., Wang, P., Lu, J., Zhou, J., Li, G., and Garelik, S. (2022). Episodic future thinking and anticipatory emotions: effects on delay discounting and preventive behaviors during COVID-19. Appl. Psychol. 14, 1–20. doi: 10.1111/aphw.12350

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: neuropsychology, social decision making, group decision, emotions, descriptive vs. experiential decisions, interpersonal decision-making

Citation: Wang XT, Hill S and Cao H (2024) Editorial: Neuropsychology of human social decision-making: the role of emotions. Front. Neurosci. 18:1405057. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2024.1405057

Received: 22 March 2024; Accepted: 21 May 2024;
Published: 03 June 2024.

Edited and reviewed by: Bumseok Jeong, Korea Advanced Institute of Science and Technology (KAIST), Republic of Korea

Copyright © 2024 Wang, Hill and Cao. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: X. T. Wang, xtwang@cuhk.edu.cn

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.