Skip to main content

SYSTEMATIC REVIEW article

Front. Neurosci., 12 August 2024
Sec. Neurodevelopment

Executive functions in children with developmental language disorder: a systematic review and meta-analysis

Tengfei NiuTengfei Niu1Shiqiang WangShiqiang Wang2Jingxi Ma,Jingxi Ma3,4Xiaoping ZengXiaoping Zeng5Ruiling Xue,
Ruiling Xue3,5*
  • 1Department of Basic Courses, Chongqing Medical and Pharmaceutical College, Chongqing, China
  • 2Department of Neuro-Oncology, Chongqing University Cancer Hospital, Chongqing, China
  • 3Chongqing Medical University, Chongqing, China
  • 4Department of Neurology, Chongqing General Hospital, Chongqing, China
  • 5Department of Rehabilitation, Chongqing General Hospital, Chongqing, China

Background: The purpose of the current study was to assess the differences between children with developmental language disorder (DLD) and typically developing (TD) children in their performance of executive functions from working memory, inhibitory control, and cognitive flexibility.

Methods: We performed a systematical search of PubMed, Embase, Cochrane, and Web of Science for case control studies (published in English between January 1, 1950, and October 11, 2023) comparing the differences in the performance of executive functions between DLD and TD children.

Results: Forty eligible studies were included in the present study (N = 3,168 participants). In comparison with TD children, DLD children exhibited significantly poorer performances in all six verbal working memory tasks (backward digit recall task, SMD –1.4321, 95% CI –2.2692 to –0.5950; listening recall task, SMD –1.4469, 95% CI –1.7737 to –1.1202; counting recall task, SMD –0.9192, 95% CI –1.4089 to –0.4295; digit recall task, SMD –1.2321, 95% CI –1.4397 to –1.0244; word list recall task, SMD –1.1375, 95% CI –1.5579 to –0.7171; non-word recall task, SMD –1.5355, 95% CI –1.8122 to –1.2589). However, regarding inhibitory control and cognitive flexibility, the differences between DLD and TD children depended on specific circumstances. In subgroup analyses of all verbal working memory tasks, DLD children presented notably lower performance than TD children in both the monolingual English and monolingual non-English groups, and in both the preschooler and school-aged groups.

Conclusion: This study proves that verbal working memory deficits can be seen as a marker for children with DLD and are not affected by age or language type.

Systematic Review Registration: https://www.crd.york.ac.uk/PROSPERO/display_record.php?RecordID=391070, CRD42023391070.

1 Introduction

Executive function (EF) is a comprehensive term that encompasses various intricate cognitive abilities. These abilities work together to regulate lesser cognitive skills, aiming to achieve a specific objective or goal (Doebel, 2020). Currently, EFs are most accurately depicted as a sophisticated collection of cognitive abilities, incorporating elements like working memory, inhibitory control, cognitive flexibility, planning, reasoning, as well as problem-solving (Jay, 2002). Classically, EFs have been subdivided into three domains: working memory, inhibition, and cognitive flexibility (Doebel, 2020). Language serves as an organized system relying on speech, gesture, signs, and writing. It allows us to communicate, express emotions, preferences, and thoughts, contemplate our surroundings, define our identity, and articulate our history and culture (Kamkar and Morton, 2017). The close connection between EF and language becomes evident, considering that language primarily functions through symbolic representation. This involves applying a set of rules to interpret symbols (grammar), arranging symbols in a specific order for meaning (syntax), and utilizing symbols in spoken language with adjustments in pitch, intensity, and emotion (prosody) (Purves et al., 2018).

A substantial body of research indicates a positive correlation between EF and various language skills. For example, studies demonstrate a connection between EF and vocabulary knowledge (Carlson et al., 2005), sentence reading (Lewis et al., 2006), reading comprehension (Booth and Boyle, 2009; Sesma et al., 2009; Booth et al., 2010; Butterfuss and Kendeou, 2018), and syntax (White et al., 2017). As for the relationship between the three components of EF and language, evidence indicates that working memory (WM) plays a role in children’s perception of complex syntax (Georgiou and Theodorou, 2023), language acquisition is influenced by the abilities of inhibitory control (Gandolfi and Viterbori, 2020; Usai et al., 2020; Yuile and Sabbagh, 2021), and children who possess superior cognitive flexibility may be more adept at applying the diverse rules of language (Blair and Raver, 2015). Conversely, studies have shown that language could directly influence the development of EFs (Doebel, 2020; Merchán et al., 2022). Some theories propose that language skills come into play during the execution of executive tasks (Winsler et al., 2009). Furthermore, Zelazo and colleagues proposed that language serves as a foundational precursor to the emergence of EF in children (Zelazo et al., 2003). Empirical findings from longitudinal studies have provided further support for the notion that early language abilities can predict subsequent EF skills (Marcovitch and Zelazo, 2009; Vallotton and Ayoub, 2011; Kuhn et al., 2016).

Developmental language disorder (DLD), which is previously known as specific language impairment (SLI), is a condition that is characterized by difficulties in learning and utilizing language without any accompanying neurological damage, hearing loss, or cognitive impairment, as well as a lack of behavioral or emotional disorders (Bishop et al., 2017). As a condition that can affect various aspects of language such as phonology, morphology, lexicon, syntax, and pragmatics, DLD persists from preschool to adolescence and adulthood and can lead to challenges in social, emotional, academic, and vocational domain (Bishop, 2006; Brizzolara et al., 2011; St Clair et al., 2011). Therefore, examining EFs in DLD children could be especially significant in correctly understanding these children’s language and communication challenges.

According to recent studies, children with DLD may exhibit poorer performance in various oral language skills and EFs when compared to their typically developing (TD) peers (Henry et al., 2012; Leonard, 2014; Lukács et al., 2016). In exploring EFs, both cognitive tasks and behavioral evaluations are employed. Cognitive tasks have been utilized to study various critical components of EFs in children with DLD. However, there is significant variability in the number of studies that examine different components.

Working memory (WM) is the most studied key component of EFs among children with DLD. Most of these studies find evidence that children with DLD demonstrate poorer performance when compared with TD, age-matched children on WM tasks that require the storage and processing of auditory information (Dodwell and Bavin, 2008; Coady et al., 2010; Duinmeijer et al., 2012; Claessen et al., 2013; Engel de Abreu et al., 2014; Delage and Durrleman, 2018; Evans et al., 2018; Arslan et al., 2020; Balilah and Archibald, 2022). However, whether DLD children perform worse in visuospatial WM compared with TD peers is inconclusive (Henry et al., 2012; Engel de Abreu et al., 2014; Lukács et al., 2016; Balilah and Archibald, 2022), although Vugs et al. discovered significant visuospatial WM deficits in DLD children in a meta-analysis (Vugs et al., 2013; Kapa et al., 2017).

In terms of inhibitory control, Pauls et al. identified a significant difference between DLD and TD children in a meta-analysis (Pauls and Archibald, 2016). However, a study using the Bielefeld Screening for Early Recognition of Dyslexia (BISC) test found no significant differences between the DLD and TD children (Reichenbach et al., 2016). What’s more, Agnes Lukacs et al. reported controversial inhibitory control results in DLD children (Lukács et al., 2016).

Regarding cognitive flexibility, like the inhibitory control, Pauls et al. identified a significant difference between DLD and TD children in a meta-analysis (Pauls and Archibald, 2016). However, both the study using the task-switching test (Lukács et al., 2016) and the study using the Dimensional Change Card Sort (DCCS) test (Reichenbach et al., 2016) found no significant differences between the DLD and TD children.

Although the above studies on verbal WM have shown relatively consistent results, little attention has been paid to the effects of age and language on EFs in children with DLD; and studies on nonverbal WM inhibitory control, and cognitive flexibility have shown mixed results. Moreover, in previous meta-analyses (Vugs et al., 2013; Pauls and Archibald, 2016; Kapa et al., 2017) subtle discrepancies among different outcome measures were concealed by amalgamating an extensive array of data from various measurement tasks. Hence, we conducted this systematic review and meta-analysis to address the relationship between EFs and DLD children from an evidence-based perspective. In this study, to ensure the comparability of data, when an outcome is analyzed, we only pooled the data measured by the same experimental task or the same outcome indicator. For the most studied WM, we adopted Baddeley’s multicomponent WM model, comparing the performance of DLD and TD children in terms of each specific subcomponent (Baddeley, 2003). Besides, based on subgroup analyses, we further explored whether preschoolers and school-aged children presented significant differences, and whether monolingual English-speaking and monolingual non-English-speaking children had significant differences in terms of performance on WM. What’s more, this meta-analysis included not only neurocognitive measures but also the rating scale of behavior BRIEF-P in assessing different components of EFs to check whether the result depended on the specific measurement method. In addition, the current study is, to the best of our knowledge, the first systematic review and meta-analysis investigating all three key components of EFs when comparing differences in EF performance between DLD and TD children. Based on the above, the following research questions are proposed:

1. Compared with TD children, do DLD children perform significantly worse on Baddeley’s four WM subcomponents, verbal inhibitory control and nonverbal inhibitory control, and verbal cognitive flexibility and nonverbal cognitive flexibility after controlling task types?

2. Is the performance of DLD children on the three major components of EFs affected by age and language compared to TD children?

3. Do the neurocognitive measures and the rating scale of behavior BRIEF-P get the same results in assessing different components of EFs?

2 Materials and methods

This systematic review and meta-analysis was reported in accordance with the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA 2020) Statement and was registered at International Prospective Register of Systematic Reviews (No.: CRD42023391070).

2.1 Selection criteria

2.1.1 Inclusion criteria

1. In the included studies, in comparison with the TD children, children with DLD were diagnosed by a speech-language pathologist, with test scores indicating at least 1.25 SDs below the mean on various language tests (Tomblin et al., 1996).

2. There was no remarkable difference between DLD and TD children in their performance on nonverbal intelligence test (Bishop, 2014).

3. To ensure the comparability of data, for WM, we only included studies in which the outcome was measured by the same experimental task, including Backward Digit Recall, Counting Recall, Listening Recall, Working Memory Test Battery for Children, Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children, Automated Working Memory Assessment, Backward Block Tapping, Odd-one-out, Spatial Span, Digit Recall, Nonword Recall, Word List Recall, Block Recall, Dot Matrix.

2.1.2 Exclusion criteria

Duplicates; case reports, abstracts, conference papers, reviews, meta-analyses, and other non-original articles; full text not available or original research data not extractable; studies in which none of the three outcomes (WM, inhibition, and cognitive flexibility) were reported; studies in which Intelligence Quotient (IQ) of participants were not controlled; studies in which the WM task was different from that of all the other included studies; and studies from which the data obtained was not comparable to those of other included studies.

2.2 Search strategy

We collected relevant studies that were published between January 1, 1950, and Dec 31, 2022, by a systematic search of Embase, PubMed, Cochrane, and Web of Science. The language was limited to English. To reduce the risk of missing newly published literature, we conducted a supplementary search on October 11, 2023. The detailed search strategy can be seen in Supplementary Table S1.

2.3 Study selection and data extraction

We imported the retrieved literature into EndNote, excluded duplicates, read the titles and abstracts to screen the original studies for initial matches and downloaded the full text, and then finalized the included literature by reading the full text. Before carrying out data extraction, we developed a data extraction spreadsheet containing first author, publication year, study design type, language used by the participants, total number of participants, age (mean [SD]), sex, outcomes reported. Literature screening and data extraction were completed independently by two researchers and cross-checked upon completion, with a third researcher assisting in the adjudication of disputes if needed.

2.4 Quality evaluation

Two authors independently finished quality evaluation of the eligible studies using the Newcastle-Ottawa Scale (NOS), which included eight items and was divided into three dimensions: selection, comparability, and exposure (Wells et al., 2014). Quality scores on the NOS had a range from 0 to 9, with studies scoring 7 or more being rated as having high quality, those scoring 4 to 6 as having medium quality, and those scoring 3 or less as having low quality.

2.5 Outcomes

Working memory refers to a system with limited capacity responsible for temporarily maintaining and storing information in an active, online state during cognitive tasks (Baddeley, 2003). The multicomponent WM model developed by Baddeley has been widely utilized in research concerning children with DLD (Baddeley and Hitch, 1974; Baddeley, 2003). This model posits a central executive (CE) system connected to subsystems such as the phonological loop, the visuospatial sketchpad, as well as the episodic buffer. The CE system is responsible for coordinating and controlling activities within WM, but its limited attentional capacity necessitates attentional control. Since the phonological loop and visuospatial sketchpad are in charge of temporarily storing verbal and visuospatial information, respectively, they are referred to as “slave” systems. The episodic buffer, a more recent addition to the model, is believed to facilitate the integration of information from various sources into meaningful chunks (Baddeley, 2003). Because working memory is the most studied outcome and the one that was further subdivided into four components, in this meta-analysis, based on Baddeley’s model, WM is divided into four components: verbal CE, visuospatial CE, phonological loop, and visuospatial storage. Only studies that measured a subcomponent of working memory with the same experimental task were eligible to be included in this meta-analysis. Of the included literature, the verbal CE contained three tasks Backward Digit Recall, Counting Recall, and Listening Recall; the visuospatial CE included three tasks Backward Block Tapping, Odd-one-out, and Spatial Span; the phonological loop covered three tasks Digit Recall, Nonword Recall, and Word List Recall; and the visuospatial storage contained Block Recall, Dot Matrix, and Mazes Memory. Given that a task can be tested differently, this study also conducted subgroup analyses based on the specific test method.

According to Diamond’s definition, inhibition, also known as inhibitory control, involves avoiding being distracted by external factors or refraining from making a predominant but erroneous response (Diamond, 2013). To ensure comparability of data, in this study, verbal inhibition was analyzed according to the measurement of accuracy; nonverbal inhibition was analyzed according to the measurement of accuracy, reaction time, and error numbers, respectively.

Cognitive flexibility, also known as shifting or task switching, is the capacity to modify one’s thoughts and actions in response to changes in the environment (Seiferth and Thienel, 2013). In this study, cognitive flexibility was statistically analyzed separately by verbal and non-verbal cognitive flexibility.

2.6 Statistical analysis

We assessed the difference between DLD and TD children on three outcomes: WM, inhibitory control, and cognitive flexibility.

The pooled estimates of the mean differences between DLD and TD children were calculated using a random-effects model or a fixed-effects model. The results were analyzed by calculating the SMD and the 95% CI. We used the Cochran I2 test to assess heterogeneity across the studies, with I2 > 50% suggesting moderate-to-high heterogeneity, and I2 less than or equal to 50% as low heterogeneity. When I2 was greater than 50%, a random-effects model was used for meta-analysis and when I2 was less than or equal to 50%, a fixed-effects model was adopted. Here’s a simplified explanation of how the Cochran I2 test works (Huedo-Medina et al., 2006):

1. Q Test: The I2 statistic is derived from the Q statistic, which is calculated as part of the meta-analysis process. The Q statistic follows a chi-square distribution and tests the null hypothesis that all studies are estimating the same effect size.

2. Degrees of Freedom (df): The degrees of freedom for the Q test are calculated as the number of studies minus one (k – 1).

3. I2 Calculation: The I2 statistic is then calculated using the formula:

I2 = Q d f Q  × 100%, where Q is the Cochran’s Q heterogeneity statistic.

For the three outcomes, we also analyzed subgroups according to the specific test method, age (the preschooler and the school-aged subgroups), and language (English and non-English subgroups). We used R (version 4.3.1) for meta-analysis.

3 Results

3.1 Study selection

We initially retrieved 4,815 reports, and after excluding 1,688 duplicate publications, we read the titles and abstracts of the remaining 3,127 reports and screened 96 studies relevant to our topic. After reading the full text of 96 reports, we found that 25 reports did not report the outcome relevant to this meta-analysis, the tasks for measuring the outcomes in 9 reports could not be compared with those of the included studies, and the data for the outcomes in 10 reports could not be compared with the data of the included studies, 9 reports were non-English, and 3 reports did not control the IQ of the children studied. After further exclusion of the 56 reports mentioned above, a final total of 40 studies (Dodwell and Bavin, 2008; Coady et al., 2010; Lum and Zarafa, 2010; Mainela-Arnold et al., 2010; Spanoudis and Natsopoulos, 2011; Duinmeijer et al., 2012; Farrant et al., 2012; Henry et al., 2012; Hutchinson et al., 2012; Lum et al., 2012; Lum and Bleses, 2012; Claessen et al., 2013; Wittke et al., 2013; Engel de Abreu et al., 2014; Taylor et al., 2014; Vugs et al., 2014, 2017; Jackson et al., 2016; Lukács et al., 2016, 2017; Reichenbach et al., 2016; Kapa et al., 2017; Kuusisto et al., 2017; Laloi et al., 2017; Delage and Durrleman, 2018; Evans et al., 2018; Schaeffer et al., 2018; Fyfe et al., 2019; Ladányi and Lukács, 2019; Sikora et al., 2019; Arslan et al., 2020; Guiberson and Rodríguez, 2020; Kapa and Erikson, 2020; Marini et al., 2020; Gillam et al., 2021; Ralli et al., 2021; Balilah and Archibald, 2022; Kalliontzi et al., 2022; Thomas et al., 2022) were included (Figure 1).

Figure 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Flow chart of literature screening.

3.2 Study characteristics and quality assessment of the included studies

The 40 studies included in this meta-analysis were all case–control studies containing 1,307 children with DLD (age range 3.44–11.53 years) and 1861 TD children (age range 3.52–10.7 years). Thirty-four papers reported gender matched DLD children and TD children, while the remaining 6 papers did not disclose the gender of the participants. Of all 40 studies, 37 studies reported on working memory; 11 studies reported on inhibitory control, and 7 studies reported on cognitive flexibility. In terms of language, 18 studies dealt with monolingual English-speaking participants, 18 studies focused on monolingual non-English-speaking participants, 4 studies reported bilingual participants, and 2 studies had both monolingual and bilingual participants. In terms of age, 15 studies reported preschoolers and 28 studies reported school-aged children. Based on the NOS scores, 7 studies were of moderate quality, while the other 33 were of high quality (Table 1).

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Characteristics of eligible studies.

3.3 Meta-analysis

3.3.1 Working memory (WM)

3.3.1.1 WM-verbal CE
3.3.1.1.1 Merging of effect sizes

Thirteen studies reported on the Backward Digit Recall (BDR) task, and a meta-analysis with a random-effects model (I2 = 88.5%) revealed that children in the TD group significantly outperformed children in the DLD group (SMD = –1.4321, 95% CI: −2.2692 to –0.5950). Five studies reported the Counting Recall (CR) task, and a meta-analysis with a random-effects model (I2 = 86.6%) revealed that children in the DLD group underperformed significantly compared with those in the TD group (SMD = –0.9192, 95% CI: −1.4089 to –0.4295). Eleven studies reported on the Listening Recall (LR) task, and a meta-analysis using a random-effects model (I2 = 76.5%) demonstrated that children in the TD group performed remarkably better compared with those in the DLD group (SMD = –1.4469, 95% CI: −1.7737 to –1.1202) (Supplementary Table S2).

3.3.1.1.2 Subgroup analyses

In our subgroup analyses by the specific test, the BDR task was dominated by the Working Memory Test Battery for Children (WMTB-C, studies = 4) and the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (WIS-C, studies = 5); the CR task included the Automated Working Memory Assessment (AWMA, studies = 3) and WMTB-C (Studies = 2); and the WMTB-C (Studies = 6) predominated in the LR task. For the different tests of the BDR, CR and LR tasks, children in the DLD group performed significantly worse than children in the TD group (Supplementary Table S2). For age subgroups, in both the preschooler subgroup and the school-aged subgroup the TD children significantly outperformed their respective DLD children in the three tasks of BDR, CR, and LR (p < 0.05). Similarly, for the language subgroups, in both the English and non-English subgroups the TD children significantly outperformed their respective DLD children (p < 0.05) in the above three tasks (Supplementary Table S2).

3.3.1.2 WM-visuospatial CE
3.3.1.2.1 Merging of effect sizes

Two studies reported Backward Block Tapping (BBT) task, and a fixed-effects model (I2 = 0.0%) was adopted for meta-analysis revealing that children in the TD group significantly outperformed children in the DLD group (SMD = –0.6089, 95% CI: −1.0469 to –0.1709). Odd-one-out (OOO) task was reported in seven studies, and a random-effects model (I2 = 85.8%) was used for the meta-analysis, which suggested that children in the DLD group performed significantly worse than children in the TD group (SMD = -0.4923, 95% CI: −0.9140 to –0.0706). Four studies reported Spatial Span (SS) task, and the meta-analysis was completed with a random-effects model (I2 = 77.7%), showing that children in the TD group significantly outperformed children in the DLD group (SMD = –0.6787, 95%CI: −1.0812 to –0.2762) (Supplementary Table S3).

3.3.1.2.2 Subgroup analyses

In our subgroup analysis by the specific test, OOO task was mainly implemented through Odd-one-out Test (Henry, 2001, studies = 3) and Automated Working Memory Assessment (Alloway 2007, studies = 3). According to the Odd-one-out Test (Henry, 2001), the DLD group performed remarkably worse compared with the TD group (p < 0.05), but AWMA revealed no significant difference between the DLD group and the TD group (p > 0.05) (Supplementary Table S3).

For both OOO and SS tasks, in the subgroup of preschoolers, the DLD children performed remarkably worse than the TD counterparts (p < 0.05), but no significant difference was observed between the DLD and the TD children in the school-age subgroup (p > 0.05). Based on the subgroup analysis by language in OOO task, significant differences were found between the DLD and the TD children in the English subgroup (p < 0.05), while no significant difference was observed between the DLD and the TD children in the non-English subgroup (p > 0.05) (Supplementary Table S3).

3.3.1.3 WM-phonological loop
3.3.1.3.1 Merging of effect sizes

A random-effects model (I2 = 64.7%) was employed for the meta-analysis of Digit Recall (DR) task in 18 studies, and the results showed that children in the DLD group remarkably underperformed children in the TD group (SMD = –1.2321, 95% CI: −1.4397 to –1.0244). Nineteen studies reported on the Nonword Recall (NWR) task, and the meta-analysis using a random-effects model (I2 = 79.1%) indicated that children in the TD group significantly outperformed children in the DLD group (SMD = –1.5355, 95% CI: –1.8122 to –1.2589). Seven studies reported the Word List Recall (WLR) task and a random-effects model (I2 = 82.0%) was adopted for meta-analysis. The result revealed that children in the DLD group had significantly worse performance than children in the TD group (SMD = –1.1375, 95% CI: −1.5579 to –0.7171) (Supplementary Table S4).

3.3.1.3.2 Subgroup analyses

In our subgroup analyses by the specific test, the DR task was dominated by WMTB-C (Studies = 3), WIS-C (Studies = 4), and AWMA (Studies = 5); the NWR task mainly included the NWR Task (Dollaghan and Campbell 1998, studies = 7), and the WLR was dominated by the AWMA (Studies = 1) and WMTB-C (Studies = 2). Children in the DLD group remarkably underperformed children in the TD group in all tests of the DR, NWR, and WLR tasks (Supplementary Table S4).

In our subgroup analyses by age, in both the preschooler subgroup and the school-aged subgroup the DLD children performed significantly worse (p < 0.05) compared to their respective TD children in the DR, NWR, and WLR tasks. In the subgroup analyses by language, DLD children in both the English and non-English subgroups performed significantly worse (p < 0.05) compared to their respective TD children in the DR, NWR and WLR tasks (Supplementary Table S4).

3.3.1.4 WM-visuospatial CE
3.3.1.4.1 Merging of effect sizes

Seven studies reported the Block Recall (BR) task, and a random-effects model (I2 = 65.8%) was employed for meta-analysis, which revealed that children in the TD group performed significantly better than children in the DLD group (SMD = –0.3569, 95% CI: −0.6361 to –0.0777). Five studies reported the Dot Matrix (DM) task, and a random-effects model (I2 = 85.4%) was employed for meta-analysis, which showed that children in the DLD group markedly underperformed children in the TD group (SMD = –0.6561, 95% CI: −1.1879 to –0.1244). Three studies reported the Mazes Memory (MM) task, and a meta-analysis with a random effects model (I2 = 91.7%) indicated no significant difference between the DLD and TD groups (SMD = –0.5525, 95% CI: −1.4187 to 0.3137) (Supplementary Table S5).

3.3.1.4.2 Subgroup analyses

In our subgroup analysis by the specific test, BR task mainly included WMTB-C (Studies = 3) and AWMA (Studies = 2). WMTB-C test showed that the DLD group markedly underperformed the TD group, while other test methods showed no significant difference between the DLD group and the TD group. Unlike the condition in BR task, WMTB-C (Studies = 2) in MM task showed no significant difference between the DLD group and the TD group.

For all the BR, DM, and MM tasks, in the preschooler subgroup the DLD children markedly underperformed TD children (p < 0.05), whereas no significant difference was observed between the DLD and the TD children in the school-aged subgroup (p > 0.05). Language subgroup analysis in BR task showed that in the English subgroup, the DLD children significantly performed worse than the TD children (p < 0.05), while in the non-English subgroup, the difference between the two groups was not significant (p > 0.05). Contrary to the above result, language subgroup analysis in MM task showed no significant difference between DLD and TD children in the English subgroup (p > 0.05), but significant differences were observed between DLD and TD children in the non-English subgroup (p < 0.05).

3.3.2 Inhibitory control

3.3.2.1 Merging of effect sizes

Three studies reported verbal inhibition with Accuracy Rate as the outcome indicator, and the meta-analysis using a random-effects model (I2 = 97.7%) indicated that the difference between the DLD and TD groups was not significant (SMD = -1.9915, 95% CI: −4.5188 to 0.5358). Three studies reported nonverbal inhibition with Accuracy Rate as the outcome indicator, and the meta-analysis using a random-effects model (I2 = 94.6%) revealed that the DLD children markedly underperformed the TD counterparts (SMD = –1.9343, 95% CI: −3.4606 to –0.4079). Another six studies reported nonverbal inhibition using Reaction Time as the outcome indicator, and the meta-analysis using a random-effects model (I2 = 94.5%) demonstrated that the difference between the DLD and TD children was not significant (SMD = 1.1651, 95% CI: −0.2643 to 2.5944). Two other studies reported nonverbal inhibition using Error Numbers as the outcome indicator, and the meta-analysis using a random-effects model (I2 = 57.2%) revealed that the TD children markedly outperformed the DLD children (SMD = 0.6643, 95% CI: 0.1092 to 1.2193) (Supplementary Table S6).

3.3.2.2 Subgroup analyses

In the experiments where nonverbal inhibition was measured using Reaction Time as the outcome indicator, no significant difference was found between the DLD children and their respective TD children in either the preschooler subgroup or the school-aged subgroup (p > 0.05), as well as between the DLD and TD children in the non-English subgroup (p > 0.05), whereas TD children performed better compared to DLD children in the bilingual subgroup (Supplementary Table S6).

3.3.3 Cognitive flexibility

3.3.3.1 Merging of effect sizes

Four included studies measured verbal cognitive flexibility and the meta-analysis with a fixed-effects model (I2 = 0.0%) suggested a marked worse performance in children with DLD than in children with TD (SMD = –0.4690, 95% CI: −0.6397 to –0.2982). Nonverbal cognitive flexibility was measured in five studies and the meta-analysis using a random-effects model (I2 = 96.9%) revealed that the difference between DLD and TD children was not significant (SMD –1.9792, 95% CI: –4.7906 to 0.8323) (Supplementary Table S7).

3.3.3.2 Subgroup analyses

In experiments measuring verbal cognitive flexibility, in both the preschool and school-age subgroups DLD children performed significantly worse (p < 0.05) compared to their respective TD children, and in both the English and non-English subgroups the DLD children performed significantly worse (p < 0.05) compared to their respective TD children. In the experiment measuring nonverbal cognitive flexibility, no significant difference was observed between the DLD and TD children in both the English subgroup and the non-English subgroup (Supplementary Table S7).

3.3.4 BRIEF-parent behavioral measurement

Three studies tested elements of EF using the BRIEF-parent scale, and the meta-analysis with a fixed-effects model (I2 = 0.0%) revealed that the DLD children performed significantly worse compared to TD children (SMD = 0.837, 95% CI: 0.657 to 1.018). Subgroup analyses of the three main components WM, Inhibition, and Cognitive Flexibility indicated that children with DLD markedly underperformed TD children on all three components (p < 0.05) (Supplementary Table S8).

4 Discussion

Our findings from 40 studies involving 3,168 participants demonstrate that in neurocognitive tasks DLD children exhibited notably lower performance than TD children in verbal WM. However, regarding visuospatial WM, whether the difference between DLD and TD children is significant depends on the specific task. In terms of verbal inhibitory control, no significant difference was found between the DLD and TD children whereas as for nonverbal inhibitory control, whether there is a significant difference between DLD and TD children depends on how the outcome was measured. With respect to verbal cognitive flexibility, children with DLD markedly underperformed TD children, whereas there was no significant difference between children with DLD and TD children in terms of nonverbal cognitive flexibility. In contrast, across all measured outcomes using the BRIEF-P Scale, DLD children presented markedly worse performance compared with TD children.

A few recent studies have also revealed that children with DLD had significantly poorer performance than TD children on verbal WM tasks (Guiberson and Rodríguez, 2020; Kapa and Erikson, 2020; Jackson et al., 2021; Larson and Ellis Weismer, 2022). Consistent with these studies, the present analysis provides further support and elaboration on previous discoveries in several aspects. Unlike previous studies, the current analysis further divided verbal WM into two components, verbal CE and verbal storage, and measured them separately using three tasks, yielding the same result as previous studies. Furthermore, since Klara Marton et al. found that the performance of WM is influenced by the linguistic attributes of the language accessed, the present analysis further divided children into monolingual English and monolingual non-English subgroups (Marton et al., 2007). It concluded that children with DLD underperformed TD children on verbal WM in all languages. We believe that although each language has its specific linguistic attributes that can affect verbal WM performance, the effects of these linguistic attributes on the WM performance in children with DLD and TD children are similar under a particular language and so do not affect the result that children with DLD perform significantly worse than TD children on verbal WM tasks. Given that single studies can only focus on a particular age group and that children at different ages have different levels of neurological development, the present analysis further divided the children into preschool and school-aged subgroups and concluded that children with DLD underperformed TD children on verbal WM in both preschool and school-aged subgroups.

In a prior meta-analysis, Vugs et al. found deficits in both storage and CE components of visuospatial WM in children with DLD (Vugs et al., 2013). Consistent with Vugs’s study, DLD children had significant weaker CE capacities on three visuospatial WM tasks than TD children in this study. However, regarding visuospatial storage, two of three tasks revealed that the difference between DLD and TD children was significant, while another MM task showed that the difference between the two groups was not significant. We believe that the different results between this analysis and Vugs’s study are mainly because Vugs’s data comes from different tasks. By combining a wide range of data collected from various measurement tasks, the subtle discrepancies among different measurement tasks in Vugs’s study were masked. Unlike Vugs’s analysis, in this study, only data obtained by the same experimental task were included and compared to ensure the accuracy of the analysis. Given that MM task was only reported in three papers, more original studies are needed in the future to explore further results.

Through subgroup analysis, we found that in the preschool subgroup children with DLD markedly underperformed TD children in all visuospatial WM tasks, but that in the school-aged subgroup children with DLD did not differ significantly from TD children in all the tasks. The above findings can be interpreted in two ways. One possibility is that at preschool age, the elements of EF have not yet undergone complete differentiation so other EF elements may influence visuospatial WM performance on the specific task. As EF elements differentiate in late childhood, children’s performance on visuospatial WM tasks can reflect their actual level. It is the gradual emergence of distinct EF elements that contribute to different performances between the preschool group and the school-aged group. Several studies, including Shing et al. and Gandolfi et al. have discovered evidence supporting this gradual emergence of EF elements (Shing et al., 2010; Gandolfi et al., 2014). The other possible reason is that the potential difficulties in verbal and visuospatial skills during preschool years might decrease as children with DLD develop visual recoding abilities over time, allowing them to compensate for their verbal impairments while maintaining their visual capacity (Engel de Abreu et al., 2014). What’s more, the subgroup analysis based on the age factor demonstrated that in the English subgroup the DLD children performed significantly worse than the TD children in both the visuospatial CE and storage tasks. In contrast, in the non-English subgroup, the DLD children did not differ significantly from its TD counterparts. We believe that specific characteristics of different languages may affect DLD children’s visuospatial WM abilities, depending on the language they speak.

In terms of inhibitory control, Archibald and Leonard found that although children with DLD underperformed TD children, this difference was insignificant (Archibald and Gathercole, 2006; Leonard, 2014). In this study, the meta-analysis on inhibitory control is partially consistent with the above result. Given that the analysis only included a limited number of studies, more original studies are needed in the future to explore further results. And another meta-analysis by Pauls et al. concluded that children with DLD were significantly worse on inhibitory control and cognitive flexibility than TD children (Pauls and Archibald, 2016). In this study, we further divided cognitive flexibility into the verbal part and the nonverbal part and got a partially different conclusion. We believe that this difference may be due to two factors. One is that Pauls’s study did not present the original data. Because of the different data conversion standards, the accuracy of the data included in the study may have been affected, which in turn may have further affected the result of the data analysis. Second, his study was a comparative analysis of data obtained through different experimental tasks, ignoring the differences in the different experimental tasks themselves.

In addition to the findings based on the neurocognitive tasks described above, the BRIEF-P scale was used to assess the performance in children with DLD and TD children in everyday life. This behavioral measure revealed that children with DLD markedly underperformed TD children on the three main components of EF. This above finding is consistent with that in the study of Cuperus et al. (2014). We believe that the different results between neurocognitive tasks and the behavioral scale BRIEF-P is because in neurocognitive tasks we can influence the results by artificially adjusting the difficulty of the task, whereas the problems children encounter in their everyday life are more natural and flexible.

5 Limitations

There are several limitations of the current meta-analysis. The strict inclusion criteria for this study, whereby the included comparable experimental data were derived from the same task, objectively limited the amount of included literature. Of the 21 sets of data analysis in this study, 9 sets of data analysis included less than 5 pieces of literature, so the results of this study must be treated with caution. Furthermore, heterogeneity was high in 16 of the 22 sets of data analysis, particularly in all three neurocognitive tasks of the three outcomes, making it even more essential for us to treat the results of this study with caution. The author suggests that the high heterogeneity of these data analyses may be due to the differences in task difficulty and the way scores were tallied across experiments, even though each set of data originated from the same experimental task. What’s more, this study does not divide DLD children into receptive and productive DLD subtypes to compare their performance in EFs.

6 Conclusion and clinical implications

In conclusion, this research reinforces the idea that deficits in verbal WM can be considered a marker for children with DLD. The extent to which children with DLD differ from TD children in visuospatial WM performance varies depending on the specific neurocognitive task. The performance of children with DLD on visuospatial WM tasks remains unaffected by language and age, whereas age influences their performance on visuospatial WM tasks. Moreover, different languages may influence the performance of children with DLD on visuospatial WM tasks. Further studies are needed to explore the difference in the performance of children with DLD on inhibitory control and cognitive flexibility.

This study has important clinical implications. First, it suggests that professionals should consider multiple aspects of EF such as WM, inhibitory control, and cognitive flexibility when assessing and treating children with DLD. Second, it supports the development and implementation of targeted interventions to improve EF in children with DLD, potentially improving their language ability and other related cognitive skills. Third, the study suggests that when we design interventions, we should take into account that children with DLD may have different needs and responses at different ages and stages of development. Moreover, by designing a measure encompassing both neurocognitive and behavioral modalities, we will comprehensively assess the EFs of children with DLD to formulate interventions, allowing for further development of the children’s EFs.

Data availability statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/Supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding authors.

Author contributions

TN: Conceptualization, Supervision, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. SW: Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – original draft. JM: Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – original draft. XZ: Resources, Supervision, Writing – original draft. RX: Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare that financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. This research was partly supported by Medical Research Project of Chongqing Municipal Health Commission, grant number 2023WSJK008.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnins.2024.1390987/full#supplementary-material

Abbreviations

SMD, standard mean difference; 95% CI, 95% confidence interval; DLD, developmental language disorder; TD, typically developing; EF, executive function; SLI, specific language impairment; WM, working memory; CE, central executive; DCCS, dimensional change card sort; BDR, backward digit recall; CR, counting recall; LR, listening recall; WMTB-C, working memory test battery for children; WIS-C, Wechsler intelligence scale for children.; AWMA, automated working memory assessment; BBT, backward block tapping; OOO, odd-one-out; SS, spatial span; DR, digit recall; NWR, non-word recall; WLR, word list recall; BR, block recall; DM, dot matrix

References

Alloway, T. P. (2007). Working memory, reading, and mathematical skills in children with developmental coordination disorder. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 96, 20–36.

Google Scholar

Archibald, L. M., and Gathercole, S. E. (2006). Nonword repetition: a comparison of tests. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 49, 970–983. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2006/070)

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Arslan, S., Broc, L., Olive, T., and Mathy, F. (2020). Reduced deficits observed in children and adolescents with developmental language disorder using proper nonverbalizable span tasks. Res. Dev. Disabil. 96:103522. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2019.103522

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Baddeley, A. (2003). Working memory and language: an overview. J. Commun. Disord. 36, 189–208. doi: 10.1016/s0021-9924(03)00019-4

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Baddeley, A. D., and Hitch, G. (1974). The psychology of learning and motivation: working memory. ed. G. Bower (New York: Academic Press).

Google Scholar

Balilah, A., and Archibald, L. (2022). Processing-dependent measures sensitive to language performance differences in Arabic-speaking English language learners compared to children with developmental language disorder. Can. J. Speech Lang. Pathol. Audiol. 46, 171–184.

Google Scholar

Bishop, D. V. (2006). What causes specific language impairment in children? Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 15, 217–221. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2006.00439.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bishop, D. V. (2014). Uncommon understanding (classic edition): Development and disorders of language comprehension in children; psychology press. London, UK: Taylor & Francis Group.

Google Scholar

Bishop, D. V. M., Snowling, M. J., Thompson, P. A., and Greenhalgh, T. (2017). Phase 2 of CATALISE: a multinational and multidisciplinary Delphi consensus study of problems with language development: terminology. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 58, 1068–1080. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12721

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Blair, C., and Raver, C. C. (2015). School readiness and self-regulation: a developmental psychobiological approach. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 66, 711–731. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-010814-015221

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Booth, J. N., and Boyle, J. M. E. (2009). The role of inhibitory functioning in children’s reading skills. Educ. Psychol. Pract. 25, 339–350. doi: 10.1080/02667360903315164

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Booth, J. N., Boyle, J. M., and Kelly, S. W. (2010). Do tasks make a difference? Accounting for heterogeneity of performance of children with reading difficulties on tasks of executive function: findings from a meta-analysis. Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 28, 133–176. doi: 10.1348/026151009x485432

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Brizzolara, D., Gasperini, F., Pfanner, L., Cristofani, P., Casalini, C., and Chilosi, A. M. (2011). Long-term reading and spelling outcome in Italian adolescents with a history of specific language impairment. Cortex 47, 955–973. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2011.02.009

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Butterfuss, R., and Kendeou, P. (2018). The role of executive functions in Reading comprehension. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 30, 801–826. doi: 10.1007/s10648-017-9422-6

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Carlson, S. M., Davis, A. C., and Leach, J. G. (2005). Less is more: executive function and symbolic representation in preschool children. Psychol. Sci. 16, 609–616. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2005.01583.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Claessen, M., Leitão, S., Kane, R., and Williams, C. (2013). Phonological processing skills in specific language impairment. Int. J. Speech Lang. Pathol. 15, 471–483. doi: 10.3109/17549507.2012.753110

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Coady, J., Evans, J. L., and Kluender, K. R. (2010). Role of phonotactic frequency in nonword repetition by children with specific language impairments. Int. J. Lang. Commun. Disord. 45, 494–509. doi: 10.3109/13682820903222783

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cuperus, J., Vugs, B., Scheper, A., and Hendriks, M. (2014). Executive function behaviours in children with specific language impairment (SLI). Int. J. Develop. Disabil. 60, 132–143. doi: 10.1179/2047387714Y.0000000049

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Delage, H., and Durrleman, S. (2018). Developmental dyslexia and specific language impairment: distinct syntactic profiles? Clin. Linguist. Phon. 32, 758–785. doi: 10.1080/02699206.2018.1437222

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 64, 135–168. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dodwell, K., and Bavin, E. L. (2008). Children with specific language impairment: an investigation of their narratives and memory. Int. J. Lang. Commun. Disord. 43, 201–218. doi: 10.1080/13682820701366147

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Doebel, S. (2020). Rethinking executive function and its development. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 15, 942–956. doi: 10.1177/1745691620904771

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dollaghan, C., and Campbell, T. F. (1998). Nonword repetition and child language Impairment. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 41, 1136–1146.

Google Scholar

Duinmeijer, I., de Jong, J., and Scheper, A. (2012). Narrative abilities, memory and attention in children with a specific language impairment. Int. J. Lang. Commun. Disord. 47, 542–555. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-6984.2012.00164.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Engel de Abreu, P. M., Cruz-Santos, A., and Puglisi, M. L. (2014). Specific language impairment in language-minority children from low-income families. Int. J. Lang. Commun. Disord. 49, 736–747. doi: 10.1111/1460-6984.12107

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Evans, J. L., Gillam, R. B., and Montgomery, J. W. (2018). Cognitive predictors of spoken word recognition in children with and without developmental language disorders. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 61, 1409–1425. doi: 10.1044/2018_jslhr-l-17-0150

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Farrant, B. M., Maybery, M. T., and Fletcher, J. (2012). Language, cognitive flexibility, and explicit false belief understanding: longitudinal analysis in typical development and specific language impairment. Child Dev. 83, 223–235. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01681.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fyfe, E. R., Matz, L. E., Hunt, K. M., and Alibali, M. W. (2019). Mathematical thinking in children with developmental language disorder: the roles of pattern skills and verbal working memory. J. Commun. Disord. 77, 17–30. doi: 10.1016/j.jcomdis.2018.11.001

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gandolfi, E., and Viterbori, P. (2020). Inhibitory control skills and language Acquisition in Toddlers and Preschool Children. Lang. Learn. 70, 604–642. doi: 10.1111/lang.12388

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gandolfi, E., Viterbori, P., Traverso, L., and Usai, M. C. (2014). Inhibitory processes in toddlers: a latent-variable approach. Front. Psychol. 5:381. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00381

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Georgiou, G. P., and Theodorou, E. (2023). Comprehension of complex syntax by non-English-speaking children with developmental language disorder: a scoping review. Clin. Linguist. Phon. 37, 1050–1068. doi: 10.1080/02699206.2022.2135024

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gillam, R. B., Serang, S., Montgomery, J. W., and Evans, J. L. (2021). Cognitive processes related to memory capacity explain nearly all of the variance in language test performance in school-age children with and without developmental language disorder. Front. Psychol. 12:724356. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.724356

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Guiberson, M. M., and Rodríguez, B. L. (2020). Working memory and linguistic performance of dual language learners with and without developmental language disorders. Am. J. Speech Lang. Pathol. 29, 1301–1306. doi: 10.1044/2019_ajslp-19-00109

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Henry, L. A. (2001). How does the severity of a learning disability affect working memory performance? Memory 9, 233–247.

Google Scholar

Henry, L. A., Messer, D. J., and Nash, G. (2012). Executive functioning in children with specific language impairment. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 53, 37–45. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2011.02430.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Huedo-Medina, T. B., Sánchez-Meca, J., Marín-Martínez, F., and Botella, J. (2006). Assessing heterogeneity in meta-analysis: Q statistic or I2 index? Psychol. Methods 11, 193–206. doi: 10.1037/1082-989X.11.2.193

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hutchinson, E., Bavin, E., Efron, D., and Sciberras, E. (2012). A comparison of working memory profiles in school-aged children with specific language impairment, attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder, comorbid SLI and ADHD and their typically developing peers. Child Neuropsychol. 18, 190–207. doi: 10.1080/09297049.2011.601288

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Jackson, E., Leitao, S., and Claessen, M. (2016). The relationship between phonological short-term memory, receptive vocabulary, and fast mapping in children with specific language impairment. Int. J. Lang. Commun. Disord. 51, 61–73. doi: 10.1111/1460-6984.12185

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Jackson, E., Leitão, S., Claessen, M., and Boyes, M. (2021). Word learning and verbal working memory in children with developmental language disorder. Autism Develop. Lang. Impair. 6, 239694152110041–239694152110020. doi: 10.1177/23969415211004109

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Jay, B. (2002). The psychology of language. London: Pearson College Division, Prentice Hall.

Google Scholar

Kalliontzi, E., Ralli, A. M., Palikara, O., and Roussos, P. (2022). Examining the relationship between oral language skills and executive functions: evidence from Greek-speaking 4-5-year-old children with and without developmental language disorder. Res. Dev. Disabil. 124:104215. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2022.104215

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kamkar, N. H., and Morton, J. B. (2017). CanDiD: a framework for linking executive function and education. Front. Psychol. 8:1187. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01187

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kapa, L. L., and Erikson, J. A. (2020). The relationship between word learning and executive function in preschoolers with and without developmental language disorder. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 63, 2293–2307. doi: 10.1044/2020_jslhr-19-00342

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kapa, L. L., Plante, E., and Doubleday, K. (2017). Applying an integrative framework of executive function to preschoolers with specific language impairment. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 60, 2170–2184. doi: 10.1044/2017_jslhr-l-16-0027

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kuhn, L. J., Willoughby, M. T., Vernon-Feagans, L., and Blair, C. B. (2016). The contribution of children's time-specific and longitudinal expressive language skills on developmental trajectories of executive function. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 148, 20–34. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2016.03.008

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kuusisto, M. A., Nieminen, P. E., Helminen, M. T., and Kleemola, L. (2017). Executive and intellectual functioning in school-aged children with specific language impairment. Int. J. Lang. Commun. Disord. 52, 127–136. doi: 10.1111/1460-6984.12263

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ladányi, E., and Lukács, Á. (2019). Word retrieval difficulties and cognitive control in specific language impairment. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 62, 918–931. doi: 10.1044/2018_jslhr-l-17-0446

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Laloi, A., Jong, J. D. D., and Baker, A. E. (2017). Can executive functioning contribute to the diagnosis of SLI in bilingual children?: a study on response inhibition. Linguistic Approaches Biling. 7, 431–459. doi: 10.1075/lab.15020.lal

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Larson, C., and Ellis Weismer, S. (2022). Working memory performance in children with developmental language disorder: the role of domain. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 65, 1906–1920. doi: 10.1044/2022_JSLHR-21-00420

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Leonard, L. B. (2014). Children with specific language impairment. Cambridge, MA: MIT press.

Google Scholar

Lewis, R. L., Vasishth, S., and Van Dyke, J. A. (2006). Computational principles of working memory in sentence comprehension. Trends Cogn. Sci. 10, 447–454. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2006.08.007

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lukács, Á., Kemény, F., Lum, J. A., and Ullman, M. T. (2017). Learning and overnight retention in declarative memory in specific language impairment. PLoS One 12:e0169474. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0169474

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lukács, Á., Ladányi, E., Fazekas, K., and Kemény, F. (2016). Executive functions and the contribution of short-term memory span in children with specific language impairment. Neuropsychology 30, 296–303. doi: 10.1037/neu0000232

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lum, J. A., and Bleses, D. (2012). Declarative and procedural memory in Danish speaking children with specific language impairment. J. Commun. Disord. 45, 46–58. doi: 10.1016/j.jcomdis.2011.09.001

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lum, J. A., Conti-Ramsden, G., Page, D., and Ullman, M. T. (2012). Working, declarative and procedural memory in specific language impairment. Cortex 48, 1138–1154. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2011.06.001

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lum, J. A., and Zarafa, M. (2010). Relationship between verbal working memory and the SCAN-C in children with specific language impairment. Lang. Speech Hear. Serv. Sch. 41, 521–530. doi: 10.1044/0161-1461(2010/09-0035)

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mainela-Arnold, E., Evans, J. L., and Coady, J. A. (2010). Explaining lexical-semantic deficits in specific language impairment: the role of phonological similarity, phonological working memory, and lexical competition. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 53, 1742–1756. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2010/08-0198)

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Marcovitch, S., and Zelazo, P. D. (2009). A hierarchical competing systems model of the emergence and early development of executive function. Dev. Sci. 12, 1–18. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00754.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Marini, A., Piccolo, B., Taverna, L., Berginc, M., and Ozbič, M. (2020). The complex relation between executive functions and language in preschoolers with developmental language disorders. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 17:1772. doi: 10.3390/ijerph17051772

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Marton, K., Kelmenson, L., and Pinkhasova, M. (2007). Inhibition control and working memory capacity in children with SLI. Psikhologyah 50, 110–121. doi: 10.2117/psysoc.2007.110

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Merchán, A., Fernández García, L., Gioiosa Maurno, N., Ruiz Castañeda, P., and Daza González, M. T. (2022). Executive functions in deaf and hearing children: the mediating role of language skills in inhibitory control. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 218:105374. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2022.105374

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pauls, L. J., and Archibald, L. M. (2016). Executive functions in children with specific language impairment: a Meta-analysis. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 59, 1074–1086. doi: 10.1044/2016_jslhr-l-15-0174

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Purves, D., Augustine, G., Fitzpatrick, D., Hall, W., LaMantia, A., Mooney, R., et al. (2018). Neuroscience. 6th Edn. Sunderland, MA: Sinauer Associates.

Google Scholar

Ralli, A. M., Chrysochoou, E., Roussos, P., Diakogiorgi, K., Dimitropoulou, P., and Filippatou, D. (2021). Executive function, working memory, and verbal fluency in relation to non-verbal intelligence in Greek-speaking school-age children with developmental language disorder. Brain Sci. 11:604. doi: 10.3390/brainsci11050604

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Reichenbach, K., Bastian, L., Rohrbach, S., Gross, M., and Sarrar, L. (2016). Cognitive functions in preschool children with specific language impairment. Int. J. Pediatr. Otorhinolaryngol. 86, 22–26. doi: 10.1016/j.ijporl.2016.04.011

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Schaeffer, J., Van Witteloostuijn, M., and Creemers, A. (2018). Article choice, theory of mind, and memory in children with high-functioning autism and children with specific language impairment. Appl. Psycholinguist. 39, 89–115. doi: 10.1017/S0142716417000492

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Seiferth, N., and Thienel, R. (2013). “Exekutive Funktionen” in Funktionelle MRT in Psychiatrie und Neurologie. eds. F. Schneider and G. R. Fink (Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer Berlin Heidelberg), 359–374.

Google Scholar

Sesma, H. W., Mahone, E. M., Levine, T., Eason, S. H., and Cutting, L. E. (2009). The contribution of executive skills to reading comprehension. Child Neuropsychol. 15, 232–246. doi: 10.1080/09297040802220029

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Shing, Y. L., Lindenberger, U., Diamond, A., Li, S. C., and Davidson, M. C. (2010). Memory maintenance and inhibitory control differentiate from early childhood to adolescence. Dev. Neuropsychol. 35, 679–697. doi: 10.1080/87565641.2010.508546

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sikora, K., Roelofs, A., Hermans, D., and Knoors, H. (2019). Executive control in language production by children with and without language impairment. Int. J. Lang. Commun. Disord. 54, 645–655. doi: 10.1111/1460-6984.12470

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Spanoudis, G. C., and Natsopoulos, D. (2011). Memory functioning and mental verbs acquisition in children with specific language impairment. Res. Dev. Disabil. 32, 2916–2926. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2011.05.011

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

St Clair, M. C., Pickles, A., Durkin, K., and Conti-Ramsden, G. (2011). A longitudinal study of behavioral, emotional and social difficulties in individuals with a history of specific language impairment (SLI). J. Commun. Disord. 44, 186–199. doi: 10.1016/j.jcomdis.2010.09.004

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Taylor, L. J., Maybery, M. T., Grayndler, L., and Whitehouse, A. J. (2014). Evidence for distinct cognitive profiles in autism spectrum disorders and specific language impairment. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 44, 19–30. doi: 10.1007/s10803-013-1847-2

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Thomas, S., Shipp, N. J., and Ryder, N. (2022). Inhibition in preschool children at risk of developmental language disorder. Child Lang. Teach. Therapy 38, 241–253. doi: 10.1177/02656590221111341

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tomblin, J. B., Records, N. L., and Zhang, X. (1996). A system for the diagnosis of specific language impairment in kindergarten children. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 39, 1284–1294. doi: 10.1044/jshr.3906.1284

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Usai, M. C., Viterbori, P., Gandolfi, E., and Zanobini, M. (2020). The relationship between temperamental dimensions and inhibitory control in early childhood: implications for language acquisition. Infant Behav. Dev. 61:101495. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2020.101495

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Vallotton, C., and Ayoub, C. (2011). Use your words: the role of language in the development of Toddlers' self-regulation. Early Child Res. Q. 26, 169–181. doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2010.09.002

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Vugs, B., Cuperus, J., Hendriks, M., and Verhoeven, L. (2013). Visuospatial working memory in specific language impairment: a meta-analysis. Res. Dev. Disabil. 34, 2586–2597. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2013.05.014

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Vugs, B., Hendriks, M., Cuperus, J., Knoors, H., and Verhoeven, L. (2017). Developmental associations between working memory and language in children with specific language impairment: a longitudinal study. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 60, 3284–3294. doi: 10.1044/2017_jslhr-l-17-0042

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Vugs, B., Hendriks, M., Cuperus, J., and Verhoeven, L. (2014). Working memory performance and executive function behaviors in young children with SLI. Res. Dev. Disabil. 35, 62–74. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2013.10.022

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wells, G.A., Shea, B., O’Connell, D., Peterson, J., Welch, V., Losos, M., et al. (2014). The Newcastle-Ottawa scale (NOS) for assessing the quality of nonrandomised studies in meta-analyses. Available at: http://www.ohri.ca/programs/clinical_epidemiology/oxford.asp.

Google Scholar

White, L. J., Alexander, A., and Greenfield, D. B. (2017). The relationship between executive functioning and language: examining vocabulary, syntax, and language learning in preschoolers attending head start. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 164, 16–31. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2017.06.010

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Winsler, A. E., Fernyhough, C. E., and Montero, I. E. (2009). Private speech, executive functioning, and the development of verbal self-regulation. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Google Scholar

Wittke, K., Spaulding, T. J., and Schechtman, C. J. (2013). Specific language impairment and executive functioning: parent and teacher ratings of behavior. Am. J. Speech Lang. Pathol. 22, 161–172. doi: 10.1044/1058-0360(2012/11-0052)

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Yuile, A. R., and Sabbagh, M. A. (2021). Inhibitory control and Preschoolers' use of irregular past tense verbs. J. Child Lang. 48, 480–498. doi: 10.1017/s0305000920000355

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zelazo, P. D., Müller, U., Frye, D., and Marcovitch, S. (2003). The development of executive function in early childhood: I. The development of executive function. Monogr. Soc. Res. Child Dev. 68, 1–27. doi: 10.1111/j.0037-976X.2003.00261.x

Google Scholar

Keywords: developmental language disorder, executive functions, children, systematic review, meta-analysis

Citation: Niu T, Wang S, Ma J, Zeng X and Xue R (2024) Executive functions in children with developmental language disorder: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Front. Neurosci. 18:1390987. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2024.1390987

Received: 24 February 2024; Accepted: 29 July 2024;
Published: 12 August 2024.

Edited by:

Yitzchak Frank, Mount Sinai Hospital, United States

Reviewed by:

Georgios Georgiou, University of Nicosia, Cyprus
Debora Maria Befi-Lopes, University of São Paulo, Brazil

Copyright © 2024 Niu, Wang, Ma, Zeng and Xue. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Ruiling Xue, xueruiling@hotmail.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.