Skip to main content

REVIEW article

Front. Neurosci., 30 August 2023
Sec. Visual Neuroscience
This article is part of the Research Topic Insights in Visual Neuroscience: 2023 View all 8 articles

The dorsal lateral geniculate nucleus and the pulvinar as essential partners for visual cortical functions

Christian Casanova
Christian Casanova1*Leo M. ChalupaLeo M. Chalupa2
  • 1School of Optometry, University of Montreal, Montreal, QC, Canada
  • 2School of Medicine and Health Sciences, The George Washington University, Washington, DC, United States

In most neuroscience textbooks, the thalamus is presented as a structure that relays sensory signals from visual, auditory, somatosensory, and gustatory receptors to the cerebral cortex. But the function of the thalamic nuclei goes beyond the simple transfer of information. This is especially true for the second-order nuclei, but also applies to first-order nuclei. First order thalamic nuclei receive information from the periphery, like the dorsal lateral geniculate nucleus (dLGN), which receives a direct input from the retina. In contrast, second order thalamic nuclei, like the pulvinar, receive minor or no input from the periphery, with the bulk of their input derived from cortical areas. The dLGN refines the information received from the retina by temporal decorrelation, thereby transmitting the most “relevant” signals to the visual cortex. The pulvinar is closely linked to virtually all visual cortical areas, and there is growing evidence that it is necessary for normal cortical processing and for aspects of visual cognition. In this article, we will discuss what we know and do not know about these structures and propose some thoughts based on the knowledge gained during the course of our careers. We hope that these thoughts will arouse curiosity about the visual thalamus and its important role, especially for the next generation of neuroscientists.

1. Foreword

Much has been accomplished since the first recordings were made from the mammalian dorsal lateral geniculate nucleus (dLGN) by Barlow, Fitzhugh and Kuffler in 1957. These pioneering investigators found that the visual receptive field properties of dLGN neurons were in many ways similar to those reported earlier for retinal ganglion cells by Kuffler (1953). These early studies, as well as the observations of Hubel (1960), laid the groundwork for the long-maintained viewpoint that the dLGN functions largely to relay visual information from the retina to the primary visual cortex. Indeed, this notion can still be found in some neuroscience and medical textbooks today. Similarly, interest in the pulvinar, the largest thalamic nucleus in mammals, originally stems from its connectivity with the mesencephalon and the cortex. The pulvinar was also often considered as a relay nucleus, providing a route for signals from the superior colliculus, a structure involved in the control of fixation and eye movements, to the visual cortex.

The problem with the term “relay” is that it suggests that information is transmitted without significant modification or transformation. We now know that this is not the case. Indeed, while the precise roles of the dLGN and the pulvinar in visual and extra-visual functions are yet to be established, a rather substantial body of literature has clearly refuted the notion that these are “relay” nuclei. Our intent in this paper is to provide an overview of some of the key studies in this field and to propose some thoughts as to how this aspect of visual neuroscience could be advanced in future work.

2. The dorsal lateral geniculate nucleus

The dLGN is undoubtedly the most studied subcortical region of the visual brain (Werner and Chalupa, 2004). In mammals with front-facing eyes, this knee-shaped structure is organized in distinct layers each receiving signals from one of the two eyes. Thus, information from the two eyes remains separate until it reaches the primary visual cortex where binocularity is formally established. Hubel (1960) was the first to record visual activity of optic tract units as well as those of dLGN neurons in the same animal preparation, which led him to conclude that the organization of the receptive fields of dLGN neurons are strikingly similar to those of retinal ganglion cells. Later, Enroth-Cugell and Robson (1966) discovered three parallel retinal pathways (X, Y and W) that differ in spatial and temporal resolution, contrast sensitivity and conduction velocity. The latter were subsequently revealed in primates (Kaplan and Shapley, 1982), and it is now well established that the four dorsal layers (parvocellular) of the dLGN receive signals about form and color from X-type ganglion cells and the two ventral layers (magnocellular), receive signals about movement from Y-type ganglion cells. Ventral to each of the magno- and parvocellular layers lie the koniocellular layers, which contain a heterogenous group of neurons whose functions remain to be fully determined. While the main emphasis of these early studies was the role of the retinal input in the organization of dLGN receptive fields, subsequent anatomical studies showed that only about 10 percent of the synaptic contacts onto dLGN neurons derive from the retina. The astonishing conclusion from the anatomical evidence was that most of the input to the dLGN is from non-retinal sources, mainly the visual cortex and various brainstem nuclei (Jones, 1985).

How is it that the retina, which represents only about 10% of the dLGN afferents, has the strongest functional input? The answer, in part, comes from the work of Sherman and Guillery (1998). Based on several morphological and functional criteria, these authors have identified two types of inputs: drivers and modulators (type 2 and type 1 projections, respectively). Essentially, drivers determine the main properties of the activity of their target cells, whereas modulators provide contextual modulation of the recipient neuron’s activity. Studies have shown that retinal ganglion cells provide a driver input to dLGN neurons, which in turn send driver signals to layer 4 neurons in V1. In return, layer 6 neurons of V1 projecting to the dLGN exert a modulatory influence (Figure 1). These laminar patterns are in accordance with the assumption made by several authors (cf, Jones, 1985), that drivers and modulators terminate and originate from distinct cortical layers. For bottom-up projections, thalamocortical terminals ending in layer 4 are drivers, while those ending in layer 1 are modulators. For top-down projections, cortico-thalamic cells in layer 5 provide driver signals, while those lying in layer 6 send modulatory signals. Thus, visual processing in the dLGN is continuously influenced by patterns of activity occurring in V1. By considering the visual response latency and conduction latency of corticogeniculate neurons, Briggs and Usrey (2007) estimated that cortical feedback signals can reach the dLGN within ∼50 ms of the presentation of a visual stimulus. It is acknowledged that these signals have little effect on the spatial characteristics of dLGN receptive fields inherited from retinal inputs or on their “sharpness” (Murphy and Sillito, 1987; Sillito and Jones, 2002; Webb et al., 2002). However, there is mounting evidence that the visual cortex enhances response precision of dLGN neurons by reducing their gain variability, thereby increasing their information coding capacity (Hasse and Briggs, 2017; Murphy et al., 2021; Spacek et al., 2022).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. The main predicted laminar connections involving the pulvinar are based on the general rules proposed by Jones (1985), with additional constraints advanced by Crick and Koch (1998). The geniculo-cortical pathway is provided for comparison. Cortico-cortical projections are omitted for clarity. d: drivers, m: modulators. When in bold, d and m were confirmed functionally.

So, what does the dLGN do besides transferring retinal signals to the cortex? In most textbooks, one will learn that the dLGN is gating information according to the state of vigilance. Depending on the influence of signals from the reticular activating system of the brainstem, dLGN neurons will discharge with regularly spaced action potentials (tonic mode, often associated with arousal) or with clusters of spikes (burst mode, associated with sleep), dissociating the dLGN from external signals. We invite the reader to consult the comprehensive reviews of Weyand (2016) and Ghodrati et al. (2017), who propose several functions of the dLGN reflecting the numerous non-retinal inputs that allow the dLGN to transform retinal signals in dynamic ways. We would like to mention here two notable facts. The tonic-burst mode of dLGN neurons is not solely related to arousal as previously thought. It has been shown that V1 feedback projections shift the firing mode of geniculate neurons between burst and tonic patterns (Sherman, 2001). Alitto et al. (2019) showed that retinogeniculate communication is enhanced during the burst mode and visually evoked thalamic bursts, thereby augmenting retinal signals transmitted to cortex. Computational models suggest that burst neurons can encode stimulus features similarly to their tonic counterparts (Elijah et al., 2015; Alitto et al., 2019). Thus, by transitioning between the two modes in a dynamic way, cells in dLGN may allow the most relevant information to be transferred to the visual cortex during normal waking behavior. Another interesting fact is that the temporal aspect of retinal and dLGN responses differ considerably. This has led to the proposal that dLGN neurons improve visual coding by reducing redundancy through temporal decorrelation of the retinal signals, much in the same way that the retina improves efficiency by spatially decorrelating incoming images (Truccolo and Dong, 2001; Tan and Yao, 2009; Alexander et al., 2022).

Clearly, the dLGN is more than a “simple” relay nucleus. The retinal signals inputs to this thalamic structure are modulated by non-retinal inputs, such as those originating from the visual cortex, the mesencephalon, the thalamic reticular nucleus, the extraocular muscles, and the brainstem. The dLGN is not merely a gate that opens and closes, nor simply a linear filter. Its plays a fundamental role in regulating information transmission to the visual cortex as a function of the activity taking place in subcortical and cortical areas, thereby enhancing the message being transferred to the first site of cortical computation, i.e., V1 (Fisher et al., 2017). But is the dLGN involved in higher-level processing as proposed by some authors (Zabbah et al., 2014; Ghodrati et al., 2017), or should we turn our attention to the pulvinar nucleus for that?

3. The pulvinar nucleus

Situated next to the dLGN (the “knee”) is the pulvinar nucleus (the “cushion”). The pulvinar lies over the dorsolateral posterior thalamus and runs along the medial edge of the dLGN. The size and differentiation of the pulvinar increased strikingly during evolution, in parallel with the neocortex (Chalfin et al., 2007; Kaas and Baldwin, 2019). Thus, it is the largest visual nucleus of the thalamus in primates and cats, which have been the two most studied species in pulvinar research. In both species, it has established extensive reciprocal connections with all visual cortical areas (see Casanova, 2004). Six subdivisions have been identified in the pulvinar of primates, including humans, and several representations of the visual field have been delineated (Bender, 1981; Arcaro et al., 2015; DeSimone et al., 2015; Baldwin et al., 2017). In the cat, the lateral posterior-pulvinar complex is generally subdivided into three main regions (up to five have been proposed, Hutchins and Updyke, 1989), each containing a coarse representation of the contralateral visual field: the lateral and medial parts of lateral posterior nucleus (LPl and LPm) and the pulvinar per se, also named the striate-, tecto-, and retino-recipient zones of the complex, respectively (Berson and Graybiel, 1983; Abramson and Chalupa, 1985). The nomenclature of this nucleus across species is rather confusing. In animals like rodents and rabbits, it is named the lateral posterior (LP) nucleus. In cats, the two main subdivisions of the LP are also considered as the homolog of the primate pulvinar, while a portion of the cat pulvinar has been considered as part of the geniculate complex (Mason, 1978). A reevaluation of the terminology is necessary. For clarity, only the term “pulvinar” will be used thereafter.

While there is a plethora of studies on the dLGN and the primary visual cortex, fewer have focused on the pulvinar, particularly on the properties of its neurons. This is undoubtedly due to the difficulty of documenting the visual response properties of pulvinar cells, which are much more capricious than those of the dLGN and more sensitive to anesthetics. Interest in the pulvinar stems from the discovery of the existence of two visual systems (Schneider, 1969): one for discrimination (retina-dLGN-visual cortex) and the other for localization (retina-superior colliculus). At the time, some laboratories such as that of Donald B. Lindsley (Chalupa et al., 1972), focused their work on the pulvinar, as signals from the superior colliculus must pass through the pulvinar to reach the visual cortex, a research theme that has been addressed recently by Beltramo and Scanziani (2019). Unlike the dLGN, which receives its main visual inputs from the retina, the pulvinar gets its visual signals mainly from the neocortex and the mesencephalon. The strong cortical input is reflected in the response properties of its neurons, which remarkably resemble those found in the visual cortex (Bender, 1982; Chalupa and Abramson, 1988; Casanova et al., 1989; Chalupa and Abramson, 1989). Essentially, pulvinar cells have large receptive fields, largely encompassing those of V1 neurons, with a complex cell-like organization (Casanova et al., 1989; Piche et al., 2015). In contrast to dLGN neurons, most pulvinar cells are binocular, sensitive to retinal disparity, and selective for stimulus orientation and direction of motion. Moreover, in cats (as well as in humans), pulvinar neurons can signal the true direction of motion of complex stimuli such as plaid patterns and random dot kinematograms (Merabet et al., 1998; Dumbrava et al., 2001; Villeneuve et al., 2005; Thompson et al., 2012; Villeneuve et al., 2012). This level of computation is generally described in higher-order cortical areas only, further suggesting that the pulvinar actively participates to the processing of visual signals transferred along the cortical hierarchy. Pattern-motion neurons were mostly found in the LPm subdivision of pulvinar. Differences between the response properties of the two main subregions of the pulvinar, the LPl and LPm, have been described and likely reflect the predominance of V1 and collicular inputs targeting the LPl and LPm, respectively (Chalupa and Abramson, 1989; Piche et al., 2015). The interest of researchers toward the pulvinar was increased by the discovery that the activity of a subset of cells is linked to visual attention (Petersen et al., 1985). Subsequent studies on behaving monkeys and humans (normal and with lesions) have indeed confirmed that this nucleus is involved in visual attention, visuo-motor functions, target selection among distractors and feature binding (Chalupa et al., 1976; Ward et al., 2002; Danziger et al., 2004; Shipp, 2004; Zhou et al., 2016; Fiebelkorn et al., 2019). In recent years, given it connectivity with the limbic system, the pulvinar has been linked with the visual processing of fearful stimuli (Ward et al., 2005; Ren and Tao, 2020). Moreover, pulvinar is considered to play an important role in unconscious vision or blindsight (Cowey et al., 1994; Fox et al., 2020).

This array of functions associated with the pulvinar not only reflects the extensive connectivity of this thalamic complex with the neocortex but also illustrates why researchers are still struggling to assign a clear role to the pulvinar. This is why we must return to the roots and determine the nature of the signals being exchanged between the pulvinar and the visual cortex. As mentioned above, given its extensive reciprocal connectivity with the visual cortex, the pulvinar is in an exceptional position to regulate cortical processing along the visual hierarchy, both in the feedforward and feedback directions. For decades, the perception of external stimuli was considered to result solely from the processing of thalamic signals through direct cortico-cortical connections between hierarchically organized areas. We must now consider the fact that, besides direct communication between cortical areas through cortico-cortical connections, indirect communication through cortico-pulvinar-cortical projections also occurs (Figure 1). Through these transthalamic pathways, each cortical area of the visual cortex is potentially only one thalamic synapse away from another cortical area, allowing a rapid modulation of cortical activity according to external signals and internal computations. What do we know about these transthalamic cortical pathways, which has been the subject of research by several laboratories (e.g., Rockland, 1996; Baldauf et al., 2005; Huppe-Gourgues et al., 2006; Rockland, 2019). We know that V1 provides a strong driver input to the pulvinar from layer 5 complex cells (Casanova, 1993). Permanent or transient inactivation of V1 results in an almost total disappearance of visual responses in the striate-recipient zone of the pulvinar in cats and primates, indicating that V1 is necessary for establishing the fundamental representation of the visual world in the pulvinar (Bender, 1983; Casanova et al., 1997). The contribution of other areas to the subregions of the pulvinar is still not fully understood. Studies have shown that pattern-motion neurons in the LPm of cats only disappear when both V1 and the ectosylvian cortex are lesioned (Merabet et al., 1998). Recently, Abbas Farishta et al. (2020) revealed that the ratio of type 1/type 2 terminals (thus, putatively, the modulator/driver ratio) from layers 5 and 6 neurons terminating in the pulvinar increases along the cortical hierarchy (Figure 2). This organizational scheme predicts that higher-order visual cortical areas will primarily modulate activity in the pulvinar. A theoretical model based on this organization suggests that the pulvinar shows a bistable spiking activity, oscillatory or regular asynchronous spiking, whose responses are gated by the different activation of cortico-pulvinar projections from lower to higher-order areas (Cortes et al., 2021), but this has yet to be demonstrated functionally.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Simplified diagram illustrating the increase in modulator terminals (type 1) and the concomitant decrease in driver terminals (type 2) along the cortical hierarchy (refer to Abbas Farishta et al., 2020).

A greater number of studies have explored the impact of the pulvinar on the visual cortex. However, during some of these studies, the understanding of drivers and modulators was either unknown or in its early stages. As a result, the visual stimuli and experimental approaches were not specifically designed to differentiate between these two types of signals (e.g., Minville and Casanova, 1998). Vanni et al. (2015) discovered that direct stimulation of the pulvinar and dLGN in tree shrews produced distinct spatiotemporal profiles of voltage-sensitive dye responses in the visual cortex. Stimulation of the dLGN resulted in rapid, strong, and localized responses in the primary visual cortex, characteristic of a driver input. On the other hand, pulvinar activation evoked only weak and diffuse responses in the same area, indicative of a modulatory input. Interestingly, pulvinar stimulation evoked fast and robust responses in higher-order visual areas. This finding supports the notion that the pulvinar predominantly projects to layer 1 of V1 (modulatory input) and layer 4 (driver input) in areas beyond V1 (Abramson and Chalupa, 1985). However, Logothetis et al. (2010) reported no compelling differences between the positive blood oxygen level-dependent (BOLD) signals in V1 and extrastriate cortical areas when the pulvinar was electrically stimulated in anesthetized monkeys. Subsequently, Roth et al. (2016) revealed a distinct organization of thalamic input from the dLGN (circumscribed and visuotopically organized) and the pulvinar (more distributed) in the primary visual cortex of mice. This suggests again that the pulvinar is more likely involved in the contextual modulation of activity in V1.

In a different set of recordings conducted on macaque monkeys, Saalmann et al. (2012) demonstrated that the pulvinar is capable of synchronizing activity between cortical areas based on attentional allocation. This finding aligns with a study in cats showing that the pulvinar modulates the oscillatory information between areas 17 and 21a in gamma and alpha bands for feedforward and feedback processing, respectively (Wrobel et al., 2007; Cortes et al., 2020). Together, these findings indicate that the pulvinar is involved in the mechanisms underlying oscillatory communication along the visual cortex. The role of the pulvinar in attentional processing was further suggested by Zhou et al. (2016), who showed that inactivation of the macaque pulvinar leads to a decrease in attentional effects on firing rates and gamma synchrony and coherence of neurons in area V4. Additionally, a more recent study conducted in ferrets demonstrated that fluctuations in pupil-linked arousal result in dynamic changes in frequencies along the thalamocortical network (Stitt et al., 2018). During low arousal states, synchronized activity between the pulvinar and the posterior parietal cortex was characterized by alpha oscillations, while higher arousal states exhibited theta frequency bands. The authors proposed that the pulvinar may act as a gate or suppressor of incoming sensory information in the early stages of the visual cortex during low arousal but facilitate visual exploration and attentional selection during increased arousal. A similar pattern of synchronization was observed in recordings between the mediodorsal pulvinar, the frontal eye fields (FEF), and the lateral intraparietal region (LIP) of macaques by Fiebelkorn et al. (2019). According to these authors, the pulvinar may gate communication between cortical areas by aligning the phases of their oscillatory responses.

De Souza et al. (2020) conducted a study to examine the impact of the pulvinar on the contrast response function of neurons in two hierarchically distinct areas of the cat visual cortex. Their findings highlight the complexity of the functional relationship between the pulvinar and the cortex. In the primary visual cortex (V1), pulvinar inactivation resulted in a modest decrease in the response gain of neurons to contrast. This effect was less pronounced than the near-complete suppression of activity observed in the supragranular layers of V1 in prosimian primates when the pulvinar is silenced (Purushothaman et al., 2012). Surprisingly, contrary to expectations based on anatomical evidence, pulvinar inactivation led to a significant increase in response gain in the majority of neurons in area 21a (often considered the homolog of primate area V4), with only a subset of cells exhibiting changes in contrast gain. Similar increases in response amplitude were also observed in area V2 of Cebus monkeys when the pulvinar was inactivated (Soares et al., 2004). The study by De Souza et al. (2020) demonstrates that the pulvinar can influence functions across the visual cortex through the modulation of neuronal activity. This suggests that the mechanisms underlying the transthalamic flow of information and its role in cortical contrast processing involve the pulvinar’s ability to modulate neuronal responses. The authors proposed a model that explains the observed changes in response gain in hierarchically distinct cortical areas based on the interaction between the pulvinar and feedforward visual cortex signals.

These findings underscore the significance of computational models in advancing our understanding of pulvinar functions. Over the years, various cognitive and computational models of visual perception have put forth roles for the pulvinar. Three decades ago, Mumford (1992) proposed that the thalamus, including nuclei like the pulvinar, functions as an active blackboard that maintains an updated representation of the visual world. This enables cortical areas to be informed about relevant changes in the visual scene, requiring new neuronal computations for planning action strategies. A more recent theoretical framework, known as predictive coding, also recognizes the unique role of the pulvinar in influencing cortical processing within and between cortical areas (Kanai et al., 2015; Haarsma et al., 2021). In essence, predictive coding is a model of neuronal organization that suggests the brain constantly generates and updates an internal representation of its environment. The brain generates predictions about the state of the world based on stored sensory inputs and compares them to incoming sensory information, resulting in prediction errors. To contextualize and coordinate these predictions and prediction errors, the brain requires a regulatory mechanism that assigns “precision” to the message. Within this framework, the pulvinar could play a crucial role in processing the variability of cortical signals to modulate the transfer of feedforward and feedback information. Disruptions in this “precision-weighting” of neuronal activity have been proposed as a key mechanism underlying the pathogenesis of psychosis, including schizophrenia, which is characterized by dysfunctions in thalamocortical communication involving the pulvinar (Byne et al., 2009; Adams et al., 2013; Benarroch, 2015; Friston, 2022).

4. Future avenues of study and challenges

Our current understanding of the role of thalamic nuclei in vision, particularly the pulvinar and its associated pathways, still has significant gaps. It is crucial to acknowledge that a solid understanding of the anatomy is a prerequisite to comprehending the physiology. Therefore, we must continue to characterize the anatomical organization of pathways involving the lateral geniculate nucleus and the pulvinar, and explore the effects of activating or silencing their specific components. The remarkable progress in technical approaches, such as optogenetics, may prove to be valuable in this pursuit. The ability to manipulate single cell types within an intact cortico-thalamic network can provide more precise information compared to conventional silencing techniques that affect all neuron classes. This technique, combined with connectomics approaches like three-dimensional visualization of neural networks involving thalamic nuclei in transparent tissue and resting-state functional imaging, can unveil cortico-thalamic connectivity in both animal models and humans. With the knowledge we have acquired in recent years, it is crucial to refine our protocols when studying the impact of thalamic lesions on visual cognition and take advantage of technical advancements (e.g., the visualization of dLGN layers in fMRI studies). However, there are still some challenges to address. Research on the lateral geniculate nucleus may not be as glamorous as it once was, making it difficult to secure funding from granting agencies. Recording from pulvinar neurons is challenging, and only a limited number of laboratories have the expertise to do so. Furthermore, animal models are currently limited, with mice being the preferred model. However, mounting evidence suggests that the organization of transthalamic and cortico-cortical pathways in mice differs significantly from that of higher mammals.

Nevertheless, the visual thalamus remains a fascinating area of study for the next generation of neuroscientists. They now have access to powerful tools that allow for investigations into the constituent nuclei and associated pathways, enabling a better understanding of the exact role of thalamic nuclei in sensory processing and the consequences of their dysfunction. This knowledge is essential for developing tools to restore vision and holds great promise for future research.

Author contributions

CC: Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. LC: Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.

This review received financial support from CIHR (PJT-148959 & 202209PJT) and NSERC (RGPIN-2019-04982).

Acknowledgments

We express our gratitude to all the students, postdocs, and staff of our laboratories, whose invaluable contributions were essential for the successful completion of our research. We also extend our thanks to those who recognized the importance of funding research on the thalamus, especially the pulvinar.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest. The author(s) declared that they were an editorial board member of Frontiers, at the time of submission. This had no impact on the peer review process and the final decision.

The author(s) declared that they were an editorial board member of Frontiers, at the time of submission. This had no impact on the peer review process and the final decision.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Abbas Farishta, R., Boire, D., and Casanova, C. (2020). Hierarchical organization of corticothalamic projections to the pulvinar. Cereb. Cortex. Commun. 1:tgaa030. doi: 10.1093/texcom/tgaa030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Abramson, B. P., and Chalupa, L. M. (1985). The laminar distribution of cortical connections with the tecto- and cortico-recipient zones in the cat's lateral posterior nucleus. Neuroscience 15, 81–95. doi: 10.1016/0306-4522(85)90125-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Adams, R. A., Stephan, K. E., Brown, H. R., Frith, C. D., and Friston, K. J. (2013). The computational anatomy of psychosis. Front. Psych. 4:47. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2013.00047

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Alexander, P. C., Alitto, H. J., Fisher, T. G., Rathbun, D. L., Weyand, T. G., and Usrey, W. M. (2022). Dynamics of temporal integration in the lateral geniculate nucleus. eNeuro 9:ENEURO.0088-22.2022. doi: 10.1523/ENEURO.0088-22.2022

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Alitto, H., Rathbun, D. L., Vandeleest, J. J., Alexander, P. C., and Usrey, W. M. (2019). The augmentation of retinogeniculate communication during thalamic burst mode. J. Neurosci. 39, 5697–5710. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2320-18.2019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arcaro, M. J., Pinsk, M. A., and Kastner, S. (2015). The anatomical and functional organization of the human visual pulvinar. J. Neurosci. 35, 9848–9871. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1575-14.2015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baldauf, Z. B., Chomsung, R. D., Carden, W. B., May, P. J., and Bickford, M. E. (2005). Ultrastructural analysis of projections to the pulvinar nucleus of the cat. I: middle suprasylvian gyrus (areas 5 and 7). J. Comp. Neurol. 485, 87–107. doi: 10.1002/cne.20480

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baldwin, M. K. L., Balaram, P., and Kaas, J. H. (2017). The evolution and functions of nuclei of the visual pulvinar in primates. J. Comp. Neurol. 525, 3207–3226. doi: 10.1002/cne.24272

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beltramo, R., and Scanziani, M. (2019). A collicular visual cortex: Neocortical space for an ancient midbrain visual structure. Science 363, 64–69. doi: 10.1126/science.aau7052

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Benarroch, E. E. (2015). Pulvinar: associative role in cortical function and clinical correlations. Neurology 84, 738–747. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000001276

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bender, D. B. (1981). Retinotopic organization of macaque pulvinar. J. Neurophysiol. 46, 672–693. doi: 10.1152/jn.1981.46.3.672

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bender, D. B. (1982). Receptive-field properties of neurons in the macaque inferior pulvinar. J. Neurophysiol. 48, 1–17. doi: 10.1152/jn.1982.48.1.1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bender, D. B. (1983). Visual activation of neurons in the primate pulvinar depends on cortex but not colliculus. Brain Res. 279, 258–261. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(83)90188-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berson, D. M., and Graybiel, A. M. (1983). Organization of the striate-recipient zone of the cats lateralis posterior-pulvinar complex and its relations with the geniculostriate system. Neuroscience 9, 337–372. doi: 10.1016/0306-4522(83)90299-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Briggs, F., and Usrey, W. M. (2007). A fast, reciprocal pathway between the lateral geniculate nucleus and visual cortex in the macaque monkey. J. Neurosci. 27, 5431–5436. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1035-07.2007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Byne, W., Hazlett, E. A., Buchsbaum, M. S., and Kemether, E. (2009). The thalamus and schizophrenia: current status of research. Acta Neuropathol. 117, 347–368. doi: 10.1007/s00401-008-0404-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Casanova, C. (1993). Response properties of neurons in area 17 projecting to the striate-recipient zone of the cat's lateralis posterior-pulvinar complex: comparison with cortico-tectal cells. Exp. Brain Res. 96, 247–259. doi: 10.1007/BF00227105

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Casanova, C. (2004). “Function of the pulvinar in vision” in The visual neurosciences. eds. L. M. Chalupa and J. S. Werner (Cambridge: MIT Press)

Google Scholar

Casanova, C., Freeman, R. D., and Nordmann, J. P. (1989). Monocular and binocular response properties of cells in the striate-recipient zone of the cat's lateral posterior-pulvinar complex. J. Neurophysiol. 62, 544–557. doi: 10.1152/jn.1989.62.2.544

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Casanova, C., Savard, T., and Darveau, S. (1997). Contribution of area 17 to cell responses in the striate-recipient zone of the cat's lateral posterior-pulvinar complex. Eur. J. Neurosci. 9, 1026–1036. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.1997.tb01453.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chalfin, B. P., Cheung, D. T., Muniz, J. A., De Lima Silveira, L. C., and Finlay, B. L. (2007). Scaling of neuron number and volume of the pulvinar complex in New World primates: comparisons with humans, other primates, and mammals. J. Comp. Neurol. 504, 265–274. doi: 10.1002/cne.21406

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chalupa, L. M., and Abramson, B. P. (1988). Receptive-field properties in the tecto- and striate-recipient zones of the cat's lateral posterior nucleus. Prog. Brain Res. 75, 85–94. doi: 10.1016/S0079-6123(08)60468-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chalupa, L. M., and Abramson, B. P. (1989). Visual receptive fields in the striate-recipient zone of the lateral posterior-pulvinar complex. J. Neurosci. 9, 347–357. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.09-01-00347.1989

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chalupa, L. M., Anchel, H., and Lindsley, D. B. (1972). Visual input to the pulvinar via lateral geniculate, superior colliculus and visual cortex in the cat. Exp. Neurol. 36, 449–462. doi: 10.1016/0014-4886(72)90005-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chalupa, L. M., Coyle, R. S., and Lindsley, D. B. (1976). Effect of pulvinar lesions on visual pattern discrimination in monkeys. J. Neurophysiol. 39, 354–369. doi: 10.1152/jn.1976.39.2.354

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cortes, N., Abbas Farishta, R., Ladret, H. J., and Casanova, C. (2021). Corticothalamic projections gate alpha rhythms in the pulvinar. Front. Cell. Neurosci. 15:787170. doi: 10.3389/fncel.2021.787170

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cortes, N., De Souza, B. O. F., and Casanova, C. (2020). Pulvinar modulates synchrony across visual cortical areas. Vision 4:22. doi: 10.3390/vision4020022

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cowey, A., Stoerig, P., and Bannister, M. (1994). Retinal ganglion cells labelled from the pulvinar nucleus in macaque monkeys. Neuroscience 61, 691–705. doi: 10.1016/0306-4522(94)90445-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crick, F., and Koch, C. (1998). Constraints on cortical and thalamic projections: the no-strong-loops hypothesis. Nature 391, 245–250. doi: 10.1038/34584

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Danziger, S., Ward, R., Owen, V., and Rafal, R. (2004). Contributions of the human pulvinar to linking vision and action. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 4, 89–99. doi: 10.3758/CABN.4.1.89

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Souza, B. O. F., Cortes, N., and Casanova, C. (2020). Pulvinar modulates contrast responses in the visual cortex as a function of cortical hierarchy. Cereb. Cortex 14, 1068–1086. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhz149

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Desimone, K., Viviano, J. D., and Schneider, K. A. (2015). Population receptive field estimation reveals new retinotopic maps in human subcortex. J. Neurosci. 35, 9836–9847. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3840-14.2015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dumbrava, D., Faubert, J., and Casanova, C. (2001). Global motion integration in the cat's lateral posterior-pulvinar complex. Eur. J. Neurosci. 13, 2218–2226. doi: 10.1046/j.0953-816x.2001.01598.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Elijah, D. H., Samengo, I., and Montemurro, M. A. (2015). Thalamic neuron models encode stimulus information by burst-size modulation. Front. Comput. Neurosci. 9:113. doi: 10.3389/fncom.2015.00113

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Enroth-Cugell, C., and Robson, J. G. (1966). The contrast sensitivity of retinal ganglion cells of the cat. J. Physiol. 187, 517–552. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1966.sp008107

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fiebelkorn, I. C., Pinsk, M. A., and Kastner, S. (2019). The mediodorsal pulvinar coordinates the macaque fronto-parietal network during rhythmic spatial attention. Nat. Commun. 10:215. doi: 10.1038/s41467-018-08151-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fisher, T. G., Alitto, H. J., and Usrey, W. M. (2017). Retinal and nonretinal contributions to extraclassical surround suppression in the lateral geniculate nucleus. J. Neurosci. 37, 226–235. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1577-16.2016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fox, D. M., Goodale, M. A., and Bourne, J. A. (2020). The age-dependent neural substrates of blindsight. Trends Neurosci. 43, 242–252. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2020.01.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Friston, K. (2022). Computational psychiatry: from synapses to sentience. Mol. Psychiatry 28, 256–268. doi: 10.1038/s41380-022-01743-z

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ghodrati, M., Khaligh-Razavi, S. M., and Lehky, S. R. (2017). Towards building a more complex view of the lateral geniculate nucleus: recent advances in understanding its role. Prog. Neurobiol. 156, 214–255. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2017.06.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Haarsma, J., Fletcher, P. C., Griffin, J. D., Taverne, H. J., Ziauddeen, H., Spencer, T. J., et al. (2021). Precision weighting of cortical unsigned prediction error signals benefits learning, is mediated by dopamine, and is impaired in psychosis. Mol. Psychiatry 26, 5320–5333. doi: 10.1038/s41380-020-0803-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hasse, J. M., and Briggs, F. (2017). Corticogeniculate feedback sharpens the temporal precision and spatial resolution of visual signals in the ferret. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 114, E6222–E6230. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1704524114

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hubel, D. H. (1960). Single unit activity in lateral geniculate body and optic tract of unrestrained cats. J. Physiol. 150, 91–104. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1960.sp006375

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huppe-Gourgues, F., Bickford, M. E., Boire, D., Ptito, M., and Casanova, C. (2006). Distribution, morphology, and synaptic targets of corticothalamic terminals in the cat lateral posterior-pulvinar complex that originate from the posteromedial lateral suprasylvian cortex. J. Comp. Neurol. 497, 847–863. doi: 10.1002/cne.21024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hutchins, B., and Updyke, B. V. (1989). Retinotopic organization within the lateral posterior complex of the cat. J. Comp. Neurol. 285, 350–398. doi: 10.1002/cne.902850306

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jones, E. G. (1985). In the thalamus, Penum Press, New York

Google Scholar

Kaas, J. H., and Baldwin, M. K. L. (2019). The evolution of the pulvinar complex in primates and its role in the dorsal and ventral streams of cortical processing. Vision 4:3. doi: 10.3390/vision4010003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kanai, R., Komura, Y., Shipp, S., and Friston, K. (2015). Cerebral hierarchies: predictive processing, precision and the pulvinar. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. Ser. B Biol. Sci. 370:20140169. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2014.0169

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kaplan, E., and Shapley, R. M. (1982). X and Y cells in the lateral geniculate nucleus of macaque monkeys. J. Physiol. 330, 125–143. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1982.sp014333

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kuffler, S. W. (1953). Discharge patterns and functional organization of mammalian retina. J. Neurophysiol. 16, 37–68. doi: 10.1152/jn.1953.16.1.37

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Logothetis, N. K., Augath, M., Murayama, Y., Rauch, A., Sultan, F., Goense, J., et al. (2010). The effects of electrical microstimulation on cortical signal propagation. Nat. Neurosci. 13, 1283–1291. doi: 10.1038/nn.2631

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mason, R. (1978). Functional organization in the cat's pulvinar complex. Exp. Brain Res. 31, 51–66. doi: 10.1007/BF00235804

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Merabet, L., Desautels, A., Minville, K., and Casanova, C. (1998). Motion integration in a thalamic visual nucleus. Nature 396, 265–268. doi: 10.1038/24382

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Minville, K., and Casanova, C. (1998). Spatial frequency processing in posteromedial lateral suprasylvian cortex does not depend on the projections from the striate-recipient zone of the cat's lateral posterior-pulvinar complex. Neuroscience 84, 699–711. doi: 10.1016/S0306-4522(97)00525-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mumford, D. (1992). On the computational architecture of the neocortex. Ii. The role of cortico-cortical loops. Biol. Cybern. 66, 241–251. doi: 10.1007/BF00198477

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Murphy, A. J., Shaw, L., Hasse, J. M., Goris, R. L. T., and Briggs, F. (2021). Optogenetic activation of corticogeniculate feedback stabilizes response gain and increases information coding in Lgn neurons. J. Comput. Neurosci. 49, 259–271. doi: 10.1007/s10827-020-00754-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Murphy, P. C., and Sillito, A. M. (1987). Corticofugal feedback influences the generation of length tuning in the visual pathway. Nature 329, 727–729. doi: 10.1038/329727a0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Petersen, S. E., Robinson, D. L., and Keys, W. (1985). Pulvinar nuclei of the behaving rhesus monkey: visual responses and their modulation. J. Neurophysiol. 54, 867–886. doi: 10.1152/jn.1985.54.4.867

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Piche, M., Thomas, S., and Casanova, C. (2015). Spatiotemporal profiles of receptive fields of neurons in the lateral posterior nucleus of the cat Lp-pulvinar complex. J. Neurophysiol. 114, 2390–2403. doi: 10.1152/jn.00649.2015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Purushothaman, G., Marion, R., Li, K., and Casagrande, V. A. (2012). Gating and control of primary visual cortex by pulvinar. Nat. Neurosci. 15, 905–912. doi: 10.1038/nn.3106

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ren, C., and Tao, Q. (2020). Neural circuits underlying innate fear. Adv. Exp. Med. Biol. 1284, 1–7. doi: 10.1007/978-981-15-7086-5_1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rockland, K. S. (1996). Two types of corticopulvinar terminations: round (type 2) and elongate (type 1). J. Comp. Neurol. 368, 57–87. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1096-9861(19960422)368:1<57::AID-CNE5>3.0.CO;2-J

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rockland, K. S. (2019). Distinctive spatial and laminar organization of single axons from lateral pulvinar in the macaque. Vision 4:1. doi: 10.3390/vision4010001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roth, M. M., Dahmen, J. C., Muir, D. R., Imhof, F., Martini, F. J., and Hofer, S. B. (2016). Thalamic nuclei convey diverse contextual information to layer 1 of visual cortex. Nat. Neurosci. 19, 299–307. doi: 10.1038/nn.4197

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Saalmann, Y. B., Pinsk, M. A., Wang, L., Li, X., and Kastner, S. (2012). The pulvinar regulates information transmission between cortical areas based on attention demands. Science 337, 753–756. doi: 10.1126/science.1223082

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schneider, G. E. (1969). Two visual systems. Science 163, 895–902. doi: 10.1126/science.163.3870.895

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sherman, S. M. (2001). Tonic and burst firing: dual modes of thalamocortical relay. Trends Neurosci. 24, 122–126. doi: 10.1016/S0166-2236(00)01714-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sherman, S. M., and Guillery, R. W. (1998). On the actions that one nerve cell can have on another: distinguishing "drivers" from "modulators". Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 95, 7121–7126. doi: 10.1073/pnas.95.12.7121

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shipp, S. (2004). The brain circuitry of attention. Trends Cogn. Sci. 8, 223–230. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2004.03.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sillito, A. M., and Jones, H. E. (2002). Corticothalamic interactions in the transfer of visual information. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. Ser. B Biol. Sci. 357, 1739–1752. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2002.1170

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Soares, J. G., Diogo, A. C., Fiorani, M., Souza, A. P., and Gattass, R. (2004). Effects of inactivation of the lateral pulvinar on response properties of second visual area cells in Cebus monkeys. Clin. Exp. Pharmacol. Physiol. 31, 580–590. doi: 10.1111/j.1440-1681.2004.04051.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spacek, M. A., Crombie, D., Bauer, Y., Born, G., Liu, X., Katzner, S., et al. (2022). Robust effects of corticothalamic feedback and behavioral state on movie responses in mouse dlgn. eLife 11:e70469. doi: 10.7554/eLife.70469

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stitt, I., Zhou, Z. C., Radtke-Schuller, S., and Frohlich, F. (2018). Arousal dependent modulation of thalamo-cortical functional interaction. Nat. Commun. 9:2455. doi: 10.1038/s41467-018-04785-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tan, Z., and Yao, H. (2009). The spatiotemporal frequency tuning of Lgn receptive field facilitates neural discrimination of natural stimuli. J. Neurosci. 29, 11409–11416. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1268-09.2009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thompson, B., Villeneuve, M. Y., Casanova, C., and Hess, R. F. (2012). Abnormal cortical processing of pattern motion in amblyopia: evidence from FMRI. NeuroImage 60, 1307–1315. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.01.078

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Truccolo, W. A., and Dong, D. W. (2001). Dynamic temporal decorrelation: an information-theoretic and biophysical model of the functional role of the lateral geniculate nucleus. Neurocomputing 38-40, 993–1001. doi: 10.1016/S0925-2312(01)00440-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vanni, M. P., Thomas, S., Petry, H. M., Bickford, M. E., and Casanova, C. (2015). Spatiotemporal profile of voltage-sensitive dye responses in the visual cortex of tree shrews evoked by electric microstimulation of the dorsal lateral geniculate and pulvinar nuclei. J. Neurosci. 35, 11891–11896. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0717-15.2015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Villeneuve, M. Y., Kupers, R., Gjedde, A., Ptito, M., and Casanova, C. (2005). Pattern-motion selectivity in the human pulvinar. NeuroImage 28, 474–480. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.06.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Villeneuve, M. Y., Thompson, B., Hess, R. F., and Casanova, C. (2012). Pattern-motion selective responses in Mt, Mst and the pulvinar of humans. Eur. J. Neurosci. 36, 2849–2858. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2012.08205.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ward, R., Danziger, S., and Bamford, S. (2005). Response to visual threat following damage to the pulvinar. Curr. Biol. 15, 571–573. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2005.01.056

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ward, R., Danziger, S., Owen, V., and Rafal, R. (2002). Deficits in spatial coding and feature binding following damage to spatiotopic maps in the human pulvinar. Nat. Neurosci. 5, 99–100. doi: 10.1038/nn794

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Webb, B. S., Tinsley, C. J., Barraclough, N. E., Easton, A., Parker, A., and Derrington, A. M. (2002). Feedback from V1 and inhibition from beyond the classical receptive field modulates the responses of neurons in the primate lateral geniculate nucleus. Vis. Neurosci. 19, 583–592. doi: 10.1017/S0952523802195046

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Werner, J. S., and Chalupa, L. M. (2004). The visual neurosciences. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Google Scholar

Weyand, T. G. (2016). The multifunctional lateral geniculate nucleus. Rev. Neurosci. 27, 135–157. doi: 10.1515/revneuro-2015-0018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wrobel, A., Ghazaryan, A., Bekisz, M., Bogdan, W., and Kaminski, J. (2007). Two streams of attention-dependent beta activity in the striate recipient zone of cat's lateral posterior-pulvinar complex. J. Neurosci. 27, 2230–2240. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4004-06.2007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zabbah, S., Rajaei, K., Mirzaei, A., Ebrahimpour, R., and Khaligh-Razavi, S. M. (2014). The impact of the lateral geniculate nucleus and corticogeniculate interactions on efficient coding and higher-order visual object processing. Vis. Res. 101, 82–93. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2014.05.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhou, H., Schafer, R. J., and Desimone, R. (2016). Pulvinar-cortex interactions in vision and attention. Neuron 89, 209–220. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.11.034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: vision, transthalamic cortical pathways, receptive fields, lateral posterior nucleus, thalamus

Citation: Casanova C and Chalupa LM (2023) The dorsal lateral geniculate nucleus and the pulvinar as essential partners for visual cortical functions. Front. Neurosci. 17:1258393. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2023.1258393

Received: 13 July 2023; Accepted: 14 August 2023;
Published: 30 August 2023.

Edited by:

Benjamin Thompson, University of Waterloo, Canada

Reviewed by:

Vallabh Das, University of Houston, United States
W. Martin Usrey, University of California, Davis, United States

Copyright © 2023 Casanova and Chalupa. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Christian Casanova, christian.casanova@umontreal.ca

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.