- 1Department of Physiology and Cell Biology, Faculty of Health Sciences, Ben-Gurion University of the Negev, Beer Sheva, Israel
- 2Carl-Ludwig-Institute for Physiology, University of Leipzig, Leipzig, Germany
- 3Department of Neurogenetics, Max-Planck-Institute for Multidisciplinary Sciences, Göttingen, Germany
- 4Azrieli National Center for Autism and Neurodevelopment Research, Ben-Gurion University of the Negev, Beer Sheva, Israel
Introduction: The implications of folate deficiency in neuropsychiatric disorders were demonstrated in numerous studies. Genetic deficiency in a key folate metabolism enzyme, MTHFR, is an example of the interaction between genetic and environmental risk factors: the maternal MTHFR deficiency governs in-utero nutrient availability, and the embryo’s Mthfr genotype influences its ability to metabolize folates. Here, we explore how the maternal and offspring Mthfr genotypes affect cortical interneuron densities and distributions, mouse social outcome, and the relation of the different interneuron patterns to cortical excitability.
Methods: Two experiments were conducted to examine the effects of maternal and offspring Mthfr-KO heterozygosity. Mice were tested for direct social interactions (DSIs), repetitive behavior and cortical laminar distribution of interneuron populations expressing glutamate-decarboxylase-65, parvalbumin and somatostatin. Susceptibility to seizure was tested by exposure to pentylenetetrazole (PTZ).
Results: Maternal Mthfr+/− genotype was associated with suppressed social activities and reduced interneuron densities in all layers of the retrosplenial cortex (RSC). Somatostatin density and the somatostatin/parvalbumin ratio in the RSC and frontal cortex positively correlated with social behavior in the mice. An interaction between maternal and offspring Mthfr genotypes resulted in higher susceptibility of wild-type offspring to PTZ induced seizure.
Discussion: Maternal folate metabolism was shown to be critical to interneuron ontogenesis. Our results demonstrate that interneurons have a specific susceptibility to folate deficiency that may mediate folate’s involvement in neuropsychiatric disease. The relations between cortical somatostatin interneuron patterns and social behavior highlight this subpopulation of interneurons as a target for further research.
Introduction
The importance of folate metabolism for brain development and the implications of folate deficiency in neuropsychiatric disorders have been repeatedly demonstrated in a wide range of studies (Lewis et al., 2005; Muntjewerff et al., 2006; Roza et al., 2010; Levine et al., 2018; Zou et al., 2021). In the light of accumulating evidence supporting the beneficial effects of folic acid (FA) supplementation during pregnancy to prevent neural tube defects (MRC Vitamin Study Research Group, 1991; Wilson and O’Connor, 2021), FA fortification was recommended in 1992 by the CDC for woman of childbearing ages. Epidemiological studies have further supported FA’s potential to reduce the risks of giving birth to a baby that will be diagnosed later in life with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) (Levine et al., 2018). Moreover, an interaction between maternal FA intake and maternal polymorphism in a gene coding for key folate metabolism enzyme, methylene-tetrahydrofolate reductase (Mthfr, Mthfr677C > T, rs1801133), was reported to rescue the risk for neurodevelopmental outcome in the newborn (Schmidt et al., 2011, 2012; Pu et al., 2013; Rai, 2016).
Considering that maternal Mthfr genetics and FA intake contribute to the psychiatric outcome of the newborn via a single metabolic pathway, the Mthfr677C > T polymorphism constitutes an example of gene–environment interaction. In this case, the maternal polymorphism is an environmental risk factor that leads to an MTHFR-deficient intrauterine environment, and the presence of the polymorphism in offspring is a genetic risk factor. The maternal MTHFR deficiency not only reduces the availability of various metabolites in the intrauterine environment, it also dictates placental gene expression (Luan et al., 2021). The significance of the FA metabolic pathway to brain development is further supported by the interactions with other genetic variations (Mthfr1289A > C, rs1801131), FA dosage and the presence of folate receptor autoantibodies (Hoxha et al., 2021).
The folate cycle coupled with the methionine cycle is referred to as one carbon metabolism. Indications that one carbon metabolism function was disturbed due to the Mthfr677C > T polymorphism in humans or the Mthfr haploinsufficiency (Mthfr+/− genotype) in mice were previously shown (Chew et al., 2011; Yan et al., 2011). Moreover, the effects of maternal and offspring Mthfr+/− genotype on methionine, betaine and choline levels in the cerebral cortex and basal ganglia were associated with mice behavior (Orenbuch et al., 2019).
Dysregulation of the excitation-inhibition balance in brain circuits was suggested to be an underlying mechanism for autism and schizophrenia (Marenco and Weinberger, 2000; Cotter et al., 2002; Lewis and Hashimoto, 2007; Charych et al., 2009; Taylor and Tso, 2015; Buckley and Holmes, 2016; Frye et al., 2016; Jacob, 2016; Kumar et al., 2021). A weak inhibitory tone in ASD is supported by vast evidence of the differences that have been observed in the inhibitory cells, proteins and gene expression patterns in the postmortem brains of ASD patients (Fatemi et al., 2002, 2014; Oblak et al., 2010; Gaetz et al., 2014; Hashemi et al., 2016; Robertson et al., 2016; Hong et al., 2020; Satterstrom et al., 2020; Amina et al., 2021) and in animal models of ASD (Eagleson et al., 2010; Jedlicka et al., 2011; Han et al., 2012; Wohr et al., 2015; Filice et al., 2016). Moreover, it appears that several of the genes associated with ASD, such as Scn1A, Shank1 and Dlx1/2, are expressed preferentially in inhibitory interneurons during an early developmental window (Cobos et al., 2005; Han et al., 2012; Mao et al., 2015). The largest classes of cortical interneurons are characterized by the expression of parvalbumin (PV) and somatostatin (SST), which differ in their laminar and targets of connectivity (Pfeffer et al., 2013; Tremblay et al., 2016). PV and SST interneurons play important roles in the initiation and maintenance of cortical beta-gamma band oscillations in the resting state and upon sensory activation (Kato et al., 2015; Hayden et al., 2021), wherein they exhibit a layer specific response (Kuki et al., 2015). Proper interneuron incorporation in the cortical circuits is a fundamental prerequisite for orchestrated function.
Our previous studies linked the mouse Mthfr+/− genotype to ASD-like behavior and low levels of GABA pathway proteins in the cerebral cortex (Sadigurschi and Golan, 2018; Orenbuch et al., 2019). Here, we broaden the examination of the maternal and offspring Mthfr genotypes, and the association between ASD-like behavior and the density and laminar distribution of cortical interneurons.
Materials and methods
Mouse colonies
Two mouse colonies were raised: heterozygous Mthfr-KO mice (Mthfr+/−) on a Balb/cAnNCrlBR background (Chen et al., 2001), and GAD65-tdTomato: Transgene GAD65-tdTomato mice on a C57/Bl6 BAC background (Besser et al., 2015). The colonies were maintained on a 12:12 h light/dark schedule, and food and water were provided ad libitum. All procedures were performed according to the guidelines of the Israeli Council on Animal Care and approved by the Animal Care and Use Committee of Ben-Gurion University of the Negev (protocols IL-16-07-14 and IL 10–03-18D).
Study design
Experiment 1
To assess the effects of the maternal Mthfr+/− genotype vs. the offspring genotype, the tested mice comprised the first generation (F1) produced by Mthfr+/− (HT) and Mthfr+/+ (WT) females with GAD65-tdTomato males (WT). Three groups are represented by the genotype of maternal:paternal:offspring: (group 1) WT offspring of WT parents (WT:WT:WT, N = 6), (group 2) WT offspring from pairs of HT dams and WT males (HT:WT:WT, N = 8), and (group 3) HT offspring produced by HT dams and WT males (HT:WT:HT, N = 8).
Experiment 2
To exclude the in-utero contribution of the Mthfr+/− genotype to the phenotype, we mated GAD65-tdTomato (WT) females with Mthfr+/− males to create two additional groups – (group 4) WT offspring of WT dams and HT males (WT:HT:WT, N = 10), and (group 5) HT offspring of WT dams and HT males (WT:HT:HT, N = 10). During pup lactation, cages were observed and adverse or neglect maternal care was excluded. Tests were performed on 3-month-old mice, not more than 2 offspring from each litter/group were included.
We generated an additional batch of similar groups for the seizure susceptibility test. Mice were tested beginning on postnatal day 90, as shown in Figure 1. We previously showed that the maternal and offspring Mthfr+/− genotypes were associated with an ASD-like phenotype in male mice, and minor effect in female mice (Sadigurschi and Golan, 2018). In the current study, therefore, we focused on male mice.
Figure 1. Experimental design and timeline of experiments. Two batches of mice were used for the study, presented in blue and green. P, Postnatal day; DSI, Direct social interaction; PTZ, pentylenetetrazol.
Genotyping
Mothers and offspring were genotyped as described previously by using polymerase chain reaction amplification of DNA isolated from toe clips. The primers used were as follows: Mthfr: sense primer 1 (5′-GAAGCAGAGGGAAGGAGGCTTCAG-3′) in exon 3, sense primer 2 (5′-AGCCTGAAGAACGAGATCAGCAG C-3′) in the neor gene, and antisense primer 3 (5′-GACTA GCTGGCTATCCTCTCATCC-3′) in intron 3. GAD65-tdTomato: Sense primer 1 (5′-GTGCAGGGTCGAGGCAAAGGCA-3′), and antisense primer 2 (5′-GGACAGGATGTCCCAGGCGAAG-3′) (Chen et al., 2001; Besser et al., 2015).
Behavioral assessments
Nest building
Animal welfare and repetitive behavior were assessed based on several nest features (Gaskill et al., 2013; Sadigurschi and Golan, 2018). Prior to testing, mice were separated and placed in individual cages. Nest material comprised tissue paper folded into a fixed size of 5 × 7 cm and placed in each cage. The size and quality of the nests were measured 24 h after the insertion of the bedding. Scores (0–3) were given for material processing, centralization and symmetry. The volume of the nest was calculated by length × width × height.
Marble burying test
Repetitive behavior was additionally evaluated by recording the number of buried (“hidden”) marbles (Thomas et al., 2009). Each mouse was placed in a cage with 15 marbles organized in 5 × 3 rows for a duration of 10 min. At the end of the test the mouse was removed from the cage. Each cage was photographed before mouse introduction into it and after mouse removal from it. The number of buried marbles was counted.
Direct social interaction test
To measure sociability, two mice from the same group were inserted into a clean cage where their behavior was then videotaped for 15 min. The following behaviors were measured offline (duration and number of occurrences) by a blinded experimenter as previously described (Defensor et al., 2011; Sadigurschi and Golan, 2018) with some modification to the types of interactions that were tested:
Sniffing: Different aspects of sniffing of the companion mouse were quantified:
Nose tip-to-nose tip: Subject’s nose tip and/or vibrissae contacts the nose tip and/or vibrissae of the other mouse.
Nose-to-head: Subject’s nose or vibrissae contacts the dorsal, lateral, or ventral surface of other mouse’s head.
Nose-to-anogenital: Subject’s nose or vibrissae contacts the base of the tail or anus of the other mouse.
Body Sniffing: Subject’s nose tip and/or vibrissae contacts body areas not specified before.
In addition, play and grooming were quantified by the following:
Whisker trimming: Representative of grooming and dominant behavior.
Crawl Over: Subject’s forelimbs cross the midline of the dorsal surface of the other mouse.
Crawl Under: Subject’s head goes under the ventral surface of the other mouse to a depth at which at least the ears of the subject animal cross the midline of the other mouse’s body.
Because the crawl over and crawl under behaviors are fast events, only the number of events was analyzed.
All sniffing and play aspects were considered Non-Aggressive behaviors.
Aggressive behaviors were quantified by:
Attack: Initiation of aggressive behavior.
Fight: Participation in an aggressive event during which the two mice tussle violently and the observer is unable to distinguish the subject mouse from the other mouse.
Aggressive behavior: Sum of attacks and fighting events.
Behavioral phenotype
Mice were phenotyped based on how social and repetitive behaviors manifested themselves in the animals.
We used the Direct social interaction (DSI) test to categorize mouse behavior as either (a) “Social” or (b) “Non-social” in mice that engaged in sniffing behavior for longer or shorter times, respectively, than the median duration for sniffing behavior.
We evaluated repetitive behavior with the marble burying test. Mouse behavior was classified as either (a) “Non repetitive” or (b) “Repetitive” in mice that hid fewer or more marbles, respectively, than the median number of marbles hidden.
The categories thus determined for mouse social and repetitive behaviors were used to define “ASD-like” behavior (i.e., mice that presented sniffing times of longer durations than the median and that hid larger numbers of marbles than the median) and “non-ASD-like” behavior (i.e., mice that presented sniffing times of shorter durations than the median and that hid smaller numbers of marbles than the median). In addition, we defined a third group of “partially symptomatic” mice (i.e., sniffing times of shorter durations than the median or numbers of marbles hidden larger than the median).
Immuno-fluorescence analysis of brain tissue
Mice interaction with the experimenter and the participation in the DSI test may induce a stress response, which could include gene regulation, glia activation and more. To avoid the effects of these factors on the variables measured in brain tissue, after the completion of the DSI test, we allowed the mice to spend a week in their home cage basal conditions before sampling brain tissue. Mice were anesthetized by inhalation of 30% isoflurane (Minrad Inc., NY, United States) diluted in isopropanol (Fluka Chemie GmbH, Buchs, Netherlands), after which they were transcardially perfused with paraformaldehyde (PFA) 4%. Brains were rapidly removed into a 4% PFA solution and stored at 4°C overnight. Each brain was then washed in phosphate buffer and transferred to a 10% sucrose solution for 2 h or until it sank to the bottom of the solution. Afterwards, brains were transferred to a 30% sucrose solution for 24 h at 4°C and then immersed in OCT compound embedding matrix (Tissue-Tek #4583) inside cryo standard plastic vinyl disposable molds (25 × 20 × 5 mm) placed on dry ice and stored at −80°C. The brains were sliced into 10-μm thick sagittal sections between the bregma lateral plane, 0.24–0.36 mm. Neurons were examined by using the following primary antibodies: Polyclonal guinea pig anti-NeuN (Millipore, 1:400), Monoclonal mouse anti-PV (1:3,000, Sigma-Aldrich, Cat # P 3088), Monoclonal mouse anti-SST (Santa Cruz Biotechnology, 1:250), and Polyclonal rabbit anti-RFP (1:500, Rockland, Cat# 600–401-379) was used to detect the tdTomato expression that occurred under the regulation of the GAD65 promoter, and therefore, it marked all interneurons. In addition, Monoclonal mouse anti-NKCC1 [(T4), 1:500, DSHB] and Monoclonal mouse anti-Gephyrin (Synaptic system, 1:1,000). The following were used as secondary antibodies: Goat anti-rabbit Cy3 (1,600, Chemicon, Cat # AP-124C), Donkey anti-mouse Alexa488 and Donkey anti-guinea pig Alexa488 (Jackson Immuno Research, 1:400). Images were taken using a PD73 Olympus CCD attached to an IX-70 Olympus fluorescence microscope and then collected by CellSense software (Olympus) at a × 20 magnification. Images of the frontal cortex (FC), retrosplenial cortex (RSC) and hippocampus regions were captured from each brain section and examined (see Supplementary Figure S1). The analysis was performed using NIH image J. software by an experimenter blind to the tissue identity. For the analysis, cortical images from the pia to the ventricle were combined and divided into 10 bins of equal sizes for cell density and fluorescence optical density analyses. In each bin, the number of cells and the fluorescence optical density were analyzed to calculate cell density and innervation, respectively. Cortical layers (L) were defined as follows: L1 (bin 1), L2 (bins 2–3), L3 (bins 4–6), L4 (bin 7) L5 (bin 8), L6 (bins 9–10). Hippocampal images were divided by fields and cellular region as follows: CA1 and CA3 were divided into the stratum oriens (SO), stratum pyramidale (SP) and stratum radiatum (SR), and the dentate gyrus (DG) was divided into the hillus and stratum granulare (SC) and stratum moleculare (SM).
Seizure induction by PTZ
Mice were transferred into new individual cages and injected with 50 mg/Kg of the GABA blocker pentylenetetrazole (PTZ, Sigma-Aldrich, CAS 54–95-5) diluted in saline. Mice were observed for the following 20 min, and the seizure intensities were recorded according to a five-score scale described by Powell et al. (2003).
• 1- Ear and facial twitching.
• 2- One or more myoclonic twitches of the whole body.
• 3- A weak to moderate generalized clonic seizure without loss of righting reflexes.
• 4- Generalized clonic convulsions with rearing and falling down episodes.
• 5- Clonic seizures with loss of righting reflexes followed by tonic hindlimb extension.
• The highest score during each minute was recorded.
Statistical analysis
Statistical analysis was performed using SPSS 26 software. Univariate two-way ANOVA analysis was used to test the effects of the independent factors, genotype and maternal genotype, and ANOVA for repeated measurement was used when applicable. A two-tailed Student’s t-test was used to analyze the effect of genotype in experiment 2. Correlation between cell density and behavior was tested by Spearman’s correlation test. Differences with p-values <0.05 were regarded as significant. Data are presented as means ± SEM.
Results
ASD-like behavior in adult and pup mice was associated with maternal and offspring Mthfr+/− genotypes (Sadigurschi and Golan, 2018; Orenbuch et al., 2019; Agam et al., 2020; Shekel et al., 2021). Since previous studies have been done with the balb/c strain, which is known for low sociability, here we crossed these mice with the C57/Bl6 strain, which is known for its sociability, to generate the experimental groups. In addition, to isolate the effect of offspring genotype, WT and HT offspring of WT dams were tested (experiment 2).
Maternal Mthfr+/− genotype is associated with enhanced ASD-like behavior in the offspring
Sociability: The total duration of sniffing was suppressed by the maternal Mthfr+/− genotype from 62.6 ± 31.2 s in the wt:wt:wt group to 32.6 ± 16.7 s and 42.8 ± 21.1 s in the offspring of Mthfr+/− dams (ht:wt:wt and ht.:wt:ht., respectively, F1,18 = 26.5, p < 0.001) as shown in Figures 2A,B and Table 1. The duration of all non-aggressive behaviors in the wt:wt:wt groups, 81 ± 21.8 s, was longer than those of Mthfr+/− dam offspring in the ht.:wt:wt and ht.:wt:ht. groups, 35.3 ± 16.5 s and 55.6 ± 32.5 s, respectively (F1,20 = 10.2, p = 0.005). The similar number of non-aggressive events observed in the latter two groups led us to conclude that the duration of each non-aggressive event was shorter in the affected mice. Whisker trimming, representing a dominant behavior in the mice was differentially affected by the maternal and offspring genotypes, such that it was elevated by the maternal and suppressed by the offspring Mthfr+/− genotype (F1,21 = 5.5, p = 0.03, F1,21 = 8.46, p = 009, respectively Figures 2C,D).
Figure 2. ASD-like behaviors observed in adult mice. Sociability was evaluated by the “DSI” test (A,B). The duration of “Sniffing” behavior was decreased by maternal Mthfr+/− genotype in the total 15 min of the test (F1,18 = 26.54, p < 0.001, two-way ANOVA) (A) and when analyzing the cumulative time (F1,16 = 20.15, p = 0.005, ANOVA for repeated measures) (B). (C) The number of “Whisker-trimming” events was increased by maternal Mthfr+/− genotype and decreased by offspring Mthfr+/− genotype (F1,21 = 5.5, p = 0.03, F1,21 = 8.46, p = 0.009, respectively, two-way ANOVA), and when analyzing the cumulative number of events (F1,19 = 6.09, p = 0.023, F1,19 = 20.15, p < 0.001, respectively, GLM for repeated measures) (D). Repetitive behavior was assessed by the “Nest building” (E) and the “Marble burying” tests (F). Nest symmetry was increased by maternal Mthfr+/− genotype and decreased by offspring Mthfr+/− genotype (F1,13 = 9.24, p = 0.011, F1,13 = 9.24, p = 0.01, respectively, two-way ANOVA). Nest centralization was increased by maternal genotype (F1,13 = 9.8, p = 0.01). Data are presented as means ± SEM. For the Sociability test, the N for groups 1, 2, & 3 = 6, 8, & 8, respectively. For the repetitive behavior tests, N = 6 per group. A one-way ANOVA with a Bonferroni post-hoc test *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.005 between wt:wt:wt and ht.:wt:wt, #p < 0.05, ##p < 0.01 between wt:wt:wt and ht.:wt:ht., $p < 0.05 between ht.:wt:wt and ht.:wt:ht. When averaged data are shown, bars represent SEM.
Processing of nesting material as representative of repetitive/rigid behavior highlighted the effect of maternal genotype. Nest symmetry was increased by the maternal Mthfr+/− genotype and decreased by the offspring Mthfr+/− genotype (F1,13 = 9.24, p = 0.011, F1,13 = 9.25, p = 0.01, respectively), and nest centralization was increased by maternal genotype (F1,13 = 9.8, p = 0.01, Figure 2E). Similar performances were observed in all of the groups in the marble burying test (Figure 2F).
Similar behavioral analyses of the Mthfr+/+ and Mthfr+/− offspring of Mthfr+/+ dams did not show any effect of offspring genotype on adult mouse behavior (see Supplementary Figure S2 and Table 1), a finding that emphasizes the significance of the in-utero environment.
Taken together, the findings show that the maternal Mthfr+/− genotype had significant effects on social and repetitive behaviors in the F1 offspring.
Maternal Mthfr+/− genotype is associated with cortical interneuron defect
To explore the possible association of the above-described behaviors with interneuron dysregulation, the brains of the mice tested above were analyzed. The expression of GAD65-tdTomato in interneurons in the RSC region, and colocalization of GAD65 and NeuN staining are shown in Figures 3A–D. Comparisons of the laminar distributions of GAD65-tdTomato interneurons sampled from the RSC and FC regions showed that their respective distribution patterns differed in these brain regions (Figures 3E,F). GAD65-tdTomato interneuron density across the entire RSC, was suppressed by the maternal Mthfr+/− genotype (F1,12 = 15.84, p = 0.003), and the effect was proportional in all cortical layers (F1,10 = 36.4, p < 0.001, ANOVA for repeated measures) (Figures 3E,G). The maternal Mthfr+/− genotype had strong effects in layers 3 and 5 (F1,12 = 7.83, p = 0.019, F1,12 = 16.74, p = 0.002, respectively). In the FC, GAD65-tdTomato interneuron densities in layers 4 and 5 were non-significantly higher in the offspring of Mthfr+/− dams compared to that in the wt:wt:wt group, but all groups exhibited similar total densities (Figures 3F,H). Lastly, the effects of the maternal and offspring genotypes on interneuron density in the hippocampus, restricted to the CA1 and DG regions, are shown in Table 2 and Supplementary Figure S3. In contrast to the interneurons, NeuN+ cells were found in similar densities in both the RSC and FC regions and in all the groups (Figure 3I). Thus, cortical interneuron density was affected mainly by the maternal Mthfr genotype in a region-specific manner.
Figure 3. Maternal Mthfr+/− genotype is associated with interneuron deficiency in mice RSC. (A–C) Images of/ GAD65-tdTomato interneurons in the RSC of each experimental group, from top – pia, to bottom – ventricle. (D) An image of GAD65-tdTomato (red) and NeuN+ neurons (green) in the RSC. (E,G) GAD65-tdTomato interneuron density was reduced by the maternal Mthfr+/− genotype in the RSC (F1,12 = 15.84, p = 0.003, two-way ANOVA). The decrease in cell density was observed in all layers, most prominently in layers 3 and 5 (F1,10 = 36. 4, p < 0.001, ANOVA for repeated measures, F1,12 = 7.83, p = 0.019, F1,12 = 16.74, p = 0.002 two-way ANOVA, respectively). (F,H) A similar analysis of GAD65-tdTomato in the FC revealed no difference between groups. (I) NeuN cell density in the RSC. Data are presented as means ± SEM, N of group 1 = 4, of group 2 = 5, of group 3 = 5. One-way ANOVA with a Bonferroni post-hoc test *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01 between wt:wt:wt and ht.:wt:wt, ##p < 0.01 between wt:wt:wt and ht.:wt:ht.
Effects of Mthfr+/− genotype on PV and SST interneurons
Two major populations of interneurons in the cortex, PV and SST, are associated with a variety of external and internal input processing tasks that are performed by the cerebral cortex. Examples of PV and SST interneuron immunostaining in the RSC of all experimental groups are shown in Figures 4A,B. The maternal Mthfr+/− genotype was associated with an increase in total PV interneuron density (F1,13 = 7.19, p = 0.021, Figure 4C) that was also observed throughout the layers of the RSC (F1,11 = 5.32, p = 0.042, ANOVA for repeated measures), where a stronger effect was found in layer 6 (F1,13 = 6.8, p = 0.024, Figure 4D). In the FC, in contrast, these effects were not observed (Supplementary Figures S4A-D). In the DG region of the hippocampus, the maternal Mthfr+/− genotype was associated with a greater than 50% decrease in the density of PV (F1,13 = 6.11, p = 0.031, see Table 2).
Figure 4. Effects of Mthfr+/− genotype on cortical PV and SST interneurons. (A,B) An example of PV and SST interneurons in Layer 3 of the RSC in the different experimental groups. (C,D) PV+ cell density was increased by maternal Mthfr+/− genotype (F1,13 = 7.19, p = 0.021, two-way ANOVA). (D) The alteration in PV+ cell density was seen throughout the entire cortical depth, with the most prominent change seen in L6 (F1,11 = 5.32, p = 0.042, ANOVA for repeated measures, F1,13 = 6.8, p = 0.024, two-way ANOVA, respectively). (E) SST+ cell density in the RSC. (F) When dividing the cortex into layers, offspring Mthfr+/− genotype increased and maternal Mthfr+/− genotype decreased SST+ cell density in layer 3 (F1,12 = 7.09, p = 0.024 and F1,12 = 7.79, p = 0.019, two-way ANOVA, respectively). (G) Cortical PV/STT ratio in the RSC. (H) Separate analyses of each layer showed that maternal Mthfr+/− genotype increased the PV/SST ratio in layer 3 (F1,10 = 6.14, p = 0.038, two-way ANOVA). (I) Average PV+ cell body cross sectional area (J) PV+ cell body cross sectional area was increased by the maternal Mthfr+/− genotype in layer 2 of the RSC (F1,16 = 4.82, p = 0.045, two-way ANOVA). (K) Average SST+ cell body cross sectional area. (L) SST+ cell body cross sectional area was increased by offspring Mthfr+/− genotype in layers 4 and 5 of the RSC (F1,13 = 11.66, p = 0.006 and F1,13 = 6.96, p = 0.023, two-way ANOVA respectively). Data are presented as means ± SEM. N = 6 groups 1, 2, and 3, respectively. One-way ANOVA with a Bonferroni post-hoc test *p < 0.05 between wt:wt:wt and ht.:wt:wt. #p < 0.05 between wt:wt:wt and ht:wt:ht., ##p < 0.01 between wt:wt:wt and ht.:wt:ht., $p < 0.05 between ht.:wt:wt and ht.:wt:ht.
Total SST interneuron density under the maternal Mthfr+/− genotype tended overall to decrease by maternal Mthfr+/− genotype (F1,12 = 3.84, p = 0.079), but contrasting layer specific effects were observed: the offspring Mthfr+/− genotype increased SST densities in layers 1 and 3 (F1,12 = 5.76, p = 0.037 and F1,12 = 7.09, p = 0.024, respectively), and the maternal Mthfr+/− genotype decreased them in cortical layer 3 compared to the offspring of Mthfr+/+ dams (F1,12 = 7.79, p = 0.019, Figures 4E,F). Consistent with this finding, SST interneuron density in the FC was reduced by the maternal Mthfr+/− genotype relative to the Mthfr+/+ genotype in layer 3 (F1,12 = 7.45, p = 0.021, Supplementary Figures S4E,F). The analysis of SST interneuron densities in the hippocampus is presented in Table 2.
The PV/SST ratio represents the functional balance between the two major interneuron populations in the cerebral cortex, each of which is active in a different functional compartment of the glutamatergic projection neurons. A higher PV/SST ratio in layer 3 was associated with the maternal Mthfr+/− genotype (F1,10 = 6.14, p = 0.038, see Figures 4G,H).
Cellular volume is affected by physiological and pathophysiological processes. To estimate the effects of the Mthfr genotype on interneuron volumes, we measured the cross-sectional areas of PV and SST interneurons in the RSC, the brain regions in which our analyses revealed the strongest effects on interneuron densities. The maternal Mthfr+/− genotype was associated with increased soma cross sectional area in PV interneurons in layer 2 (F1,16 = 4.82, p = 0.045) compared to Mthfr+/+ offspring (Figures 4I,J). The offspring Mthfr+/− genotype was associated with increased soma cross sectional area in SST interneurons in deeper layers, layers 4 and 5, of the RSC (F1,13 = 11.66, p = 0.006 and F1,13 = 6.96, p = 0.023, respectively), suggesting that the SST interneurons in these layers swelled, see Figures 4K,L.
Similar analyses in the Mthfr+/+ and Mthfr+/− offspring of Mthfr+/+ dams did not expose any effect of offspring genotype on interneuron sub-populations in the RSC, FC and limited effect on PV density in the CA1 region of the hippocampus (Supplementary Figure S5; Supplementary Table S3). Thus, the effect of offspring genotype could be detected when interacting with deficient maternal in utero environment.
ASD-like phenotype and cortical interneurons
Evaluation of the correlation between the observed phenotype and interneuron appearance found positive correlations (Table 3) between total SST interneuron density in the RSC and sniffing behavior (R = 0.502, p < 0.01) and total neuron density in the RSC and repetitive behavior (R = 0.528, p < 0.04). The correlation between SST interneuron density and behavior was more prominent in the FC, where we found a positive correlation with sniffing and a negative correlation with repetitive behavior (R = 0.666, p = 0.001 and R = −0.456, p = 0.03, respectively, Figures 5A–D, see also Supplementary Figure S6). We also evaluated whether the PV/SST ratio correlates with behavioral phenotype. A stronger correlation was observed in the FC, the PV/SST ratio negatively correlated with sniffing (R = −0.7) and positively correlated with repetitive behavior (R = 0.4, see Table 3).
Figure 5. ASD-like phenotype and cortical interneurons. Relations between sniffing duration and number of hidden marbles with SST and PV interneuron densities in the RSC (A,B) and in the FC (C,D). (E) Mice phenotypes – mice were grouped by behavior as described in the methods. (F–J) Neuron densities in the RSC of the “Non-social” compared to the “Social” group: (F) SST+ cell density. (G) PV+ cell density. (H) Cortical cell densities of GAD65-tdTomato interneurons. (I) Density of the entire neuron population. (J) When analyzing each cortical layer separately, the decrease in SST+ cell density was observed in layers 4–6 of the RSC of the “Non-social” compared to the “Social” group (K,L) PV+/SST+ ratio in FC. (K) An increase in PV/SST ratio in layer 3 of the “Non-social” compared to the “Social” group. (L) An increase in PV+/SST+ ratio in layer 3 of the “Repetitive” compared to the “Non-repetitive” group. Data are presented as means ± SEM, N = 13 per group. *p < 0.05 student t-test between “Social” and “Non-social” groups, between “Non-ASD-like” and “ASD-like” and between “Non-repetitive” and “Repetitive” groups.
To assess whether a particular behavioral phenotype was enriched, all of the experimental mice were categorized by their social and repetitive behaviors. Thirty three percent of the mice presented an ASD-like phenotype, exhibiting both impaired social behavior and increased repetitive behavior, while about 40% of the mice presented only one of these behavioral features (see Figure 5E). The correlation of SST interneuron appearance in the RSC with social behavior was also obtained by the lower SST densities observed in non-social compared to social mice, while PV, GAD65-tdTomato, and NeuN neuron densities did not differ in social vs. non-social mice (Figures 5F–J).
To assess whether interneuron densities in a particular layer of the RSC and FC regions contributed to the observed behaviors, the correlation was calculated by cortical layer (Table 4). SST densities in layers 3 and below and layers 2 and below in the RSC and FC regions, respectively, positively correlated with sniffing behavior. Moreover, the negative correlation of PV/SST ratio with sniffing was observed in most cortical layers, while the positive correlation with repetitive behavior was limited mainly to the contribution of FC layer 3 (Table 4 and Figures 5K,L).
The use of correlation analysis to evaluate the link between behavioral phenotype and interneuron distribution highlighted the contribution to the endophenotype made by FC interneuron organization.
Hints of the synaptic contribution to behavioral phenotype
The inhibitory tone in general depends on interneuron density, distribution and synaptic connectivity. Evaluation of the immunoreactivity of gephyrin as a marker for GABAergic postsynaptic clusters found similar levels of the protein in the RSC of all groups (Figures 6A,B). We then examined the levels of a sodium-potassium-chloride transporter, NKCC1, which helps control the intracellular chloride equilibrium. The contribution of NKCC1 to normal neurological function is exemplified by the positive effect of its antagonist, bumetanide, in neurological conditions (Löscher and Kaila, 2022). Layer specific analysis revealed that the maternal Mthfr+/− genotype was associated with enhanced NKCC1 levels in layer 2 of the RSC (Figures 6C,D), while NKCC1 levels in layer 1 negatively correlated with repetitive behavior (R = −0.67, Table 5). Evaluation by behavioral phenotype exposed a negative correlation between gephyrin levels in RSC layers 5 and 6 and social behavior (R = −0.69 and R = −0.54, respectively, Table 5), associating inhibitory synapses that populate the deep cortical layers with social phenotype.
Figure 6. Effects of Mthfr genotype on mice susceptibility to seizure and molecular markers. (A) Example of gephyrin and GAD65-tdTomato in layer 3 of the RSC. (B) Gephyrin density in the RSC cortex. (C) Example of NKCC1 and GAD65-tdTomato in Layer 3 of the RSC. (D) NKCC1 density was increased by the maternal Mthfr+/− genotype in layer 2 of the RSC (F1,16 = 5.45, p = 0.035, two-way ANOVA). (E,F) Recordings of seizure scores for each minute after PTZ injection. (E) Cumulative seizure score for each minute of the 20-min test was enhanced by the maternal Mthfr+/− genotype and reduced by the offspring Mthfr+/− genotype (F1,55 = 6.06, p = 0.017, F1,55 = 8.99, p = 0.004, respectively, ANOVA for repeated measurements). N = 6 per group. One-way ANOVA with a Bonferroni post-hoc test #p < 0.05 between wt:wt:wt and ht.:wt:het. (F) Number of seizure events. The number of minutes during which the mouse’s number of seizure events was in the range of 2–5. The number of seizure events was increased by maternal Mthfr+/− genotype and decreased by offspring Mthfr+/− genotype (F1,58 = 5.47, p = 0.023, F1,58 = 4.96, p = 0.03, respectively, two-way ANOVA). The sum of the score for seizure events was also increased by maternal genotype (F1,58 = 4.71, p = 0.034, two-way ANOVA). Data are presented as means ± SEM. One way ANOVA with Bonferroni post-hoc test *p < 0.05 between wt:wt:wt and ht.:wt:wt. N: wt:wt:wt = 21, ht.:wt:wt = 12, ht.:wt:ht. = 12.
Effects of Mthfr genotype on mice susceptibility to seizure
Interneuron defects are a major cause of epilepsy, a neurological disorder with high comorbidity in ASD (Bozzi et al., 2018). We evaluated whether an Mthfr deficiency altered the susceptibility of mice to a PTZ induced seizure. The cumulative seizure score for each minute of the 20-min test is presented in Figure 6C. Higher seizure scores were induced by the maternal Mthfr+/− genotype and lower scores were induced by the offspring Mthfr+/− genotype (F1,55 = 6.06, p = 0.017, F1,55 = 8.99, p = 0.004, respectively, ANOVA for repeated measurements). Seizure recurrence (number of seizure events, score 2–5) was increased by the maternal Mthfr+/− genotype and decreased by the offspring Mthfr+/− genotype (F1,58 = 5.47, p = 0.023, F1,58 = 4.96, p = 0.03, respectively). The sum of the score for seizure events was also increased by the maternal Mthfr+/− genotype (F1,58 = 4.71, p = 0.034, Figure 6D). Similar analyses in Mthfr+/+ and Mthfr+/− offspring to Mthfr+/+ dams did not show any effect of offspring genotype on the susceptibility to PTZ induced seizure (Supplementary Figure S7).
Discussion
Maternal deficiency in MTHFR, a key regulatory enzyme of the folate cycle, caused a robust interneuron deficiency in the RSC that was reflected to lesser extents in two of its input sources. The maternal Mthfr+/− genotype was the main factor that affected interneuron quantity and laminar distribution, i.e., the total interneuron population and its largest sub-types, PV and SST. Furthermore, major domains of ASD-like behavior significantly correlated with SST interneuron distributions and PV/SST ratios in the RSC and FC. The functional relevance of interneuron defects to brain excitability was shown by an elevated response to the convulsant agent, PTZ.
Attempts to identify the genetic origin of a neurodevelopmental disorder typically focus on the genotype of the affected subject. Interactions between a genetic factor and the environment are more difficult to dissect. The maternal Mthfr genetic deficiency constitutes an example of a gene–environment interaction that results in modification of the in-utero environment and subsequent nutrient availability for the developing fetus by an impaired gene. The separate contributions of the offspring and maternal genotypes and that of the interaction between the two profoundly affect developmental trajectories in the brain and result in a variable spectrum of phenotypes. In humans, the maternal homozygous Mthfr677TT polymorphism was shown to increase the risk for ASD in the child (James et al., 2006; Goin-Kochel et al., 2009; Mohammad et al., 2009; Liu et al., 2011; Schmidt et al., 2011; Guo et al., 2012; Pu et al., 2013; Rai, 2016) and to interact with the child’s genotype (i.e., its ability to metabolize FA). However, the differential impact of maternal genotype on the risk for the Mthfr677CC newborn cannot be explored. In the current study, the Mthfr+/− genotype alone in the offspring of WT mothers (as tested in experiment 2) was not sufficient to modulate the tested behaviors in offspring, and it had only a minor effect on cortical interneuron lamination and susceptibility to PTZ induced seizure. As one may expect, not all brain circuits are affected similarly by maternal Mthfr+/− and its interaction with offspring genotype. Similar interaction was observed in the susceptibility to seizure, whisker trimming and nesting behavior of offspring, among which the WT offspring exhibited the strongest effect, suggesting the presence of compensatory mechanisms in the Mthfr+/− offspring. Supporting data for the notion of cell- or circuit-specific interactions between offspring and maternal genotype comprising PV quantity and the layer 3 PV/SST ratio were also collected. Considering the regulatory nature of the MTHFR enzyme, with multiple regulatory sites by other enzymes or products of the biochemical pathway, i.e., 5- methyl-tetrahydrofolate, S-adenosyl methionine and S-adenosylhomocysteine (Froese et al., 2018), it is possible that the response to the in-utero deficiency in the Mthfr+/− offspring differs from that of its WT littermate.
The reduced RSC interneuron densities observed in the offspring of Mthfr+/− mothers cannot be attributed solely to changes in either the PV or SST interneuron population, which undergoes more restricted change. The lower number of interneurons may be compensated for by an excessive inhibitory innervation, a possibility that was partially excluded by the restricted effects observed for gephyrin and by NKCC1 cortical density.
PV and SST interneurons differently contribute to the processing of sensory, cognitive and emotional functions (Pfeffer et al., 2013). Exerting differential and concerted regulation on cortical beta and gamma oscillations (Kato et al., 2015; Kuki et al., 2015; Chen et al., 2017; Montgomery et al., 2022), they are inversely active in response to novel vs. familiar visual stimuli (Hayden et al., 2021). The observed changes in interneuron densities and proportions in the offspring of Mthfr+/− dams may therefore engender modifications in mice abilities to process audio-visual stimuli during social encounters that lead to social deficits. Providing support for this notion, PV or SST ablation was sufficient to cause social and cognitive impairment, with a stronger effect obtained by SST ablation (Perez et al., 2019). Moreover, an altered PV/SST ratio was associated with poor responses to social stimuli in the Pten deficient mice model (Vogt et al., 2015; Shin et al., 2021). In addition to the quantitative changes, SST interneuron swelling in deep cortical layers may represent energetic failure (Brisson and Andrew, 2012) and/or a volumetric response to changes in cortical fluid ionic strengths that potentially involve changes in their activity (Lauderdale et al., 2015).
Although the Mthfr deficiency is known to confer increased risks for ASD in humans and for ASD-like features in mice, due to biological variability and in-utero factors, human and mice present a variable phenotype. Work to find subgroups of patients that share behavioral profiles has been done in both human research (Vargason et al., 2019; Nordahl et al., 2022) and in animal models of psychiatric disorders (El-Kordi et al., 2013; Ardi et al., 2016; Zilkha et al., 2017; Orenbuch et al., 2019), with the goal of enabling biological origin to be determined. Employing this approach, we show that SST densities in the RSC and FC positively correlated with prosocial behavior and SST density in the FC negatively correlated with repetitive behavior. An observed negative correlation between the PV/SST ratio and prosocial behavior was specific to the FC. As a whole, our analyses of brain-behavior relations enhanced our ability to detect neural correlates to behavior and to point specifically to the SST laminar organization in the RSC and FC as targets with significant translational relevance that warrants further research.
Input from the FC, hippocampus and other brain regions is integrated by the RSC and is forwarded by feedback projections to primary sensory cortices (Vann et al., 2009; Zhang et al., 2014). fMRI studies have implicated the RSC in a wide range of cognitive functions, including episodic and autobiographical memory, imagining future events, and executive function, some of which are altered in ASDs (Chan et al., 2009; Spreng et al., 2009; Vann et al., 2009; Demetriou et al., 2018). The RSC was also suggested to translate between egocentric (self-centered) and allocentric (world-centered) spatial reference frames (Marchette et al., 2014; Miller et al., 2014). Here we show that the maternal Mthfr+/− genotype affects both the RSC and its major input sources with a main effect on SST interneurons. The resultant lower numbers of SST interneurons in the RSC may have significant implications for network processing, social perception and behavior that can result in asocial behavior as reported here. Circuit-wise, the dendrites of principal neurons in layer 1 of the RSC integrate inputs from local inhibitory SST synapses (Tremblay et al., 2016; Urban-Ciecko and Barth, 2016) and other brain regions. Lower SST interneuron counts may result in weaker inhibitory input to layer 1, thereby interfering with processing. Although no evidence exists of such changes in the RSC of ASD patients, mice models carrying human mutations were reported to possess irregular RSC function that manifested in impaired social and sensory-motor function and that involved abnormal SST cell quantities (Vesuna et al., 2020; Yang et al., 2021). Our findings support the notion that the RSC regulates social behavior by interfering with the integration of sensorimotor information. Longer recognized for its association with ASD is the FC (Carper and Courchesne, 2005; Geschwind and Levitt, 2007; Teffer and Semendeferi, 2012; van Rooij et al., 2018), with several reports of hypoactivation during social cognition tasks (Hadjikhani et al., 2007; Yang et al., 2017). Although initially, the correlation found between sociability and SST interneurons in the FC seems to contradict this observation, considering the significant cortical interneuron innervation by the SST (Pfeffer et al., 2013), reduced SST cell quantities may result in disinhibition of PV and VIP cells, which would increase the inhibitory input to principal cells. This may also explain the lower GABA levels observed in the FC of autistic patients (Harada et al., 2011).
Insofar as interneuron quantities and distributions may affect inhibitory innervation, our initial estimations of postsynaptic elements and their cortical distributions showed layer specific effects of maternal folate metabolism and the relation of the observed distribution patterns to behavioral phenotypes. Considering these proteins as representative of inhibitory synapses, we conclude that in the tested brain regions, the effect of the Mthfr genotype on inhibitory synapses is restricted. Lastly, whether female offspring exhibit similar associations between social behavior and SST interneurons remains to be determined. An improved understanding of these complex interactions will require further study to elucidate interneuron connectivity.
Data availability statement
The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/Supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.
Ethics statement
The animal study was reviewed and approved by the Animal Care and Use Committee of Ben-Gurion University of the Negev, Beer-Sheva, Israel.
Author contributions
NS conceptualized, planned and performed the experiments, developed method for analysis, and analyzed the data. GS performed the experiments and data analysis. JH developed and provided the Transgene GAD65-tdTomato mice and commented on the manuscript. HG conceptualized and planned the experiments, supervised the experiments and analysis, and funded the study. NS and HG wrote the first draft of the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.
Funding
This study was supported by the Israel Science Foundation grant 515/17 to HG.
Acknowledgments
We thank Milkovski from Alon Friedman’s group for his guidance in the performance of PTZ induced seizures, and Danielle Barda Coronel for her assistance with NKCC1 imaging. The monoclonal anti-NKCC1 antibody, developed by Lytle, C./Forbush III, B., Yale University, was obtained from the Developmental Studies Hybridoma Bank, created by the NICHD of the NIH and maintained at The University of Iowa, Department of Biology, Iowa City, IA 52242.
Conflict of interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Publisher’s note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
Supplementary material
The Supplementary material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnins.2023.1203262/full#supplementary-material
Abbreviations
ASD, autism spectrum disorder; DSI, direct social interaction; FA, folic acid; FC, frontal cortex; GABA, gamma-aminobutyric acid; GAD65, glutamate-decarboxylase 65 isoform; HT, Mthfr+/− heterozygote; MTHFR, methylene-tetrahydrofolate reductase protein; NeuN, neuronal enolase; NKCC1, Na+-K+-2Cl co-transporter; PV, parvalbumin; PTZ, pentylenetetrazol; RSC, retrosplenial cortex; SST, somatostatin; WT, wild-type.
References
Agam, G., Taylor, Z., Vainer, E., and Golan, H. M. (2020). The influence of choline treatment on behavioral and neurochemical autistic-like phenotype in Mthfr-deficient mice. Transl. Psychiatry 10:316. doi: 10.1038/s41398-020-01002-1
Amina, S., Falcone, C., Hong, T., Wolf-Ochoa, M. W., Vakilzadeh, G., Allen, E., et al. (2021). Chandelier cartridge density is reduced in the prefrontal cortex in autism. Cereb. Cortex 31, 2944–2951. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhaa402
Ardi, Z., Albrecht, A., Richter-Levin, A., Saha, R., and Richter-Levin, G. (2016). Behavioral profiling as a translational approach in an animal model of posttraumatic stress disorder. Neurobiol. Dis. 88, 139–147. doi: 10.1016/j.nbd.2016.01.012
Besser, S., Sicker, M., Marx, G., Winkler, U., Eulenburg, V., Hulsmann, S., et al. (2015). A transgenic mouse line expressing the red fluorescent protein tdTomato in GABAergic neurons. PLoS One 10:e0129934. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0129934
Bozzi, Y., Provenzano, G., and Casarosa, S. (2018). Neurobiological bases of autism-epilepsy comorbidity: a focus on excitation/inhibition imbalance. Eur. J. Neurosci. 47, 534–548. doi: 10.1111/ejn.13595
Brisson, C. D., and Andrew, R. D. (2012). A neuronal population in hypothalamus that dramatically resists acute ischemic injury compared to neocortex. J. Neurophysiol. 108, 419–430. doi: 10.1152/jn.00090.2012
Buckley, A. W., and Holmes, G. L. (2016). Epilepsy and autism. Cold Spring Harb. Perspect. Med. 6:a022749. doi: 10.1101/cshperspect.a022749
Carper, R. A., and Courchesne, E. (2005). Localized enlargement of the frontal cortex in early autism. Biol. Psychiatry 57, 126–133. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.11.005
Chan, A. S., Cheung, M. C., Han, Y. M., Sze, S. L., Leung, W. W., Man, H. S., et al. (2009). Executive function deficits and neural discordance in children with autism spectrum disorders. Clin. Neurophysiol. 120, 1107–1115. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2009.04.002
Charych, E. I., Liu, F., Moss, S. J., and Brandon, N. J. (2009). GABA(a) receptors and their associated proteins: implications in the etiology and treatment of schizophrenia and related disorders. Neuropharmacology 57, 481–495. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2009.07.027
Chen, Z., Karaplis, A. C., Ackerman, S. L., Pogribny, I. P., Melnyk, S., Lussier-Cacan, S., et al. (2001). Mice deficient in methylenetetrahydrofolate reductase exhibit hyperhomocysteinemia and decreased methylation capacity, with neuropathology and aortic lipid deposition. Hum. Mol. Genet. 10, 433–443. doi: 10.1093/hmg/10.5.433
Chen, G., Zhang, Y., Li, X., Zhao, X., Ye, Q., Lin, Y., et al. (2017). Distinct inhibitory circuits orchestrate cortical beta and gamma band oscillations. Neuron 96, 1403–1418.e6. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2017.11.033
Chew, T. W., Jiang, X., Yan, J., Wang, W., Lusa, A. L., Carrier, B. J., et al. (2011). Folate intake, Mthfr genotype, and sex modulate choline metabolism in mice. J. Nutr. 141, 1475–1481. doi: 10.3945/jn.111.138859
Cobos, I., Calcagnotto, M. E., Vilaythong, A. J., Thwin, M. T., Noebels, J. L., Baraban, S. C., et al. (2005). Mice lacking Dlx1 show subtype-specific loss of interneurons, reduced inhibition and epilepsy. Nat. Neurosci. 8, 1059–1068. doi: 10.1038/nn1499
Cotter, D., Landau, S., Beasley, C., Stevenson, R., Chana, G., MacMillan, L., et al. (2002). The density and spatial distribution of gabaergic neurons, labelled using calcium binding proteins, in the anterior cingulate cortex in major depressive disorder, bipolar disorder, and schizophrenia. Biol. Psychiatry 51, 377–386. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(01)01243-4
Defensor, E. B., Pearson, B. L., Pobbe, R. L., Bolivar, V. J., Blanchard, D. C., and Blanchard, R. J. (2011). A novel social proximity test suggests patterns of social avoidance and gaze aversion-like behavior in BTBR T+ tf/J mice. Behav. Brain Res. 217, 302–308. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2010.10.033
Demetriou, E. A., Lampit, A., Quintana, D. S., Naismith, S. L., Song, Y. J. C., Pye, J. E., et al. (2018). Autism spectrum disorders: a meta-analysis of executive function. Mol. Psychiatry 23, 1198–1204. doi: 10.1038/mp.2017.75
Eagleson, K. L., Gravielle, M. C., Schlueter McFadyen-Ketchum, L. J., Russek, S. J., Farb, D. H., and Levitt, P. (2010). Genetic disruption of the autism spectrum disorder risk gene PLAUR induces GABAA receptor subunit changes. Neuroscience 168, 797–810. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2010.03.066
El-Kordi, A., Winkler, D., Hammerschmidt, K., Kästner, A., Krueger, D., Ronnenberg, A., et al. (2013). Development of an autism severity score for mice using Nlgn4 null mutants as a construct-valid model of heritable monogenic autism. Behav. Brain Res. 251, 41–49. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2012.11.016
Fatemi, S. H., Halt, A. R., Stary, J. M., Kanodia, R., Schulz, S. C., and Realmuto, G. R. (2002). Glutamic acid decarboxylase 65 and 67 kDa proteins are reduced in autistic parietal and cerebellar cortices. Biol. Psychiatry 52, 805–810. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(02)01430-0
Fatemi, S. H., Reutiman, T. J., Folsom, T. D., Rustan, O. G., Rooney, R. J., and Thuras, P. D. (2014). Downregulation of GABAA receptor protein subunits alpha6, beta2, delta, epsilon, gamma2, theta, and rho2 in superior frontal cortex of subjects with autism. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 44, 1833–1845. doi: 10.1007/s10803-014-2078-x
Filice, F., Vorckel, K. J., Sungur, A. O., Wohr, M., and Schwaller, B. (2016). Reduction in parvalbumin expression not loss of the parvalbumin-expressing GABA interneuron subpopulation in genetic parvalbumin and shank mouse models of autism. Mol. Brain 9:10. doi: 10.1186/s13041-016-0192-8
Froese, D. S., Kopec, J., Rembeza, E., Bezerra, G. A., Oberholzer, A. E., Suormala, T., et al. (2018). Structural basis for the regulation of human 5,10-methylenetetrahydrofolate reductase by phosphorylation and S-adenosylmethionine inhibition. Nat. Commun. 11:2261. doi: 10.1038/s41467-018-04735-2
Frye, R. E., Casanova, M. F., Fatemi, S. H., Folsom, T. D., Reutiman, T. J., Brown, G. L., et al. (2016). Neuropathological mechanisms of seizures in autism Spectrum disorder. Front. Neurosci. 10:192. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2016.00192
Gaetz, W., Bloy, L., Wang, D. J., Port, R. G., Blaskey, L., Levy, S. E., et al. (2014). GABA estimation in the brains of children on the autism spectrum: measurement precision and regional cortical variation. NeuroImage 86, 1–9. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.05.068
Gaskill, B. N., Karas, A. Z., Garner, J. P., and Pritchett-Corning, K. R. (2013). Nest building as an indicator of health and welfare in laboratory mice. J. Visual. Exp. 82:51012. doi: 10.3791/51012
Geschwind, D. H., and Levitt, P. (2007). Autism spectrum disorders: developmental disconnection syndromes. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 17, 103–111. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2007.01.009
Goin-Kochel, R. P., Porter, A. E., Peters, S. U., Shinawi, M., Sahoo, T., and Beaudet, A. L. (2009). The MTHFR 677C-->T polymorphism and behaviors in children with autism: exploratory genotype-phenotype correlations. Autism Res. 2, 98–108. doi: 10.1002/aur.70
Guo, T., Chen, H., Liu, B., Ji, W., and Yang, C. (2012). Methylenetetrahydrofolate reductase polymorphisms C677T and risk of autism in the Chinese Han population. Genet. Test. Mol. Biomarkers 16, 968–973. doi: 10.1089/gtmb.2012.0091
Hadjikhani, N., Joseph, R. M., Snyder, J., and Tager-Flusberg, H. (2007). Abnormal activation of the social brain during face perception in autism. Hum. Brain Mapp. 28, 441–449. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20283
Han, S., Tai, C., Westenbroek, R. E., Yu, F. H., Cheah, C. S., Potter, G. B., et al. (2012). Autistic-like behaviour in Scn1a+/− mice and rescue by enhanced GABA-mediated neurotransmission. Nature 489, 385–390. doi: 10.1038/nature11356
Harada, M., Taki, M. M., Nose, A., Kubo, H., Mori, K., Nishitani, H., et al. (2011). Non-invasive evaluation of the GABAergic/glutamatergic system in autistic patients observed by MEGA-editing proton MR spectroscopy using a clinical 3 tesla instrument. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 41, 447–454. doi: 10.1007/s10803-010-1065-0
Hashemi, E., Ariza, J., Rogers, H., Noctor, S. C., and Martínez-Cerdeño, V. (2016). The number of Parvalbumin-expressing interneurons is decreased in the medial prefrontal cortex in autism. Cereb. Cortex 27, bhw021–bh1943. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhw021
Hayden, D. J., Montgomery, D. P., Cooke, S. F., and Bear, M. F. (2021). Visual recognition is heralded by shifts in local field potential oscillations and inhibitory networks in primary visual cortex. J. Neurosci. 41, 6257–6272. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0391-21.2021
Hong, T., Falcone, C., Dufour, B., Amina, S., Castro, R. P., Regalado, J., et al. (2020). GABA(a)Rα2 is decreased in the axon initial segment of pyramidal cells in specific areas of the prefrontal cortex in autism. Neuroscience 437, 76–86. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2020.04.025
Hoxha, B., Hoxha, M., Domi, E., Gervasoni, J., Persichilli, S., Malaj, V., et al. (2021). Folic acid and autism: a systematic review of the current state of knowledge. Cells 10:1976. doi: 10.3390/cells10081976
Jacob, J. (2016). Cortical interneuron dysfunction in epilepsy associated with autism spectrum disorders. Epilepsia 57, 182–193. doi: 10.1111/epi.13272
James, S. J., Melnyk, S., Jernigan, S., Cleves, M. A., Halsted, C. H., Wong, D. H., et al. (2006). Metabolic endophenotype and related genotypes are associated with oxidative stress in children with autism. Am. J. Med. Geneti. B, Neuropsychiatr. Genet. 141B, 947–956. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.30366
Jedlicka, P., Hoon, M., Papadopoulos, T., Vlachos, A., Winkels, R., Poulopoulos, A., et al. (2011). Increased dentate gyrus excitability in neuroligin-2-deficient mice in vivo. Cereb. Cortex 21, 357–367. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhq100
Kato, H. K., Gillet, S. N., and Isaacson, J. S. (2015). Flexible sensory representations in auditory cortex driven by behavioral relevance. Neuron 88, 1027–1039. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.10.024
Kuki, T., Fujihara, K., Miwa, H., Tamamaki, N., Yanagawa, Y., and Mushiake, H. (2015). Contribution of parvalbumin and somatostatin-expressing GABAergic neurons to slow oscillations and the balance in beta-gamma oscillations across cortical layers. Front. Neural Circ. 9:6. doi: 10.3389/fncir.2015.00006
Kumar, V., Vajawat, B., and Rao, N. P. (2021). Frontal GABA in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis of (1)H-MRS studies. World J. Biol. Psychiatry 22, 1–13. doi: 10.1080/15622975.2020.1731925
Lauderdale, K., Murphy, T., Tung, T., Davila, D., Binder, D. K., and Fiacco, T. A. (2015). 2015. osmotic edema rapidly increases neuronal excitability through activation of NMDA receptor-dependent slow inward currents in juvenile and adult Hippocampus. ASN Neuro 7:175909141560511. doi: 10.1177/1759091415605115. Print
Levine, S. Z., Kodesh, A., Viktorin, A., Smith, L., Uher, R., Reichenberg, A., et al. (2018). Association of maternal use of folic acid and multivitamin supplements in the periods before and during pregnancy with the risk of autism spectrum disorder in offspring. JAMA Psychiaty 75, 176–184. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2017.4050
Lewis, D. A., and Hashimoto, T. (2007). Deciphering the disease process of schizophrenia: the contribution of cortical GABA neurons. Int. Rev. Neurobiol. 78, 109–131. doi: 10.1016/S0074-7742(06)78004-7
Lewis, S. J., Zammit, S., Gunnell, D., and Smith, G. D. (2005). A meta-analysis of the MTHFR C677T polymorphism and schizophrenia risk. Am. J. Med. Genet. B Neuropsychiatr. Genet. 135B, 2–4. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.30170
Liu, X., Solehdin, F., Cohen, I. L., Gonzalez, M. G., Jenkins, E. C., Lewis, M. E., et al. (2011). Population- and family-based studies associate the MTHFR gene with idiopathic autism in simplex families. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 41, 938–944. doi: 10.1007/s10803-010-1120-x
Löscher, W., and Kaila, K. (2022). CNS pharmacology of NKCC1 inhibitors. Neuropharmacology 205:108910. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2021.108910
Luan, Y., Leclerc, D., Cosín-Tomás, M., Malysheva, O. V., Wasek, B., Bottiglieri, T., et al. (2021). Moderate folic acid supplementation in pregnant mice results in altered methyl metabolism and in sex-specific placental transcription changes. Mol. Nutr. Food Res. 65:e2100197. doi: 10.1002/mnfr.202100197
Mao, W., Watanabe, T., Cho, S., Frost, J. L., Truong, T., Zhao, X., et al. (2015). Shank1 regulates excitatory synaptic transmission in mouse hippocampal Parvalbumin-expressing inhibitory interneurons. Eur. J. Neurosci. 41, 1025–1035. doi: 10.1111/ejn.12877
Marchette, S. A., Vass, L. K., Ryan, J., and Epstein, R. A. (2014). Anchoring the neural compass: coding of local spatial reference frames in human medial parietal lobe. Nat. Neurosci. 17, 1598–1606. doi: 10.1038/nn.3834
Marenco, S., and Weinberger, D. R. (2000). The neurodevelopmental hypothesis of schizophrenia: following a trail of evidence from cradle to grave. Dev. Psychopathol. 12, 501–527. doi: 10.1017/S0954579400003138
Miller, A. M., Vedder, L. C., Law, L. M., and Smith, D. M. (2014). Cues, context, and long-term memory: the role of the retrosplenial cortex in spatial cognition. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8:586. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00586
Mohammad, N. S., Jain, J. M., Chintakindi, K. P., Singh, R. P., Naik, U., and Akella, R. R. (2009). Aberrations in folate metabolic pathway and altered susceptibility to autism. Psychiatr. Genet. 19, 171–176. doi: 10.1097/YPG.0b013e32832cebd2
Montgomery, D. P., Hayden, D. J., Chaloner, F. A., Cooke, S. F., and Bear, M. F. (2022). Stimulus-selective response plasticity in primary visual cortex: progress and puzzles. Front. Neural Circ. 15:815554. doi: 10.3389/fncir.2021.815554
MRC Vitamin Study Research Group (1991). Prevention of neural tube defects: results of the Medical Research Council vitamin study. MRC vitamin study research group. Lancet 338, 131–137. doi: 10.1016/0140-6736(91)90133-A
Muntjewerff, J. W., Kahn, R. S., Blom, H. J., and den Heijer, M. (2006). Homocysteine, methylenetetrahydrofolate reductase and risk of schizophrenia: a meta-analysis. Mol. Psychiatry 11, 143–149. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4001746
Nordahl, C. W., Andrews, D. S., Dwyer, P., Waizbard-Bartov, E., Restrepo, B., Lee, J. K., et al. (2022). The autism phenome project: toward identifying clinically meaningful subgroups of autism. Front. Neurosci. 15:786220. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2021.786220
Oblak, A. L., Gibbs, T. T., and Blatt, G. J. (2010). Decreased GABA(B) receptors in the cingulate cortex and fusiform gyrus in autism. J. Neurochem. 114, 1414–1423. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-4159.2010.06858.x
Orenbuch, A., Fortis, K., Taesuwan, S., Yaffe, R., Caudill, M. A., and Golan, H. M. (2019). Prenatal nutritional intervention reduces autistic-like behavior rates among Mthfr-deficient mice. Front. Neurosci. 13:383. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2019.00383
Perez, S. M., Boley, A., and Lodge, D. J. (2019). Region specific knockdown of Parvalbumin or somatostatin produces neuronal and behavioral deficits consistent with those observed in schizophrenia. Transl. Psychiatry 9:264. doi: 10.1038/s41398-019-0603-6
Pfeffer, C. K., Xue, M., He, M., Huang, Z. J., and Scanziani, M. (2013). Inhibition of inhibition in visual cortex: the logic of connections between molecularly distinct interneurons. Nat. Neurosci. 16, 1068–1076. doi: 10.1038/nn.3446
Powell, E. M., Campbell, D. B., Stanwood, G. D., Davis, C., Noebels, J. L., and Levitt, P. (2003). Genetic disruption of cortical interneuron development causes region- and GABA cell type-specific deficits, epilepsy, and behavioral dysfunction. J. Neurosci. 23, 622–631. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-02-00622.2003
Pu, D., Shen, Y., and Wu, J. (2013). Association between MTHFR gene polymorphisms and the risk of autism spectrum disorders: a meta-analysis. Autism Res. 6, 384–392. doi: 10.1002/aur.1300
Rai, V. (2016). Association of methylenetetrahydrofolate reductase (MTHFR) gene C677T polymorphism with autism: evidence of genetic susceptibility. Metab. Brain Dis. 31, 727–735. doi: 10.1007/s11011-016-9815-0
Robertson, C., Ratai, E., and Kanwisher, N. (2016). Reduced GABAergic action in the autistic brain. Curr. Biol. 26, 80–85. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2015.11.019
Roza, S. J., van Batenburg-Eddes, T., Steegers, E. A., Jaddoe, V. W., Mackenbach, J. P., Hofman, A., et al. (2010). Maternal folic acid supplement use in early pregnancy and child behavioural problems: the generation R study. Br. J. Nutr. 103, 445–452. doi: 10.1017/S0007114509991954
Sadigurschi, N., and Golan, H. M. (2018). Maternal and offspring MTHFR genotypes interact in a mouse model to induce ASD-like behavior. Genes Brain Behav. :18:e12547. doi: 10.1111/gbb.12547
Satterstrom, F. K., Kosmicki, J. A., Wang, J., Breen, M. S., De Rubeis, S., An, J. Y., et al. (2020). Large-scale exome sequencing study implicates both developmental and functional changes in the neurobiology of autism. Cells 180, 568–584.e23. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2019.12.036
Schmidt, R. J., Hansen, R. L., Hartiala, J., Allayee, H., Schmidt, L. C., Tancredi, D. J., et al. (2011). Prenatal vitamins, one-carbon metabolism gene variants, and risk for autism. Epidemiology 22, 476–485. doi: 10.1097/EDE.0b013e31821d0e30
Schmidt, R. J., Tancredi, D. J., Ozonoff, S., Hansen, R. L., Hartiala, J., Allayee, H., et al. (2012). Maternal periconceptional folic acid intake and risk of autism spectrum disorders and developmental delay in the CHARGE (CHildhood autism risks from genetics and environment) case-control study. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 96, 80–89. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.110.004416
Shekel, I., Giladi, S., Raykin, E., Weiner, M., Chalifa-Caspi, V., Lederman, D., et al. (2021). Isolation-induced ultrasonic vocalization in environmental and genetic mice models of autism. Front. Neurosci. 15:769670. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2021.769670
Shin, S., Santi, A., and Huang, S. (2021). Conditional Pten knockout in parvalbumin- or somatostatin-positive neurons sufficiently leads to autism-related behavioral phenotypes. Mol. Brain 14:24. doi: 10.1186/s13041-021-00731-8
Spreng, R. N., Mar, R. A., and Kim, A. S. (2009). The common neural basis of autobiographical memory, prospection, navigation, theory of mind, and the default mode: a quantitative meta-analysis. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 21, 489–510. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2008.21029
Taylor, S. F., and Tso, I. F. (2015). GABA abnormalities in schizophrenia: a methodological review of in vivo studies. Schizophr. Res. 167, 84–90. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2014.10.011
Teffer, K., and Semendeferi, K. (2012). Human prefrontal cortex: evolution, development, and pathology. Prog. Brain Res. 195, 191–218. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-444-53860-4.00009-X
Thomas, A., Burant, A., Bui, N., Graham, D., Yuva-Paylor, L. A., and Paylor, R. (2009). Marble burying reflects a repetitive and perseverative behavior more than novelty-induced anxiety. Psychopharmacology 204, 361–373. doi: 10.1007/s00213-009-1466-y
Tremblay, R., Lee, S., and Rudy, B. (2016). GABAergic interneurons in the neocortex: from cellular properties to circuits. Neuron 91, 260–292. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2016.06.033
Urban-Ciecko, J., and Barth, A. L. (2016). Somatostatin-expressing neurons in cortical networks. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 17, 401–409. doi: 10.1038/nrn.2016.53
van Rooij, D., Anagnostou, E., Arango, C., Auzias, G., Behrmann, M., Busatto, G. F., et al. (2018). Cortical and subcortical brain morphometry differences between patients with autism Spectrum disorder and healthy individuals across the lifespan: results from the ENIGMA ASD working group. Am. J. Psychiatry 175, 359–369. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2017.17010100
Vann, S. D., Aggleton, J. P., and Maguire, E. A. (2009). What does the retrosplenial cortex do? Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 10, 792–802. doi: 10.1038/nrn2733
Vargason, T., Frye, R. E., McGuinness, D. L., and Hahn, J. (2019). Clustering of co-occurring conditions in autism spectrum disorder during early childhood: a retrospective analysis of medical claims data. Autism Res. 12, 1272–1285. doi: 10.1002/aur.2128
Vesuna, S., Kauvar, I. V., Richman, E., Gore, F., Oskotsky, T., Sava-Segal, C., et al. (2020). Deep posteromedial cortical rhythm in dissociation. Nature 586, 87–94. doi: 10.1038/s41586-020-2731-9
Vogt, D., Cho, K. K. A., Lee, A. T., Sohal, V. S., and Rubenstein, J. L. R. (2015). The parvalbumin/somatostatin ratio is increased in Pten mutant mice and by human PTEN ASD alleles. Cell Rep. 11, 944–956. doi: 10.1016/j.celrep.2015.04.019
Wilson, R. D., and O’Connor, D. L. (2021). Maternal folic acid and multivitamin supplementation: international clinical evidence with considerations for the prevention of folate-sensitive birth defects. Prev. Med. Rep. 24:101617. doi: 10.1016/j.pmedr.2021.101617
Wohr, M., Orduz, D., Gregory, P., Moreno, H., Khan, U., Vorckel, K. J., et al. (2015). Lack of parvalbumin in mice leads to behavioral deficits relevant to all human autism core symptoms and related neural morphofunctional abnormalities. Transl. Psychiatry 5:e525. doi: 10.1038/tp.2015.19
Yan, J., Wang, W., Gregory, J. F. 3rd, Malysheva, O., Brenna, J. T., Stabler, S. P., et al. (2011). MTHFR C677T genotype influences the isotopic enrichment of one-carbon metabolites in folate-compromised men consuming d9-choline. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 93, 348–355. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.110.005975
Yang, K., Shi, Y., Du, X., Wang, J., Zhang, Y., Shan, S., et al. (2021). SENP1 in the retrosplenial agranular cortex regulates core autistic-like symptoms in mice. Cell Rep. 37:109939. doi: 10.1016/j.celrep.2021.109939
Yang, Y. J., Sukhodolsky, D. G., Lei, J., Dayan, E., Pelphrey, K. A., and Ventola, P. (2017). Distinct neural bases of disruptive behavior and autism symptom severity in boys with autism spectrum disorder. J. Neurodev. Disord. 9:1. doi: 10.1186/s11689-017-9183-z
Zhang, S., Xu, M., Kamigaki, T., Hoang Do, J. P., Chang, W. C., Jenvay, S., et al. (2014). Selective attention. Long-range and local circuits for top-down modulation of visual cortex processing. Science 345, 660–665. doi: 10.1126/science.1254126
Zilkha, N., Kuperman, Y., and Kimchi, T. (2017). High-fat diet exacerbates cognitive rigidity and social deficiency in the BTBR mouse model of autism. Neuroscience 345, 142–154. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2016.01.070
Keywords: MTHFR, folic acid, interneurons, GABA, neurodevelopment, autism spectrum disorder
Citation: Sadigurschi N, Scrift G, Hirrlinger J and Golan HM (2023) Genetic impairment of folate metabolism regulates cortical interneurons and social behavior. Front. Neurosci. 17:1203262. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2023.1203262
Edited by:
Edward Quadros, Downstate Health Sciences University, United StatesReviewed by:
Zhonghua Hu, Central South University, ChinaRuchi Malik, University of California, San Francisco, United States
Copyright © 2023 Sadigurschi, Scrift, Hirrlinger and Golan. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Hava M. Golan, havag@bgu.ac.il