Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Neurosci., 18 October 2022
Sec. Perception Science
This article is part of the Research Topic The Study of Multiple Models of Neural Function and Behavior in Sensory Neuroscience View all 5 articles

Assessment of EEG-based functional connectivity in response to haptic delay

  • 1Engineering Division, New York University Abu Dhabi, Abu Dhabi, United Arab Emirates
  • 2Tandon School of Engineering, New York University, New York City, NY, United States

Haptic technologies enable users to physically interact with remote or virtual environments by applying force, vibration, or motion via haptic interfaces. However, the delivery of timely haptic feedback remains a challenge due to the stringent computation and communication requirements associated with haptic data transfer. Haptic delay disrupts the realism of the user experience and interferes with the quality of interaction. Research efforts have been devoted to studying the neural correlates of delayed sensory stimulation to better understand and thus mitigate the impact of delay. However, little is known about the functional neural networks that process haptic delay. This paper investigates the underlying neural networks associated with processing haptic delay in passive and active haptic interactions. Nineteen participants completed a visuo-haptic task using a computer screen and a haptic device while electroencephalography (EEG) data were being recorded. A combined approach based on phase locking value (PLV) functional connectivity and graph theory was used. To assay the effects of haptic delay on functional connectivity, we evaluate a global connectivity property through the small-worldness index and a local connectivity property through the nodal strength index. Results suggest that the brain exhibits significantly different network characteristics when a haptic delay is introduced. Haptic delay caused an increased manifestation of the small-worldness index in the delta and theta bands as well as an increased nodal strength index in the middle central region. Inter-regional connectivity analysis showed that the middle central region was significantly connected to the parietal and occipital regions as a result of haptic delay. These results are expected to indicate the detection of conflicting visuo-haptic information at the middle central region and their respective resolution and integration at the parietal and occipital regions.

1. Introduction

Haptic technologies are becoming a vital component in human-machine communication to empower virtual and extended reality applications with multimodal and bilateral interactions (Holland et al., 2019). Telemedicine, teletraining, teleeducation, and the metaverse are a few application areas that are driving the rapid integration of haptics into human-machine interaction (Tzafestas et al., 2008). To benefit from a multimodal experience involving haptics in such applications, users should be able to experience meaningful perception of the virtual environment by simultaneously integrating inputs from all modalities (visual, auditory, and haptics). A delay in the haptic information is probable due to limited computational resources or disruption in the transmission over the network (Van Den Berg et al., 2017). Haptic delay is well-documented to cause disruption in the task completion time (Ferrell, 1965), degrade the performance (Tatematsu et al., 2011) and falsely manipulate the haptic experience (Knorlein et al., 2009).

Designing reliable and robust haptic technologies intended for use over a computer network is thus dependent on our understanding of the human experience of haptic delay. Neurohaptics is an emerging field that employs brain imaging techniques to analyze the complex neural representations provoked in response to haptic stimulation (Alsuradi et al., 2020). Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) and electroencephalography (EEG) are the two leading neuroimaging methods employed in the neurohaptics field (Alsuradi et al., 2020). However, EEG is more widespread due to its compatibility with electronic devices, lower cost, and higher temporal resolution (Alsuradi et al., 2020). The latter advantage is particularly crucial in investigating time-related perceptual attributes such as haptic delay.

Meanwhile, a few studies have been conducted to explore the neural mechanisms and activation associated with haptic delay. A study on visuo-haptic mismatch in virtual reality showed that there exists an event-related potential (ERP) based neural marker of haptic delay, namely a significant modulation of the early negativity component of the ERP detected at FCz electrode (Gehrke et al., 2019). Several other studies used time-frequency analysis to find spectral features of visual-motor incongruency; they reported that theta synchronization at the midfrontal cortex is more pronounced under incongruent cross-modal stimulation, reflecting conflict monitoring and resolution processes (Cohen and Donner, 2013; Göschl et al., 2015; Arrighi et al., 2016). We have previously shown that there exists a clear oscillatory signature of haptic delay when a discrete force feedback is delayed during passive and active interactions (Alsuradi et al., 2021). The previous work suggests an increase in the mid-frontal theta power upon the detection of haptic delay. P200 peak is also found to be modulated with the presence of haptic delay at FCz (Alsuradi et al., 2021).

However, to our knowledge, no work has investigated the functional neural networks involved in processing haptic delay. Functional connectivity refers to a group of measures that describe how spatially distant brain areas are functionally connected (Nentwich et al., 2020). Typically, functional connectivity is measured in one frequency band at a time; two brain regions are called functionally connected if either the phase or amplitude fluctuates in unison (Cohen, 2014). Neural processes associated with experiencing haptic delay are thought to be complex, and thus, examining neural activation at the electrode level could be insufficient (Lee and Hsieh, 2014). On the contrary, functional connectivity-based analysis considers the intercommunication between different brain regions, which is helpful in better understanding the underlying neural mechanisms associated with haptic delay.

The main objective of this work is to investigate the impact of haptic delay on functional neural networks and their graph theoretical characteristics. The contributions of this study are listed below:

• Examining EEG-derived connectivity patterns associated with haptic delay utilizing a hybrid approach based on phase locking value (PLV) and graph theory.

• Assessing the impact of haptic delay on global and local connectivity patterns through examining the small-worldness index and the strength index, respectively, during passive and active visuo-haptic interactions.

• Identifying the frequency bands in which significant modulation of the connectivity pattern is observed due to the experience of haptic delay.

• Locating the cortical regions(s) that act as the hub of connectivity and capturing the functional connectivity between the hub and the other brain regions during the haptic delay experience.

Toward this end, a visuo-haptic task is designed and participants are recruited to perform the task under passive and active haptic interactions. Haptic delay is introduced to evaluate its impact on the functional neural networks in action.

2. Materials and methods

2.1. Participants

Nineteen healthy subjects (9 males and 10 females; 18–40 years old) were enrolled in the study. All participants were right-handed with normal or corrected-to-normal vision. None of the participants reported a history of neurological or psychiatric disorders. Participants below the age of 18 or left-handed with reported traumatic brain injuries, neural abnormalities, or muscle atrophy were excluded from the study. The study was conducted after obtaining approval of the experimental protocol from the New York University Abu Dhabi Institutional Review Board (IRB: #HRPP-2019-120). The study was carried out in full compliance with the ethical standards outlined in the Declaration of Helsinki, following its guidelines and regulations. Before enrolling in the study, each participant signed a written consent in accordance with the IRB ethics. All participants received monetary compensation upon completion of the task.

2.2. Experimental setup and task

Participants were introduced to the haptic-visual task in which they used a computer screen and a haptic device (Geomagic Touch, 3D Systems, United States). A tennis ball and a racket were shown on the screen; the goal of the task was to bounce the ball using the racket, which is controlled by the stylus of the haptic device. Participants were asked to stay seated on a chair in front of the screen and were instructed to hold the stylus of the haptic device in their right hand. Participants had to perform two types of tasks, namely passive and active, under two haptic delay conditions (0 ms delay or synchronous and 220 ms delay). During the passive task, participants held the stylus passively and pressed the button on the stylus end to initiate a free fall of the ball toward the racket. During the active task, however, participants had to move the stylus actively and hit the stationary ball. In both scenarios, and upon the ball's visual collision with the racket, a discrete force feedback is delivered through the haptic device. While the haptic feedback was delivered instantly along with the visual collision in the no delay condition, it was delayed by 220 ms from the visual collision in the delay condition. The task was developed using the Unity game engine version 2018.4.5f1 (Unity Technologies, United States) and the Openhaptics Unity toolkit (3D Systems, United States).

Participants were trained to use the haptic device before the start of the experiment to maintain minimal body movement and thus avoid excessive motor EEG artifacts. In the active task, participants were asked to only move their wrists up or down while keeping both the elbow and the forearm rested on the table. Figure 1A shows the experimental setup, with one of the participants correctly holding the stylus of the haptic device. The timeline of a single trial for the passive and active tasks is shown in Figure 1B. The trial consists of a rest period of 1.5 or 2.5 s (randomized) during which a blank screen is displayed, followed by a single bounce move. The experimental session consisted of 10 runs (5 passive and 5 active), each comprising 20 trials. In a single run, trials were sequenced randomly and split equally between the synchronous and delay conditions. In total, each participant performed 200 trials equally divided between the four experimental conditions [Passive No Delay (PND), Passive Delay (PD), Active No Delay (AND), and Active Delay (AD)]. To maintain participants' attention and to obtain a performance measure, a question was asked at the end of 30 randomly selected trials on whether the haptic feedback was delayed. Participants had to answer using a keypad. They were also asked to fill in a post-experiment questionnaire.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. (A) Experimental setup showing a participant correctly holding the stylus of the haptic device (B) Timeline of the passive and active tasks.

2.3. EEG data recordings and pre-processing

EEG data were acquired from 64 active electrodes using an actiCHamp amplifier (Brainproducts GmbH, Germany) and the Brain Vision Recorder software (BVR; Version 1.21.0201 Brain Products, Germany). The locations of the electrodes were selected based on the international 10–20 system where the online reference electrode was placed at (FCz) and the ground electrode was placed at the frontal pole (Fpz). The sampling rate was 1,000 Hz. The impedance at each electrode was maintained below 10 KΩ.

EEG data were pre-processed using MATLAB release 2019b (MathWorks, United States) and the EEGLAB toolbox (v14.1.2) (Delorme and Makeig, 2004). EEG data were first filtered between 0.5 and 50 Hz using a zero-phase Hamming windowed sinc FIR filter (order: 3300) and epoched from –1 to 2 s around the visual collision event. High-amplitude artifacts, including eye blinks and muscle bursts, were tackled using the artifact subspace reconstruction (ASR) (Kothe and Jung, 2016) method. Data was then re-referenced using the Common Average Referencing (CAR) method (Lakshmi et al., 2014) and the data of the online reference channel, FCz, was restored back to the dataset.

Following re-referencing, the infomax algorithm was applied to implement independent component analysis (ICA) (Jung et al., 2000) and to maximize the statistical independence between the components of the signals. An extracted component is marked as artifactual if: (1) spurious bursts of activity are observed over the time-course of the epoch, and (2) an increase in power with an increase in frequency is observed (not a natural EEG data behavior (Buzsaki, 2006) but a common pattern of muscle artifacts' spectral power (Goncharova et al., 2003)), or (3) the topography map of the component was confined to the distant edges of the scalp, as such patterns are commonly attributed to eye or muscle artifacts. The clean components were then reflected back into the channel space. Subsequently, EEG data were spatially filtered using the current source density (CSD) method (m-constant: 4, head radius: 10 cm, smoothing constant: 10−4) implemented as part of the CSD toolbox in Matlab (version 1.1) (Kayser and Tenke, 2006). It is evident that applying CSD to EEG data improves the spatio-temporal features and attenuates distant effects due to volume conduction (Burle et al., 2015; Vidal et al., 2015). Spatial filtering is particularly important in studies based on channel-space functional connectivity analysis.

2.4. EEG data analysis

2.4.1. Phase locking value

Before choosing and computing any functional connectivity measure, the pre-processed EEG data of all electrodes are band-pass filtered into five frequency bands: delta (1–4 Hz), theta (4–8 Hz), alpha (8–13 Hz), beta (13–30 Hz), and gamma (30–50 Hz). Filtered data are subsequently transformed to the time-frequency domain using Hilbert transform to obtain the phase information. The phase value is extracted for all the time-bins of each trial. Several studies emphasized the role of phase synchronization mechanisms in binding segregated brain regions (Varela et al., 2001) to fulfill several cognitive functions such as attention (Gross et al., 2004), conscious perception (Pockett et al., 2009) and memory processes (Kitzbichler et al., 2011). PLV is one of the commonly used measures in computing functional connectivity using EEG data which is intrinsically a measure of coordination across different brain regions. PLV can be used to measure the synchrony between two channels in a particular frequency band at a particular time-bin averaged over trials, forming a single epoch. The below equation shows the formula used to calculate the PLV index between electrodes i and j at a particular time-bin t (Lachaux et al., 1999; Bruña et al., 2018):

PLVi,j(t)=1N|n=1Nei(ϕi(t,n)-ϕj(t,n))|

where N is the total number of trials per subject, and ϕi(t, n) - ϕj(t, n) is the instantaneous phase difference between channel i and channel j in trial n at time t. To reduce the computational cost, trials were divided into windows of 50 ms duration with 50% overlap, where the phase values were averaged for each window prior to calculating PLVs as described in Niso et al. (2013). A strong phase synchronization is expressed by a PLV close to 1 while a weak phase synchronization is expressed by a PLV close to 0. The choice of the baseline in this relatively complex setting was selected such that it is close to the onset (visual collision event) and consistent across conditions. The rest period is far from the onset, as can be seen from Figure 1B, while the period preceding the haptic feedback is not consistent across conditions. Thus, an appropriate choice is to use the time interval before the visual collision as a baseline. PLVs were baseline-normalized to the 200 ms data that preceded the visual collision onset. Scripts from HERMES toolbox (Niso et al., 2013) were used and adjusted for the calculation and analysis of the PLVs.

2.4.2. Graph theoretical analysis

PLVs were calculated for each pair of electrodes under the five frequency bands for all participants, creating weighted undirected adjacency (i.e., connectivity) matrices of baseline-normalized PLVs. PLV matrices were analyzed using indices from graph theory (Bullmore and Sporns, 2009). In a graph theoretical analysis, the brain is modeled as a graph network of nodes and links. Generally, nodes represent brain region while links represent functional, anatomical or effective connections (Friston, 1994) depending on the neuroimaging modality as well as the calculated connectivity measure.

In this work, we consider EEG channels as nodes and PLVs as links representing functional connectivity, which generally may occur between anatomically unconnected regions. We focus on two main indices from graph theory; a global index describing the general topology of the network which is the small-worldness index (SW) (Muller et al., 2014) and a nodal index that describes local properties of the network which is the strength index (S), also called node strength (Fallani et al., 2010).

The SW index is defined as the ratio between the weighted average clustering coefficient (C) and the weighted average characteristic path length (L) individually normalized with respect to the frequency bands and participants. A SW index that is larger than 1 indicates that the network possesses small-world properties (Humphries and Gurney, 2008). A network with small-world characteristics has a high-degree of local clustering (segregation) as well as long-distance communication (integration) (Watts and Strogatz, 1998). In other words, small-worldness describes the strength of the 1) collective activations of neighbor nodes and 2) communication strength between distant nodes. The index C of a node is defined as the fraction of triangles around a node, where triangles are defined as simple graphs consisting of three nodes connected in the form of a triangle. The higher the number of triangles around a node, the more clustered it is. The index L, on the other hand, is defined as the average shortest path length between all pairs of nodes in the network. The equations below define Cp, f and Lp, f as a function of the time-bin (t) for a particular participant (p) and frequency band (f):

Cp,f(t)=1Ej=1ECj(t)
Lp,f(t)=1(E2)i=1(E2)Li(t)

where Cj is the clustering coefficient for channel j, E is the number of channels, Li is the characteristic path length for combination i of pairs of channels, and (E2) is the total number of channel pair combinations. Normalized Cp, f(t) and Lp, f(t) indices were obtained by dividing indices of the epoch to the mean of the epoch as described in Vecchio et al. (2017, 2018). The normalized Cp, f(t), Lp, f(t), and SWp, f(t) are defined below:

Cnorm(t)=Cp,f(t)1TNt=t0tnCp,f(t)
Lnorm(t)=Lp,f(t)1TNt=t0tnLp,f(t)
SWnorm(t)=Cnorm(t)Lnorm(t)

where Cnorm is the normalized clustering coefficient, Lnorm is the normalized characteristic path length, SWnorm is the normalized small-worldenss index, t0 is the first time bin in the epoch, tn is the last time bin in the epoch, and TN is the total number of time bins in an epoch. Weighted indices of an epoch are obtained by averaging the indices over the epoch length to achieve a single index for every participant at each frequency band and can be realized by the equations below (Vecchio et al., 2017):

Cavg-norm=1TNt=t0tnCnorm(t)
Lavg-norm=1TNt=t0tnLnorm(t)
SWavg-norm=1TNt=t0tnSWnorm(t)

The strength index (S) is defined as the sum of links' weights connected to the node. The S index was subsequently calculated for the frequency bands that showed a statistically significant difference in the SWavgnorm index between the synchronous and delayed conditions. The index S is used to identify the highly connected nodes in a specific frequency band. We used the Brain Connectivity Toolbox for the graph theoretical analysis on this work, along with custom written MATLAB routines (Rubinov and Sporns, 2010).

2.4.3. Inter-regional connectivity analysis

A global graph-theoretical property such as the SW index is useful to identify the frequency bands at which the connectivity pattern is modulated under haptic delay, while a nodal graph-theoretical property such as the S index is useful to locate the hubs of connectivity over the scalp. Once a hub(s) is identified, a hub-seeded connectivity analysis can be performed to capture the functional connectivity between the hub and the other brain regions. By hub seeded, we mean that we focus on evaluating the connectivity between a single electrode (the hub) and the rest of the electrodes (van Driel et al., 2012). Graph theory analysis is useful to filter out the significantly changed PLVs from the enormous adjacency matrices calculated in Section 2.4.1. Figure 2 shows a summary of the main steps taken to analyze the EEG data from a functional connectivity perspective using graph theory.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Block diagram of the sequence of steps followed for the functional connectivity analysis.

2.4.4. Statistical analysis

Distributions of SWavgnorm index were statistically tested across conditions (PND vs PD and AND vs AD) over the five frequency bands. The normality of the SWavgnorm index distributions was tested using the Shapiro-Wilk test (Royston, 1995) implemented based on Royston R94 algorithm, which works well with sample sizes ≤ 50 (Razali and Wah, 2011; Ghasemi and Zahediasl, 2012). The test yielded an affirmation that the distributions under consideration are all Gaussian. Consequently, we used the paired t-test to evaluate the significant differences in SWavgnorm index between the synchronous and delayed conditions for both the passive and active tasks across the delta, theta, alpha, beta, and gamma bands. False discovery rate (FDR) method was used to correct for multiple comparisons across the five frequency bands by adjusting the raw p-values; we report the adjusted p-values in the manuscript.

Frequency bands that showed significant differences (p < 0.05) were further analyzed using the S index. The S index which was calculated over the time-course of the epoch, was compared across the synchronous and delayed conditions. The statistical comparison of the S index was computed for the hub(s) of connectivity at the peak values of the index. This is motivated by the temporal difference of the peak values caused by haptic delay. Lastly, hub-seeded connectivity analysis is conducted. The time-course of the difference topography maps for the hub-seeded PLVs are obtained per condition. Channels that show significantly different connectivity between the synchronous and delay conditions are highlighted at every time bin. To limit type 1 error due to multiple comparisons of the 64 channels and 10 time bins, p-values were corrected using the FDR method. For all analyzes in the manuscript, the statistics are computed at the group level and the reported p-values are the adjusted values after considering the multiple comparison factor.

3. Results

3.1. Global connectivity properties

The examination of the SWavgnorm index revealed a statistically significant difference between the synchronous and delayed conditions for the passive and active tasks (PD and AD) only in the delta and theta bands. Figure 3A shows a clear increase in the SWavgnorm index when the haptic delay is introduced. On the contrary, the SWavgnorm index showed no significant change upon the introduction of the haptic delay in the alpha, beta, or gamma bands. To better understand whether the change in the SWavgnorm index is motivated by an increase in the Cavgnorm index or a decrease in the Lavgnorm index, both indices were examined. The details of the statistical analysis results on the SWavgnorm, Cavgnorm and Lavgnorm indices are found in Table 1. The time-course of the SWnorm index is further examined over the delta-theta bands and plotted in Figure 4 for both the passive and active tasks. Analysis shows that the peak value is significantly greater for the PD compared to the PND condition (Passive: p-value < 0.05, tPND=[175 ms] and tPD = [350 ms], paired t-test). However, no significant difference was found over the peaks for the active task (Active: p-value > 0.05, tAND=[225 ms] and tAD = [350 ms], paired t-test). As per Figure 3A, it is evident that the SWavgnorm was higher on average for AD compared to the AND condition, but not over the peak values.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Normalized average (A) small-worldness (SWavgnorm), (B) clustering coefficient (Cavgnorm), and (C) characteristic path length (Lavgnorm) indices over the five frequency bands. PND and PD refer to (passive, no delay) and (passive, delay) while AND and AD refer to (active, no delay) and (active, delay) conditions, respectively.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Paired t-test statistics for the normalized average SWavgnorm, Cavgnorm, and Lavgnorm indicies over the five frequency bands.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Time-course of SWnorm index in the delta-theta band for the (A) passive task and the (B) active task. The onset (0 ms) is the visual collision event. *Indicates statistically significant difference.

3.2. Nodal connectivity properties

Consequently, further analysis was focused only on the bands that showed significant differences in the SWavgnorm index, namely, delta (1–4 Hz) and theta (4–8 Hz). The S index was computed for each channel (node) over the time-course of the epoch; Figure 5 shows the temporal change of the S index topography for the four conditions (PND, PD, AND, and AD). In the passive task, a high localized S index is observed in the middle central region centered around Cz. In the presence of delay, the high localized S index is temporally shifted and increased in magnitude. In the active task on the other hand, a high S index is observed over the frontal and ipsilateral parietal regions under the synchronous condition. In the presence of delay, however, a high localized S index is again observed over the middle central region. In all four conditions, the described activation fades out 700 ms after the onset. Since Cz is the center of the activation in the middle central region, the time-course of the S index at Cz is extracted and shown in Figure 6. From a temporal perspective, it can be clearly observed that the S index peaks later and higher in the presence of delay (PD and AD conditions). The difference is statistically significant and observed over peak values (Passive: p-value < 0.05, tPND=[150 ms] and tPD = [350 ms], paired t-test; Active: p-value < 0.05, tAND=[225 ms] and tAD = [350 ms], paired t-test).

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Time-course of the strength index (S) topography over the delta-theta bands (1–8 Hz).

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. Time-course of the strength index S at Cz (1–8 Hz) under the synchronous and delayed conditions for the (A) passive task and the (B) active task. Arrows point to peaks where statistical tests were performed. *Indicates statistically significant difference.

3.3. Inter-regional functional connectivity

Both, the frequency bands and the hub of connectivity were identified through the SW and S indices, respectively. Connectivity in the delta and theta bands was found to be impacted by haptic delay, and the mid-central region, centered around Cz, was found to be the hub of connectivity. Thus, the Cz-seeded connectivity is plotted in Figures 7, 8 for the passive and active tasks, respectively. Figure 7 shows that there is considerable intra-regional connectivity between Cz and the rest of the middle central channels, particularly connected in PD condition. The difference between PND and PD shows that in addition to the intra-regional connectivity in the middle-central region, there is a significant functional connectivity between Cz and (P1, P3, P5, and CP3) as well as (POz and Oz) located at the contralateral parietal and occipital regions, respectively. Those differences are most pronounced between 400 and 500 ms post the visual collision event. Similar analysis for the active task demonstrates a difference in functional connectivity between Cz and (P3, P5, PO3, PO7 and O1) in the contralateral parieto-occipital region maximally pronounced between 300–400 ms post the onset. However, this difference became insignificant after considering the correction for multiple comparisons.

FIGURE 7
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 7. Time course of topography plots of Cz-seeded delta-theta connectivity for the passive task under both conditions and their difference. The highlighted channels in the third row show a significant difference between the delay and no delay conditions.

FIGURE 8
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 8. Time course of scalp topographies plots of Cz-seeded delta-theta connectivity for the active task under both conditions and their difference.

4. Discussion

This study applied a graph theory-driven approach to PLV connectivity patterns derived from EEG data with the objective of identifying distinct global and nodal properties of the neural networks associated with haptic delay during visuo-haptic tasks (passive and active). Our results suggest that the brain exhibits significantly different network characteristics when a haptic delay is introduced. In particular, a significantly increased manifestation of the small-world topology in the theta and delta bands was observed when the haptic delay was introduced (Table 1). An increase in the SW index could be attributed to either an increase in the global clustering (neural segregation) or a decrease in the characteristic path length (neural integration). The results in Figures 3B,C and Table 1, show that this increased manifestation in small-worldness is due to both, the significant increase in the clustering coefficient and the decrease in the characteristic path length. In other words, an enhanced small-world topology in the presence of haptic delay indicates increased segregation within the neural clusters and increased integration among the clusters, demonstrating high communication speed between nodes (electrodes) (Bassett and Sporns, 2017). However, the significance test shows that haptic delay had a larger impact on the network properties (SW,C, and L) during the passive task compared to the active task. As reported in self-reporting research (Vogels, 2004), the sensitivity to haptic delay is stronger during passive interactions as compared to active ones. One possible explanation is that during the passive task, participants are perceptually less distracted, making them more receptive to and aware of the delayed haptic feedback.

A delay in an expected haptic stimulus is hypothesized to elicit higher levels of attention when compared to synchronous stimulation. This is, in turn, functionally related to the ability of an individual to detect and respond to rapid and subtle changes in the environment, such as the presence, absence, or delay of an expected stimulus; this ability is known as the psychomotor speed (Bigler, 2012). In light of this hypothesis, the importance of theta band phase synchrony is evident in the literature with respect to cognition (Klimesch, 1999; Axmacher et al., 2006). In particular, theta band clustering and small-worldness were associated with attention (Gootjes et al., 2006), psychomotor speed, and working memory (Douw et al., 2011), which corroborate the current results.

Further analysis of the graph nodal properties in the delta and theta bands revealed high connectivity localized at the middle central region for the PD and AD conditions, as well as the PND condition, see Figure 5. As for the PND condition, the main neural processes involved while performing the task could be more perceptual and less related to motor coordination. A study based on visuo-tactile stimulation (Kanayama et al., 2015) found increased theta phase synchrony at the middle central region (originated at the anterior cingulate) following multisensory (visuo-tactile) stimulation, relative to unisensory (visual) stimulation. However, the pattern of high S index at the middle central region in the PND condition is delayed and amplified significantly upon the introduction of the haptic delay, as shown in Figure 6A. The delay could simply relate to deferred multisensory processing upon the late delivery of the haptic feedback, while the significant increase in the S index is similar to reported findings indicating a relationship between tasks that involve conflict processing and a subsequent increase in the middle central theta-based connectivity (Wang et al., 2005; Cavanagh et al., 2009, 2010; van Driel et al., 2012).

The AND condition, on the contrary, showed a distinct S index pattern, mainly eminent at the frontal and contralateral parietal regions. Previous clinical studies stated that patients with an impairment in central executive functions (i.e., directing attention, maintaining task goals) show reduced fronto-parietal EEG phase synchrony in the theta band (Babiloni et al., 2004); thus, the observed connectivity could imply active employment of central executive functions during the AND condition. However, the connectivity pattern is diverted to the middle central region when the haptic delay is introduced (AD condition). At this stage of understanding, it is tempting to think that the processes related to conflict resolution could be more prominent compared to executive functions generally present in the AND condition.

In addition to the strong intra-regional connectivity in the middle-central regions, significant inter-regional connectivity was also observed. The contralateral parietal and occipital regions were significantly more connected to Cz under the presence of haptic delay for the passive task. Due to the scarcity of research studies that explore the brain functional networks associated with the haptic modality, it is hard to directly relate the current results to previous work. However, a strong speculation relates the observed theta phase synchrony at the parietal region to hippocampal theta activity, pointing to mechanisms of sensorimotor integration (Bland et al., 2007; Watrous et al., 2011). This hypothesis ties well with previous studies wherein theta activity at the parietal cortex plays a major role in supporting body motor movements necessary for goal pursuit or task completion (Guterstam et al., 2015; Lin et al., 2022). By extension, we believe that theta phase synchrony played a significant role in detecting the delay in the haptic modality (middle central region) and communicating the conflict to the parietal cortex for integrating the flow of the incongruent sensory information. Additionally, it is interesting to observe the connectivity between Cz and the occipital electrodes (POz and Oz) indicating a sort of information transfer possibly falling under reactive control processes required to resolve conflict in the involved sensory modalities (motor, visual) (Cooper et al., 2015). In Figure 8, it can be observed that there is higher connectivity between Cz and the contralateral parieto-occipital region (P3, P5, PO3, and PO7) in the AD compared to the AND condition. Despite the difference, it was found insignificant after correcting for the number of channels and the time-bins.

One limitation of this study is that we used the PLV measure to construct the adjacency matrices to evaluate the connectivity between electrode pairs. It is known that the PLV measure is sensitive to the problem of volume conduction, which could result in spurious connectivity (Bruña et al., 2018). However, PLV is one of the standard methods that is used to study connectivity at the scalp level. To address this limitation, we used spatial filtering (CSD method), which is known to attenuate the volume conduction effect (Srinivasan et al., 2006). An interesting future direction would be to study the effects of changing the type of haptic feedback (i.e., vibrotactile or kinesthetic), the amount of haptic delay, and the variation in delay (i.e., jitter) on the functional networks involved in processing haptic delay.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by New York University Abu Dhabi Institutional Review Board (IRB: #HRPP-2019-120). The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

ME and HA conceived the study. HA and WP designed the experimental protocol. HA conducted the experiment and analyzed the results. WP and ME reviewed the results. All authors contributed intellectually in writing and revising the manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by the New York University Abu Dhabi PhD Fellowship Program. This work is also supported in part by the NYUAD Center for Artificial Intelligence and Robotics, funded by Tamkeen under the Research Institute Award CG010.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Alsuradi, H., Park, W., and Eid, M. (2020). Eeg-based neurohaptics research: a literature review. IEEE Access 8, 49313–49328. doi: 10.1109/ACCESS.2020.2979855

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Alsuradi, H., Park, W., and Eid, M. (2021). Midfrontal theta oscillation encodes haptic delay. Sci. Rep. 11, 1–12. doi: 10.1038/s41598-021-95631-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arrighi, P., Bonfiglio, L., Minichilli, F., Cantore, N., Carboncini, M. C., Piccotti, E., et al. (2016). Eeg theta dynamics within frontal and parietal cortices for error processing during reaching movements in a prism adaptation study altering visuo-motor predictive planning. PLoS ONE 11, e0150265. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0150265

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Axmacher, N., Mormann, F., Fernández, G., Elger, C. E., and Fell, J. (2006). Memory formation by neuronal synchronization. Brain Res. Rev. 52, 170–182. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2006.01.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Babiloni, C., Babiloni, F., Carducci, F., Cincotti, F., Vecchio, F., Cola, B., et al. (2004). Functional frontoparietal connectivity during short-term memory as revealed by high-resolution eeg coherence analysis. Behav. Neurosci. 118, 687. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.118.4.687

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bassett, D. S., and Sporns, O. (2017). Network neuroscience. Nat. Neurosci. 20, 353–364. doi: 10.1038/nn.4502

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bigler, E. D. (2012). Symptom validity testing, effort, and neuropsychological assessment. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 18, 632–640. doi: 10.1017/S1355617712000252

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bland, B. H., Declerck, S., Jackson, J., Glasgow, S., and Oddie, S. (2007). Septohippocampal properties of n-methyl-d-aspartate-induced theta-band oscillation and synchrony. Synapse 61, 185–197. doi: 10.1002/syn.20357

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bruña, R., Maestú, F., and Pereda, E. (2018). Phase locking value revisited: teaching new tricks to an old dog. J. Neural Eng. 15, 056011. doi: 10.1088/1741-2552/aacfe4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bullmore, E., and Sporns, O. (2009). Complex brain networks: graph theoretical analysis of structural and functional systems. Nature Rev. Neurosci. 10, 186–198. doi: 10.1038/nrn2575

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burle, B., Spieser, L., Roger, C., Casini, L., Hasbroucq, T., and Vidal, F. (2015). Spatial and temporal resolutions of eeg: Is it really black and white? a scalp current density view. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 97, 210–220. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2015.05.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buzsaki, G. (2006). Rhythms of the Brain. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Cavanagh, J. F., Cohen, M. X., and Allen, J. J. (2009). Prelude to and resolution of an error: EEG phase synchrony reveals cognitive control dynamics during action monitoring. J. Neurosci. 29, 98–105. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4137-08.2009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cavanagh, J. F., Frank, M. J., Klein, T. J., and Allen, J. J. (2010). Frontal theta links prediction errors to behavioral adaptation in reinforcement learning. Neuroimage 49, 3198–3209. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.11.080

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen, M. X. (2014). Analyzing Neural Time Series Data: Theory and Practice. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Google Scholar

Cohen, M. X., and Donner, T. H. (2013). Midfrontal conflict-related theta-band power reflects neural oscillations that predict behavior. .J Neurophysiol. 110, 2752–2763. doi: 10.1152/jn.00479.2013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cooper, P. S., Wong, A. S., Fulham, W. R., Thienel, R., Mansfield, E., Michie, P. T., et al. (2015). Theta frontoparietal connectivity associated with proactive and reactive cognitive control processes. Neuroimage 108, 354–363. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.12.028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Delorme, A., and Makeig, S. (2004). Eeglab: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial eeg dynamics including independent component analysis. J. Neurosci. Methods 134, 9–21. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2003.10.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Douw, L., Schoonheim, M., Landi, D., Van der Meer, M., Geurts, J., Reijneveld, J., et al. (2011). Cognition is related to resting-state small-world network topology: an magnetoencephalographic study. Neuroscience 175, 169–177. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2010.11.039

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fallani, F. D. V., Costa, L. d,. F, Rodriguez, F. A., Astolfi, L., Vecchiato, G., et al. (2010). “A graph-theoretical approach in brain functional networks. Possible Implications in EEG Studies. Nonlinear Biomed. 4, 1–13.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Ferrell, W. R. (1965). Remote manipulation with transmission delay. IEEE Trans. Hum. Factors Electron. 6, 24–32. doi: 10.1109/THFE.1965.6591253

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Friston, K. J. (1994). Functional and effective connectivity in neuroimaging: a synthesis. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2, 56–78. doi: 10.1002/hbm.460020107

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gehrke, L., Akman, S., Lopes, P., Chen, A., Singh, A. K., Chen, H.-T., et al. (2019). “Detecting visuo-haptic mismatches in virtual reality using the prediction error negativity of event-related brain potentials,” in Proceedings of the 2019 CHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems, 1–11.

Google Scholar

Ghasemi, A., and Zahediasl, S. (2012). Normality tests for statistical analysis: a guide for non-statisticians. Int. J. Endocrinol. Metabol. 10, 486. doi: 10.5812/ijem.3505

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goncharova, I. I., McFarland, D. J., Vaughan, T. M., and Wolpaw, J. R. (2003). Emg contamination of EEG: spectral and topographical characteristics. Clin. Neurophysiol. 114, 1580–1593. doi: 10.1016/S1388-2457(03)00093-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gootjes, L., Bouma, A., Van Strien, J. W., Scheltens, P., and Stam, C. J. (2006). Attention modulates hemispheric differences in functional connectivity: evidence from meg recordings. Neuroimage 30, 245–253. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.09.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Göschl, F., Friese, U., Daume, J., König, P., and Engel, A. K. (2015). Oscillatory signatures of crossmodal congruence effects: an EEG investigation employing a visuotactile pattern matching paradigm. Neuroimage 116, 177–186. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.03.067

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gross, J., Schmitz, F., Schnitzler, I., Kessler, K., Shapiro, K., Hommel, B., et al. (2004). Modulation of long-range neural synchrony reflects temporal limitations of visual attention in humans. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 101, 13050–13055. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0404944101

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guterstam, A., Björnsdotter, M., Gentile, G., and Ehrsson, H. H. (2015). Posterior cingulate cortex integrates the senses of self-location and body ownership. Curr. Biol. 25, 1416–1425. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2015.03.059

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Holland, O., Steinbach, E., Prasad, R. V., Liu, Q., Dawy, Z., Aijaz, A., et al. (2019). The ieee 1918.1 “tactile internet” standards working group and its standards. Proc. IEEE 107, 256–279. doi: 10.1109/JPROC.2018.2885541

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Humphries, M. D., and Gurney, K. (2008). Network ‘small-world-ness: a quantitative method for determining canonical network equivalence. PLoS ONE. 3, e0002051. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0002051

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jung, T.-P., Makeig, S., Humphries, C., Lee, T.-W., Mckeown, M. J., Iragui, V., et al. (2000). Removing electroencephalographic artifacts by blind source separation. Psychophysiology 37, 163–178. doi: 10.1111/1469-8986.3720163

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kanayama, N., Kimura, K., and Hiraki, K. (2015). Cortical eeg components that reflect inverse effectiveness during visuotactile integration processing. Brain Res. 1598, 18–30. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2014.12.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kayser, J., and Tenke, C. E. (2006). Principal components analysis of laplacian waveforms as a generic method for identifying erp generator patterns: I. evaluation with auditory oddball tasks. Clin. Neurophysiol. 117, 348–368. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2005.08.034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kitzbichler, M. G., Henson, R. N., Smith, M. L., Nathan, P. J., and Bullmore, E. T. (2011). Cognitive effort drives workspace configuration of human brain functional networks. J. Neurosci. 31, 8259–8270. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0440-11.2011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klimesch, W. (1999). Eeg alpha and theta oscillations reflect cognitive and memory performance: a review and analysis. Brain Res. Rev. 29, 169–195. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0173(98)00056-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Knorlein, B., Di Luca, M., and Harders, M. (2009). “Influence of visual and haptic delays on stiffness perception in augmented reality,” in 2009 8th IEEE International Symposium on Mixed and Augmented Reality (Orlando, FL: IEEE), 49–52.

Google Scholar

Kothe, C. A. E., and Jung, T.-P. (2016). Artifact removal techniques with signal reconstruction. U.S. Patent App. 14/895,440.

Google Scholar

Lachaux, J.-P., Rodriguez, E., Martinerie, J., and Varela, F. J. (1999). Measuring phase synchrony in brain signals. Hum. Brain Mapp. 8, 194–208. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1097-0193(1999)8:4andlt;194::AID-HBM4andgt;3.0.CO;2-C

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lakshmi, M. R., Prasad, T., and Prakash, D. V. C. (2014). Survey on EEG signal processing methods. Int. J. Adv. Res. Comput. Sci. Software Eng. 4, 84–91. Available online at: https://scholar.google.com/scholar_lookup?title=Survey%20on%20EEG%20signal%20processing%20methods&publication_year=2014&author=M.%20Rajya%20Lakshmi&author=T.V.%20Prasad&author=Dr%20V.%20Chandra%20Prakash

Google Scholar

Lee, Y.-Y., and Hsieh, S. (2014). Classifying different emotional states by means of eeg-based functional connectivity patterns. PLoS ONE 9, e95415. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0095415

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lin, M.-H., Liran, O., Bauer, N., and Baker, T. E. (2022). Scalp recorded theta activity is modulated by reward, direction, and speed during virtual navigation in freely moving humans. Sci. Rep. 12, 1–18. doi: 10.1038/s41598-022-05955-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Muller, L., Destexhe, A., and Rudolph-Lilith, M. (2014). Brain networks: small-worlds, after all? New J. Phys. 16, 105004. doi: 10.1088/1367-2630/16/10/105004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nentwich, M., Ai, L., Madsen, J., Telesford, Q. K., Haufe, S., Milham, M. P., et al. (2020). Functional connectivity of eeg is subject-specific, associated with phenotype, and different from fmri. Neuroimage 218, 117001. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.117001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Niso, G., Bru na, R., Pereda, E., Gutiérrez, R., Bajo, R., Maestú, F., et al. (2013). Hermes: towards an integrated toolbox to characterize functional and effective brain connectivity. Neuroinformatics 11, 405–434. doi: 10.1007/s12021-013-9186-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pockett, S., Bold, G. E., and Freeman, W. J. (2009). Eeg synchrony during a perceptual-cognitive task: widespread phase synchrony at all frequencies. Clin. Neurophysiol. 120, 695–708. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2008.12.044

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Razali, N. M., and Wah, Y. B. (2011). Power comparisons of shapiro-wilk, kolmogorov-smirnov, lilliefors and anderson-darling tests. J. Stat. Model. Anal. 2, 21–33.

Google Scholar

Royston, J. (1995). A remark on algorithm as-181-the w test for normality (algorithm r94). J. Appl. Stat. 44, 547–551. doi: 10.2307/2986146

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rubinov, M., and Sporns, O. (2010). Complex network measures of brain connectivity: uses and interpretations. Neuroimage 52, 1059–1069. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.10.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Srinivasan, R., Winter, W. R., and Nunez, P. L. (2006). Source analysis of EEG oscillations using high-resolution eeg and meg. Prog. Brain Res. 159, 29–42. doi: 10.1016/S0079-6123(06)59003-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tatematsu, A., Ishibashi, Y., Fukushima, N., and Sugawara, S. (2011). “Qoe assessment in tele-operation with 3d video and haptic media,” in 2011 IEEE International Conference on Multimedia and Expo (Barcelona: IEEE), 1–6.

Google Scholar

Tzafestas, C., Velanas, S., and Fakiridis, G. (2008). “Adaptive impedance control in haptic teleoperation to improve transparency under time-delay,” in 2008 IEEE International Conference on Robotics and Automation (Pasadena, CA: IEEE), 212–219.

Google Scholar

Van Den Berg, D., Glans, R., De Koning, D., Kuipers, F. A., Lugtenburg, J., Polachan, K., et al. (2017). Challenges in haptic communications over the tactile internet. IEEE Access 5, 23502–23518. doi: 10.1109/ACCESS.2017.2764181

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Driel, J., Ridderinkhof, K. R., and Cohen, M. X. (2012). Not all errors are alike: theta and alpha eeg dynamics relate to differences in error-processing dynamics. J. Neurosci. 32, 16795–16806. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0802-12.2012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Varela, F., Lachaux, J.-P., Rodriguez, E., and Martinerie, J. (2001). The brainweb: phase synchronization and large-scale integration. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2, 229–239. doi: 10.1038/35067550

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vecchio, F., Di Iorio, R., Miraglia, F., Granata, G., Romanello, R., Bramanti, P., et al. (2018). Transcranial direct current stimulation generates a transient increase of small-world in brain connectivity: an eeg graph theoretical analysis. Exp. Brain Res. 236, 1117–1127. doi: 10.1007/s00221-018-5200-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vecchio, F., Miraglia, F., Piludu, F., Granata, G., Romanello, R., Caulo, M., et al. (2017). “small world” architecture in brain connectivity and hippocampal volume in alzheimer's disease: a study via graph theory from eeg data. Brain Imaging Behav. 11, 473–485. doi: 10.1007/s11682-016-9528-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vidal, F., Burle, B., Spieser, L., Carbonnell, L., Meckler, C., Casini, L., et al. (2015). Linking eeg signals, brain functions and mental operations: advantages of the laplacian transformation. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 97, 221–232. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2015.04.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vogels, I. M. (2004). Detection of temporal delays in visual-haptic interfaces. Hum. Factors 46, 118–134. doi: 10.1518/hfes.46.1.118.30394

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, C., Ulbert, I., Schomer, D. L., Marinkovic, K., and Halgren, E. (2005). Responses of human anterior cingulate cortex microdomains to error detection, conflict monitoring, stimulus-response mapping, familiarity, and orienting. J. Neurosci. 25, 604–613. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4151-04.2005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Watrous, A. J., Fried, I., and Ekstrom, A. D. (2011). Behavioral correlates of human hippocampal delta and theta oscillations during navigation. J. Neurophysiol. 105, 1747–1755. doi: 10.1152/jn.00921.2010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Watts, D. J., and Strogatz, S. H. (1998). Collective dynamics of ‘small-world'networks. Nature 393, 440–442. doi: 10.1038/30918

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: functional connectivity, neurohaptics, haptics, PLV, graph theory, neural signal processing, EEG

Citation: Alsuradi H, Park W and Eid M (2022) Assessment of EEG-based functional connectivity in response to haptic delay. Front. Neurosci. 16:961101. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2022.961101

Received: 03 June 2022; Accepted: 03 October 2022;
Published: 18 October 2022.

Edited by:

Birgitta Dresp-Langley, Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique (CNRS), France

Reviewed by:

Miriam Zacksenhouse, Technion Israel Institute of Technology, Israel
Mark W. Greenlee, University of Regensburg, Germany

Copyright © 2022 Alsuradi, Park and Eid. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Haneen Alsuradi, haneen@nyu.edu; Mohamad Eid, mohamad.eid@nyu.edu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.