Skip to main content

SYSTEMATIC REVIEW article

Front. Neurosci., 11 March 2024
Sec. Translational Neuroscience
This article is part of the Research Topic Translational Applications of Neuroimaging View all 12 articles

Interpersonal neural synchrony and mental disorders: unlocking potential pathways for clinical interventions

  • 1Child Neuropsychology Section, Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Psychosomatics and Psychotherapy, University Hospital RWTH, Aachen, Germany
  • 2JARA Brain Institute II, Molecular Neuroscience and Neuroimaging (INM-11), Jülich Research Centre, Jülich, Germany
  • 3Department of Applied Mathematics and Theoretical Physics, Cambridge Centre for Data-Driven Discovery, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, United Kingdom
  • 4Department of Psychiatry, Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, Medical School, RWTH Aachen University, Aachen, Germany
  • 5Institute for Translational Psychiatry, University of Münster, Münster, Germany
  • 6School of Psychology, Cardiff University Brain Research Imaging Center (CUBRIC), Cardiff University, Cardiff, United Kingdom
  • 7Institute of Medical Psychology and Medical Sociology, University Hospital RWTH, Aachen, Germany
  • 8Department of Psychology, School of Social Sciences, Nanyang Technological University, Singapore, Singapore

Introduction: Interpersonal synchronization involves the alignment of behavioral, affective, physiological, and brain states during social interactions. It facilitates empathy, emotion regulation, and prosocial commitment. Mental disorders characterized by social interaction dysfunction, such as Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD), Reactive Attachment Disorder (RAD), and Social Anxiety Disorder (SAD), often exhibit atypical synchronization with others across multiple levels. With the introduction of the “second-person” neuroscience perspective, our understanding of interpersonal neural synchronization (INS) has improved, however, so far, it has hardly impacted the development of novel therapeutic interventions.

Methods: To evaluate the potential of INS-based treatments for mental disorders, we performed two systematic literature searches identifying studies that directly target INS through neurofeedback (12 publications; 9 independent studies) or brain stimulation techniques (7 studies), following PRISMA guidelines. In addition, we narratively review indirect INS manipulations through behavioral, biofeedback, or hormonal interventions. We discuss the potential of such treatments for ASD, RAD, and SAD and using a systematic database search assess the acceptability of neurofeedback (4 studies) and neurostimulation (4 studies) in patients with social dysfunction.

Results: Although behavioral approaches, such as engaging in eye contact or cooperative actions, have been shown to be associated with increased INS, little is known about potential long-term consequences of such interventions. Few proof-of-concept studies have utilized brain stimulation techniques, like transcranial direct current stimulation or INS-based neurofeedback, showing feasibility and preliminary evidence that such interventions can boost behavioral synchrony and social connectedness. Yet, optimal brain stimulation protocols and neurofeedback parameters are still undefined. For ASD, RAD, or SAD, so far no randomized controlled trial has proven the efficacy of direct INS-based intervention techniques, although in general brain stimulation and neurofeedback methods seem to be well accepted in these patient groups.

Discussion: Significant work remains to translate INS-based manipulations into effective treatments for social interaction disorders. Future research should focus on mechanistic insights into INS, technological advancements, and rigorous design standards. Furthermore, it will be key to compare interventions directly targeting INS to those targeting other modalities of synchrony as well as to define optimal target dyads and target synchrony states in clinical interventions.

1 Introduction

The evolution of humans as social creatures has prepared our brains to be ideally primed for interpersonal interactions (Hari and Kujala, 2009). When we naturally mirror each other’s smiles or laughter during social engagement or unintentionally align our body language with those we are speaking to, these are moments of behavioral synchrony. Moreover, interpersonal synchrony is evident in hormonal states and the autonomous nervous system (physiological synchrony), as well as in neural responses among two (or more) individuals interacting with each other (interpersonal neural synchrony, INS).

Measures of the autonomous nervous system (ANS) become synchronized in interacting dyads, reflecting the activity of the sympathetic and parasympathetic nervous system. This includes, e.g., heartbeat, electrodermal activity, or breathing rhythm (Davis et al., 2018; Bell, 2020). Furthermore, hormonal levels like oxytocin or cortisol get attuned in interacting dyads. This phenomenon can be seen from birth on and in the case, e.g., of parents and their infants critically depends on the quality of their behavioral synchrony (Feldman et al., 2011; Feldman, 2012b).

Interpersonal neural synchrony (INS) is the temporal relationship between two person’s brain signals while interacting and seems to reflect a fundamental mechanism of bi-directional attunement (Azhari et al., 2019). In particular, neural activity is coordinated in frontotemporal cortices when humans are interacting (see Lotter et al., 2023 for a review). INS can be captured by hemodynamic or electrophysiological measures. Electroencephalography (EEG) / magnetoencephalography (MEG), near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS), and functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) are the most widely used imaging techniques to capture neural synchrony patterns of two or more subjects concurrently, which is called hyperscanning (Czeszumski et al., 2022).

INS is associated with both external and internal factors: External influences might include non-social triggers such as common sensory input, as well as social triggers like shared eye contact, language, or movement. On the other hand, internal synchronizers might encompass personality traits, the mental states of those interacting, the social closeness between them, and motivational states (Dikker et al., 2021).

Synchrony across all known modalities, such as in behavior, hormonal states, autonomous nervous and central nervous activity can be seen as a crucial underlying factor of social participation and social cohesion (Feldman, 2017). Being able to adaptively get “in-sync” and “out-of-sync” with others may represent important requirements for successful interaction in terms of communication, social bonding, and affiliation (Hove and Risen, 2009; Rennung and Göritz, 2016; Vicaria and Dickens, 2016; Mogan et al., 2017; Hu et al., 2022). Some of these requirements are the individuals’ adaptive capacities to access another’s internal arousal state (Mizugaki et al., 2015), share and regulate emotions (Davis et al., 2018; Birk et al., 2022), to learn from each other (Pan et al., 2021), and to adapt to collective behaviors and group norms (Wiltermuth and Heath, 2009; Reinero et al., 2021).

A lack of social interaction abilities and maladaptive relationships during the early formative years as well as throughout the life span are among the most significant factors leading to mental disorders (Schilbach, 2016; Schilbach and Lahnakoski, 2023). Vice versa, mental disorders can affect our abilities to successfully interact with others and to enjoy social interactions. Psychotherapy, an effective treatment for many mental disorders, utilizes the structured therapeutic relationship to favor the patients’ well-being. Thus, it has been suggested that mental disorders in general can be construed as disorders of social interaction (Schilbach, 2016). However, there are some mental disorders, such as autism spectrum disorder (ASD), (Reactive) attachment disorder (RAD), or social anxiety disorder (SAD), that are particularly characterized by disruption of social interaction and communication as an essential part of their underlying patho-mechanisms. In line with this, there is first evidence derived from recent hyperscanning studies that decreased INS is associated with (i) increased level of social difficulties in everyday life in subjects with ASD (Quiñones-Camacho et al., 2021), (ii) predictive of poor attachment quality in high-risk mother–child dyads (Miller et al., 2019) and (iii) linked to symptom severity in SAD when assessed in emotionally negative situations (Deng et al., 2022).

However, despite the compelling evidence for the broad relevance of social interaction in mental health and the emerging evidence for a specific role of synchrony as an important underlying mechanism, so far, the majority of neuroscientific studies focused on the mechanisms, antecedents, and consequences of INS under typical and atypical conditions largely ignoring INS as a therapeutic target for mental health interventions. However, the recent technical advances in the field of hyperscanning, real-time neurofeedback, and neurostimulation techniques open up new avenues to translate this second-person neuroscience approach into interactive-based neuroscientific interventions. Tackling directly the neural mechanism involved in social function and dysfunction might thus help to develop novel effective prevention strategies and interventions for a variety of mental disorders.

In principle, a range of methods can be employed to influence INS. Neural synchrony can be targeted through direct manipulation or indirectly by targeting alternative modalities of synchrony to enhance INS. Directly INS can be targeted using brain stimulation and hyperscanning neurofeedback (hyper-NF), indirectly it can be targeted using pharmacological approaches, ANS-biofeedback and behavioral interventions aiming to enhance INS (Figure 1).

Figure 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Interpersonal Synchrony occurs at multiple levels (i.e., behavior, autonomous nervous system, hormones and brain) in interacting subjects. Interpersonal neural synchrony (INS) can be targeted through direct manipulation (depicted in RED) via dual brain stimulation or hyperscanning neurofeedback (hyper-NF). INS can be targeted indirectly by behavioral interventions (depicted in GREEN), or by peripher-physiological biofeedback or pharmacological approaches (depicted in BLUE).

In this work, we will outline these translational approaches and provide an overview how they impact on INS. We will systematically review those approaches that directly target INS by (i) hyper-NF and (ii) neurostimulation approaches, and (iii) how well such approaches are generally accepted by patients. Furthermore, we will exemplify how manipulation of INS can contribute to treatment of three selected mental disorders, all characterized by social interactions deficits, ASD, RAD and SAD. While here we highlight these three mental disorders, we note that INS-based approaches may also be relevant to other psychiatric or neurological diseases that are characterized by impaired social interactions. Finally, we will provide a critical outlook on open research questions, technical challenges, and clinical caveats to be considered in future studies.

2 Methods

In this review, we offer a concise overview of indirect strategies for manipulating INS, while also presenting a comprehensive state-of-the-art illustration of all translational techniques that directly target INS. The latter insights are derived from systematic database searches, providing a thorough analysis of the current landscape. To foster discussions about progress towards clinical utility, open technical challenges for these novel approaches will be outlined. Finally, we will narratively review promising clinical applications for improving interaction-based mental health outcomes in the future along with a systematic review on their general acceptability by patients. Of note, in the clinical translation part we will exclusively focus on future avenues for INS-based approaches for treatment of social interaction disorder while leaving out the scope in terms of INS-based diagnostics or patient stratification.

To provide a comprehensive overview of techniques directly targeting INS and assess their feasibility and acceptability, we conducted three structured literature searches (i: neurostimulation, ii: hyper-NF, iii: feasibility / acceptability). Literature searches were conducted using the open-source search engine SetYouFree (v. 0.1.2, https://github.com/ChristianGerloff/set-you-free, Gerloff et al., 2022b) following the best practices of the PRISMA guidelines for systematic reviews. As the reviewed techniques are in their early stages, the search was conducted across scientific publication databases as well as preprint servers. All searches were performed starting from 1st January 2000 up to the 31th December 2023 (date of conduction of ii), which includes the first hyperscanning studies (Montague et al., 2002). Publications were screened in SetYouFree using deterministic exclusion criteria, which were a-priori-defined (see Supplementary Tables S1–S3). For one of the searches (ii), the screening was conducted by two independent reviewers to assess their interrater reliability (Cohen’s kappa = 0.95). To ensure reproducibility, technical details, parameters, search flows and human readable SetYouFree files are provided in the Supplementary materials (Text “Structured Literature Search”, Supplementary Tables S1–S3, Supplementary Figures S1–S3).

To guide the reader, the kind of search (systematic or unsystematic) is explicitly described at the beginning of each paragraph in the Result section. In Section 3.2.1 we provide an overview of brain stimulation results based on a systemic literature search using search strings that combined “neurostimulation”, “TMS”, “tDCS” or “tACS” with hyperscanning-related terms. From the initial pool of N = 695 studies, we identified seven studies that applied tACS (N = 5), tDCS (N = 1) or both (N = 1) in one or both members of a dyad with the goal to manipulate INS and examining changes in behavioral and/or neural synchrony.

In Section 3.2.2 we investigated the general feasibility and training success of hyper-NF based on twelve eligible publications (EEG: N = 10, fNIRS: N = 1; fMRI: N = 1) out of 129 initial search results, using a search string that combined “neurofeedback” with hyperscanning-related terms (see i).

Finally, in Section 3.4 we analyzed the general acceptability and feasibility of direct intervention techniques in patients with social interaction dysfunction across 8 out of 321 studies based on neurofeedback (N = 4) and neurostimulation (N = 4). For this search, we combined the search terms from (i) and (ii) with relevant disorder related terms (“social”, “autism”, “attachment”) and outcome specific terms (“acceptability”, “feasibility”, “acceptance”, “compliance”, “adherence”). The specific search terms are provided in Supplementary Tables S1–S3.

3 Results

3.1 Techniques targeting INS indirectly through other modalities of synchrony

One of the most widely used indirect approaches to target INS is through behavioral techniques that aim to modify behavior and foster shared experiences and emotional bonding. Hyperscanning studies using EEG/MEG, fNIRS, and fMRI have shown increased INS in a variety of social-interactive tasks, e.g., emotional sharing, joint action, coordinated and synchronized movement, communication, making music, eye contact and joint attention, cooperation and competition, decision making and learning tasks e.g., (see Nam et al., 2020 for a review). Based on these studies, we will non-systematically summarize several behavioral techniques that may be particularly well-suited for increasing INS and promoting positive outcomes in Section 3.1.1. Given the close link between the brain and other bodily systems, INS may additionally be enhanced by targeting synchrony in the ANS and by hormonal manipulations. These techniques will be non-systematically summarized in Section 3.1.2.

3.1.1 Behavioral techniques

Some promising candidates for enhancing INS include (i) coordinating or synchronizing body movements, (ii) making music together, (iii) playing cooperative or interactive games, and (iv) engaging in shared eye contact or joint attention. Of note is that while these possibilities are not the exclusive ones, they are highlighted here because there is fairly robust evidence for all four of them.

3.1.1.1 Coordinated and synchronized movements

Evidence suggests that coordinated or synchronized body movements are associated with increased INS. Examples of tasks that induce INS include synchronized button press after counting a time in mind (Hu et al., 2017), synchronized arm movement (Nozawa et al., 2019) and imitation tasks (Dumas et al., 2010; Holper et al., 2012; Miyata et al., 2021), as well as many cooperative tasks that involve action coordination or synchronization (e.g., Cui et al., 2012; Li et al., 2020; see below for more information). Further, experimental manipulations of movement synchrony have been shown to induce higher affiliation (Hove and Risen, 2009), altruistic and prosocial behavior (Valdesolo and DeSteno, 2011; Cirelli et al., 2014), cooperation (Wiltermuth and Heath, 2009) and joint action performance (Valdesolo et al., 2010). Some of these effects may be facilitated through increased INS (Hu et al., 2017). While most hyperscanning studies have measured the effects of behavioral synchrony on INS concurrently, Nozawa et al. (2019) have probed the prolonged effects of an experimental movement synchrony manipulation on subsequent social interaction. In their study, pairs of participants had to move their arm to a beat of a sound, presented either at the same or at a different tempo. After the rhythmic movement block, participants engaged in an educational communication where they taught and learned unknown words to/from each other during which their neural activities in medial and left lateral prefrontal cortices were measured using fNIRS hyperscanning. Results showed that prior movement synchrony enhanced both teacher-learner rapport and INS in the left lateral prefrontal cortex in the subsequent teaching-learning task, and these changes were interrelated. Thus, although these findings need to be replicated, this suggests that movement synchrony induction can have at least short-term carry-over effects on social interaction.

3.1.1.2 Cooperative behavior and games

As shown by a recent systematic review and quantitative meta-analysis, cooperative behavior has been associated with statistically significant INS, with a large overall effect sizes in frontal and temporoparietal areas, across diverse cooperative tasks, including Jenga game, Tangram puzzle, creativity tasks, joint finger tapping, drawing and singing, realistic problem solving and a math task (N = 13 fNIRS-based hyperscanning studies with 890 human subjects; Czeszumski et al., 2022). Many of the cooperative tasks have been applied or are potentially suitable for children and adolescents (e.g., Reindl et al., 2018, 2022; Kruppa et al., 2021). Importantly, compared to simple movement coordination/synchronization tasks, gamifying behavioral synchrony induction tasks (e.g., as in Cui et al., 2012 or Reindl et al., 2018) may make the tasks more engaging, particularly for developmental populations, and thus potentially more suitable for behavioral interventions.

3.1.1.3 Music

Singing or humming together, drumming, playing musical instruments like guitar or piano, musical improvisation and listening to music have been associated with INS in multiple studies (e.g., Lindenberger et al., 2009; Sänger et al., 2012; Müller et al., 2013, 2018; Osaka et al., 2015; Zamm et al., 2018; Müller and Lindenberger, 2019, 2022, 2023; Hou et al., 2020; Liu et al., 2021; Chabin et al., 2022; Gugnowska et al., 2022). The close link between music and INS is not surprising given that making music involves sensorimotor coupling in musicians, being entrained to the same pace and rhythm as well as coordinating and synchronizing actions. Further, music is a pleasurable experience that is linked to emotional sharing in musicians, audience as well as between musicians and audience. In addition to its emotional effects, it promotes social functions, such as communication, cooperation, and social attachment, and thereby might possess inherent therapeutic potential (Koelsch, 2014).

3.1.1.4 Eye contact

Irrespective of the specific task, ostensive signals have been proposed to entrain oscillatory brain signals during the social interaction (e.g., via mutual phase resetting in sender and receiver; Wass et al., 2020). Ostensive signals are cues that a communicator uses to convey their communicative intention to an addres(see Wass et al., 2020); one of the most notable is eye contact. Studies in adults and adult-child dyads show that eye contact elicits increased INS (e.g., Dikker et al., 2017; Hirsch et al., 2017; Koike et al., 2019; Dravida et al., 2020; Noah et al., 2020; Guglielmini et al., 2022; Luft et al., 2022; but see Haresign et al., 2023 for contradictory findings). This has been demonstrated both in naturalistic tasks, e.g., during communication (Piazza et al., 2020), as well as in well-controlled experimental set-ups, e.g., eye-to-eye contact compared to mutual gaze at the eyes of a picture face (Hirsch et al., 2017). Further, Dikker et al. (2017) demonstrated that engaging in a short eye-to-eye contact activity could serve as an intervention to enhance subsequent INS. In their study, the EEGs were recorded of 12 students simultaneously over the course of a semester during regular classroom activities. Prior to the class, students engaged in a 2-min eye contact with an assigned peer. Students showed highest INS with their face-to-face partner during class, which was correlated with student’s mutual closeness rating. Such a correlation was only observed for face-to-face partners; thus, it seemed to “activate” interpersonal relationship features.

Taken together, behavioral studies indicate that social interactions that involve coordinating or synchronizing actions, cooperation, music and eye contact are associated with increased INS, and thus are potentially suitable as behavioral interventions. However, the majority of studies have evaluated INS during the behavioral intervention or experimental condition. Only a very limited number of studies have explored the effects of behavioral interventions on INS measured shortly thereafter, indicating that these tasks may have carry-over effects (Dikker et al., 2017; Nozawa et al., 2019; Khalil et al., 2022). While long-term effects on INS manipulations are rather unexplored, some of these techniques have been implemented in therapeutic approaches (e.g., in a dance/movement interventions based on interpersonal movement imitation and synchronization; Koehne et al., 2016) (for further information see Section 3.3). Nevertheless, before moving behavioral synchrony manipulations towards clinical application in patients with social dysfunction more research is needed to demonstrate whether they do show longer-lasting effects on social interactive behaviors, INS or both.

3.1.2 ANS-based biofeedback and hormonal manipulations

Another indirect technique to manipulate synchrony in interacting individuals is to provide feedback systems to measures of the ANS. This form of biofeedback training can for instance be provided based on heart rate variability. As such, it can not only be employed in single-person designs to entrain self-regulation of the underlying biological signal (Mather and Thayer, 2018), but is also considered a suitable approach for dyadic contexts to increase interpersonal synchrony. For example, it may help to increase interpersonal synchrony in a therapeutic setting (Fiskum, 2019) or promote empathy and social entrainment between two people (Tennant et al., 2019). Besides heart rate variability, skin conductance has been used to provide (simulated) dyadic feedback (Feijt et al., 2020) and other studies have provided ANS-based biofeedback based on breathing rhythm (Järvelä et al., 2019, 2021; Stepanova et al., 2020; Salminen et al., 2022). Popular feedback modalities in this context are pure visual (Feijt et al., 2020), or auditory feedback (Tennant et al., 2019) as well as a multisensorial approach via virtual realities (Järvelä et al., 2019, 2021; Stepanova et al., 2020; Salminen et al., 2022).

Along with biofeedback, hormonal approaches can be employed to intentionally enhance INS. Notably, maternal chemo-signals have been found to heighten INS between parents and infants and can also foster increased INS between an infant and an unfamiliar person (Endevelt-Shapira et al., 2021). Moreover, studies have demonstrated that the administration of intranasal oxytocin can effectively boost INS (Mu et al., 2016) and promote behavioral synchrony in adult participants (Spengler et al., 2017). Again, nothing is known about any longer-lasting effects of such manipulations.

3.2 Techniques directly targeting INS

Promising manipulation approaches that target synchrony directly on the neural level include brain stimulation through methods like transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) or transcranial alternating current stimulation (tACS), as well as hyper-NF. These techniques aim to directly influence INS in the brain.

Both tDCS and tACS pass a low electrical current through the brain to modulate neural activity (Kropotov, 2016) and can be used to influence the activity of specific brain regions, such as regions that are involved in social cognition and interpersonal interaction. Specifically, tDCS delivers a weak, direct current to the brain through scalp electrodes and can thereby manipulate the membrane potential of neurons and modulate spontaneous firing rates, producing facilitatory or inhibitory effects upon a variety of behaviors (Paulus, 2011; Thair et al., 2017). Instead of using a direct current, tACS delivers sinusoidally varying transcranial stimulation that may interact with ongoing rhythms in the cortex (Paulus, 2011). tDCS may create a stable environment that predisposes neural circuits towards synchronization, potentially affecting INS through sustained shifts in excitability and plasticity. tACS, on the other hand, could immediately and dynamically entrain neural oscillations and directly modulate the temporal dynamics of brain activity, providing a more immediate but possibly less enduring impact on INS. Thus, while both, tDCS and tACS can modulate INS, they may do so through different mechanisms and with different temporal dynamics. tDCS might promote a general state of heightened plasticity and readiness for synchronization, while tACS could directly entrain and synchronize oscillatory activity in real-time, which is typically considered the foundation of INS (Lu et al., 2023). In comparison, tDCS applies a constant stimulation and can thus modulate brain activities in regions associated with the mental processes that are being probed. tDCS can also reduce INS, e.g., by applying stimulation to one participant but not the other (Long et al., 2023). The influence of those two brain stimulation techniques has been shown to impact on INS in dyads with respect to synchrony in communication (Long et al., 2023), movement (Novembre et al., 2017; Szymanski et al., 2017; Pan et al., 2021) as well as enhanced learning performances (Pan et al., 2021).

While brain stimulation influences the brain signal most directly, neurofeedback allows participants to intentionally manipulate their neural activity through real-time information about their brain activity (Marzbani et al., 2016). Recently, initial studies have focused on hyper-NF also called dual neurofeedback or so-called cross-brain neurofeedback (Duan et al., 2013; Zhang and Zhao, 2018). In this context, mainly EEG (Järvelä et al., 2019, 2021; Dikker et al., 2021; Müller et al., 2021; Putri et al., 2022; Salminen et al., 2022; Ceccato et al., 2023; Vrins et al., 2023) but also fNIRS (Duan et al., 2013) and fMRI (Kerr et al., 2022) have been used.

So far, little is known about how direct approaches can be used to manipulate INS intentionally, even though brain stimulation and hyper-NF are promising techniques and represent two of the most suitable, practicable, and relatively cost-effective methods for clinical translation. Therefore, we will provide a systematic overview of the current literature on brain stimulation approaches in section 3.2.1 and hyper-NF in Section 3.2.2. Additionally, we will discuss considerations of optimizing technical and design issues for hyper-NF (Boxes 1, 2).

BOX 1: Methodological considerations for hyper-NF.

From the reviewed hyper-NF studies, we will explore key methodological and design considerations, aiming to provide a comprehensive overview of the selected parameters and methodological considerations.

Typically, in neurofeedback, predefined brain regions are selected that have been shown to be associated with the psychological or biological construct of interest to calculate the feedback. The selection of these target regions varies considerably across studies. Some studies focus on distinct parietal regions targeted by a single EEG channel (Susnoschi Luca et al., 2021; Putri et al., 2022) while others consider multiple regions, such as the left and bilateral sensorimotor cortex in motor-related experiments (Duan et al., 2013; Zhang and Zhao, 2018). Dikker et al. (2021) and Vrins et al. (2023) performed whole-brain-based neurofeedback. Notably, all studies targeted only homologous brain regions between participants in a dyad or averaged the signals across regions.

The neural recordings from these targeted regions are subsequently online preprocessed. Online preprocessing can be performed either directly on an infinite stream of data or by slicing a finite number of subsequent samples into batches, termed batch processing (Marz and Warren, 2015). The reported batch sizes varied considerably across studies, ranging from 0.5 s up to 9 s for EEG experiments. Increasing the overlap of batches allows for a higher refresh frequency (the frequency at which the preprocessed signal is updated) of the preprocessed signal, approaching the sampling frequency. The refresh frequency in the reviewed studies ranged from 0.5 Hz in fMRI up to 60 Hz in EEG, enabling continuously perceived visual feedback. Signal processing operations on these batches involved signal averaging across target regions and time, transformations (e.g., Hilbert Transform), and applying filters, e.g., to select the desired frequency band in EEG studies. The choice of frequency band differed across EEG studies. For instance, Müller et al. (2021) targeted both the theta band and the delta band in separate training runs. In fNIRS, an important methodological consideration pertains to the signal type since fNIRS measures changes in oxyhemoglobin (HbO) and deoxyhemoglobin concentrations in the brain. Duan et al. (2013) targeted HbO to derive feedback features, a common approach in fNIRS neurofeedback studies (see Kohl et al., 2020). Notably, none of the studies applied bad channel detection or artifact correction beyond filters (see Nolan et al., 2010; Mullen et al., 2015; Aranyi et al., 2016; Kohl et al., 2023).

Based on the preprocessed signals, a single or a set of features are derived, forming the basis of the subsequently presented feedback. The features in the studies reviewed here can be differentiated into three categories: amplitude and power-based estimators, or functional connectivity estimators. Both amplitude and power-based feedback provide independent metrics for each subject ( f x 1 , f x 2 ). These can be feedbacked independently, e.g., as two bars, or a difference between subjects’ features can be calculated and presented to the participants, e.g., as a see-saw, resulting in symmetric feedback ( f ( x 1 , x 2 ) = | f ( x 2 , x 1 ) | , see Figure 2) In contrast to amplitude and power-based feedback features, connectivity estimators quantify synchrony directly ( f ( x 1 , x 2 ) ). Here, generally, non-directed and directed estimators can be differentiated (Bastos and Schoffelen, 2016). While non-directed metrics aim to capture some form of interdependence between signals, without considering any direction of influence, directed metrics seek to establish a statistical causation from the data that is based on the principle that cause precedes effect (e.g., from parent to child or vice versa). Among the reviewed studies, only undirected functional connectivity estimators were used. These undirected estimators, such as the Pearson correlation, provide a single value that is equal for both participants ( f ( x 1 , x 2 ) = f x 2 , x 1 ). This value indicating high or low INS is feedbacked to the participants, e.g., as a pendulum (joint feedback; Figure 2). In contrast, directed FC, such as Granger causality, can provide individual feedback for each person which is dependent but non-symmetric ( f ( x 1 , x 2 ) != f ( x 2 , x 1 ) ), thus allowing to remove individual training differences within a dyad. The feedback signal should fit the task at hand. For example, joint feedback may be less applicable for a competitive task where the winner should be displayed, while in a cooperative task both joint and individual feedback can provide valuable information.

To quantify success based on the feedback signal, either the continuous feedback signal itself can be used, for example, in an offline analysis, or a discrete performance measure can be calculated from the feedback signal. For instance, three studies thresholded feedback signals to estimate discrete states of synchronization or desynchronization, and then counted the occurrence of the desired state if the state lasted over a period, such as 1 s (Müller et al., 2021; Susnoschi Luca et al., 2021; Putri et al., 2022).

Figure 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Study design considerations. Hyper-NF begins with the observation of neural activity of multiple persons using EEG/MEG, fNIRS or fMRI in target brain regions or network of regions. In an online analysis, signals are preprocessed and a feedback signal is calculated based on features such as the signal’s amplitude, band power or directed / undirected functional connectivity. This is then fed back to the participants through visual, auditory, or other modalities using independent, symmetric, non-symmetric or joint feedback.

BOX 2: Design considerations for hyper-NF.

Conceptually, cooperative, and competitive tasks can be differentiated. Cooperative tasks typically reinforce a synchronization of neural activity between dyads. In contrast, competitive tasks reinforce a desynchronization, e.g., by targeting the upregulation of one’s neural activities above those of the opponent, or by targeting neural activities into opposite directions (i.e., bidirectional training). However, whether indeed competition leads to lower, or possibly higher, INS than cooperation remains to be explored. Most studies employed a cooperative task, such as dyadic meditation (Järvelä et al., 2019, 2021; Salminen et al., 2022) or generating live music together (Ceccato et al., 2023; Vrins et al., 2023), while other studies employed competitive tasks, such as tug-of-war games (Duan et al., 2013; Zhang and Zhao, 2018). Depending on the task and neurofeedback target region, studies provide either specific instructions, e.g., motor imagery to regulate motor areas (Duan et al., 2013; Zhang and Zhao, 2018), or empathic, warm and compassionate feelings to regulate prefrontal EEG frequency bands (Järvelä et al., 2019, 2021; Salminen et al., 2022), or rather loose instructions as in Müller et al. (2021) who provide a few exemplary strategies but encourage a trial-and-error approach. A conceptional exception is the study by Kerr et al. (2022), in which mothers are instructed to regulate not their own brain activation but that of their daughters.

The presentation of feedback may differ with regard to (1) sensory modality (e.g., visual, auditory), (2) timing (immediate vs. delayed), (3) complexity (simple vs. complex), (4) reward (reward vs. no reward, type and time point of reward, see also Kohl et al. (2020). In previous hyper-NF studies, the feedback animations ranged from very simple visual designs such moving bar designs (Kerr et al., 2022) or intuitive approaching-ball-, pendulum- or seesaw-designs reflecting the IBS of both participants (Müller et al., 2021) to complex immersive audiovisual VR environments of a campfire scene using glowing connecting bridges between two avatars (Järvelä et al., 2019, 2021; Salminen et al., 2022). In the artistic approach of Dikker et al. (2021) participants sat face-to-face in a dome-like neurofeedback environment that immerses pairs of participants in a real-time audiovisual (AV) reflection of their EEG signals. In the study by Ceccato et al. (2023) and Vrins et al. (2023) participants received musical feedback. The pitch, intensity and pleasantness of the tone were determined by the different EEG features of the participants. Notably, nearly all studies provided immediate feedback on brain activity or synchrony. None of the studies provided delayed or post-block feedback, i.e., feedback after regulation. Two studies (Susnoschi Luca et al., 2021; Putri et al., 2022) presented additional rewards in the form of points that participants could collect throughout the training. Notably none of the studies used a form of social or monetary reward, which may boost regulation performance (Mathiak et al., 2015; Sepulveda et al., 2016; Kohl et al., 2023).

A hyper-NF protocol can be organized into trials/blocks, nested in runs, nested in sessions, although it should be noted that taxonomies differ between hyper-NF studies. A training run can be defined as a sequence of trials or blocks which is presented once or several times in one session, whereas different sessions are conducted on different days. Past protocols consisted of several training runs (1–8) on each day/session and up to six regulation trials per run. Besides the study by Susnoschi Luca et al. (2021) and Putri et al. (2022) (3 sessions) all studies employed singles-session training regimes. Whereas studies based on hemodynamic neuroimaging followed blocked designs with alternating regulation and resting/baseline blocks of 40 s each (Duan et al., 2013; Kerr et al., 2022), EEG studies involved longer regulation trials/blocks without breaks in between up to 5 min (Susnoschi Luca et al., 2021; Putri et al., 2022) or with breaks only after 5–10 trials (Müller et al., 2021), but resting baselines before the experiment, which was used for normalization purposes (Susnoschi Luca et al., 2021; Putri et al., 2022).

Neurofeedback experiments can employ several different control conditions (Sorger et al., 2019). Most studies were lacking a control condition, while some used sham or implicit feedback (i.e., participants were unaware that they received neurofeedback) (Dikker et al., 2021), no feedback, solo-neurofeedback or biofeedback (Järvelä et al., 2019, 2021; Salminen et al., 2022) and fake or inverted feedback (Müller et al., 2021). The implementation of random sham feedback can be challenging. For example, in Vrins et al. (2023), participants anticipated whether they were assigned to the control group and reported that the sham feedback was perceived as noisy music. Although no statistical comparison between cooperation and competition was conducted, the approach proposed by Susnoschi Luca et al. (2021) offers conceptually a bidirectional control mechanism that involves for one party a collaborative task to reinforce synchronization and for the other party a competitive task to reinforce desynchronization.

3.2.1 Brain stimulation

Seven studies were identified that used brain stimulation protocols with the goal to modify INS and measure its effects on behavioral outcomes, with five studies using tACS (Novembre et al., 2017; Szymanski et al., 2017; Pan et al., 2021; Chen et al., 2022; Liu et al., 2023), one study using tDCS (Long et al., 2023) and one study using both (Lu et al., 2023) (see Table 1 for an overview). In a hyper-tACS protocol, the brains of a pair of participants are simultaneously stimulated and a behavioral outcome, such a movement synchrony, is measured during the stimulation. The stimulation can be applied with the same or different frequency and with the same or different phase. While same-phase-same-frequency stimulation is expected to enhance INS, different-phase-same-frequency or different-phase-different-frequency stimulation are expected to reduce INS, although it should be noted that INS was not actually measured in three hyper-tACS studies. In the first study, Novembre et al. (2017) showed that in-phase 20 Hz stimulation over the participants’ left motor cortices (same-phase-same-frequency) enhanced interpersonal movement synchrony in a finger tapping task, compared with anti-phase or sham stimulation, particularly for the initial taps following a preparatory period. This effect was specific for 20 Hz (beta oscillations) stimulation and was not found for 10 Hz (alpha oscillations) or 2 Hz (finger tapping frequency). In contrast to these findings, Szymanski et al. (2017) demonstrated that same-phase-same-frequency and different-phase-different-frequency stimulation over right frontal and parietal sites in the theta frequency range were associated with greater dyadic drumming asynchrony relative to a sham condition. This indicates that artificial modulation of inter-brain synchronization can actually impair rather than improve joint action coordination and highlight the importance of finding the optimal stimulation protocol to enhance synchrony. Investigating the potential of brain stimulation for social learning, Pan et al. (2021) found that tACS stimulation of learners and instructors over inferior frontal brain regions with in-phase 6 Hz alternating currents (same-phase-same-frequency) led to spontaneous and synchronized body movements and enhanced song learning performance (intonation accuracy) compared to a sham condition. Interestingly, the effects of in-phase stimulation on learning performance were partially mediated by interpersonal movement synchrony, suggesting that brain stimulation can facilitate learning through enhancing interpersonal synchrony. Similarly, Chen et al. (2022) found an increased gamma-band INS in left temporoparietal region for successful versus unsuccessful conceptual alignment. This was paralleled by an observed enhancement in conceptual alignment when gamma-band in-phase tACS was applied. Notably, these findings were derived from two distinct experiments. Liu et al. (2023) used a semiotics paradigm in which participants had to establish a novel interpersonal symbolic communication system using arbitrary symbols and figures prior to which the two brains were stimulated simultaneously using either 40 Hz in-phase, 40 Hz anti-phase or sham stimulation targeting the right superior temporal gyrus (rSTG). Throughout the stimulation period and communication task, brain activities were measured using fNIRS. Results showed that in-phase stimulation not only enhanced INS in the rSTG, but also improved communicative accuracy compared to the sham or anti-phase stimulation. Importantly, higher INS in the rSTG was observed both during the stimulation and task periods for in-phase compared to anti-phase and sham stimulation, showing that it was effective in increasing INS.

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Brain stimulation study results.

While tACS may modulate the frequency of brain oscillations, e.g., in beta frequency band, tDCS does not target a specific frequency but induces subthreshold alterations in neuronal resting membrane potentials. In a tDCS protocol, stimulation was delivered to one participant of the dyad for 20 min. after which a resting-state session and social interaction task were conducted while measuring the brain activities using fNIRS hyperscanning (Long et al., 2023). Results showed that INS in the communication task was reduced after true compared to sham stimulation. Further, true stimulation decreased emotional empathy but importantly, the relationship between INS and emotional empathy was fully mediated through nonverbal behaviors. Thus, although there is ample correlational evidence for the importance of INS for learning, interpersonal understanding and affiliative bonding (e.g., Zheng et al., 2020; Zhang et al., 2022), the functional role of INS without behavior as a mediator is yet to be determined. Lu et al. (2023) compared the effects of 20 Hz in-phase tACS, tDCS and sham stimulation delivered to both participants over the right inferior frontal gyrus (rIFG) while their brain activities were measured simultaneous using fNIRS during a behavioral coordination task. They showed that tACS led to greater INS in the prefrontal cortex (PFC) for coordination after the stimulation while tDCS led to a reduced activation, indicating enhanced efficiency, in the rIFG. Importantly, tACS had longer lasting positive effects on behavioral coordination than tDCS. This shows that a comparison of tACS and tDCS can yield valuable insights into the unique effects of INS, yet these differences need to be interpreted with caution since effects may depend on parameter settings and intervention formulations, including the targeted cortex, intensity and frequency of stimulation (Lu et al., 2023).

To summarize, few studies exist examining the effects of brain stimulation on interpersonal synchrony. These studies have primarily focused on exploring the casual relationships between INS and behavioral synchrony and other outcome measures, however in some of the studies without directly assessing INS. Although some conflicting evidence exists, studies suggest that behavioral synchrony and related behavioral outcomes (e.g., coordination, learning, communication or conceptual understanding) may be enhanced by hyper-tACS.

3.2.2 Hyperscanning based neurofeedback (hyper-NF)

Twelve publications including nine independent experimental data sets were identified that investigated the general feasibility and training success of hyper-NF using EEG (N = 10), fNIRS (N = 1), or fMRI (N = 1). Detailed information on methodological and design characteristics of the studies can be found in Boxes 1, 2. In a typical hyper-NF setup, the brain activations of two or more individuals are simultaneously measured to provide feedback on a shared target parameter (amplitudes or a measure of synchrony). This feedback allows individuals to collectively learn self-regulation of the target parameter. From a clinical perspective, the ultimate goal is to modify outcomes such as empathy, social affiliation or affective mental states. To assess whether participants were able to successfully regulate their brain activities / INS, we followed established regulation success classifications from single-person neurofeedback research (Thibault et al., 2018; Kohl et al., 2020). Specifically, we explored whether a study reported a significant effect as compared to a baseline control condition (CTB), for an early session as compared to a late session (ECTL), a linear increase (linear) and lastly if a study reported a significant effect as compared to a control condition (CTC). Results are depicted in Table 2 and study details provided in Supplementary Table S4. Findings are either based on the online analysis which was used for the calculation of the neurofeedback signal or an INS derived from an additional offline analysis.

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Hyper-NF study results.

In a strictly controlled within-subject design, Müller et al. (2021) subjected participants to two different tasks in which they received real feedback, fake feedback (an enhanced signal meant to motivate the participants by giving them the impression that they are performing well) and inverted feedback (reinforcing desynchronization) of theta or delta frequencies (N = 25 dyads). Specifically, participants either had to move two balls as close as possible to each other by synchronizing their brain activities or they had to make two pendulums, each reflecting the oscillatory activity of one of the two participants, swing in phase. Across task and feedback conditions, participants demonstrated increased INS at theta and beta frequencies and partly also at delta frequencies compared to a resting state whereas INS at alpha frequencies decreased. Further validating their approach, they showed that INS at theta and beta was relatively strongly related to test partner likability and estimated ability to influence the feedback signal, as assessed using survey items. However, no significant differences between real and manipulated NF (enhanced and inverted) in INS emerged.

While in the study by Müller et al. (2021) both tasks were cooperative in nature, in the experiment by Susnoschi Luca et al. (2021) and Putri et al. (2022) participants controlled a virtual seesaw either in a cooperative or competitive interaction (N = 10 dyads each). Cooperating dyads had to maintain their Relative Alpha (RA) within 5% of each other to win shared points, while competing dyads won points if their RA was 10% above their opponent’s. The authors observed a decrease in alpha power in both competitive and cooperative tasks compared to rest. Further, significant INS in theta, alpha and beta frequency bands was found for both tasks, however, unfortunately cooperative, and competitive tasks were not directly compared in the study (Susnoschi Luca et al., 2021).

Three publications reported different analyses from the same experimental set-up (Järvelä et al., 2019, 2021; Salminen et al., 2022). In a virtual reality meditation environment, dyadic neurofeedback targeting EEG frontal asymmetry (neurophysiological measure of approach motivation) was combined with respirational biofeedback (N = 21, 39 and 36 dyads, respectively). Higher frontal asymmetry was observed in participants engaging in a dyadic neurofeedback condition compared to those in a solo neurofeedback condition. Additionally, increased synchrony of frontal asymmetry was observed when dyadic neurofeedback was combined with respiratory synchrony biofeedback, surpassing the effects of dyadic neurofeedback alone (Järvelä et al., 2019). Note, that the two later publications (Järvelä et al., 2021; Salminen et al., 2022) did not report respiratory or neural synchrony data. However, higher levels of empathy, emotion and social presence were reported following EEG-feedback and respiratory feedback, compared to a no-feedback condition, as well as after dyadic meditation compared to solo meditation conditions. Furthermore, the synchronization of EEG-frontal asymmetries between participants was associated with higher levels of empathy, with the highest ratings observed when both participants exhibited high values of frontal asymmetry (Järvelä et al., 2021; Salminen et al., 2022). Thus, these findings indicate that combining dyadic neurofeedback with biofeedback may augment both self-reported and neurophysiologically measured empathy.

Ceccato et al. (2023) built a dual brain-computer interface (BCI) that generates live music by measuring three EEG signal characteristics, which was adopted in a recent preprint by Vrins et al. (2023) to assess the effect of audio-based hyper-NF on interpersonal neural synchrony using a blinded protocol. Specifically, eight dyads (N = 4 dyads per group) listened to generated music. While in one group, three EEG signal characteristics (mean amplitude, frontal alpha asymmetry, and inter-brain phase-lock value) were mapped to the pitch, intensity, and consonance of the music, the control group was exposed to sham feedback based on randomly adjusted sound characteristics. The results showed a significant increase in INS through Hyper-NF compared to baseline only in the actual group, not in the control group. The authors also found that increased INS was associated with increased perceived synchrony and that the actual group reported a significant increased enjoyment, and changes in perceived mental state. However, it should be noted that participants anticipated ex-post correctly their group assignment. Hence, this study may indicate the potential of audio-based Hyper-NF.

In the so far largest hyper-NF study sample, Dikker et al. (2021) took a “crowdsourcing neuroscience” approach, in which they invited museum and festival visitors to sit in a “Mutual Wave Machine,” a dome-like neurofeedback environment that translates real-time correlations of each pair’s EEG activity into light patterns (final sample size: N = 784). Participants showed an increase in synchrony in the second compared to the first half of the experiment, however only if they were explicitly told that the visuals were derived from their correlated EEG signal. Further, this increase in synchrony in the second compared to first half was also observed in a sham feedback group, in which visualizations were randomly generated, indicating that the mere belief that the feedback signal was related to the success of the interaction could lead to higher social engagement irrespective of the actual relationship between inter-brain coupling and the feedback animation. Moreover, dyads´ relationship duration, social closeness, focus level, and social behavior (joint action and eye contact) positively and personal distress negatively predicted synchronization. While this crowdsourcing approach generated a very large sample, the authors also acknowledge its methodological limitations, in particular, the high likelihood of noise contamination, rendering it difficult to draw meaningful conclusions about the participants’ INS based on their online feedback.

Finally, three studies have developed and probed new experimental setups but with very limited samples sizes (N = 1–6; Duan et al., 2013; Zhang and Zhao, 2018; Kerr et al., 2022). Duan et al. (2013) developed a hyper-NF setup for fNIRS with a competitive “tug-of-war” game in which the target of both participants was to upregulate their left sensorimotor brain activities more strongly than the opponent in order to pull a ribbon to their side. Similarly, Zhang and Zhao (2018) developed a hyper-NF setup for EEG, testing it using a similar competitive “tug-of-war” game. Finally, Kerr et al. (2022) did not provide dyadic neurofeedback in the sense that the brain signals of two persons were fed back simultaneously but rather they probed whether mothers were able to downregulate the right anterior insular cortex signal of their adolescent daughter during an emotion discussion task.

To summarize, the field of hyper-NF is at a very early stage consisting mainly of proof-of-concept studies involving only limited sample sizes and oftentimes lacking stringent control conditions. These studies provide preliminary evidence that EEG-based hyper-NF is effective in modulating specifically targeted measures of INS. Not enough studies employing fMRI or fNIRS-based hyper-NF are available. While there is evidence that INS can be modulated by hyper-NF, neurofeedback based on amplitudes, based on a noisy signal or even sham neurofeedback may also enhance INS (Dikker et al., 2021; Müller et al., 2021).

3.3 Clinical translation: integration of INS-based manipulations in the treatment of mental disorders characterized by social interaction dysfunction

The three cases, presented in Box 3, although representing distinct clinical pictures, demonstrate the tremendous impact of social interaction dysfunction on our everyday functioning leading to severe impairments in building up trust and emotional connections in (close) relationships, in forming friendships with peers, and in overall academic development. Despite advancements in the last decade regarding our understanding of the neural basis of ASD, attachment issues, and anxiety disorders, the application of these insights into clinical practice remains largely confined. Over the past ten years, there have been few significant breakthroughs in the development of new therapeutic approaches, whether in psychotherapy or pharmacological treatments, for any of these mental conditions. Direct application of a transdiagnostic second-person neuroscience perspective-moving from a single brain towards at least two interacting brains—might open novel avenues for prevention and intervention of social interaction disorders.

BOX 3: Clinical case presentations.

Imagine the following scenarios:

A foster mother seeks psychotherapeutic help for her 2-year-old daughter Sally, whom she took into foster care 6 months ago due to neglect and suspicion of child abuse in her biological family. Sally seems withdrawn and anxious, shows no attachment to her foster parents, and does not play with other children in kindergarten. When she cries, Sally cannot be comforted by anyone, not even by the foster mother, who overall appears sensitive and empathetic when interacting with Sally.

Brian, a six-year-old boy, diagnosed with ASD three years ago, had been treated with intensive behavioral interventions, including applied behavioral analysis (ABA). Although his parents report that overall, Brian seems to profit from those interventions showing more adequate interactions within his family and with his little sister and less ASD-stereotypic behavior (i.e., less body rocking) he still suffers from marked social interaction deficits in the classroom. He typically misses the overall context of the lesson while focusing on specific details and he does not engage in any group activities which impairs both his integration into joint classroom activities with his peers as well as the acquisition of academic skills.

Susan, an 18-year-old university student, has always been shy and has consistently faced difficulties speaking in front of strangers. After moving to a new town for college, where she is unfamiliar with her peers and surroundings, she experiences a noticeable exacerbation of her social anxiety, including intense physical symptoms such as trembling, sweating, and palpitations as soon as she steps onto the campus. She seeks psychotherapeutic help as she is unable to attend her classes, participate in group projects, or engage in any typical social interactions on campus.

Coming back to the three cases presented above: Imagine, forming a secure attachment pattern between Sally’s foster parents and herself could be supported by engaging them in synchronous activities along with direct feedback to improve synchronization of their biological parameters in the caregiver-child dyad, such as synchronized oxytocin, cortisol levels, or simultaneous brain activities in those brain regions associated with mentalizing and trust. Imagine Brian’s learning difficulties in the classroom could be reduced by facilitating neural entrainment between the teacher and himself. For example, providing him with social ostensive signals that trigger transient moments of interpersonal teacher-pupil entrainment, in which the information presented by the teacher might arrive at a high receptivity phase for optimal encoding in Brian’s brain. And finally, imagine the effectiveness of a cognitive-behavioral therapy (CBT) for Susan’s social anxiety could be improved or shortened by synchronizing her ANS and neural activity with a socially non-anxious role model in prototype situations of social interactions.

In the following paragraphs, we will briefly describe what is already known about interpersonal synchrony, and INS in particular, in the three mental disorders, all characterized by social interaction dysfunction: ASD, RAD and SAD. We will summarize the current treatment standards according to clinical guidelines for each of these disorders and finally stimulate a critical discussion about the potential of tailored intervention to enhance INS in clinical treatment of these disorders and conclude with aspects of acceptability by participants and cost-effectiveness.

3.3.1 Autism spectrum disorder (ASD)

ASD is a very heterogeneous, pervasive developmental disorder with life-long difficulties in social affect, including social interaction and communication, as well as restricted and repetitive behaviors. It affects approximately 1–3% of children and by the year of 2025 the cost of caring only for Americans with ASD will approximately reach up to 461 billion USD in the absence of more-effective interventions and support across the life span (Autism Statistics and Facts, 2023).

More recently, temporal synchrony has been in the focus of interest and might provide new insights for the understanding of social communication and sensory difficulties in ASD as experienced in everyday tasks and in naturalistic settings such as speaking back-and-forth on the telephone without visual cues, engaging in “flowing” one-to-one in-person conversation, and taking turns in social interactions. McNaughton and Redcay (2020) and Baldwin et al. (2022) reviewed existing studies on synchrony in ASD. Results demonstrated that participants with ASD tended to show more temporally asynchronous behavior when performing tasks that required audio-visual, audio-motor, visuo-tactile, visuo-motor, social motor, and conversational sensory integration.

In line with, a growing number of studies investigated interpersonal synchrony in ASD, and more recently also focused on INS (e.g., Lyons et al., 2020; Kruppa et al., 2021; Quiñones-Camacho et al., 2021; Wang et al., 2021; Key et al., 2022). Since these studies vary tremendously in the participants’ age (children, adolescents, and adults), as well as in tasks and imaging techniques (EEG, fMRI and fNIRS), it is difficult at this point to draw any strong conclusions. Kruppa et al. (2021) for instance found differences in behavioral synchrony between typically developing children and children with ASD, but no difference with respect to INS measures (i.e., wavelet coherence was calculated for oxy- and deoxyhemoglobin brain signals during a fNIRS task). However, using the same sample, Gerloff et al. (2022a) was able to predict ASD diagnosis based on non-linear connectivity estimators and network embeddings. Quiñones-Camacho et al. (2021) on the other hand, reported that healthy adults showed more neural synchrony than participants with ASD in the TPJ during a conversation task in an fNIRS study. Similarly, Key et al. (2022) reported that in typically developing adolescents and in adolescents with ASD, lower levels of synchrony, as measured with EEG, were associated with increased behavioral symptoms of social difficulties. Furthermore, Tanabe et al. (2012) used a real-time joint-attention task combined with dual-fMRI recordings and found that detecting gaze direction was impaired in both healthy subjects and subjects with ASD, when they were paired together and inter-brain coherence in the right inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) together with intra-brain functional connectivity between the right IFG and right superior temporal sulcus (STS) was diminished in ASD. These studies –although not completely consistent—point to impaired interpersonal synchrony across multiple levels in subjects with ASD.

Up to now, numerous interventions have been developed to improve ASD symptomatology. During early childhood, intensive behavioral interventions, including applied behavioral analysis (ABA, Cooper et al., 1987) and TEAACH (Mesibov et al., 2005), aim to improve difficulties in communication and social interaction. Nonetheless, its evidence is heterogenous, and randomized control trials (RCTs) are scarce. However, increasing evidence now supports the efficacy, and in some instances, the long-term impact of interventions focusing on parent–child interactions during the early years (e.g., European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry practice parameters). From school-age on, social skills training programs are commonly applied, mostly in high-functioning children with ASD, and just like early behavioral interventions, some evidence exists that show improvement in social functioning, including, i.e., social motivation, social anxiety, social cognition, and social skills (e.g., Spain and Blainey, 2015; Freitag et al., 2016). Nevertheless, so far, no interventions are available that can provide “cure” for ASD across the lifespan. In line with this, clinical guidelines refer to a multi-sensory, multi-disciplinary approach for the treatment of ASD (Subramanyam et al., 2019). Existing behavioral, psychosocial, educational, medical, and complementary approaches are recommended to be chosen based on the age and developmental status of the individual to maximize functional independence and quality of life by minimizing core deficits in social skills and communication, facilitating development and learning, promoting socialization, reducing maladaptive behaviors, and educating and supporting families. Intervention studies targeting interpersonal synchrony are still scarce yet, however, they provide first evidence for effectiveness and give hope for a whole new branch of intervention studies in ASD. Until now four interventions exist that target synchrony in individuals with ASD. Koehne et al. (2016) included 55 adults with ASD who either received 10 weeks of a dance movement intervention focusing on interpersonal movement imitation and synchronization (SI-DMI) or a control movement intervention (CMI). Patients in the SI-SMI group increased their synchronization skills and imitation tendencies, as well as whole-body imitation/synchronization and movement reciprocity/dialogue, compared to patients in the CMI group. In a RCT Landa et al. (2011) provided 50 toddlers with 10 h/week of classroom intervention, parent education setting (38 h) and 1.5 h of home-based parent training and instructional strategies. Additionally, half of the participants received a supplementary curriculum targeting socially engaged imitation, joint attention, and affect sharing. The authors report an increase in socially engaged imitation with eye contact in autistic toddlers, who participated in the supplementary curriculum. In a more recent study, Griffioen et al. (2020) provided six weekly sessions of 30-min-long dog-assisted therapy with children with ASD and children with Down’s syndrome focusing on psychomotor and socialization skills that would ensure aligned motor action between the child and the therapy dog. The authors also report an increase in synchronous interaction between children with ASD and their therapy dog. However, the number of participants (N = 10) in the study limits the generalizability of the findings. And last, in a pilot RCT (Srinivasan et al., 2015) 36 children with ASD between 5 and 12 years of age received one of three interventions (rhythm, robotic or standard-of-care) for eight weeks four times per week. In the rhythm and robot groups children got engaged in socially embedded whole-body movement games, whereas the children in the standard-of-care group engaged in tabletop activities promoting fine motor, social communication, and academic skills within a group setting. The rhythm and robot groups improved on the body coordination assessment, whereas the standard-of-care group improved on the fine manual control assessment. All three groups improved in imitation/praxis. The rhythm and robot groups also showed improved interpersonal synchrony performance.

These studies provide first and very preliminary evidence for behavioral joint-action interventions manipulating INS indirectly in dyadic interactions (person–person, child-dog, person-robot) in individuals with ASD. Other interventions to improve INS in ASD, such as neurofeedback or neurostimulation, have not been published yet. By contrast, neurofeedback interventions based on single-brain information using EEG, thus not including/targeting INS, have not been shown to be effective to improve ASD symptoms (Holtmann et al., 2011).

3.3.2 Reactive attachment disorder (RAD)

RAD is characterized by absent or aberrant attachment behaviors in young children which are observable across settings. Children with RAD rarely interact with caregivers and they do not seek comfort or respond to comfort when distressed (American Psychiatric Association, 2013; Zeanah et al., 2016). In addition, they show limited or no positive affect, often appear socially and emotionally unresponsive and may display episodes of unexplained irritability, sadness, or fearfulness even if (familiar) adults are around. A child diagnosed with RAD should have a cognitive age of at least 9 months as this is the time, when infants usually have formed preferred attachments, and the symptomatology should develop before the age of 5 (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). RAD is a rather rare disorder, however in high-risk populations as, e.g., in children being raised in institutional care the prevalence rate is much higher and might reach up to 40% (O’Connor and Zeanah, 2003).

Children with RAD partly present similar symptoms as children diagnosed with ASD, e.g., social withdrawal and reduced social reciprocity. Both disorders are often associated with cognitive delays and stereotypes (Zeanah et al., 2016). The phenotypic similarity between the two disorders can be such that children exposed to sustained extreme early life institutional deprivation may present a symptom pattern that Rutter et al. (2007) termed “quasi-autism”. Importantly, in contrast to ASD, RAD is caused by the experience of adverse, neglectful caregiving environments including social neglect or deprivation, for example when being raised in institutions with high child-to-caregiver ratios or after having repeated changes of primary caregivers. As such, inadequate care is etiologic regarding the development of RAD as the deficits and the aberrant attachment behaviors develop in children who would have been able to develop functional social interaction processes if they had been raised in more favourable caregiving environments. Faced with severely limited opportunities to form selective attachments, affected children fail to develop attachments to any caregiver. However, it is important to bear in mind, that the RAD symptoms should not be reduced to a unique dysregulation of the children’s attachment system. Instead, the RAD symptoms rather seem to reflect a more general emotional and behavioral dysregulation (Rutter et al., 2009). This general pattern of dysregulation might be seen as a direct consequence of the lack of biobehavioral synchrony experience with a sensitive caregiver in early development. The important role of early synchrony experiences for the child’s social–emotional development has been shown in several studies. The quality of the early caregiver-child synchrony has been shown to predict the child’s later emotion and stress regulation capabilities, the expression of core social behaviors like engagement with peers and displaying empathy, as well as the exhibition of cognitive control (Feldman, 2012a, 2015b; Bell, 2020). Up to now, there are no studies investigating either behavioral synchrony or INS in children diagnosed with RAD. However, some studies considered biobehavioral synchrony in the context of insecure or disorganized attachment styles in high-risk caregiver-child conditions, such as in premature infants, in families who experienced multiple traumata or in mothers with postpartum depression (e.g., Feldman, 2015a; Granat et al., 2017; Pratt et al., 2019; Ulmer-Yaniv et al., 2023). For instance, it could be shown that depressed mothers and their infants were less synchronous, i.e., had lower levels of gaze and touch synchrony, reduced coordination of affectionate touch with mutual gazing, and diminished maternal behavior (Granat et al., 2017). Pratt et al. (2019) investigated the impact of early and persistent maternal depression on the child’s brain and general development in a prospective longitudinal study. They not only found an increased prevalence of affective disorders in these children, but also a longitudinal impact of maternal depression on the children’s oxytocin levels across early childhood and aberrant neural responses to attachment-specific and social cues in preadolescence.

Research regarding the relationship between attachment quality and INS is still in its infancy (Long et al., 2020) and so far, studies with children with RAD or with high-risk dyads are missing. Using fNIRS hyperscanning in 28 mother–child dyads during the performance of a cooperation task Miller et al. (2019) found some preliminary evidence in an ROI-based correlation analysis that avoidant attachment in the child was associated with reduced INS in the right frontopolar PFC. Another fNIRS hyperscanning study in 42 dyads of mothers and their preschool children demonstrated that INS was positively correlated with behavioral reciprocity (Nguyen et al., 2020).

With regard to treatment, the clinical guidelines for RAD recommend as treatment of first choice the child’s placement in a safe and sensitive caregiving environment with an emotionally available attachment figure as early as possible to minimize the consequences of the psychosocial deprivation on the child’s development (Practice Parameter of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry (AACAP); Zeanah et al., 2016). However, due to a lack of research in this field, it is less clear whether children who recover from RAD still have an enhanced risk for subsequent interpersonal difficulties. To help the child and the caregivers to attune to each other and to interact more positively, the AACAP-Parameter additionally recommend psychotherapeutic interventions targeting the caregiver-child-dyad (Zeanah et al., 2016). Among them the child–parent psychotherapy (CPP; Lieberman, 2004) and the Attachment and Biobehavioral Catch Up intervention (ABC; Bernard et al., 2012; Dozier and Bernard, 2017) were found to be effective in RCTs, though in samples of children with disturbed attachment relationships, not specifically in toddlers with attachment disorders. ABC, a manualized, in-home treatment program includes synchrony-based interventions and is targeted to “catch up” the missed experience of nurturing and sensitive caregiving. Consequently, ABC focuses on directly changing caregivers’ behaviors by, e.g., using direct behavior-focused in-the-moment comments when caregivers interact with the child as well as video feedback. Three main parenting targets are addressed during the intervention, namely nurturing the child when distressed, following the child’s lead (synchronous behavior), and behaving in nonfrightening ways (Dozier and Bernard, 2017). In ABC-T for toddlers the third target is replaced by helping the caregivers to serve as co-regulators when toddlers show signs of dysregulation as, e.g., anger or frustration or aggressive behavior (Lind et al., 2017; Imrisek et al., 2018). It could be shown that the ABC intervention led to enhanced caregivers’ synchronous behaviors and improved attachment behavior, cortisol production, and executive functioning in the child (Dozier et al., 2013).

These first studies addressing the enhancement of biobehavioral synchrony as a core intervention target provide evidence that attachment- and synchrony-based interventions can help to attenuate the consequences of psychosocial deprivation. However, treatment interventions directly targeting INS in socially deprived children have not been published yet.

3.3.3 Social anxiety disorder (SAD)

SAD is a persistent anxiety and avoidance reaction triggered by social or performance situations. SAD may escalate to panic attacks when facing fearful situations such as speaking or eating in the presence of others or engaging in social activities. Individuals with SAD experience distress and/or impairment in occupational and social functioning. SAD often becomes chronic without intervention. The anxiety reaction typically extends to all social contexts and varies in severity, influenced by stressors and life changes (Stein and Stein, 2008; Steinert et al., 2013). SAD is highly prevalent, with a lifetime and 12-month prevalence of 13 and 8%, respectively, among adults in the United States and similar rates in adolescents (Kessler et al., 2005, 2012; Kessler and Üstün, 2008). Onset is typically during adolescence (mean age: 13 years) and rarely after age 25 (Stein and Stein, 2008; Steinert et al., 2013).

Given the considerable difficulties individuals with SAD face in interpersonal scenarios, investigating interpersonal synchrony within this patient group holds substantial promise. Asher et al. (2020) recently examined behavioral and heart rate (HR) synchrony during closeness-generating and small talk conversation in opposite-sex dyads including subjects with and without SAD. It was shown that closeness-generating conversation compared to small-talk conversation led to increased behavioral synchrony (as derived from computer-based video analysis) in control dyads but not in subjects with SAD. Furthermore, the authors found that during intimate conversations, social anxiety correlated with increased HR synchrony in control dyads but decreased HR synchrony in SAD dyads, suggesting that SAD may hinder the ability to establish HR synchrony in closer social contexts, potentially affecting relationships negatively (Asher et al., 2021). So far, INS has been hardly addressed in the context of SAD. However, one study conducted an EEG hyperscanning study with parent-adolescent dyads, revealing that SAD in adolescents is associated with heightened gamma interbrain synchrony in positive emotional contexts and diminished gamma interbrain synchrony in emotionally negative context during socioemotional interactions (Deng et al., 2022).

Clinical guidelines for the treatment of SAD (e.g., Germany: S3 guideline, Bandelow et al., 2014); United Kingdom: NICE guidelines (National Institute for Health and Care Excellence, 2013); Canada: Canadian Clinical Practice Guidelines (Katzman et al., 2014) recommend both, cognitive behavioral therapy and pharmacotherapy for treatment. Several RCT studies in subjects with SAD demonstrated high efficacy for exposure therapy and cognitive restructuring (effect size: 1.8) and for selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (effect size: 1.5) (Fedoroff and Taylor, 2001; Stein et al., 2004; Canton et al., 2012). While divergent study designs hinder direct effect size comparisons between psychotherapy and pharmacotherapy, emerging evidence suggests faster effects with pharmaceuticals and potentially more enduring effects with CBT (Gelernter et al., 1991; Heimberg et al., 1998; Liebowitz et al., 1999; Haug et al., 2003; Davidson et al., 2004; Nordahl et al., 2016). RCTs comparing monotherapies with combined CBT and psychotropics yield heterogeneous results (Knijnik et al., 2008; Stein and Stein, 2008; Canton et al., 2012; Baldwin et al., 2014; Nordahl et al., 2016).

Moreover, given that individuals with SAD inherently fear social interactions, the question emerges about the potential of therapeutic approaches aimed at reinforcing synchronization for therapeutic efficacy. As of now and to our knowledge, no study has explicitly targeted synchronization between therapist and patient with SAD, despite its assessment as an outcome measure in previous research. Altmann et al. (2020) investigated the effect of movement synchrony in interactions between individuals with SAD and psychotherapists, without putting synchrony in the therapeutic focus. The study revealed that heightened synchrony led by the therapist correlated with improved therapeutic alliance and decreased interpersonal issues. Similarly, Schoenherr et al. (2019) found that therapist-patient pairs, which included patients with SAD who prematurely terminated psychotherapy, exhibited lower movement synchrony at the therapy onset compared to patients who completed therapy. Ramseyer and Tschacher (2011) reported analogous results across more diverse disease profiles. Notably, if patients took a leading role in synchrony at the end of the psychotherapy, a reduced therapeutic alliance was observed along with increased scores for interpersonal problems and depression (Altmann et al., 2020). Furthermore, studies on vocal synchrony in patient-therapist interactions (Schoenherr et al., 2021) revealed that heightened patient-led vocal synchrony correlated with elevated symptom severity, attachment anxiety, avoidance, and interpersonal problems in subjects with SAD.

Previous studies have demonstrated that the inclusion of neurofeedback as a treatment tool can produce effects in reducing symptoms in generalized anxiety disorder (Hou et al., 2021) and specific phobia (Zilverstand et al., 2015). In an initial pilot study, NIRS-based neurofeedback targeting the dlPFC in individuals with SAD was deemed feasible (Kimmig et al., 2019). Participants improved dlPFC control, altering social threat-related attention bias. Decreased social anxiety severity was linked to reduced attentional threat processing and successful NF training. NIRS-based NF offers potential to explore attention biases and the dlPFC’s role in SAD. Nevertheless, subsequent research should encompass larger sample sizes and suitable control groups to validate and extend these initial findings. While these studies focused on a single-subject, Saul et al. (2022) were the first to introduce hyper-NF protocol (called “InBS-NF-paradigm”) for clinical application in SAD employing multi-user neurofeedback for intervention. The paradigm aims to integrate neural activities of interacting partners with external sources to minimize subjective distortions. It enables real-time observation and treatment of SAD symptoms by updating the mental self-representation using objective external cues. This work exemplifies the first attempts to directly target INS in clinical treatment.

3.4 Feasibility of neuroscience-based intervention approaches in patients with social interaction disorders

If interventions that directly target the neural basis of a mental disorder in at least two interacting individuals, e.g., by hyper-NF or by brain stimulation, could represent novel treatment options, implementation depends not only on the effectiveness of such approaches but is also heavily influenced by the patients’ perspective on tolerability and acceptance of such treatment approaches as well as cost-effectiveness and safety of the intervention. The systematic database search for feasibility of neurofeedback and neurostimulation approaches in subjects with social interaction disorders provided overall good support for such methods (see Table 3 for study results). NF studies in patients with social interaction disorders reported no clinically meaningful adverse events. Generally, both EEG and fNIRS, typically used in NF, are considered safe and can be used in all patient groups (Strehl et al., 2017; Pinti et al., 2020). Similarly, non-invasive brain stimulation methods used in the therapy of mental disorders, such as tDCS, trigeminal nerve stimulation (TNS) or transcutaneous electrical nerve stimulation (TENS), are well tolerated with mild side effects like skin irritations from electrodes (Matsumoto and Ugawa, 2017; Wei et al., 2017). One neuromodulation study conducted with participants with social interaction disorders used TENS and reported no adverse events (Foldes et al., 2021). Three studies used tDCS in patients with ASD and reported no adverse events as well as low drop-out rates (D’Urso et al., 2015, 2022; Wang et al., 2023).

Table 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Study results for feasibility of neuroscience-based intervention approaches for social interaction disorders.

In NF studies, acceptability was high. In a study by Kimmig et al. (2019), 9 out of 12 of participants in a NF training for social anxiety disorder would recommend the treatment. A fMRI-based NF study examining adolescents with ASD reported high fidelity, feasibility, and acceptability (Direito et al., 2021). Some studies reported that children had difficulty concentrating on the NF task (Steiner et al., 2014; LaMarca et al., 2018). LaMarca et al. (2018) conducted an EEG-NF intervention with children with ASD and reported that some participants displayed problems with concentration, compliance as well as sensory issues associated with the NF protocol (e.g., electrode placement, noises).

Note, however, that the studies included here had few participants and reported on few measures of acceptability. However, the findings are in line with NF studies in other patient groups. A meta-analysis from Catalá-López et al. (2017) found lower drop-out rates for NF treatment for children and adolescents with ADHD compared to cognitive training, and no difference to a wide range of other pharmaceutical and non-pharmaceutical treatments, suggesting high acceptability.

While acceptability of single-brain-based neuroscience-based interventions was high in patients with social interaction disorders, this cannot be automatically generalized to dual-brain-based interventions. Targeting the INS requires interventions of social interacting partners. This could pose an additional challenge. Therefore, future studies need to specifically assess acceptability for neuroscience-based interventions involving synchrony.

3.5 Considerations of cost-effectiveness of neuroscience-based interventions in patients with social interaction disorders

In addition to acceptability and safety, cost-effectiveness is a crucial factor for the implementation of any novel treatment option. Commonly used behavioral therapies for patients with ASD are costly, e.g., ABA-based interventions are estimated to cost around 45,000 USD annually per patient (Hodgson et al., 2022). Treatments for social anxiety disorders, such as cognitive behavioral group therapy, typically amount to approximately 2,500 USD for a 15-session program consisting of two-hour sessions (Stuhldreher et al., 2014). Cost for neuroscience-based interventions using hyperscanning depend on the specific protocol and the type of neuroimaging method that is used. In an EEG NF study, Arnold et al. (2013) reported that after 24 sessions, the parent rating of ADHD symptom improvement plateaued. Assuming a similar number of treatment sessions for NF treatment of social interaction disorders and calculating the costs for one-person NF intervention (according to the remuneration of the German health insurance funds for NF training), the total cost of one-person NF intervention is around 1,500 USD (for 24 sessions). Two-person NF requires two neuroimaging devices, but no additional therapeutic personnel are needed, so the cost of each session for hyper-NF should be comparable to one-person NF. In contrast, fMRI-based hyper-NF constitutes an exception as it requires twice costly scanner time and additional technical personnel. Recent developments of low-cost neuroimaging devices for EEG (Niso et al., 2023) and fNIRS (Vidal-Rosas et al., 2023) have increased accessibility to mobile NF equipment. Taken together, compared to standard treatment of social interaction disorders, NF training based on EEG and fNIRS can be considered relatively low-cost. Thus, such approaches might yield high potential to develop cost-effective interventions, however, as a first step, the field requires a robust evaluation of the effectiveness of such non-pharmacological interventions.

4 Conclusion

Despite their preliminary nature, early findings outline the potential of INS-based manipulations, such as hyper-NF. Although predominantly conducted in healthy adult cohorts in the past, the findings motivate the extension of this technique to clinical and developmental samples, along with further developments of methodological approaches. Hyper-NF may exhibit greater transferability to everyday situations, particularly through its association with social experience and learning, compared to traditional neurofeedback approaches which have often struggled to demonstrate substantial real-world transfer (Kohl et al., 2020). Even other innovative and motivating treatment options, such as “serious games”, may not fully address this aspect (Dewhirst et al., 2022). In contrast, targeting directly the INS might be distinct, as it couples learning mechanisms with physiological processes, linking a natural synchronization process with a conditioned stimulus.

Nevertheless, for clinical applications, first several technical and data analytical challenges must be solved. In particular, well-controlled studies with larger samples are needed to determine the most suitable hyper-NF parameters, such as experimental task and instructions, target brain regions and feedback signal, presentation of the feedback signal, required number of trials, runs and sessions etc. (see Boxes 1, 2). In addition to neurofeedback control conditions, e.g., sham neurofeedback, it will be key to compare feedback of neural synchrony to synchrony feedback in other modalities, such as heart rate or breathing (see also Järvelä et al., 2019, 2021; Salminen et al., 2022), as well as to behavioral interventions to determine the most effective technique or combination of techniques for enhancing INS. Furthermore, task and hyper-NF settings have to be evaluated with respect to their transferability. For instance, for patients with SAD, a training that involves no or little direct interaction between participants (e.g., back-to-back setting, Müller et al., 2021) may be less suitable although it allows for better experimental control. Similarly, for dual-brain stimulation more methodological groundwork is needed. In particular, previously conflicting findings suggest that identifying the optimal stimulation protocol, e.g., with respect to the specific frequencies and stimulation sites, will be crucial for developing this technique further. In addition, it will be necessary to assess short- and long-term changes in INS in response to single and repeated stimulation. In this review, we outline the importance of tailored design and method considerations (see Section 3.2.2), as well as key factors to report (see Supplementary Table S4) when combining hyperscanning with interventional techniques. Moving forward, future work may substantially benefit from an improved mechanistic understanding of INS (Lotter et al., 2023), e.g., to efficiently target relevant brain regions, as well as from further developments of technique-specific designs, reporting standards (Ros et al., 2020) and methods and from fostering the application of open science practices (Allen and Mehler, 2019; Niso et al., 2022; Botvinik-Nezer and Wager, 2023).

Based on the evidence of the current review, one can conclude that although direct strategies have shown some promise for improving INS, there is no concrete evidence that the effects of direct stimulation are more substantial than those achieved through indirect strategies. Furthermore, so far, no data are available how long such direct effects might last. Thus, given that few studies have attempted a comparison between direct and indirect INS-based manipulations (for exceptions see Järvelä et al., 2019, 2021 and Salminen et al., 2022) and none have examined their transferability, no clear conclusions can be drawn about the efficiency of direct versus indirect techniques. Interestingly, the findings of Järvelä et al. (2019) indicate that a combination of different types of feedback (here EEG neurofeedback and respiratory-based biofeedback) could potentially surpass the effects of one type of feedback alone. This is in line with many other areas of clinical applications of direct brain manipulations, such as in depression (Xu et al., 2023) neurological rehabilitation (Xu et al., 2022) or pain management (Goudra et al., 2017). In this regard it should also be noted that not all patients may profit equally from different types of interventions. Thus, identifying which types of interventions work best for certain groups of individuals (see also “precision medicine”) may be an important avenue for future research.

The current review emphasizes the unique role of INS in selected mental disorders characterized by core deficits in social interactions. However, as outlined in this review, before INS-based interventions can be applied in clinical contexts there is quite a long way to go from the current state of knowledge towards RCTs testing the efficacy of INS-based interventions either as a stand-alone or as an add-on intervention to the current standard treatment approaches of the respective disorder. For example, in the case of ASD, future trials need to demonstrate that combined approaches including social competence trainings together with targeted interventions incorporating Hyper-NF or dual neurostimulation might be more effective than social competence training alone to facilitate not only cognitive but also affective empathy, thus creating a more holistic approach to managing social interaction dysfunctions in subjects with ASD.

Clinical applications based on a second-person neuroscience approach must be built on specific dysfunction in interpersonal synchrony. They should target those modalities of synchrony that have the largest effects to achieve the “optimal” or the “most healthy state” (see also Gordon et al., 2023). Gordon et al. (2023) proposed an alternative theory of flexible multimodal synchrony which highlights the context as a key component that defines “pulls” towards synchrony and “pulls” towards segregation inherent to the social situation. In the case of the mental disorders described above, this aspect becomes particularly relevant in the context of allostasis co-regulation in relationships, such as in the mother–child bond in RAD or in the patient-therapist-relationship in SAD. For instance, if a mother is comforting her crying child, it is intuitive to assume that maximal synchrony will not be most goal-conducive but that a temporary “desynchronization” may be more beneficial instead which allows the mother to down-regulate the child’s affective state (Long et al., 2020). Thus, clinical intervention that include a monitoring of functional and dysfunctional synchrony patterns in interacting partners should enable and support the participants not only to pull towards synchrony but also to push out of maladaptive interpersonal synchrony. Furthermore, as there is growing evidence that synchrony of body and mind is distinct and their relationship is dependent on context (Reindl et al., 2022), a second challenge concerns the identification of the most efficient target of the different (i.e., behavioral, physiological, endocrine, and neural) measures of interpersonal synchrony. For example, increasing INS between caregivers and their children might support mentalizing and empathy towards their children’s needs while ANS synchrony in some situations might be associated with higher overall distress in the dyads. Finally, for clinical translations, selecting the best patient’s partner for INS-based intervention, e.g., a therapist, romantic partner, peer or the child’s primary caregiver, will be crucial for treatment success and strongly depend on disorder-specific social dysfunction.

While further research is necessary to fine-tune these interventions and establish their efficacy, this exploration of interpersonal synchrony paves the way for promising advancements in the realm of mental health which may—in principle—be relevant for many other mental disorders as well as for neurological diseases that are characterized by impaired social interactions.

Data availability statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/Supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Author contributions

KK: Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Resources, Supervision, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. CG: Methodology, Software, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. SK: Methodology, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. DM: Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. LM: Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. EV: Methodology, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. MK: Writing – review & editing. AH: Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. MB: Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. EW: Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. VR: Conceptualization, Methodology, Resources, Supervision, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. KK has received funding for this research by the German Research Foundation –DFG: Project-ID 431549029 –SFB 1451.

Conflict of interest

SK is working for MEDIACC GmbH, Berlin, an independent clinical research organization. SK and DM received payments to consult with Mendi Innovations AB, Stockholm, Sweden. None of these activities were related to the work presented in the current manuscript.

The remaining authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnins.2024.1286130/full#supplementary-material

References

Allen, C., and Mehler, D. M. A. (2019). Open science challenges, benefits and tips in early career and beyond. PLoS Biol. 17:e3000246. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.3000246

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Altmann, U., Schoenherr, D., Paulick, J., Deisenhofer, A.-K., Schwartz, B., Rubel, J. A., et al. (2020). Associations between movement synchrony and outcome in patients with social anxiety disorder: evidence for treatment specific effects. Psychother. Res. 30, 574–590. doi: 10.1080/10503307.2019.1630779

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders DSM-5. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association

Google Scholar

Aranyi, G., Pecune, F., Charles, F., Pelachaud, C., and Cavazza, M. (2016). Affective interaction with a virtual character through an fNIRS brain-computer interface. Front. Comput. Neurosci. 10:70. doi: 10.3389/fncom.2016.00070

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Arnold, L. E., Lofthouse, N., Hersch, S., Pan, X., Hurt, E., Bates, B., et al. (2013). EEG neurofeedback for ADHD: double-blind sham-controlled randomized pilot feasibility trial. J. Atten. Disord. 17, 410–419. doi: 10.1177/1087054712446173

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Asher, M., Barthel, A. L., Hofmann, S. G., Okon-Singer, H., and Aderka, I. M. (2021). When two hearts beat as one: heart-rate synchrony in social anxiety disorder. Behav. Res. Ther. 141:103859. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2021.103859

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Asher, M., Kauffmann, A., and Aderka, I. M. (2020). Out of sync: nonverbal synchrony in social anxiety disorder. Clin. Psychol. Sci. 8, 280–294. doi: 10.1177/2167702619894566

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Autism Statistics and Facts (2023). Autism speaks. Available at: https://www.autismspeaks.org/autism-statistics-asd (Accessed August 25, 2023).

Google Scholar

Azhari, A., Leck, W. Q., Gabrieli, G., Bizzego, A., Rigo, P., Setoh, P., et al. (2019). Parenting stress undermines mother-child brain-to-brain synchrony: a hyperscanning study. Sci. Rep. 9:11407. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-47810-4

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Baldwin, D. S., Anderson, I. M., Nutt, D. J., Allgulander, C., Bandelow, B., den Boer, J. A., et al. (2014). Evidence-based pharmacological treatment of anxiety disorders, post-traumatic stress disorder and obsessive-compulsive disorder: a revision of the 2005 guidelines from the British Association for Psychopharmacology. J. Psychopharmacol. 28, 403–439. doi: 10.1177/0269881114525674

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Baldwin, M. M., Xiao, Z., and Murray, A. (2022). Temporal synchrony in autism: a systematic review. Rev. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 9, 596–617. doi: 10.1007/s40489-021-00276-5

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bandelow, B., Wiltink, J., Alpers, G. W., Benecke, C., Deckert, J., Eckhardt-Henn, A., et al. (2014). Deutsche S3-Leitlinie Behandlung von Angststörungen. Available at: www.awmf.org/leitlinien.html.

Google Scholar

Bastos, A. M., and Schoffelen, J.-M. (2016). A tutorial review of functional connectivity analysis methods and their interpretational pitfalls. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 9:175. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2015.00175

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bell, M. A. (2020). Mother-child behavioral and physiological synchrony. Adv. Child Dev. Behav. 58, 163–188. doi: 10.1016/bs.acdb.2020.01.006

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bernard, K., Dozier, M., Bick, J., Lewis-Morrarty, E., Lindhiem, O., and Carlson, E. (2012). Enhancing attachment organization among maltreated children: results of a randomized clinical trial. Child Dev. 83, 623–636. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01712.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Birk, S. L., Stewart, L., and Olino, T. M. (2022). Parent–child synchrony after early childhood: a systematic review. Clin. Child. Fam. Psychol. Rev. 25, 529–551. doi: 10.1007/s10567-022-00383-7

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Botvinik-Nezer, R., and Wager, T. D. (2023). Reproducibility in neuroimaging analysis: challenges and solutions. Biol. Psychiatry. Cogn. Neurosci. Neuroimaging. 8, 780–788. doi: 10.1016/j.bpsc.2022.12.006

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Canton, J., Scott, K. M., and Glue, P. (2012). Optimal treatment of social phobia: systematic review and meta-analysis. Neuropsychiatr. Dis. Treat. 8, 203–215. doi: 10.2147/NDT.S23317

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Catalá-López, F., Hutton, B., Núñez-Beltrán, A., Page, M. J., Ridao, M., Saint-Gerons, D. M., et al. (2017). The pharmacological and non-pharmacological treatment of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in children and adolescents: a systematic review with network meta-analyses of randomised trials. PLoS One 12:e0180355. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0180355

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ceccato, C., Pruss, E., Vrins, A., Prinsen, J., and Alimardani, M. (2023). BrainiBeats: a dual brain-computer interface for musical composition using inter-brain synchrony and emotional valence. In Extended abstracts of the 2023 CHI conference on human factors in computing systems (CHI EA ‘23) (New York, NY, USA: ACM), 1–7. doi: 10.1145/3544549.3585910

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chabin, T., Gabriel, D., Comte, A., Haffen, E., Moulin, T., and Pazart, L. (2022). Interbrain emotional connection during music performances is driven by physical proximity and individual traits. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1508, 178–195. doi: 10.1111/nyas.14711

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, D., Zhang, R., Liu, J., Wang, P., Bei, L., Liu, C.-C., et al. (2022). Gamma-band neural coupling during conceptual alignment. Hum. Brain Mapp. 43, 2992–3006. doi: 10.1002/hbm.25831

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cirelli, L. K., Einarson, K. M., and Trainor, L. J. (2014). Interpersonal synchrony increases prosocial behavior in infants. Dev. Sci. 17, 1003–1011. doi: 10.1111/desc.12193

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cooper, J. O., Heron, T. E., and Heward, W. L. (1987). Applied behavior analysis. Columbus: Merrill Publishing Co.

Google Scholar

Cui, X., Bryant, D. M., and Reiss, A. L. (2012). NIRS-based hyperscanning reveals increased interpersonal coherence in superior frontal cortex during cooperation. NeuroImage 59, 2430–2437. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.09.003

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Czeszumski, A., Liang, S. H.-Y., Dikker, S., König, P., Lee, C.-P., Koole, S. L., et al. (2022). Cooperative behavior evokes interbrain synchrony in the prefrontal and temporoparietal cortex: a systematic review and meta-analysis of fNIRS hyperscanning studies. eNeuro 9:ENEURO.0268–21.2022. doi: 10.1523/ENEURO.0268-21.2022

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

D’Urso, G., Bruzzese, D., Ferrucci, R., Priori, A., Pascotto, A., Galderisi, S., et al. (2015). Transcranial direct current stimulation for hyperactivity and noncompliance in autistic disorder. World J. Biol. Psychiatry 16, 361–366. doi: 10.3109/15622975.2015.1014411

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

D’Urso, G., Toscano, E., Sanges, V., Sauvaget, A., Sheffer, C. E., Riccio, M. P., et al. (2022). Cerebellar transcranial direct current stimulation in children with autism spectrum disorder: a pilot study on efficacy, feasibility, safety, and unexpected outcomes in tic disorder and epilepsy. J. Clin. Med. 11:143. doi: 10.3390/jcm11010143

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Davidson, J. R. T., Foa, E. B., Huppert, J. D., Keefe, F. J., Franklin, M. E., Compton, J. S., et al. (2004). Fluoxetine, comprehensive cognitive behavioral therapy, and placebo in generalized social phobia. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 61, 1005–1013. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.61.10.1005

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Davis, M., West, K., Bilms, J., Morelen, D., and Suveg, C. (2018). A systematic review of parent-child synchrony: it is more than skin deep. Dev. Psychobiol. 60, 674–691. doi: 10.1002/dev.21743

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Deng, X., Chen, X., Zhang, L., Gao, Q., Li, X., and An, S. (2022). Adolescent social anxiety undermines adolescent-parent interbrain synchrony during emotional processing: a hyperscanning study. Int. J. Clin. Health Psychol. 22:100329. doi: 10.1016/j.ijchp.2022.100329

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dewhirst, A., Laugharne, R., and Shankar, R. (2022). Therapeutic use of serious games in mental health: scoping review. BJPsych. Open. 8:e37. doi: 10.1192/bjo.2022.4

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dikker, S., Michalareas, G., Oostrik, M., Serafimaki, A., Kahraman, H. M., Struiksma, M. E., et al. (2021). Crowdsourcing neuroscience: inter-brain coupling during face-to-face interactions outside the laboratory. NeuroImage 227:117436. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.117436

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dikker, S., Wan, L., Davidesco, I., Kaggen, L., Oostrik, M., McClintock, J., et al. (2017). Brain-to-brain synchrony tracks real-world dynamic group interactions in the classroom. Curr. Biol. 27, 1375–1380. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2017.04.002

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Direito, B., Mouga, S., Sayal, A., Simões, M., Quental, H., Bernardino, I., et al. (2021). Training the social brain: clinical and neural effects of an 8-week real-time functional magnetic resonance imaging neurofeedback phase IIa clinical trial in autism. Autism 25, 1746–1760. doi: 10.1177/13623613211002052

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dozier, M., and Bernard, K. (2017). Attachment and biobehavioral catch-up: addressing the needs of infants and toddlers exposed to inadequate or problematic caregiving. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 15, 111–117. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2017.03.003

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dozier, M., Zeanah, C. H., and Bernard, K. (2013). Infants and toddlers in foster care. Child Dev. Perspect. 7, 166–171. doi: 10.1111/cdep.12033

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dravida, S., Noah, J. A., Zhang, X., and Hirsch, J. (2020). Joint attention during live person-to-person contact activates rTPJ, including a sub-component associated with spontaneous eye-to-eye contact. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 14:201. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2020.00201

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Duan, L., Liu, W.-J., Dai, R.-N., Li, R., Lu, C.-M., Huang, Y.-X., et al. (2013). Cross-brain neurofeedback: scientific concept and experimental platform. PLoS One 8:e64590. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0064590

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dumas, G., Nadel, J., Soussignan, R., Martinerie, J., and Garnero, L. (2010). Inter-brain synchronization during social interaction. PLoS One 5:e12166. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0012166

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Endevelt-Shapira, Y., Djalovski, A., Dumas, G., and Feldman, R. (2021). Maternal chemosignals enhance infant-adult brain-to-brain synchrony. Sci. Adv. 7:eabg6867. doi: 10.1126/sciadv.abg6867

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fedoroff, I. C., and Taylor, S. (2001). Psychological and pharmacological treatments of social phobia: a meta-analysis. J. Clin. Psychopharmacol. 21, 311–324. doi: 10.1097/00004714-200106000-00011

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Feijt, M., De Kort, Y., Westerink, J., Okel, S., and Ijsselsteijn, W. (2020). “The effect of simulated feedback about psychophysiological synchronization on perceived empathy and connectedness” in Virtual reality meets COVID-19: the potential of positive technology against the burden of coronavirus. eds. B. K. Wiederhold and G. Riva (San Diego, CA: Interactive Media Institute), 117–121.

Google Scholar

Feldman, R. (2012a). Bio-behavioral synchrony: a model for integrating biological and microsocial behavioral processes in the study of parenting. Parent. Sci. Pract. 12, 154–164. doi: 10.1080/15295192.2012.683342

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Feldman, R. (2012b). Oxytocin and social affiliation in humans. Horm. Behav. 61, 380–391. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2012.01.008

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Feldman, R. (2015a). Sensitive periods in human social development: new insights from research on oxytocin, synchrony, and high-risk parenting. Dev. Psychopathol. 27, 369–395. doi: 10.1017/S0954579415000048

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Feldman, R. (2015b). The adaptive human parental brain: implications for children’s social development. Trends Neurosci. 38, 387–399. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2015.04.004

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Feldman, R. (2017). The neurobiology of human attachments. Trends Cogn. Sci. 21, 80–99. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2016.11.007

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Feldman, R., Magori-Cohen, R., Galili, G., Singer, M., and Louzoun, Y. (2011). Mother and infant coordinate heart rhythms through episodes of interaction synchrony. Infant Behav. Dev. 34, 569–577. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2011.06.008

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fiskum, C. (2019). Psychotherapy beyond all the words: dyadic expansion, vagal regulation, and biofeedback in psychotherapy. J. Psychother. Integr. 29, 412–425. doi: 10.1037/int0000174

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Foldes, S. T., Jensen, A. R., Jacobson, A., Vassall, S., Foldes, E., Guthery, A., et al. (2021). Transdermal electrical neuromodulation for anxiety and sleep problems in high-functioning autism Spectrum disorder: feasibility and preliminary findings. J. Pers. Med. 11:1307. doi: 10.3390/jpm11121307

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Freitag, C. M., Jensen, K., Elsuni, L., Sachse, M., Herpertz-Dahlmann, B., Schulte-Rüther, M., et al. (2016). Group-based cognitive behavioural psychotherapy for children and adolescents with ASD: the randomized, multicentre, controlled SOSTA-net trial. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 57, 596–605. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12509

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gelernter, C. S., Uhde, T. W., Cimbolic, P., Arnkoff, D. B., Vittone, B. J., Tancer, M. E., et al. (1991). Cognitive-behavioral and pharmacological treatments of social phobia: a controlled study. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 48, 938–945. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1991.01810340070009

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gerloff, C., Konrad, K., Kruppa, J., Schulte-Rüther, M., and Reindl, V. (2022a). “Autism spectrum disorder classification based on interpersonal neural synchrony: can classification be improved by dyadic neural biomarkers using unsupervised graph representation learning?” in Machine learning in clinical neuroimaging lecture notes in computer science. eds. A. Abdulkadir, D. R. Bathula, N. C. Dvornek, M. Habes, S. M. Kia, and V. Kumar, et al. (Cham: Springer Nature Switzerland), 147–157. doi: 10.1007/978-3-031-17899-3_15

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gerloff, C., Lotter, L. D., and Maheshwari, K. (2022b). Set you free: automated structured literature search. doi: 10.5281/zenodo.6907681

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gordon, I., Tomashin, A., and Mayo, O. (2023). A theory of flexible multimodal synchrony. PsyArXiv [preprint]. doi: 10.31234/osf.io/9u7q8

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Goudra, B., Shah, D., Balu, G., Gouda, G., Balu, A., Borle, A., et al. (2017). Repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation in chronic pain: a meta-analysis. Anesth. Essays Res. 11, 751–757. doi: 10.4103/aer.AER_10_17

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Granat, A., Gadassi, R., Gilboa-Schechtman, E., and Feldman, R. (2017). Maternal depression and anxiety, social synchrony, and infant regulation of negative and positive emotions. Emotion 17, 11–27. doi: 10.1037/emo0000204

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Griffioen, R. E., van der Steen, S., Verheggen, T., Enders-Slegers, M.-J., and Cox, R. (2020). Changes in behavioural synchrony during dog-assisted therapy for children with autism spectrum disorder and children with down syndrome. J. Appl. Res. Intellect. Disabil. 33, 398–408. doi: 10.1111/jar.12682

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Guglielmini, S., Bopp, G., Marcar, V. L., Scholkmann, F., and Wolf, M. (2022). Systemic physiology augmented functional near-infrared spectroscopy hyperscanning: a first evaluation investigating entrainment of spontaneous activity of brain and body physiology between subjects. Neurophotonics. 9:026601. doi: 10.1117/1.NPh.9.2.026601

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gugnowska, K., Novembre, G., Kohler, N., Villringer, A., Keller, P. E., and Sammler, D. (2022). Endogenous sources of interbrain synchrony in duetting pianists. Cereb. Cortex 32, 4110–4127. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhab469

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Haresign, I. M., Phillips, E. A. M., Whitehorn, M., Lamagna, F., Eliano, M., Goupil, L., et al. (2023). Gaze onsets during naturalistic infant-caregiver interaction associate with ‘sender’ but not ‘receiver’ neural responses, and do not lead to changes in inter-brain synchrony. Sci. Rep. 13, 3555–3519. doi: 10.1038/s41598-023-28988-0

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hari, R., and Kujala, M. V. (2009). Brain basis of human social interaction: from concepts to brain imaging. Physiol. Rev. 89, 453–479. doi: 10.1152/physrev.00041.2007

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Haug, T. T., Blomhoff, S., Hellstrøm, K., Holme, I., Humble, M., Madsbu, H. P., et al. (2003). Exposure therapy and sertraline in social phobia: 1-year follow-up of a randomised controlled trial. Br. J. Psychiatry 182, 312–318. doi: 10.1192/bjp.182.4.312

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Heimberg, R. G., Liebowitz, M. R., Hope, D. A., Schneier, F. R., Holt, C. S., Welkowitz, L. A., et al. (1998). Cognitive behavioral group therapy vs phenelzine therapy for social phobia: 12-week outcome. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 55, 1133–1141. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.55.12.1133

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hirsch, J., Zhang, X., Noah, J. A., and Ono, Y. (2017). Frontal temporal and parietal systems synchronize within and across brains during live eye-to-eye contact. NeuroImage 157, 314–330. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.06.018

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hodgson, R., Biswas, M., Palmer, S., Marshall, D., Rodgers, M., Stewart, L., et al. (2022). Intensive behavioural interventions based on applied behaviour analysis (ABA) for young children with autism: a cost-effectiveness analysis. PLoS One 17:e0270833. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0270833

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Holper, L., Scholkmann, F., and Wolf, M. (2012). Between-brain connectivity during imitation measured by fNIRS. NeuroImage 63, 212–222. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.06.028

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Holtmann, M., Steiner, S., Hohmann, S., Poustka, L., Banaschewski, T., and Bölte, S. (2011). Neurofeedback in autism spectrum disorders. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 53, 986–993. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8749.2011.04043.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hou, Y., Song, B., Hu, Y., Pan, Y., and Hu, Y. (2020). The averaged inter-brain coherence between the audience and a violinist predicts the popularity of violin performance. NeuroImage 211:116655. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.116655

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hou, Y., Zhang, S., Li, N., Huang, Z., Wang, L., and Wang, Y. (2021). Neurofeedback training improves anxiety trait and depressive symptom in GAD. Brain Behav. 11:e02024. doi: 10.1002/brb3.2024

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hove, M. J., and Risen, J. L. (2009). It’s all in the timing: interpersonal synchrony increases affiliation. Soc. Cogn. 27, 949–960. doi: 10.1521/soco.2009.27.6.949

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hu, Y., Cheng, X., Pan, Y., and Hu, Y. (2022). The intrapersonal and interpersonal consequences of interpersonal synchrony. Acta Psychol. 224:103513. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2022.103513

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hu, Y., Hu, Y., Li, X., Pan, Y., and Cheng, X. (2017). Brain-to-brain synchronization across two persons predicts mutual prosociality. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. Neuroscience 12, 1835–1844. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsx118

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Imrisek, S. D., Castaño, K., and Bernard, K. (2018). Developing self-regulation in a dysregulating world: attachment and biobehavioral catch-up for a toddler in foster care. J. Clin. Psychol. 74, 1308–1318. doi: 10.1002/jclp.22642

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Järvelä, S., Cowley, B., Salminen, M., Jacucci, G., Hamari, J., and Ravaja, N. (2021). Augmented virtual reality meditation: shared dyadic biofeedback increases social presence via respiratory synchrony. ACM Trans. Soc. Comput. 4, 1–19. doi: 10.1145/3449358

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Järvelä, S., Salminen, M., Ruonala, A., Timonen, J., Mannermaa, K., Ravaja, N., et al. (2019). DYNECOM: augmenting empathy in VR with dyadic synchrony neurofeedback. In Proceedings of the 52nd Hawaii international conference on system sciences, 4212–4220. doi: 10.24251/HICSS.2019.509

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Katzman, M. A., Bleau, P., Blier, P., Chokka, P., Kjernisted, K., Van Ameringen, M., et al. (2014). Canadian clinical practice guidelines for the management of anxiety, posttraumatic stress and obsessive-compulsive disorders. BMC Psychiatry 14:S1. doi: 10.1186/1471-244X-14-S1-S1

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kerr, K. L., Ratliff, E. L., Cohen, Z. P., Fuller, S., Cosgrove, K. T., DeVille, D. C., et al. (2022). Real-time functional magnetic resonance imaging dyadic neurofeedback for emotion regulation: a proof-of-concept study. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 16:910951. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2022.910951

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kessler, R. C., Berglund, P., Demler, O., Jin, R., Merikangas, K. R., and Walters, E. E. (2005). “Lifetime prevalence and age-of-onset distributions of DSM-IV disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey replication”: erratum. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 62:768. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.62.7.768

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kessler, R. C., Petukhova, M., Sampson, N. A., Zaslavsky, A. M., and Wittchen, H.-U. (2012). Twelve-month and lifetime prevalence and lifetime morbid risk of anxiety and mood disorders in the United States. Int. J. Methods Psychiatr. Res. 21, 169–184. doi: 10.1002/mpr.1359

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kessler, R. C., and Üstün, T. B. eds. (2008). The WHO world mental health survey: global perspectives on the epidemiology of mental disorders. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Google Scholar

Key, A. P., Yan, Y., Metelko, M., Chang, C., Kang, H., Pilkington, J., et al. (2022). Greater social competence is associated with higher interpersonal neural synchrony in adolescents with autism. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 15:790085. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2021.790085

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Khalil, A., Musacchia, G., and Iversen, J. R. (2022). It takes two: interpersonal neural synchrony is increased after musical interaction. Brain Sci. 12:409. doi: 10.3390/brainsci12030409

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kimmig, A.-C., Dresler, T., Hudak, J., Häußinger, F., Wildgruber, D., Fallgatter, A., et al. (2019). Feasibility of NIRS-based neurofeedback training in social anxiety disorder: behavioral and neural correlates. J. Neural Transm. 126, 1175–1185. doi: 10.1007/s00702-018-1954-5

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Knijnik, D. Z., Blanco, C., Salum, G. A., Moraes, C. U., Mombach, C., Almeida, E., et al. (2008). A pilot study of clonazepam versus psychodynamic group therapy plus clonazepam in the treatment of generalized social anxiety disorder. Eur. Psychiatry 23, 567–574. doi: 10.1016/j.eurpsy.2008.05.004

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Koehne, S., Behrends, A., Fairhurst, M. T., and Dziobek, I. (2016). Fostering social cognition through an imitation- and synchronization-based dance/movement intervention in adults with autism spectrum disorder: a controlled proof-of-concept study. Psychother. Psychosom. 85, 27–35. doi: 10.1159/000441111

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Koelsch, S. (2014). Brain correlates of music-evoked emotions. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 15, 170–180. doi: 10.1038/nrn3666

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kohl, S. H., Mehler, D. M. A., Lührs, M., Thibault, R. T., Konrad, K., and Sorger, B. (2020). The potential of functional near-infrared spectroscopy-based neurofeedback—a systematic review and recommendations for best practice. Front. Neurosci. 14:594. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2020.00594

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kohl, S. H., Melies, P., Uttecht, J., Lührs, M., Bell, L., Mehler, D. M. A., et al. (2023). Successful modulation of temporoparietal junction activity and stimulus-driven attention by fNIRS-based neurofeedback— a randomized controlled proof-of-concept study. Imaging Neurosci. 1, 1–26. doi: 10.1162/imag_a_00014

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Koike, T., Sumiya, M., Nakagawa, E., Okazaki, S., and Sadato, N. (2019). What makes eye contact special? Neural substrates of on-line mutual eye-gaze: a hyperscanning fMRI study. eNeuro 6:ENEURO.0284-18.2019. doi: 10.1523/ENEURO.0284-18.2019

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kropotov, J. D. (2016). “Transcranial direct current stimulation” in Functional neuromarkers for psychiatry. ed. J. D. Kropotov (San Diego, CA: Academic Press), 273–280.

Google Scholar

Kruppa, J. A., Reindl, V., Gerloff, C., Oberwelland Weiss, E., Prinz, J., Herpertz-Dahlmann, B., et al. (2021). Brain and motor synchrony in children and adolescents with ASD-a fNIRS hyperscanning study. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 16, 103–116. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsaa092

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

LaMarca, K., Gevirtz, R., Lincoln, A. J., and Pineda, J. A. (2018). Facilitating neurofeedback in children with autism and intellectual impairments using TAGteach. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 48, 2090–2100. doi: 10.1007/s10803-018-3466-4

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Landa, R. J., Holman, K. C., O’Neill, A. H., and Stuart, E. A. (2011). Intervention targeting development of socially synchronous engagement in toddlers with autism spectrum disorder: a randomized controlled trial. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 52, 13–21. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02288.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, L., Wang, H., Luo, H., Zhang, X., Zhang, R., and Li, X. (2020). Interpersonal neural synchronization during cooperative behavior of basketball players: a fNIRS-based hyperscanning study. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 14:169. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2020.00169

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lieberman, A. F. (2004). “Child-parent psychotherapy: a relationship-based approach to the treatment of mental health disorders in infancy and early childhood” in Treating parent-infant relationship problems: strategies for intervention. eds. A. J. Sameroff and K. L. Rosenblum (New York, NY: The Guilford Press), 97–122.

Google Scholar

Liebowitz, M. R., Heimberg, R. G., Schneier, F. R., Hope, D. A., Davies, S., Holt, C. S., et al. (1999). Cognitive-behavioral group therapy versus phenelzine in social phobia: long-term outcome. Depress. Anxiety 10, 89–98. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1520-6394(1999)10:3<89::AID-DA1>3.0.CO;2-5

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lind, T., Lee Raby, K., Caron, E. B., Roben, C. K. P., and Dozier, M. (2017). Enhancing executive functioning among toddlers in foster care with an attachment-based intervention. Dev. Psychopathol. 29, 575–586. doi: 10.1017/S0954579417000190

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lindenberger, U., Li, S.-C., Gruber, W., and Müller, V. (2009). Brains swinging in concert: cortical phase synchronization while playing guitar. BMC Neurosci. 10:22. doi: 10.1186/1471-2202-10-22

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, Y., Wang, T., Wang, K., and Zhang, Y. (2021). Collaborative learning quality classification through physiological synchrony recorded by wearable biosensors. Front. Psychol. 12:674369. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.674369

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, J., Zhang, R., Xie, E., Lin, Y., Chen, D., Liu, Y., et al. (2023). Shared intentionality modulates interpersonal neural synchronization at the establishment of communication system. Commun. Biol. 6:832. doi: 10.1038/s42003-023-05197-z

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Long, M., Verbeke, W., Ein-Dor, T., and Vrtička, P. (2020). A functional neuro-anatomical model of human attachment (NAMA): insights from first- and second-person social neuroscience. Cortex 126, 281–321. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2020.01.010

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Long, Y., Zhong, M., Aili, R., Zhang, H., Fang, X., and Lu, C. (2023). Transcranial direct current stimulation of the right anterior temporal lobe changes interpersonal neural synchronization and shared mental processes. Brain Stimul. 16, 28–39. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2022.12.009

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lotter, L. D., Kohl, S. H., Gerloff, C., Bell, L., Niephaus, A., Kruppa, J. A., et al. (2023). Revealing the neurobiology underlying interpersonal neural synchronization with multimodal data fusion. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 146:105042. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2023.105042

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lu, H., Wang, X., Zhang, Y., Huang, P., Xing, C., Zhang, M., et al. (2023). Increased interbrain synchronization and neural efficiency of the frontal cortex to enhance human coordinative behavior: a combined hyper-tES and fNIRS study. NeuroImage 282:120385. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2023.120385

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Luft, C. D. B., Zioga, I., Giannopoulos, A., Di Bona, G., Binetti, N., Civilini, A., et al. (2022). Social synchronization of brain activity increases during eye-contact. Commun. Biol. 5:412. doi: 10.1038/s42003-022-03352-6

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lyons, K. M., Stevenson, R. A., Owen, A. M., and Stojanoski, B. (2020). Examining the relationship between measures of autistic traits and neural synchrony during movies in children with and without autism. NeuroImage Clin. 28:102477. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2020.102477

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Marz, N., and Warren, J. (2015). Big data: principles and best practices of scalable realtime data systems. 1st ed. Shelter Island, NY: Manning Publications Co.

Google Scholar

Marzbani, H., Marateb, H. R., and Mansourian, M. (2016). Neurofeedback: a comprehensive review on system design, methodology and clinical applications. Basic Clin. Neurosci. 7, 143–158. doi: 10.15412/J.BCN.03070208

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mather, M., and Thayer, J. F. (2018). How heart rate variability affects emotion regulation brain networks. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 19, 98–104. doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2017.12.017

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mathiak, K. A., Alawi, E. M., Koush, Y., Dyck, M., Cordes, J. S., Gaber, T. J., et al. (2015). Social reward improves the voluntary control over localized brain activity in fMRI-based neurofeedback training. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 9:136. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2015.00136

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Matsumoto, H., and Ugawa, Y. (2017). Adverse events of tDCS and tACS: a review. Clin. Neurophysiol. Pract. 2, 19–25. doi: 10.1016/j.cnp.2016.12.003

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

McNaughton, K. A., and Redcay, E. (2020). Interpersonal synchrony in autism. Curr. Psychiatry Rep. 22:12. doi: 10.1007/s11920-020-1135-8

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mesibov, G. B., Shea, V., and Schopler, E. (2005). The TEACCH approach to autism spectrum disorders. New York, NY: Springer Science + Business Media.

Google Scholar

Miller, J. G., Vrtička, P., Cui, X., Shrestha, S., Hosseini, S. M. H., Baker, J. M., et al. (2019). Inter-brain synchrony in mother-child dyads during cooperation: an fNIRS hyperscanning study. Neuropsychologia 124, 117–124. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2018.12.021

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Miyata, K., Koike, T., Nakagawa, E., Harada, T., Sumiya, M., Yamamoto, T., et al. (2021). Neural substrates for sharing intention in action during face-to-face imitation. NeuroImage 233:117916. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2021.117916

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mizugaki, S., Maehara, Y., Okanoya, K., and Myowa-Yamakoshi, M. (2015). The power of an Infant’s smile: maternal physiological responses to infant emotional expressions. PLoS One 10:e0129672. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0129672

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mogan, R., Fischer, R., and Bulbulia, J. A. (2017). To be in synchrony or not? A meta-analysis of synchrony’s effects on behavior, perception, cognition and affect. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 72, 13–20. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2017.03.009

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Montague, P. R., Berns, G. S., Cohen, J. D., McClure, S. M., Pagnoni, G., Dhamala, M., et al. (2002). Hyperscanning: simultaneous fMRI during linked social interactions. NeuroImage 16, 1159–1164. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2002.1150

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mu, Y., Guo, C., and Han, S. (2016). Oxytocin enhances inter-brain synchrony during social coordination in male adults. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 11, 1882–1893. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsw106

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mullen, T. R., Kothe, C. A. E., Chi, Y. M., Ojeda, A., Kerth, T., Makeig, S., et al. (2015). Real-time neuroimaging and cognitive monitoring using wearable dry EEG. I.E.E.E. Trans. Biomed. Eng. 62, 2553–2567. doi: 10.1109/TBME.2015.2481482

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Müller, V., and Lindenberger, U. (2019). Dynamic orchestration of brains and instruments during free guitar improvisation. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 13:50. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2019.00050

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Müller, V., and Lindenberger, U. (2022). Probing associations between interbrain synchronization and interpersonal action coordination during guitar playing. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1507, 146–161. doi: 10.1111/nyas.14689

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Müller, V., and Lindenberger, U. (2023). Intra- and interbrain synchrony and hyperbrain network dynamics of a guitarist quartet and its audience during a concert. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1523, 74–90. doi: 10.1111/nyas.14987

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Müller, V., Perdikis, D., Mende, M. A., and Lindenberger, U. (2021). Interacting brains coming in sync through their minds: an interbrain neurofeedback study. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1500, 48–68. doi: 10.1111/nyas.14605

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Müller, V., Sänger, J., and Lindenberger, U. (2013). Intra- and inter-brain synchronization during musical improvisation on the guitar. PLoS One 8:e73852. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0073852

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Müller, V., Sänger, J., and Lindenberger, U. (2018). Hyperbrain network properties of guitarists playing in quartet. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1423, 198–210. doi: 10.1111/nyas.13656

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Nam, C. S., Choo, S., Huang, J., and Park, J. (2020). Brain-to-brain neural synchrony during social interactions: a systematic review on hyperscanning studies. Appl. Sci. 10:6669. doi: 10.3390/app10196669

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

National Institute for Health and Care Excellence (2013). Social anxiety: recognition, assessment and treatment: clinical guideline (NICE guideline 159). Available at: https://www.nice.org.uk/guidance/cg159 (Accessed August 5, 2023).

Google Scholar

Nguyen, T., Schleihauf, H., Kayhan, E., Matthes, D., Vrtička, P., and Hoehl, S. (2020). The effects of interaction quality on neural synchrony during mother-child problem solving. Cortex 124, 235–249. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2019.11.020

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Niso, G., Botvinik-Nezer, R., Appelhoff, S., De La Vega, A., Esteban, O., Etzel, J. A., et al. (2022). Open and reproducible neuroimaging: from study inception to publication. NeuroImage 263:119623. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2022.119623

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Niso, G., Romero, E., Moreau, J. T., Araujo, A., and Krol, L. R. (2023). Wireless EEG: a survey of systems and studies. NeuroImage 269:119774. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2022.119774

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Noah, A., Zhang, X., Dravida, S., Ono, Y., Naples, A., McPartland, J., et al. (2020). Real-time eye-to-eye contact is associated with cross-brain neural coupling in angular gyrus. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 14:19. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2020.00019

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Nolan, H., Whelan, R., and Reilly, R. B. (2010). FASTER: fully automated statistical thresholding for EEG artifact rejection. J. Neurosci. Methods 192, 152–162. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2010.07.015

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Nordahl, H. M., Vogel, P. A., Morken, G., Stiles, T. C., Sandvik, P., and Wells, A. (2016). Paroxetine, cognitive therapy or their combination in the treatment of social anxiety disorder with and without avoidant personality disorder: a randomized clinical trial. Psychother. Psychosom. 85, 346–356. doi: 10.1159/000447013

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Novembre, G., Knoblich, G., Dunne, L., and Keller, P. E. (2017). Interpersonal synchrony enhanced through 20 Hz phase-coupled dual brain stimulation. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 12, 662–670. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsw172

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Nozawa, T., Sakaki, K., Ikeda, S., Jeong, H., Yamazaki, S., Kawata, K. H. D. S., et al. (2019). Prior physical synchrony enhances rapport and inter-brain synchronization during subsequent educational communication. Sci. Rep. 9:12747. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-49257-z

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

O’Connor, T. G., and Zeanah, C. H. (2003). Attachment disorders: assessment strategies and treatment approaches. Attach. Hum. Dev. 5, 223–244. doi: 10.1080/14616730310001593974

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Osaka, N., Minamoto, T., Yaoi, K., Azuma, M., Shimada, Y. M., and Osaka, M. (2015). How two brains make one synchronized mind in the inferior frontal cortex: fNIRS-based hyperscanning during cooperative singing. Front. Psychol. 6:1811. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01811

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pan, Y., Novembre, G., Song, B., Zhu, Y., and Hu, Y. (2021). Dual brain stimulation enhances interpersonal learning through spontaneous movement synchrony. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 16, 210–221. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsaa080

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Paulus, W. (2011). Transcranial electrical stimulation (tES – tDCS; tRNS, tACS) methods. Neuropsychol. Rehabil. 21, 602–617. doi: 10.1080/09602011.2011.557292

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Piazza, E. A., Hasenfratz, L., Hasson, U., and Lew-Williams, C. (2020). Infant and adult brains are coupled to the dynamics of natural communication. Psychol. Sci. 31, 6–17. doi: 10.1177/0956797619878698

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pinti, P., Tachtsidis, I., Hamilton, A., Hirsch, J., Aichelburg, C., Gilbert, S., et al. (2020). The present and future use of functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) for cognitive neuroscience. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1464, 5–29. doi: 10.1111/nyas.13948

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pratt, M., Zeev-Wolf, M., Goldstein, A., and Feldman, R. (2019). Exposure to early and persistent maternal depression impairs the neural basis of attachment in preadolescence. Prog. Neuro-Psychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 93, 21–30. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2019.03.005

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Putri, F., Susnoschi Luca, I., Garcia Pedro, J. A., Ding, H., and Vučković, A. (2022). Winners and losers in brain computer interface competitive gaming: directional connectivity analysis. J. Neural Eng. 19:046037. doi: 10.1088/1741-2552/ac8451

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Quiñones-Camacho, L. E., Fishburn, F. A., Belardi, K., Williams, D. L., Huppert, T. J., and Perlman, S. B. (2021). Dysfunction in interpersonal neural synchronization as a mechanism for social impairment in autism spectrum disorder. Autism Res. 14, 1585–1596. doi: 10.1002/aur.2513

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ramseyer, F., and Tschacher, W. (2011). Nonverbal synchrony in psychotherapy: coordinated body movement reflects relationship quality and outcome. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 79, 284–295. doi: 10.1037/a0023419

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Reindl, V., Gerloff, C., Scharke, W., and Konrad, K. (2018). Brain-to-brain synchrony in parent-child dyads and the relationship with emotion regulation revealed by fNIRS-based hyperscanning. NeuroImage 178, 493–502. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.05.060

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Reindl, V., Wass, S., Leong, V., Scharke, W., Wistuba, S., Wirth, C. L., et al. (2022). Multimodal hyperscanning reveals that synchrony of body and mind are distinct in mother-child dyads. NeuroImage 251:118982. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2022.118982

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Reinero, D. A., Dikker, S., and Van Bavel, J. J. (2021). Inter-brain synchrony in teams predicts collective performance. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 16, 43–57. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsaa135

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rennung, M., and Göritz, A. S. (2016). Prosocial consequences of interpersonal synchrony. Z. Psychol. 224, 168–189. doi: 10.1027/2151-2604/a000252

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ros, T., Enriquez-Geppert, S., Zotev, V., Young, K. D., Wood, G., Whitfield-Gabrieli, S., et al. (2020). Consensus on the reporting and experimental design of clinical and cognitive-behavioural neurofeedback studies (CRED-nf checklist). Brain 143, 1674–1685. doi: 10.1093/brain/awaa009

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rutter, M., Kreppner, J., Croft, C., Murin, M., Colvert, E., Beckett, C., et al. (2007). Early adolescent outcomes of institutionally deprived and non-deprived adoptees. III. Quasi-autism. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 48, 1200–1207. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2007.01792.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rutter, M., Kreppner, J., and Sonuga-Barke, E. (2009). Emanuel Miller lecture: attachment insecurity, disinhibited attachment, and attachment disorders: where do research findings leave the concepts? J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 50, 529–543. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2009.02042.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Salminen, M., Järvelä, S., Ruonala, A., Harjunen, V. J., Hamari, J., Jacucci, G., et al. (2022). Evoking physiological synchrony and empathy using social VR with biofeedback. IEEE Trans. Affect. Comput. 13, 746–755. doi: 10.1109/TAFFC.2019.2958657

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sänger, J., Müller, V., and Lindenberger, U. (2012). Intra- and interbrain synchronization and network properties when playing guitar in duets. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 6:312. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00312

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Saul, M. A., He, X., Black, S., and Charles, F. (2022). Corrigendum: a two-person neuroscience approach for social anxiety: a paradigm with interbrain synchrony and neurofeedback. Front. Psychol. 13:871022. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.871022

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Schilbach, L. (2016). Towards a second-person neuropsychiatry. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. Ser. B Biol. Sci. 371:20150081. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2015.0081

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Schilbach, L., and Lahnakoski, J. M. (2023). “Clinical neuroscience meets second-person neuropsychiatry,” in Social and affective neuroscience of everyday human interaction: From theory to methodology, eds. P. S. Boggio, T. S. H. Wingenbach, M. L. Silveira Coêlhoda, W. E. Comfort, L. Murrins Marques, and M. V. C. Alves (Cham: Springer International Publishing), 177–191.

Google Scholar

Schoenherr, D., Paulick, J., Strauss, B. M., Deisenhofer, A.-K., Schwartz, B., Rubel, J. A., et al. (2019). Nonverbal synchrony predicts premature termination of psychotherapy for social anxiety disorder. Psychotherapy 56, 503–513. doi: 10.1037/pst0000216

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Schoenherr, D., Strauss, B., Stangier, U., and Altmann, U. (2021). The influence of vocal synchrony on outcome and attachment anxiety/avoidance in treatments of social anxiety disorder. Psychotherapy 58, 510–522. doi: 10.1037/pst0000393

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sepulveda, P., Sitaram, R., Rana, M., Montalba, C., Tejos, C., and Ruiz, S. (2016). How feedback, motor imagery, and reward influence brain self-regulation using real-time fMRI. Hum. Brain Mapp. 37, 3153–3171. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23228

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sorger, B., Scharnowski, F., Linden, D. E. J., Hampson, M., and Young, K. D. (2019). Control freaks: towards optimal selection of control conditions for fMRI neurofeedback studies. NeuroImage 186, 256–265. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.11.004

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Spain, D., and Blainey, S. H. (2015). Group social skills interventions for adults with high-functioning autism spectrum disorders: a systematic review. Autism 19, 874–886. doi: 10.1177/1362361315587659

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Spengler, F. B., Scheele, D., Marsh, N., Kofferath, C., Flach, A., Schwarz, S., et al. (2017). Oxytocin facilitates reciprocity in social communication. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 12, 1325–1333. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsx061

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Srinivasan, S. M., Kaur, M., Park, I. K., Gifford, T. D., Marsh, K. L., and Bhat, A. N. (2015). The effects of rhythm and robotic interventions on the imitation/praxis, interpersonal synchrony, and motor performance of children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD): a pilot randomized controlled trial. Autism Res. Treat. 2015:736516, 1–18. doi: 10.1155/2015/736516

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Stein, D. J., Ipser, J. C., and Balkom, A. J. (2004). Pharmacotherapy for social phobia. Cochrane Database Syst. Rev. 18:CD001206. doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD001206.pub2

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Stein, M. B., and Stein, D. J. (2008). Social anxiety disorder. Lancet 371, 1115–1125. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(08)60488-2

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Steiner, N. J., Frenette, E., Hynes, C., Pisarik, E., Tomasetti, K., Perrin, E. C., et al. (2014). A pilot feasibility study of neurofeedback for children with autism. Appl. Psychophysiol. Biofeedback 39, 99–107. doi: 10.1007/s10484-014-9241-1

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Steinert, C., Hofmann, M., Leichsenring, F., and Kruse, J. (2013). What do we know today about the prospective long-term course of social anxiety disorder? A systematic literature review. J. Anxiety Disord. 27, 692–702. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2013.08.002

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Stepanova, E. R., Desnoyers-Stewart, J., Pasquier, P., and Riecke, B. E. (2020). JeL: breathing together to connect with others and nature. In Proceedings of the 2020 ACM Designing Interactive Systems Conference (DIS ‘20) (New York, NY, USA: Association for Computing Machinery), 641–654. doi: 10.1145/3357236.3395532

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Strehl, U., Aggensteiner, P., Wachtlin, D., Brandeis, D., Albrecht, B., Arana, M., et al. (2017). Neurofeedback of slow cortical potentials in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a multicenter randomized trial controlling for unspecific effects. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 11:135. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2017.00135

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Stuhldreher, N., Leibing, E., Leichsenring, F., Beutel, M. E., Herpertz, S., Hoyer, J., et al. (2014). The costs of social anxiety disorder: the role of symptom severity and comorbidities. J. Affect. Disord. 165, 87–94. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2014.04.039

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Subramanyam, A. A., Mukherjee, A., Dave, M., and Chavda, K. (2019). Clinical practice guidelines for autism Spectrum disorders. Indian J. Psychiatry 61, 254–269. doi: 10.4103/psychiatry.IndianJPsychiatry_542_18

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Susnoschi Luca, I., Putri, F. D., Ding, H., and Vuckovič, A. (2021). Brain synchrony in competition and collaboration during multiuser neurofeedback-based gaming. Front. Neuroergon. 2:749009. doi: 10.3389/fnrgo.2021.749009

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Szymanski, C., Müller, V., Brick, T. R., von Oertzen, T., and Lindenberger, U. (2017). Hyper-transcranial alternating current stimulation: experimental manipulation of inter-brain synchrony. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 11:539. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2017.00539

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tanabe, H. C., Kosaka, H., Saito, D. N., Koike, T., Hayashi, M. J., Izuma, K., et al. (2012). Hard to “tune in”: neural mechanisms of live face-to-face interaction with high-functioning autistic spectrum disorder. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 6:268. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00268

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tennant, J. M., Cook, S., Moldoveanu, M. C., Peterson, J. B., and Cunningham, W. A. (2019). “Interpersonal resonance: developing interpersonal biofeedback for the promotion of empathy and social entrainment” in Advances in human factors in wearable technologies and game design advances in intelligent systems and computing. ed. T. Z. Ahram (Cham: Springer International Publishing), 208–214.

Google Scholar

Thair, H., Holloway, A. L., Newport, R., and Smith, A. D. (2017). Transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS): a beginner’s guide for design and implementation. Front. Neurosci. 11:641. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2017.00641

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Thibault, R. T., MacPherson, A., Lifshitz, M., Roth, R. R., and Raz, A. (2018). Neurofeedback with fMRI: a critical systematic review. NeuroImage 172, 786–807. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.12.071

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ulmer-Yaniv, A., Yirmiya, K., Peleg, I., Zagoory-Sharon, O., and Feldman, R. (2023). Developmental cascades link maternal-newborn skin-to-skin contact with Young adults’ psychological symptoms, oxytocin, and immunity; charting mechanisms of developmental continuity from birth to adulthood. Biology 12:847. doi: 10.3390/biology12060847

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Valdesolo, P., and DeSteno, D. (2011). Synchrony and the social tuning of compassion. Emotion 11, 262–266. doi: 10.1037/a0021302

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Valdesolo, P., Ouyang, J., and DeSteno, D. (2010). The rhythm of joint action: synchrony promotes cooperative ability. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 46, 693–695. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2010.03.004

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Vicaria, I. M., and Dickens, L. (2016). Meta-analyses of the intra- and interpersonal outcomes of interpersonal coordination. J. Nonverbal Behav. 40, 335–361. doi: 10.1007/s10919-016-0238-8

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Vidal-Rosas, E. E., Lühmann, A.Von, Pinti, P., and Cooper, R. J. (2023). Wearable, high-density fNIRS and diffuse optical tomography technologies: a perspective. Neurophotonics. 10:023513. doi: 10.1117/1.NPh.10.2.023513

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Vrins, A., Pruss, E., Ceccato, C., Prinsen, J., and Alimardani, M. (2023). Investigating the impact of a dual musical brain-computer interface on interpersonal synchrony: a pilot study. doi: 10.48550/ARXIV.2309.02079

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, H., Suveg, C., West, K. B., Han, Z. R., Zhang, X., Hu, X., et al. (2021). Synchrony of respiratory sinus arrhythmia in parents and children with autism Spectrum disorder: moderation by interaction quality and child behavior problems. Autism Res. 14, 512–522. doi: 10.1002/aur.2401

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, Y., Wang, F., Kong, Y., Gao, T., Zhu, Q., Han, L., et al. (2023). High definition transcranial direct current stimulation of the Cz improves social dysfunction in children with autism spectrum disorder: a randomized, sham, controlled study. Autism Res. 16, 2035–2048. doi: 10.1002/aur.3018

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wass, S. V., Whitehorn, M., Marriott Haresign, I., Phillips, E., and Leong, V. (2020). Interpersonal neural entrainment during early social interaction. Trends Cogn. Sci. 24, 329–342. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2020.01.006

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wei, Y., Zhu, J., Pan, S., Su, H., Li, H., and Wang, J. (2017). Meta-analysis of the efficacy and safety of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) in the treatment of depression. Shanghai Arch. Psychiatry 29, 328–342. doi: 10.11919/j.issn.1002-0829.217106

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wiltermuth, S. S., and Heath, C. (2009). Synchrony and cooperation. Psychol. Sci. 20, 1–5. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02253.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Xu, W.-W., Liao, Q.-H., and Zhu, D.-W. (2022). The effect of transcranial magnetic stimulation on the recovery of attention and memory impairment following stroke: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Expert. Rev. Neurother. 22, 1031–1041. doi: 10.1080/14737175.2022.2155515

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Xu, X., Xu, M., Su, Y., Cao, T. V., Nikolin, S., Moffa, A., et al. (2023). Efficacy of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) combined with psychological interventions: a systematic review and Meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. Brain Sci. 13:1665. doi: 10.3390/brainsci13121665

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zamm, A., Debener, S., Bauer, A.-K. R., Bleichner, M. G., Demos, A. P., and Palmer, C. (2018). Amplitude envelope correlations measure synchronous cortical oscillations in performing musicians. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1423, 251–263. doi: 10.1111/nyas.13738

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zeanah, C. H., Chesher, T., Boris, N. W., Walter, H. J., Bukstein, O. G., Bellonci, C., et al. (2016). Practice parameter for the assessment and treatment of children and adolescents with reactive attachment disorder and disinhibited social engagement disorder. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 55, 990–1003. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2016.08.004

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, L., Xu, X., Li, Z., Chen, L., and Feng, L. (2022). Interpersonal neural synchronization predicting learning outcomes from teaching-learning interaction: a meta-analysis. Front. Psychol. 13:835147. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.835147

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, R., and Zhao, X. (2018). “A cross-brain interaction platform based on neurofeedback using electroencephalogram” in Augmented cognition: intelligent technologies lecture notes in computer science. eds. D. D. Schmorrow and C. M. Fidopiastis (Cham: Springer International Publishing), 222–230.

Google Scholar

Zheng, L., Liu, W., Long, Y., Zhai, Y., Zhao, H., Bai, X., et al. (2020). Affiliative bonding between teachers and students through interpersonal synchronisation in brain activity. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 15, 97–109. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsaa016

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zilverstand, A., Sorger, B., Sarkheil, P., and Goebel, R. (2015). fMRI neurofeedback facilitates anxiety regulation in females with spider phobia. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 9:148. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2015.00148

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: interpersonal neural synchrony (INS), hyperscanning neurofeedback, mental disorders, social dysfunction, brain stimulation

Citation: Konrad K, Gerloff C, Kohl SH, Mehler DMA, Mehlem L, Volbert EL, Komorek M, Henn AT, Boecker M, Weiss E and Reindl V (2024) Interpersonal neural synchrony and mental disorders: unlocking potential pathways for clinical interventions. Front. Neurosci. 18:1286130. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2024.1286130

Received: 30 August 2023; Accepted: 30 January 2024;
Published: 11 March 2024.

Edited by:

Yumie Ono, Meiji University, Japan

Reviewed by:

Kazuyori Yagyu, Health Sciences University of Hokkaido, Japan
Takayuki Nozawa, University of Toyama, Japan

Copyright © 2024 Konrad, Gerloff, Kohl, Mehler, Mehlem, Volbert, Komorek, Henn, Boecker, Weiss and Reindl. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Kerstin Konrad, ay5rb25yYWRAZnotanVlbGljaC5kZQ==

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.