Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Neurosci., 10 November 2022
Sec. Neurogenomics

Association of PPP1R1B polymorphisms with working memory in healthy Han Chinese adults

\r\nHui MaHui Ma1Riyang Qiu,Riyang Qiu2,3Wenya Zhang,Wenya Zhang2,3Xiaohong Chen,Xiaohong Chen2,3Liguo ZhangLiguo Zhang4Man Wang,*Man Wang2,3*
  • 1Hainan Provincial Institute of Mental Health, Hainan Provincial Anning Hospital, Haikou, Hainan, China
  • 2Department of Precision Therapy, Shenzhen Kangning Hospital, Shenzhen, Guangdong, China
  • 3Department of Precision Therapy, Shenzhen Mental Health Center, Shenzhen, Guangdong, China
  • 4Department of Psychiatry, The Third Hospital of Heilongjiang Province, Bei’an, Heilongjiang, China

Aims: The dopamine- and cAMP-regulated phosphoprotein (DARPP-32), which is encoded by the PPP1R1B gene, plays a converging regulatory role in the central nervous system by mediating the actions of dopamine, serotonin, and glutamate. Previous studies have demonstrated that variations in genes related to the dopamine system influence working memory. The present study thus investigated whether polymorphisms in PPP1R1B gene were associated with working memory.

Materials and methods: A sample of 124 healthy Han Chinese were genotyped for three single nucleotide polymorphisms of PPP1R1B gene, namely rs12601930C/T, rs879606A/G, and rs3764352A/G, using polymerase chain reaction and restriction fragment length polymorphism analysis. Working memory performance was assessed using the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST).

Results: Significant differences were observed in the Total Correct (TC), Total Errors (TE), and Conceptual Level Responses (CLR) scores of the WCST among the three rs12601930C/T genotypes (p = 0.044, 0.044, and 0.047, respectively); in TC, TE, Non-Perseverative Errors (NPE), and CLR scores between participants with the CC and (CT + TT) rs12601930C/T polymorphism genotypes (p = 0.032, 0.032, 0.019, and 0.029, respectively); in TC, TE, Perseverative Errors (PE), NPE, and CLR scores between participants with the (CT + CC) and TT rs12601930C/T polymorphism genotypes (p = 0.001, 0.001, 0.011, 0.004, and 0.001, respectively); and in NPE and CLR scores between participants with the GG and (AG + AA) genotypes of the rs3764352A/G polymorphism (p = 0.011 and 0.010). Furthermore, for males only, there were significant differences in TC, TE, PE, NPE, and CLR scores among the rs12601930C/T genotypes (p = 0.020, 0.020, 0.037, 0.029, and 0.014, respectively) and NPE and CLR scores among the rs3764352 genotypes (p = 0.045 and 0.042).

Conclusion: PPP1R1B gene polymorphisms rs12601930C/T and rs3764352A/G might be associated with working memory assessed by the WCST in healthy Chinese adults, especially among males.

Introduction

For decades, mental disorders have been classified based on their observed symptoms and disease course. However, it is still disputed to what extent disorders are distinct entities with boundaries. There is abundant evidence of genetic overlap of single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) between multiple mental disorders, including schizophrenia (SCZ), bipolar disorder (BD), major depressive disorder (MDD), attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), and autism spectrum disorder (ASD) (Cross-Disorder Group of the Psychiatric Genomics Consortoium, 2013; Cross-Disorder Group of the Psychiatric Genomics Consortoium et al., 2013; Brainstorm et al., 2018; Schork et al., 2019; Hammerschlag et al., 2020). Much attention has been paid to the evidence and feasibility of cross-disease diagnosis of mental disorders. Improved classification of mental disorders based on neurobiological measures requires a set of traits that map to transdiagnostic subgroups of patients and align with heritable, core psychopathological processes at the center of the disorders of interest (Schwarz et al., 2016). One promising candidate for this approach is working memory, for which deficits have been reported across multiple diagnostic entities including SCZ (Saykin et al., 1994; Gold et al., 1997; Schwarz et al., 2016; Zakic Milas and Milas, 2019; Ruiz-Sanchez et al., 2021), BD (Arts et al., 2008; Zakic Milas and Milas, 2019), MDD (Matsuo et al., 2007; Vance and Winther, 2021), ADHD (Ramos et al., 2020), and ASD (Rabiee et al., 2020).

Working memory refers to the ability to hold information “online” over time in order to perform a task; this kind of memory is encoded in the brain by persistent neural activity that outlasts the presentation of a stimulus (Bolton and Constantine-Paton, 2018). The working memory system, which maintains a limited set of representations for immediate use in cognition, is a central part of human cognition. Working memory is centrally involved in reasoning (Jolly et al., 2020), mindfulness (Li et al., 2021; Youngs et al., 2021), fluid intelligence (Brydges et al., 2021), attention (Vaughan and Laborde, 2021), language and vocabulary acquisition (Verhagen and Leseman, 2016), and a variety of other neurocognitive tasks (Ricker et al., 2018). The Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST) is a complex, multifactorial test (Berg, 1948; Grant and Berg, 1948) that has been traditionally used to test frontal lobe function (Demakis, 2003; Liozidou et al., 2012). The WCST is a measure of many different neuropsychological functions including working memory, executive function, set-shifting capacity, and other cognitive processes (Gold et al., 1997; Rabin et al., 2005; Thurston-Snoha and Lewine, 2007; Liozidou et al., 2012; Lange et al., 2016; Kopp et al., 2021; Sogut et al., 2021). Successful WCST performance requires participants to remember their prior response and associated feedback and then use this information to select a new response. Although the ability to hold information in mind does not guarantee that a correct choice will be made, being unable to do so would preclude successful performance. Working memory is thus a necessary and important condition for successful WCST performance (Gold et al., 1997). Though the WCST is the gold standard for neuropsychological assessment of executive function (Rabin et al., 2005; Kopp et al., 2021), working memory and executive function overlap and interact with each other (Baddeley, 2012; Cristofori et al., 2019). For these reasons, the WSCT is used to assess working memory function in the present study.

At the molecular level, the neurotransmitter dopamine (DA) is a key regulatory component of executive function in the prefrontal cortex (PFC). Notably, dysfunction in dopaminergic circuitry can result in impaired working memory (Klaus and Pennington, 2019). Two established means of improving working memory performance, namely pharmacological and behavioral influences, are associated with similar biological mechanisms in the brain involving the dopaminergic system (Soderqvist et al., 2012). Working memory is a highly heritable cognitive trait with heritability estimates of up to 49% (Ando et al., 2001). Numerous studies have identified many common genetic variants that impact the function of the DA system could alter working memory performance (Soderqvist et al., 2012).

In 1983, the DA- and cyclic adenosine monophosphate (cAMP)-regulated phosphoprotein (DARPP-32) was first identified as a mediator in striatal neurons that receive dopaminergic neuron innervations (Walaas et al., 1983). DARPP-32, which is encoded by the PPP1R1B gene (located on 17q12), exhibits remarkable regional distribution in the brain that is roughly similar to that of DA innervations (Ouimet et al., 1984, 1992, 1998). Moreover, several studies have demonstrated that DARPP-32 plays a pivotal role in integrating signal transduction in dopaminoceptive neurons (Greengard et al., 1999; Svenningsson et al., 2004; Lin et al., 2021). When phosphorylated by protein kinase A (PKA) at threonine residue 34 (Thr34), DARPP-32 is converted into a potent inhibitor of protein phosphatase 1 (PP1); in contrast, when phosphorylated by cyclin-dependent kinase 5 (CDK5), it inhibits PKA (Svenningsson et al., 2004; Girault and Nairn, 2021). PP1 regulates the phosphorylation state and physiological activity of many neuronal phosphoproteins, including various neurotransmitter receptors, ion pumps, ion channels, and transcription factors (Svenningsson et al., 2004; Girault and Nairn, 2021). The effect of DARPP-32 is terminated by dephosphorylation at Thr34 by protein phosphatase 2B (PP2B, calcineurin) (Hernandez-Lopez et al., 2000). Thus, DARPP-32 is considered to play a converging regulatory role in the central nervous system by mediating the action of DA and various neurotransmitters acting on dopaminoceptive neurons, including serotonin and glutamate (Ouimet et al., 1998; Svenningsson et al., 2005).

Given the above evidence, the DARPP-32 system may relate to working memory function. However, there is no prior research on the association between PPP1R1B polymorphisms and working memory in the general Chinese population. To address this research gap, the present study investigated whether PPP1R1B variants were associated with performance on working memory, as assessed by WCST, in a healthy Chinese sample. Three PPPP1R1B SNPs, namely rs3764352A/G, rs879606A/G, and rs12601930C/T, were selected based on our previous studies of alcohol dependence (Ma et al., 2015), personality traits (Li et al., 2011b), defense mechanisms (Huang et al., 2013), and anxiety level (Ma et al., 2017), and other teams’ related studies of emotional leaning (Curcic-Blake et al., 2012) and cognitive performance (Kunii et al., 2014).

Materials and methods

Subjects

A total of 124 (55 males, 44.35%; 69 females, 55.65%) healthy undergraduate and graduate students were recruited from China Medical University, Shenyang, Liaoning Province, China. All participants were unrelated and of Han Chinese ethnic background. Individuals with a history of psychiatric, neurological, or severe/chronic physical illnesses were excluded. The age range was 20–25 years with a mean age ± standard deviation (SD) of 22.97 ± 1.55 years. The years of education ranged from 14 to 22 years (mean ± SD, 17.10 ± 1.76 years). There were no significant differences in age (t = 0.607, p = 0.545) or years of education (t = 1.374, p = 0.172) between males and females. All protocols in this study were approved by the Ethics Committee of China Medical University. Written informed consent was obtained from all participants.

Neurocognitive assessment

The WCST was developed by Berg (1948) and Grant and Berg (1948), and its reliability in clinical practice falls into the desirable range (≥0.90) (Kopp et al., 2021). For this study, we used a simplified version of the WCST using 64 cards (WCST-64), which uses only half of the master cards but has similar validity (Axelrod, 2002). As the test time is shortened to 10–15 min, the WCST-64 is especially suitable for field operations and recording. We analyzed six sub-scales of the WCST: The numbers of Total Correct (TC), Total Errors (TE), Perseverative Responses (PR), Perseverative Errors (PE), and Non-Perseverative Errors (NPE), and Conceptual Level Responses (CLR). All participants were asked to complete the test alone within 20 min.

Genotyping

A 2 ml sample of venous blood was obtained from each participant for genotyping. Amplification of gene fragments containing the PPP1R1B SNPs rs12601930C/T, rs879606A/G, and rs3764352A/G by polymerase chain reaction and subsequent genotyping by restriction fragment length polymorphism analysis were carried out as described in our previous studies (Li et al., 2011b; Huang et al., 2013; Ma et al., 2015, 2017).

Statistical analysis

Data are presented as the percent frequency or mean and SD. Allele frequencies were calculated from the genotypes of each subject. The Hardy–Weinberg equilibrium (HWE) and genotype distributions of the three SNPs were assessed using the chi-square test. One-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) was used to compare the mean WCST scores between the three different genotypic groups. The independent sample t-test was used to compare the mean age, years of education, and WCST scores between male and female participants and WCST scores between the two dominant-model groups or two recessive-model groups. The level of statistical significance was set at p < 0.05. The Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS 22.0 for Windows, SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, USA) was used to perform the above statistical analyses.

Results

Hardy–Weinberg equilibrium results

Table 1 shows the genotype counts and frequencies for the three studied polymorphisms. The genotype distributions of rs879606 A/G (χ2 = 1.620, p = 0.203) and rs3764352 A/G (χ2 = 1.233, p = 0.267) did not deviate significantly from HWE, while the genotype distribution of rs12601930C/T deviated from HWE (χ2 = 5.834, p = 0.016). The genotype frequency of rs12601930C/T in the present study was not significantly different from that observed in a previous sample of 82 Han Chinese individuals12 = 2.187, p = 0.335). Therefore, the sample in the present study can be considered representative of the general Han Chinese population.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Effects of PPP1R1B polymorphisms on WCST scores.

Relationship between PPP1R1B polymorphisms and working memory in the overall sample

Table 1 presents the WCST scores. Significant differences were demonstrated in the TC, TE, and CLR scores among three rs12601930 genotypes (F = 3.209, 3.209, and 3.128; p = 0.044, 0.044, and 0.047, respectively). The order of the above scores for the different genotypes was as follows: CC < CT < TT for TC and CLR, and TT < CT < CC for TE for the rs12601930 polymorphism. No significant differences were observed for any WCST results among the rs879606 or rs3764352 genotypes (all p > 0.05).

Next, the three genotypes for each SNP were divided into two different groups based on recessive and dominant-models. As shown in Table 1, there were significant differences in the TC, TE, NPE, and CLR scores of WCST between participants with the CC and (CT + TT) genotypes of the rs12601930 polymorphism. Participants with the CC genotype had lower TC and CLR scores (t = −2.169 and −2.208; p = 0.032 and 0.029, respectively) and higher TE and NPE scores (t = 2.169 and 2.385; p = 0.032 and 0.019, respectively) than those with the (CT + TT) genotypes. There were also significant differences in the TC, TE, PE, NPE, and CLR scores between participants with the (CT + CC) and TT genotypes of the rs12601930 polymorphism. Participants with the (CT + CC) genotypes showed lower TC and CLR scores (t = −3.607 and −3.738, p = 0.001 and 0.001, respectively) and higher TE, PE, and NPE scores (t = 3.607, 2.757, and 3.083; p = 0.001, 0.011, and 0.004, respectively) than those with the TT genotype.

In addition, there were significant differences in NPE and CLR scores between participants with the GG and (AG + AA) genotypes of the rs3764352 polymorphism. Participants with the GG genotype showed lower NPE scores (t = −2.631, p = 0.011) and higher CLR scores (t = 2.636, p = 0.010) than those with the (AG + AA) genotypes. However, there was no significant difference for any variable of the WCST between the two models of rs879606 A/G genotypes (all p > 0.05).

Relationship between PPP1R1B polymorphisms and working memory in male and female participants

Differences in WCST scores among the genotype groups of the three studied polymorphisms were analyzed separately by sex. As shown in Table 2, there were significant male-specific differences in TC, TE, PE, NPE, and CLR scores among the rs12601930 genotypes (F = 4.213, 4.213, 3.502, 3.810, and 4.637; p = 0.020, 0.020, 0.037, 0.029, and 0.014, respectively), as well as in NPE and CLR scores among the rs3764352 genotypes (F = 3.303 and 3.384; p = 0.045 and 0.042, respectively). There were no significant male-specific differences in any WCST measure among the rs879606 genotypes (all p > 0.05).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Effects of PPP1R1B polymorphisms on WCST scores in males.

The differences in WCST scores among the three PPP1R1B genotype groups in female participants are shown in Table 3. Notably, there was no significant female-specific difference in any variable for the three studied SNPs (all p > 0.05).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Effects of PPP1R1B polymorphisms on WCST scores in females.

Comparison of PPP1R1B polymorphisms’ genotype distributions and working memory between male and female participants

No significant differences were found in the genotype distributions of the PPP1R1B SNPs rs6090453, rs6011914, and rs2427422 (χ2 = 2.197, 2.492, and 5.677, p = 0.333, 0.288, and 0.059, respectively) or in any of the six WCST scores between male and female participants (Table 4, all p > 0.05).

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Comparation of WCST scores between male and female participants.

Discussion

The present study used 124 unrelated healthy Han Chinese participants to investigate associations between variants of the PPP1R1B gene and working memory function assessed by the WCST for the first time. We found that the rs12601930 (three genotypes, dominant or recessive-models) and rs3764352 (dominant-model) polymorphisms were significantly associated with some dimensions of working memory in the total sample. Moreover, these associations were demonstrated to be specific to males. The dimensions of working memory showing statistically significant differences included TC, TE, PE, NPE, and CLR among the rs12601930 groups, and NPE and CLR among the rs3764352 groups.

Working memory, which is a core part of human cognition, is a limited capacity system that integrates and manipulates information over brief periods of time and engages a network of prefrontal, parietal, and subcortical regions (Duncan et al., 2000; Gray et al., 2003). In SCZ, deficits of working memory have been found on various neuropsychological tests (Saykin et al., 1994; Karlsgodt et al., 2011; Schwarz et al., 2016; Ruiz-Sanchez et al., 2021). Working memory impairments in SCZ occur independently of antipsychotic medication use and appear to represent trait-like, rather than state-like, impairments with high consistency across symptom fluctuations and with little correlation to symptom severity (Gur et al., 2007; Schwarz et al., 2016). Meta-analyses have demonstrated the role of working memory deficits in ADHD (Ramos et al., 2020) and BD (Arts et al., 2008), and a number of neuropsychological studies support working memory deficits in MDD patients (Matsuo et al., 2007; Vance and Winther, 2021). Therefore, working memory is a promising candidate for cross-disease diagnosis of psychiatric diseases. Schwarz et al. (2016) adopted working memory as intermediate phenotype for improved illness classification. Previous studies have demonstrated that genetic factors are an important basis for common and stable changes in working memory function across various mental disorders (Gur et al., 2007; Karlsgodt et al., 2011). Twin studies suggest that a substantial part (up to 43%) of the genetic variance related to working memory modalities is due to a common genetic factor, with additional genetic variance explained by modality-specific factors (Ando et al., 2001). However, the genetic underpinnings of working memory have not been yet fully uncovered.

At the molecular level, the neurotransmitter DA is a key regulatory component of executive function in the PFC, and dysfunction in dopaminergic circuitry has been shown to result in impaired working memory (Klaus and Pennington, 2019). Pharmacological studies suggest that DA and DA agonists in the brain modulate delayed response tasks and working memory (Kimberg and D’Esposito, 2003; Vijayraghavan et al., 2007). In a randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled study, Furman et al. (2021) demonstrated that augmenting cortical DA tone preferentially improved working memory maintenance. Prior research has found that multiple common genetic variants that impact the DA system, including DA receptors (DR), catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT), the dopamine transporter (DAT), etc., could also alter working memory task performance. Functional genetic variants influencing D2-receptor function (DRD2 rs1076560) and Akt1 abundance implicated in downstream D2-signal transduction (AKT1 rs1130233) have been repeatedly found to affect prefrontal blood oxygen level-dependent activity during working memory, consistent with the complex role of prefrontal DA in human working memory (Emamian et al., 2004; Beaulieu et al., 2005; Harris et al., 2005; Zhang et al., 2007; Tan et al., 2008; Bertolino et al., 2009; Giovannetti et al., 2010). Caldu et al. (2007) found an additive effect of the COMT Val108/158 Met polymorphism and the 9-repeat allele of the DAT 40 base pair variable number of a tandem repeat polymorphism on brain activation during an N-back task in healthy subjects. They also demonstrated that the Val allele was related to higher number of PE on the WCST and a higher number of commission errors on the Continuous Performance Test (CPT) (Caldu et al., 2007). Wilkosc et al. (2010) reported an association between DRD1, DRD4, and COMT polymorphisms and performance on the WCST in healthy volunteers. Dumontheil et al. (2014, 2020) demonstrated that variation in COMT was associated with performance on verbal and visuospatial working memory tasks in adults and that the pattern of better working memory performance in Met/Met individuals observed in adulthood emerges during development, which is consistent with decreased levels of prefrontal DA during adolescence. Moreover, Li et al. (2011a) reported that polymorphisms in the NTR1 gene, which is also strongly linked to the DA system, were associated with working memory performance assessed with a 2-back working memory paradigm in healthy Chinese undergraduates. The above reports support the results of the present study since DARPP-32 is a key component of DA signaling.

The relationship between DARPP-32 and working memory has been reported at the protein level among patients with mental disorders. Ishikawa et al. (2007) examined the distribution and expression of DARPP-32 in the post-mortem dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) of 12 patients with SCZ, 10 patients with BD, and 11 controls, and found that DARPP-32 was decreased in the DLPFC of patients with SCZ and BD compared to controls. Kunii et al. (2019) examined the PFC and nucleus accumbens (NAc) of 49 post-mortem patients with SCZ, BD, and normal controls. They reported that DARPP-32 levels in the PFC of patients with SCZ were significantly decreased, while levels of DARPP-32 in the NAc showed no significant alternations in patients with SCZ or BD. Torres et al. (2009) measured DARPP-32 expression in blood cell sub-populations (CD4 + T lymphocytes, CD56 + NK cells, CD19 + B lymphocytes, and CD14 + monocytes) and found that DARPP-32 expression was diminished in CD4 + T lymphocytes, CD19 + B lymphocytes, and CD14 + monocytes of BPD patients and also decreased in CD4 + T lymphocytes and CD56 + NK cells of SCZ patients. Thus, at the protein level, DARPP-32 may underlie working memory, since deficits in working memory always occur in patients with SCZ or BD (Gold et al., 1997).

Direct studies of the relationship between PPP1R1B variants and working memory are rare. Curcic-Blake et al. (2012) demonstrated that homozygotes with GTA alleles of the three SNPs of PPP1R1B (rs879606A/G, rs907094T/C, and rs3764352A/G) might engage in a greater degree of motivational learning and integration of information to correctly perform an emotional learning task. Houlihan et al. (2009) suggested that the PPP1R1B gene merits further attention for association with cognitive ability and/or age-related cognitive change. Kunii et al. (2014) demonstrated that the increased expression of truncated-DARPP-32 in the DLPFC of patients with SCZ and BD was strongly associated with PPP1R1B genotypes at SNPs rs879606, rs90974, and rs3764352 in a sample of 709 post-mortem brains, and that PPP1R1B genetic variants predicting worse cognitive performance were associated with higher truncated-DARPP-32 expression. Another study found that a frequent 7-SNP PPP1R1B haplotype could predict mRNA expression of DARPP-32 isoforms in the post-mortem human brain and was associated with enhanced performance on several cognitive tests depending on frontostriatal function (Meyer-Lindenberg et al., 2007). Although two SNPs of PPP1R1B gene, namely rs879606 and rs3764352, were investigated in the above studies and the present study, the previous studies assessed general cognitive ability or learning ability and most subjects were post-mortem patients with mental disorders, which differs from the present study.

In the present study, we demonstrated no significant differences in the performances of all six dimensions of the WCST or genotype distributions of the three studied SNPs between males and females, indicating no sex-specific difference in working memory function or PPP1R1B genotype distribution. However, there were significant male-specific differences in TC, TE, PE, NPE, and CLR scores among the rs12601930 genotypes and NPE and CLR scores among the rs3764352 genotypes. Therefore, the interaction of PPP1R1B polymorphisms and sex could influence individual working memory. Previous studies have shown that the interaction of DA-related polymorphisms and sex can influence working memory (Wilkosc et al., 2010; Wu et al., 2020). One potential reason for sex-specific findings is androgens, which might be involved in the effect of PPP1R1B on DA and other neurotransmitter systems, and thus ultimately affect the biological determination of working memory. Biochemical experiments will be needed to test if this conjecture is indeed true.

Conclusion

The present study investigated associations between three PPP1R1B polymorphisms and working memory ability measured by the WCST in a sample of Chinese students to demonstrate possible biogenetic mechanisms affecting working memory. We found that the rs12601930 genotypes were associated with five dimensions of the six studied WCST dimensions (except PR) and rs3764352 genotypes were associated with NPE and CLR dimensions in both the overall sample and males only, which indicate that participants with the TT genotype of the rs12601930 polymorphism and/or the GG genotype of the rs3764352 polymorphism have better ability of working memory compared to those with other genotypes. These results provided evidence that genetic variants in the DARPP-32 system could influence working memory by regulating the DA system and that this effect was affected by sex. Moreover, these results provide evidence for working memory as a promising candidate for the classification of cross-disease diagnosis of psychiatric diseases. However, this conclusion should be considered with caution due to the limited size and age range of the sample, the lack of participants with a psychiatric condition, the limited number of studied SNPs, and the use of neurocognitive assessment. Further studies of these three PPP1R1B SNPs and other SNPs associated with the DA system in a larger Chinese sample and other ethnic populations are needed to verify our findings and develop a more comprehensive understanding of the effects of PPP1R1B variants on working memory.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article are available upon request.

Ethics statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Ethics Committee of China Medical University. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

HM and MW: conceptualization and writing—review and editing. HM and WZ: methodology. HM: software, formal analysis, and visualization. XC and LZ: validation. HM, LZ, and MW: investigation. MW: resources, supervision, project administration, and funding acquisition. HM, RQ, WZ, and XC: data curation. HM and RQ: writing—original draft preparation. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This project was supported by the Hainan Province Clinical Medical Center, the Scientific Research Fund of National Natural Science Foundation of China (No. 81771438), Sanming Project of Medicine in Shenzhen (No. SZSM202011014), and Shenzhen Fund for Guangdong Provincial Highlevel Clinical Key Specialties (No. SZGSP013).

Acknowledgments

We thank all the volunteers for taking part in this study.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Footnotes

  1. ^ http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov

References

Ando, J., Ono, Y., and Wright, M. J. (2001). Genetic structure of spatial and verbal working memory. Behav Genet. 31, 615–624. doi: 10.1023/a:1013353613591

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arts, B., Jabben, N., Krabbendam, L., and van Os, J. (2008). Meta-analyses of cognitive functioning in euthymic bipolar patients and their first-degree relatives. Psychol. Med. 38, 771–785. doi: 10.1017/S0033291707001675

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Axelrod, B. N. (2002). Are normative data from the 64-card version of the WCST comparable to the full WCST? Clin. Neuropsychol. 16, 7–11. doi: 10.1076/clin.16.1.7.8331

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baddeley, A. (2012). Working memory: Theories, models, and controversies. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 63, 1–29. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-120710-100422

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beaulieu, J. M., Sotnikova, T. D., Marion, S., Lefkowitz, R. J., Gainetdinov, R. R., and Caron, M. G. (2005). An Akt/beta-arrestin 2/PP2A signaling complex mediates dopaminergic neurotransmission and behavior. Cell 122, 261–273. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2005.05.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berg, E. A. (1948). A simple objective technique for measuring flexibility in thinking. J. Gen. Psychol. 39, 15–22. doi: 10.1080/00221309.1948.9918159

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bertolino, A., Fazio, L., Caforio, G., Blasi, G., Rampino, A., Romano, R., et al. (2009). Functional variants of the dopamine receptor D2 gene modulate prefronto-striatal phenotypes in schizophrenia. Brain 132, 417–425. doi: 10.1093/brain/awn248

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bolton, A. D., and Constantine-Paton, M. (2018). Synaptic Effects of Dopamine Breakdown and Their Relation to Schizophrenia-Linked Working Memory Deficits. Front. Synap. Neurosci. 10:16. doi: 10.3389/fnsyn.2018.00016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brainstorm, C., Anttila, V., Bulik-Sullivan, B., Finucane, H. K., Walters, R. K., Bras, J., et al. (2018). Analysis of shared heritability in common disorders of the brain. Science 360:eaa8757. doi: 10.1126/science.aap8757

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brydges, C. R., Ozolnieks, K. L., and Roberts, G. (2021). Working Memory and Intraindividual Variability in Response Time Mediate Fluid Intelligence Deficits Associated With ADHD Symptomology. J. Atten. Disord. 25, 63–72. doi: 10.1177/1087054718772143

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cross-Disorder Group of the Psychiatric Genomics Consortoium (2013). Identification of risk loci with shared effects on five major psychiatric disorders: A genome-wide analysis. Lancet 381, 1371–1379. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(12)62129-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cross-Disorder Group of the Psychiatric Genomics Consortoium, S. H., Ripke, S., Neale, B. M., Faraone, S. V., and Purcell, S. M. (2013). Genetic relationship between five psychiatric disorders estimated from genome-wide SNPs. Nat. Genet. 45, 984–994. doi: 10.1038/ng.2711

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Caldu, X., Vendrell, P., Bartres-Faz, D., Clemente, I., Bargallo, N., Jurado, M. A., et al. (2007). Impact of the COMT Val108/158 Met and DAT genotypes on prefrontal function in healthy subjects. Neuroimage 37, 1437–1444. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.06.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cristofori, I., Cohen-Zimerman, S., and Grafman, J. (2019). Executive functions. Handb. Clin. Neurol. 163, 197–219. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-12-804281-6.00011-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Curcic-Blake, B., Swart, M., Ter Horst, G. J., Langers, D. R., Kema, I. P., and Aleman, A. (2012). Variation of the gene coding for DARPP-32 (PPP1R1B) and brain connectivity during associative emotional learning. Neuroimage 59, 1540–1550. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.08.036

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Demakis, G. J. (2003). A meta-analytic review of the sensitivity of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test to frontal and lateralized frontal brain damage. Neuropsychology 17, 255–264. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.17.2.255

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dumontheil, I., Jensen, S. K., Wood, N. W., Meyer, M. L., Lieberman, M. D., and Blakemore, S. J. (2014). Preliminary investigation of the influence of dopamine regulating genes on social working memory. Soc. Neurosci. 9, 437–451. doi: 10.1080/17470919.2014.925503

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dumontheil, I., Kilford, E. J., and Blakemore, S. J. (2020). Development of dopaminergic genetic associations with visuospatial, verbal and social working memory. Dev. Sci. 23:e12889. doi: 10.1111/desc.12889

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Duncan, J., Seitz, R. J., Kolodny, J., Bor, D., Herzog, H., Ahmed, A., et al. (2000). A neural basis for general intelligence. Science 289, 457–460. doi: 10.1126/science.289.5478.457

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Emamian, E. S., Hall, D., Birnbaum, M. J., Karayiorgou, M., and Gogos, J. A. (2004). Convergent evidence for impaired AKT1-GSK3beta signaling in schizophrenia. Nat. Genet. 36, 131–137. doi: 10.1038/ng1296

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Furman, D. J., Zhang, Z., Chatham, C. H., Good, M., Badre, D., Hsu, M., et al. (2021). Augmenting Frontal Dopamine Tone Enhances Maintenance over Gating Processes in Working Memory. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 33, 1753–1765. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_01641

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Giovannetti, E., Zucali, P. A., Peters, G. J., Cortesi, F., D’Incecco, A., Smit, E. F., et al. (2010). Association of polymorphisms in AKT1 and EGFR with clinical outcome and toxicity in non-small cell lung cancer patients treated with gefitinib. Mol. Cancer Ther. 9, 581–593. doi: 10.1158/1535-7163.MCT-09-0665

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Girault, J. A., and Nairn, A. C. (2021). DARPP-32 40 years later. Adv. Pharmacol. 90, 67–87. doi: 10.1016/bs.apha.2020.09.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gold, J. M., Carpenter, C., Randolph, C., Goldberg, T. E., and Weinberger, D. R. (1997). Auditory working memory and Wisconsin Card Sorting Test performance in schizophrenia. Arch. Gen. Psych. 54, 159–165. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1997.01830140071013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grant, D. A., and Berg, E. A. (1948). A behavioral analysis of degree of reinforcement and ease of shifting to new responses in a Weigl-type card-sorting problem. J. Exp. Psychol. 38, 404–411. doi: 10.1037/h0059831

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gray, J. R., Chabris, C. F., and Braver, T. S. (2003). Neural mechanisms of general fluid intelligence. Nat. Neurosci. 6, 316–322. doi: 10.1038/nn1014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Greengard, P., Allen, P. B., and Nairn, A. C. (1999). Beyond the dopamine receptor: The DARPP-32/protein phosphatase-1 cascade. Neuron 23, 435–447. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)80798-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gur, R. E., Calkins, M. E., Gur, R. C., Horan, W. P., Nuechterlein, K. H., Seidman, L. J., et al. (2007). The Consortium on the Genetics of Schizophrenia: Neurocognitive endophenotypes. Schizophr. Bull. 33, 49–68. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbl055

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hammerschlag, A. R., de Leeuw, C. A., Middeldorp, C. M., and Polderman, T. J. C. (2020). Synaptic and brain-expressed gene sets relate to the shared genetic risk across five psychiatric disorders. Psychol. Med. 50, 1695–1705. doi: 10.1017/S0033291719001776

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harris, S. L., Gil, G., Robins, H., Hu, W., Hirshfield, K., Bond, E., et al. (2005). Detection of functional single-nucleotide polymorphisms that affect apoptosis. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 102, 16297–16302. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0508390102

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hernandez-Lopez, S., Tkatch, T., Perez-Garci, E., Galarraga, E., Bargas, J., Hamm, H., et al. (2000). D2 dopamine receptors in striatal medium spiny neurons reduce L-type Ca2+ currents and excitability via a novel PLC[beta]1-IP3-calcineurin-signaling cascade. J. Neurosci. 20, 8987–8995.

Google Scholar

Houlihan, L. M., Harris, S. E., Luciano, M., Gow, A. J., Starr, J. M., Visscher, P. M., et al. (2009). Replication study of candidate genes for cognitive abilities: The Lothian Birth Cohort 1936. Genes Brain Behav. 8, 238–247. doi: 10.1111/j.1601-183X.2008.00470.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huang, Y., Li, J., Ma, H., Zhao, X., Wang, Y., Jin, Q., et al. (2013). Association between PPP1R1B polymorphisms and defense mechanisms in healthy Chinese-Han subjects. J. Mol. Neurosci. 49, 618–624. doi: 10.1007/s12031-012-9907-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ishikawa, M., Mizukami, K., Iwakiri, M., and Asada, T. (2007). Immunohistochemical and immunoblot analysis of Dopamine and cyclic AMP-regulated phosphoprotein, relative molecular mass 32,000 (DARPP-32) in the prefrontal cortex of subjects with schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. Prog. Neuropsychopharmacol. Biol. Psych. 31, 1177–1181. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2007.04.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jolly, A. E., Scott, G. T., Sharp, D. J., and Hampshire, A. H. (2020). Distinct patterns of structural damage underlie working memory and reasoning deficits after traumatic brain injury. Brain 143, 1158–1176. doi: 10.1093/brain/awaa067

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Karlsgodt, K. H., Bachman, P., Winkler, A. M., Bearden, C. E., and Glahn, D. C. (2011). Genetic influence on the working memory circuitry: Behavior, structure, function and extensions to illness. Behav. Brain Res. 225, 610–622. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2011.08.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kimberg, D. Y., and D’Esposito, M. (2003). Cognitive effects of the dopamine receptor agonist pergolide. Neuropsychologia 41, 1020–1027. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3932(02)00317-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klaus, K., and Pennington, K. (2019). Dopamine and Working Memory: Genetic Variation, Stress and Implications for Mental Health. Curr. Top. Behav. Neurosci. 41, 369–391. doi: 10.1007/7854_2019_113

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kopp, B., Lange, F., and Steinke, A. (2021). The Reliability of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test in Clinical Practice. Assessment 28, 248–263. doi: 10.1177/1073191119866257

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kunii, Y., Hino, M., Matsumoto, J., Nagaoka, A., Nawa, H., Kakita, A., et al. (2019). Differential protein expression of DARPP-32 versus Calcineurin in the prefrontal cortex and nucleus accumbens in schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. Sci. Rep. 9:14877. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-51456-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kunii, Y., Hyde, T. M., Ye, T., Li, C., Kolachana, B., Dickinson, D., et al. (2014). Revisiting DARPP-32 in postmortem human brain: Changes in schizophrenia and bipolar disorder and genetic associations with t-DARPP-32 expression. Mol. Psych. 19, 192–199. doi: 10.1038/mp.2012.174

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lange, F., Kroger, B., Steinke, A., Seer, C., Dengler, R., and Kopp, B. (2016). Decomposing card-sorting performance: Effects of working memory load and age-related changes. Neuropsychology 30, 579–590. doi: 10.1037/neu0000271

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, J., Chen, C., Chen, C., He, Q., Li, H., Li, J., et al. (2011a). Neurotensin receptor 1 gene (NTSR1) polymorphism is associated with working memory. PLoS One 6:e17365. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0017365

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, J., Ma, H., Zhou, H., Huang, Y., Wu, L., Li, J., et al. (2011b). Association between DARPP-32 gene polymorphism and personality traits in healthy Chinese-Han subjects. J. Mol. Neurosci. 44, 48–52. doi: 10.1007/s12031-011-9505-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, Y., Yang, N., Zhang, Y., Xu, W., and Cai, L. (2021). The Relationship Among Trait Mindfulness, Attention, and Working Memory in Junior School Students Under Different Stressful Situations. Front. Psychol. 12:558690. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.558690

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lin, R., Learman, L. N., Na, C. H., Renuse, S., Chen, K. T., Chen, P. Y., et al. (2021). Persistently Elevated mTOR Complex 1-S6 Kinase 1 Disrupts DARPP-32-Dependent D1 Dopamine Receptor Signaling and Behaviors. Biol. Psych. 89, 1058–1072. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2020.10.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liozidou, A., Potagas, C., Papageorgiou, S. G., and Zalonis, I. (2012). The role of working memory and information processing speed on wisconsin card sorting test performance in Parkinson disease without dementia. J. Geriatr. Psych. Neurol. 25, 215–221. doi: 10.1177/0891988712466456

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ma, H., Huang, Y., Li, X., Lin, A., Cong, Z., and Zhu, G. (2015). A case-control study of the association between DARPP-32 gene polymorphisms and alcohol dependence in Chinese Han subjects. Psych. Res. 229, 1052–1054. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2015.08.026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ma, H., Li, X., Lin, A., Yuan, Z., Zhou, J., Yang, X., et al. (2017). Associations Between PPP1R1B Gene Polymorphisms and Anxiety Levels in the Chinese Population. Neurosci. Bull. 33, 107–110. doi: 10.1007/s12264-016-0088-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Matsuo, K., Glahn, D. C., Peluso, M. A., Hatch, J. P., Monkul, E. S., Najt, P., et al. (2007). Prefrontal hyperactivation during working memory task in untreated individuals with major depressive disorder. Mol. Psych. 12, 158–166. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4001894

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meyer-Lindenberg, A., Straub, R. E., Lipska, B. K., Verchinski, B. A., Goldberg, T., Callicott, J. H., et al. (2007). Genetic evidence implicating DARPP-32 in human frontostriatal structure, function, and cognition. J. Clin. Invest. 117, 672–682. doi: 10.1172/JCI30413

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ouimet, C. C., LaMantia, A. S., Goldman-Rakic, P., Rakic, P., and Greengard, P. (1992). Immunocytochemical localization of DARPP-32, a dopamine and cyclic-AMP-regulated phosphoprotein, in the primate brain. J. Comp. Neurol. 323, 209–218. doi: 10.1002/cne.903230206

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ouimet, C. C., Langley-Gullion, K. C., and Greengard, P. (1998). Quantitative immunocytochemistry of DARPP-32-expressing neurons in the rat caudatoputamen. Brain Res. 808, 8–12. doi: 10.1016/s0006-8993(98)00724-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ouimet, C. C., Miller, P. E., Hemmings, H. C. Jr., Walaas, S. I., and Greengard, P. (1984). DARPP-32, a dopamine- and adenosine 3’:5’-monophosphate-regulated phosphoprotein enriched in dopamine-innervated brain regions. III. Immunocytochemical localization. J. Neurosci. 4, 111–124. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.04-01-00111.1984

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rabiee, A., Vasaghi-Gharamaleki, B., Samadi, S. A., Amiri-Shavaki, Y., and Alaghband-Rad, J. (2020). Working Memory Deficits and its Relationship to Autism Spectrum Disorders. Iran J. Med. Sci. 45, 100–109. doi: 10.30476/IJMS.2019.45315

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rabin, L. A., Barr, W. B., and Burton, L. A. (2005). Assessment practices of clinical neuropsychologists in the United States and Canada: A survey of INS, NAN, and APA Division 40 members. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 20, 33–65. doi: 10.1016/j.acn.2004.02.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ramos, A. A., Hamdan, A. C., and Machado, L. (2020). A meta-analysis on verbal working memory in children and adolescents with ADHD. Clin. Neuropsychol. 34, 873–898. doi: 10.1080/13854046.2019.1604998

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ricker, T. J., Nieuwenstein, M. R., Bayliss, D. M., and Barrouillet, P. (2018). Working memory consolidation: Insights from studies on attention and working memory. Ann. N Y Acad. Sci. 1424, 8–18. doi: 10.1111/nyas.13633

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ruiz-Sanchez, E., Jimenez-Genchi, J., Alcantara-Flores, Y. M., Castaneda-Gonzalez, C. J., vina-Cervantes, C. L. A., Yescas, P., et al. (2021). Working memory deficits in schizophrenia are associated with the rs34884856 variant and expression levels of the NR4A2 gene in a sample Mexican population: A case control study. BMC Psych. 21:86. doi: 10.1186/s12888-021-03081-w

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Saykin, A. J., Shtasel, D. L., Gur, R. E., Kester, D. B., Mozley, L. H., Stafiniak, P., et al. (1994). Neuropsychological deficits in neuroleptic naive patients with first-episode schizophrenia. Arch. Gen. Psych. 51, 124–131. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1994.03950020048005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schork, A. J., Won, H., Appadurai, V., Nudel, R., Gandal, M., Delaneau, O., et al. (2019). A genome-wide association study of shared risk across psychiatric disorders implicates gene regulation during fetal neurodevelopment. Nat. Neurosci. 22, 353–361. doi: 10.1038/s41593-018-0320-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schwarz, E., Tost, H., and Meyer-Lindenberg, A. (2016). Working memory genetics in schizophrenia and related disorders: An RDoC perspective. Am. J. Med. Genet B Neuropsychiatr. Genet. 171B, 121–131. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.32353

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Soderqvist, S., Bergman Nutley, S., Peyrard-Janvid, M., Matsson, H., Humphreys, K., Kere, J., et al. (2012). Dopamine, working memory, and training induced plasticity: Implications for developmental research. Dev. Psychol. 48, 836–843. doi: 10.1037/a0026179

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sogut, M., Goksun, T., and Altan-Atalay, A. (2021). The role of numeracy skills on the Wisconsin card sorting test (WCST) performances of 5- to 8-Year-old turkish children. Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 39, 231–246. doi: 10.1111/bjdp.12353

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Svenningsson, P., Nairn, A. C., and Greengard, P. (2005). DARPP-32 mediates the actions of multiple drugs of abuse. AAPS J. 7, E353–E360. doi: 10.1208/aapsj070235

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Svenningsson, P., Nishi, A., Fisone, G., Girault, J. A., Nairn, A. C., and Greengard, P. (2004). DARPP-32: An integrator of neurotransmission. Annu. Rev. Pharmacol. Toxicol. 44, 269–296. doi: 10.1146/annurev.pharmtox.44.101802.121415

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tan, H. Y., Nicodemus, K. K., Chen, Q., Li, Z., Brooke, J. K., Honea, R., et al. (2008). Genetic variation in AKT1 is linked to dopamine-associated prefrontal cortical structure and function in humans. J. Clin. Invest. 118, 2200–2208. doi: 10.1172/JCI34725

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thurston-Snoha, B. J., and Lewine, R. R. (2007). Intact Wisconsin Card Sorting Test performance: Implications for the role of executive function in schizophrenia. Br. J. Clin. Psychol. 46, 361–369. doi: 10.1348/014466507X173772

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Torres, K. C., Souza, B. R., Miranda, D. M., Nicolato, R., Neves, F. S., Barros, A. G., et al. (2009). The leukocytes expressing DARPP-32 are reduced in patients with schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. Prog. Neuropsychopharmacol. Biol. Psych. 33, 214–219. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2008.10.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vance, A., and Winther, J. (2021). Spatial working memory performance in children and adolescents with major depressive disorder and dysthymic disorder. J. Affect. Disord. 278, 470–476. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2020.09.093

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vaughan, R. S., and Laborde, S. (2021). Attention, working-memory control, working-memory capacity, and sport performance: The moderating role of athletic expertise. Eur. J. Sport Sci. 21, 240–249. doi: 10.1080/17461391.2020.1739143

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Verhagen, J., and Leseman, P. (2016). How do verbal short-term memory and working memory relate to the acquisition of vocabulary and grammar? A comparison between first and second language learners. J. Exp. Child. Psychol. 141, 65–82. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2015.06.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vijayraghavan, S., Wang, M., Birnbaum, S. G., Williams, G. V., and Arnsten, A. F. (2007). Inverted-U dopamine D1 receptor actions on prefrontal neurons engaged in working memory. Nat. Neurosci. 10, 376–384. doi: 10.1038/nn1846

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Walaas, S. I., Aswad, D. W., and Greengard, P. (1983). A dopamine- and cyclic AMP-regulated phosphoprotein enriched in dopamine-innervated brain regions. Nature 301, 69–71. doi: 10.1038/301069a0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wilkosc, M., Hauser, J., Tomaszewska, M., Dmitrzak-Weglarz, M., Skibinska, M., Szczepankiewicz, A., et al. (2010). Influence of dopaminergic and serotoninergic genes on working memory in healthy subjects. Acta Neurobiol. Exp. 70, 86–94.

Google Scholar

Wu, S., Upadhyay, N., Lu, J., Jiang, X., Li, S., Qing, Z., et al. (2020). Interaction of Catechol-O-methyltransferase Val(158) Met polymorphism and sex influences association of parietal intrinsic functional connectivity and immediate verbal memory. Brain Behav. 10:e01784. doi: 10.1002/brb3.1784

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Youngs, M. A., Lee, S. E., Mireku, M. O., Sharma, D., and Kramer, R. S. S. (2021). Mindfulness Meditation Improves Visual Short-Term Memory. Psychol. Rep. 124, 1673–1686. doi: 10.1177/0033294120926670

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zakic Milas, D., and Milas, G. (2019). Working Memory in Patients with Schizophrenia and Bipolar Affective Disorder: Quantitative or Qualitative Differences? Psychiatr. Danub. 31, 54–61. doi: 10.24869/psyd.2019.54

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, Y., Bertolino, A., Fazio, L., Blasi, G., Rampino, A., Romano, R., et al. (2007). Polymorphisms in human dopamine D2 receptor gene affect gene expression, splicing, and neuronal activity during working memory. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 104, 20552–20557. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0707106104

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: PPP1R1B, gene polymorphisms, working memory, Wisconsin Card Sorting Test, Chinese

Citation: Ma H, Qiu R, Zhang W, Chen X, Zhang L and Wang M (2022) Association of PPP1R1B polymorphisms with working memory in healthy Han Chinese adults. Front. Neurosci. 16:989046. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2022.989046

Received: 17 August 2022; Accepted: 10 October 2022;
Published: 10 November 2022.

Edited by:

Rossen Donev, MicroPharm Ltd., United Kingdom

Reviewed by:

Hani Susianti, University of Brawijaya, Indonesia
Xiaofeng Zhao, The First Affiliated Hospital of Zhengzhou University, China

Copyright © 2022 Ma, Qiu, Zhang, Chen, Zhang and Wang. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Man Wang, d2FuZ21wc3lAMTYzLmNvbQ==

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.