Skip to main content

OPINION article

Front. Neurosci., 14 June 2022
Sec. Neuroprosthetics
This article is part of the Research Topic Women in Neuroengineering and Neurotechnologies View all 8 articles

Sensorimotor Interaction Against Trauma

\nGiada PersichilliGiada Persichilli1Joy Grifoni,,Joy Grifoni1,2,3Marco PaganiMarco Pagani1Massimo Bertoli,Massimo Bertoli1,2Eugenia Gianni,Eugenia Gianni1,4Teresa L&#x;Abbate,Teresa L'Abbate1,2Luca CernigliaLuca Cerniglia3Gabriela BevacquaGabriela Bevacqua5Luca PaulonLuca Paulon6Franca Tecchio
Franca Tecchio1*
  • 1Laboratory of Electrophysiology for Translational Neuroscience LET'S, Institute of Cognitive Sciences and Technologies ISTC, Consiglio Nazionale Delle Ricerche CNR, Rome, Italy
  • 2Department of Neuroscience, Imaging and Clinical Sciences, University “Gabriele D'Annunzio” of Chieti-Pescara, Chieti, Italy
  • 3Faculty of Psychology, International Telematic University Uninettuno, Rome, Italy
  • 4Unit of Neurology, Neurophysiology, Neurobiology, Department of Medicine, University Campus Bio-Medico of Rome, Rome, Italy
  • 5Studio Psyche Neuroscienze, EMDR Therapist, Rome, Italy
  • 6Independent Researcher, Rome, Italy

Introduction

Our body-brain system, under physiological conditions, interacts adaptively with the environment. Our central hypothesis is that the triadic principle of feedback-synchrony-plasticity (FeeSyCy, Figure 1) governs the adaptive capacity of the body-brain system. In this opinion, with the perspective goal of contributing to the fight against disorders of major importance, we aim to show how the FeeSyCy principle underlies the efficacy of an elective treatment against an illustrative case of impaired adaptive ability. To this end, we referred to Eye Movement Desensitization and Reprocessing (EMDR) indicated by the World Health Organization (WHO) guidelines (World Health Organization, 2013) as the treatment of choice for post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. FeeSyCy & EMDR efficacy against trauma. Graphic representation of our central hypothesis, that the triadic principle feedback-synchrony-plasticity (FeeSyCy) governs the adaptive capacity of the body-brain system and underlies the effectiveness of Eye Movement Desensitisation and Reprocessing (EMDR), an elective treatment for post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD), an illustrative case of reduced adaptive capacity. The neuronal network, shown separately for graphic simplicity, refers to both the patient and the therapist.

The FeeSyCy Principle

The adaptive body-brain interaction with the environment is essentially rooted in recursive sensorimotor feedback loops that compare the expected sensory information coming from the environment due to the enacted behavior and what comes back. The comparison is expressed in the (de) synchronizations of the electrical activities of the neuronal involved regions, on whose level depends the plastic changes that allow the brain to adapt to ever-changing environmental demands. Thus, the governing principle emerges as the functional triad feedback-synchrony-plasticity (FeeSyCy) (Tecchio et al., 2020). This model of individual interplay with the surrounding requirements is in line with the theories of predictive processing (Hohwy, 2013), according to which the body-brain system tends to reduce the distance with the target by minimizing the prediction error, resulting from the mismatch between top-down predictions and bottom-up sensory information (Friston and Kiebel, 2009), depending on the delicate modulatory gain control of the involved neuronal population's hierarchies (Kanai et al., 2015). It is becoming increasingly clear how neural networks operate by implementing learning mechanisms that regulate sensory precision according to its functional value that depends on the objective (Grujic et al., 2022).

PTSD and EMDR

Post-traumatic disorder (PTSD) is one of the possible consequences of a traumatic event that implies the exposition to a threat that may concern the person's integrity and physical condition (Ozer et al., 2003; Vance et al., 2018). PTSD prevalence varies in geographical areas, cultural factors, and different degrees of exposure to traumatic events (Kessler et al., 2017), and occurs twice as often in women than men (Christiansen and Hansen, 2015; Kessler et al., 2017). In this regard, trauma is a relevant example of the need to consider how gender and sex interact with socio-cultural aspects that drive a different manifestation of suffering and access to health care and treatment between men and women (Navarro et al., 2015). Moreover, biological factors underlying sex, such as neuroendocrinology, genomics, and developmental differentiation programmed by gonadal steroids, mediate the individual's impact and outcome of the traumatic event (Sherin and Nemeroff, 2011).

The PTSD provides an exemplificative case of a dramatic alteration of the adaptive physiological nature of the body-brain system. The traumatic experience, when inducing dysregulation of neuronal, hormonal, and immune mechanisms (Ressler et al., 2022) can result in a psychopathological condition characterized by altered adaptive behaviors. According to the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (APA, 2022), criteria to diagnose PTSD include the appearance of intrusive symptoms, avoidance of stimuli associated with trauma, alteration of mood and thinking significant alteration of arousal, and the possible presence of dissociative experiences. At the brain level, evidence shows that neural, neurochemical, and neurobiological alterations induce processing segregation within short-circuits, with structures belonging to the limbic, motor, and sensory systems, storing implicit traumatic memories, and becoming separated from hippocampal-cortical system processing episodic autobiographical memories (Vermetten and Lanius, 2012; Ressler et al., 2022).

Grounding on the theoretical framework of the Adaptive Information Processing (AIP) (Shapiro, 2001), EMDR (Figure 1) consists of a psychotherapy treatment with one session per week typically lasting 50–60 min, comprising 8 phases adapted by the therapist to the patient's situation:

1. Collection of history and treatment plan.

2. Preparation of the patient entering a relationship with the therapist and the identification of a “safe place.”

3. Assessment with identification of TARGET, image, cognition, emotion, feeling, and core of the EMDR treatment. The therapist, in agreement with the patient, stimulates the primary aspects of memory in a safe and structured way to facilitate access to and processing of the most representative or disturbing event. Two scales are assessed: Validity of Positive Cognition (VoC) and Subjective Units of Distress (SUD).

4. Desensitization and reprocessing, stimulating mnemonic activity to allow the TARGET memory to connect to more adaptive processing, involving all associated channels. Treatment is completed with an SUD score 0.

5. Installation, when the patient fully integrates the positive cognition by linking it to the original TARGET event.

6. Body scan: when the body is free from negative sensations, treatment is completed.

7. Closure.

8. Reassessment: verifies the complete processing of the relevant material and collects the SUD and VoC score.

The hallmark of EMDR therapy protocols is the rhythmic bilateral sensorimotor stimulations in phases 4 and 5, typically performed by the patient following with the gaze the index and middle fingers of the therapist, who rhythmically moves them from left to right at a distance of half a meter in front of the patient. EMDR typically requires 12 sessions to resolve the effects of a single trauma.

FeeSyCy Principle and EMDR Efficacy Mechanisms

The visual system works par excellence governed by the FeeSyCy principle, with motor control being the key to sensory perception (Harris and Wolpert, 1998), as the observation of an even static image, we enact ocular motor programs guided by cognitive-emotional representations developed throughout our lives (Yarbus, 1967). Notably, eye movements are essential, if the ocular saccades are blocked, we are perceptually blind (Alexander and Martinez-Conde, 2019). Thus, to target and unlock brain circuits triggered by traumatic events, EMDR utilizes the most powerful tool of sensorimotor interaction, namely vision.

The strategic function of the gaze for human life is evident when recalling its role in establishing the relationship at the origin of survival, the one between the infant and their mother (Stern, 1990; Winnicot, 2005). The gaze creates an expressive form of communication—long before the baby has access to speech or intentional action, and sustains a pattern of social interactions essential for his/her future relationships (Bowlby, 1958). The role of the gaze can also be recognized in the physiological hierarchy being the last function to be lost, to the extent that even in locked-in patients gaze remains under active control despite the loss of all other motor functions (Yang et al., 1989).

In deepening the important role of bilateral eye movements in the EMDR treatment, we consider here a study that compared the efficacy against PTSD of three procedures (Sack et al., 2016). People either fixed their gaze on the hand of the therapist while it was moving (eye movements), or while it was not (fixed gaze) or had no specific focus (absence of focus). The two conditions of eye movements and fixed gaze both improved PTSD symptoms compared to the absence of focus. Moreover, bilateral rhythmic stimulation (BRHYS) induced by eye movements has higher efficacy than auditory BRHYS (van den Hout et al., 2012).

Plastic adaptive abilities subtending reacquired equilibration of intracerebral functional connectivity in reaction to trauma (Santarnecchi et al., 2019) require a re-balance of the excitatory-inhibitory mechanisms that at the cortical level mediate the acquisition of new skills (Wehr and Zador, 2003; Falkner et al., 2010; Kolasinski et al., 2017), as well as the balance between hemispheric homologs (Meyer et al., 1998). EMDR–typical bilateral rhythmic procedures that are likely to facilitate this re-balancing emergence, enabling a neurobiological condition capable of promoting the integration of traumatic memories at the cortical level (Pagani et al., 2007, 2012; Landin-Romero et al., 2018).

We describe below three main mechanisms indicated as underlying the EMDR procedure, activated by the eyes' rhythmic bilateral sensorimotor stimulations in the Desensitization and Installation phases.

The Orienting Reflex Mechanism

The orienting reflex is a natural response to new environmental stimuli (Donchin, 1981), increasing readiness to respond to danger (Vanderwolf, 1969; Korte et al., 2005). It is typically triggered by sight or hearing and induces the shift of spatial attention in the direction of the new stimulus. Brief alarming stimuli evoke an immediate orienting response, typically characterized by cutaneous vasoconstriction and respiratory activation, which increases sympathetic activity. In the absence of danger, the reaction to novelty, rather than activating the fear response—expressed in the fight, flight, or freeze—relaxes the system. By strengthening the psychotherapist's interaction with the patient within the therapeutic relationship, the BRHYS, typical of the EMDR strategy, supports the reprocessing of the fear response generated by the traumatic memory (de Voogd et al., 2018). In agreement with the FeeSyCy principle, key feedback processing is reactivated within nuclei of the brainstem, cerebral cortex, and regions involved in the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis (Lupien and McEwen, 1997; de Voogd et al., 2018).

The Working Memory Mechanism

Working memory is crucial for information storage and has a task- and time-related limited capacity. When involved in the BRHYS-mediated interaction of the EMDR session during the recall of traumatic memories, the working memory retrieves the traumatic information without the same vividness and this leads to an emotional detachment from the traumatic material (Van Den Hout et al., 2011). In terms of the FeeSyCy principle, in prefrontal cortical neuronal populations, the neural hallmark of working memory, synchronizations induced by bilateral rhythmic stimulations, saturate reverberating synaptic excitatory neuronal activation by inhibiting the delay period activity that supports working memory (Wang, 1999; Grossberg, 2013).

The Slow-Wave Sleep Mechanism

Under normal conditions, during the waking state, information with emotional and episodic values is transferred to the amygdala and hippocampus, respectively, where it is temporarily retained. Among the complex phenomena that sustain memory consolidation during sleep (Klinzing et al., 2019), during the slow-wave sleep (SWS) phase, the short-term memories are transferred back to the neo-cortex where the reverberant activities of thalamic origin favor the epigenetic modifications of neuronal dendrites and synapses that represent the long-term memory engram of this information (Ribeiro et al., 2007; Almeida-Filho et al., 2018). Concurrent to the neo-cortex modifications, amygdala and hippocampus synapses undergo a depotentiation. Upon extreme stress situations, like in PTSD, a maximal potentiation of the amygdala synapses occurs, causing a sort of local short-circuit that prevents the transfer of the emotional memory traced to the cortical areas where integration with the episodic memories coming from the hippocampus takes place. Meaning, the emotional traumatic memory cannot be merged with its autobiographical episodic memory trace (Harper et al., 2009). According to the SWS hypothesis of EMDR, the rhythmic eye movements, while following the therapist's fingers concomitant with the traumatic episodic, recall reproduces the SWS stimulations, reactivating the integration process (Pagani et al., 2017). In terms of the FeeSyCy principle, a systemic feedback mechanism synchronizes circadian control throughout the body, where each cell—with its rhythmicity—in turn, depends on metabolic regulation as a result of nutrient, energy, and redox levels signal sent to the cellular “clock” to reinforce circadian rhythmicity and adapt physiology to tissue-specific time demands (Reinke and Asher, 2019). In the brain, the cortico-thalamic cycles that regulate neuronal activity in support of sleep plasticity have a feedback structure (Contreras et al., 1996).

Discussion

What is expressed here (Figure 1) suggests that, in clinical practice, the EMDR therapist can enhance the Assessment, Desensitization and Reprocessing, and Installation phases by favoring the choice of multisensory inputs with deep evocative power (Silva et al., 2015; Brillantes-Evangelista, 2016; Matthijssen et al., 2017; Gerge et al., 2019) and high level of gratification (Bee et al., 2008; Van Ommeren, 2013; Berliner et al., 2019). The contents of the stimulation will be chosen together with the therapist by the patient according to her/his preferences. These personalized sensorimotor interactions enter, along the same lines drawn by personalized neuromodulation interventions inspired by the FeeSyCy model (Tecchio et al., 2014; Cottone et al., 2017, 2018; Cancelli et al., 2018; Armonaite et al., 2021), in the global approach of precision medicine informed by the individual bio-psycho-social history (Collins and Varmus, 2015).

Author Contributions

GP and FT drafted the paper. MP deepened the EMDR mechanism. GB deepened the EMDR clinical protocol descriptions. LC and MB integrated the clinical perspective. LP contributed in the service development ideas. JG, EG, and TL'A shared the neurophysiology-psychotherapy connection. All authors contributed to the final writing, contributed to the article, and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This study was supported by the POR FESR LAZIO 2014-2020 (Lazio Innova on behalf of Regione Lazio, Gecoweb A0320-2019-28109, cup B89C20001430002), Project n. 28109, Title: Digital Helpers per e-Communities in Sanitá, Acronym: DHelp4H.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Alexander, R. G., and Martinez-Conde, S. (2019). “Fixational eye movements,” in Eye Movement Research: An Introduction to its Scientific Foundations and Applications, eds. C. Klein, and U. Ettinger (Cham: Springer International Publishing), 73–115. doi: 10.1007/978-3-030-20085-5_3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Almeida-Filho, D. G., Queiroz, C. M., and Ribeiro, S. (2018). Memory corticalization triggered by REM sleep: mechanisms of cellular and systems consolidation. Cell. Mol. Life Sci. 75, 3715–3740. doi: 10.1007/s00018-018-2886-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

APA (2022). DSM-5. Available online at: https://psychiatry.org/psychiatrists/practice/dsm (accessed April 27, 2022).

Armonaite, K., Bertoli, M., Paulon, L., Gianni, E., Balsi, M., Conti, L., et al. (2021). Neuronal electrical ongoing activity as cortical areas signature: an insight from mni intracerebral recording atlas. Cereb. Cortex. 2, bhab389. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhab389

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bee, P. E., Bower, P., Lovell, K., Gilbody, S., Richards, D., Gask, L., et al. (2008). Psychotherapy mediated by remote communication technologies: a meta-analytic review. BMC Psychiatry 8, 60. doi: 10.1186/1471-244X-8-60

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berliner, L., Bisson, J., Cloitre, M., Forbes, D., Goldbeck, L., Jensen, T., et al. (2019). PTSD Prevention and Treatment Guidelines. Cham: Springer.

Bowlby, J. (1958). The nature of the child's tie to his mother. Int. J. Psychoanal. 5, 350–373.

PubMed Abstract

Brillantes-Evangelista, G. (2016). Visual Arts and Poetry Usage for PTSD: Considerations for Treatment. Cham: Springer. 1935–1955. doi: 10.1007/978-3-319-08613-2_115-1

CrossRef Full Text

Cancelli, A., Cottone, C., Giordani, A., Migliore, S., Lupoi, D., Porcaro, C., et al. (2018). Personalized, bilateral whole-body somatosensory cortex stimulation to relieve fatigue in multiple sclerosis. Mult. Scler. J. 24, 1366–1374. doi: 10.1177/1352458517720528

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Christiansen, D. M., and Hansen, M. (2015). Accounting for sex differences in PTSD: a multi-variable mediation model. Eur. J. Psychotraumatol. 6, 1–10. doi: 10.3402/ejpt.v6.26068

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Collins, F. S., and Varmus, H. (2015). A new initiative on precision medicine. N. Engl. J. Med. 372, 793–795. doi: 10.1056/NEJMp1500523

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Contreras, D., Destexhe, A., Sejnowski, T. J., and Steriade, M. (1996). Control of spatiotemporal coherence of a thalamic oscillation by corticothalamic feedback. Science 274, 771–774. doi: 10.1126/science.274.5288.771

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cottone, C., Cancelli, A., Pasqualetti, P., Porcaro, C., Salustri, C., and Tecchio, F. (2018). A new, high-efficacy, noninvasive transcranial electric stimulation tuned to local neurodynamics. J. Neurosci. 38, 586–594. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2521-16.2017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cottone, C., Porcaro, C., Cancelli, A., Olejarczyk, E., Salustri, C., and Tecchio, F. (2017). Neuronal electrical ongoing activity is a signature of cortical areas. Brain Struct. Funct. 222, 2115–2126. doi: 10.1007/s00429-016-1328-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Voogd, L. D., Kanen, J. W., Neville, D. A., Roelofs, K., Fernández, G., and Hermans, E. J. (2018). Eye-Movement intervention enhances extinction via amygdala deactivation. J. Neurosci. 38, 8694–8706. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0703-18.2018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Donchin, E. (1981). Surprise!…surprise? Psychophysiology 18, 493–513. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1981.tb01815.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Falkner, A. L., Krishna, B. S., and Goldberg, M. E. (2010). Surround suppression sharpens the priority map in the lateral intraparietal area. J. Neurosci. 30, 12787–12797. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2327-10.2010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Friston, K., and Kiebel, S. (2009). Predictive coding under the free-energy principle. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 364, 1211–1221. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2008.0300

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gerge, A., Hawes, J., Eklöf, L., and Pedersen, I. N. (2019). Proposed mechanisms of change in arts-based psychotherapies. Voices A World Forum Music Ther. 19, 31. doi: 10.15845/voices.v19i2.2564

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grossberg, S. (2013). Adaptive resonance theory: how a brain learns to consciously attend, learn, and recognize a changing world. Neural Netw. 37, 1–47. doi: 10.1016/j.neunet.2012.09.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grujic, N., Brus, J., Burdakov, D., and Polania, R. (2022). Rational inattention in mice. Sci. Adv. 8, eabj8935. doi: 10.1126/sciadv.abj8935

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harper, M. L., Rasolkhani-Kalhorn, T., and Drozd, J. F. (2009). On the neural basis of EMDR therapy: insights from qEEG studies. Traumatology 15, 81–95. doi: 10.1177/1534765609338498

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harris, C. M., and Wolpert, D. M. (1998). Signal-dependent noise determines motor planning. Nature 394, 780–784. doi: 10.1038/29528

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hohwy, J. (2013). The Predictive Mind. Oxford: Oxford University Press. doi: 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199682737.001.0001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kanai, R., Komura, Y., Shipp, S., and Friston, K. (2015). Cerebral hierarchies: predictive processing, precision and the pulvinar. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 370, 20140169. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2014.0169

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kessler, R. C., Aguilar-Gaxiola, S., Alonso, J., Benjet, C., Bromet, E. J., Cardoso, G., et al. (2017). Trauma and PTSD in the WHO world mental health surveys. Eur. J. Psychotraumatol. 8, 1353383. doi: 10.1080/20008198.2017.1353383

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klinzing, J. G., Niethard, N., and Born, J. (2019). Mechanisms of systems memory consolidation during sleep. Nat. Neurosci. 22, 1598–1610. doi: 10.1038/s41593-019-0467-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kolasinski, J., Logan, J. P., Hinson, E. L., Makin, T. R., Emir, U. E., and Stagg Correspondence, C. J. (2017). A mechanistic link from GABA to cortical architecture and perception. Curr. Biol. 27, 1685–1691.e3. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2017.04.055

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Korte, S. M., Koolhaas, J. M., Wingfield, J. C., and McEwen, B. S. (2005). The Darwinian concept of stress: benefits of allostasis and costs of allostatic load and the trade-offs in health and disease. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 29, 3–38. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2004.08.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Landin-Romero, R., Moreno-Alcazar, A., Pagani, M., and Amann, B. L. (2018). How does eye movement desensitization and reprocessing therapy work? A systematic review on suggested mechanisms of action. Front. Psychol. 9, 1395. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01395

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lupien, S. J., and McEwen, B. S. (1997). The acute effects of corticosteroids on cognition: integration of animal and human model studies. Brain Res. Brain Res. Rev. 24, 1–27. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0173(97)00004-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Matthijssen, S. J. M. A., Verhoeven, L. C. M., van den Hout, M. A., and Heitland, I. (2017). Auditory and visual memories in PTSD patients targeted with eye movements and counting: the effect of modality-specific loading of working memory. Front. Psychol. 8, 1937. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01937

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meyer, B. U., Röricht, S., and Woiciechowsky, C. (1998). Topography of fibers in the human corpus callosum mediating interhemispheric inhibition between the motor cortices. Ann. Neurol. 43, 360–369. doi: 10.1002/ana.410430314

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Navarro, P., Hurtado, I., Navarro, P., and Hurtado, I. (2015). “Corporality and trauma,” in Psychopathology Women Incorporating Gender Perspective into Descriptive Psychopathology, ed M. Saenz-Herrero (Cham: Springer), 161–183. doi: 10.1007/978-3-319-05870-2_8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ozer, E. J., Best, S. R., Lipsey, T. L., and Weiss, D. S. (2003). Predictors of posttraumatic stress disorder and symptoms in adults: a meta-analysis. Psychol. Bull. 129, 52–73. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.129.1.52

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pagani, M., Amann, B. L., Landin-Romero, R., and Carletto, S. (2017). Eye movement desensitization and reprocessing and slow wave sleep: a putative mechanism of action. Front. Psychol. 8, 1935. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01935

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pagani, M., Högberg, G., Salmaso, D., Nardo, D., Sundin, Ö., Jonsson, C., et al. (2007). Effects of EMDR psychotherapy on 99mTc-HMPAO distribution in occupation-related post-traumatic stress disorder. Nucl. Med. Commun. 28, 757–765. doi: 10.1097/MNM.0b013e3282742035

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pagani, M., Lorenzo, G., Di Verardo, A. R., Nicolais, G., Monaco, L., Lauretti, G., et al. (2012). Neurobiological Correlates of EMDR Monitoring – An EEG Study. PLoS One 7, e45753. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0045753

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reinke, H., and Asher, G. (2019). Crosstalk between metabolism and circadian clocks. Nat. Rev. Mol. Cell Biol. 20, 227–241. doi: 10.1038/s41580-018-0096-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ressler, K. J., Berretta, S., Bolshakov, V. Y., Rosso, I. M., Meloni, E. G., Rauch, S. L., et al. (2022). Post-traumatic stress disorder: clinical and translational neuroscience from cells to circuits. Nat. Rev. Neurol. 18, 273–288. doi: 10.1038/s41582-022-00635-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ribeiro, S., Shi, X., Engelhard, M., Zhou, Y., Zhang, H., Gervasoni, D., et al. (2007). Novel experience induces persistent sleep-dependent plasticity in the cortex but not in the hippocampus. Front. Neurosci. 1, 43–55. doi: 10.3389/neuro.01.1.1.003.2007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sack, M., Zehl, S., Otti, A., Lahmann, C., Henningsen, P., Kruse, J., et al. (2016). A comparison of dual attention, eye movements, and exposure only during eye movement desensitization and reprocessing for posttraumatic stress disorder: results from a randomized clinical trial. Psychother. Psychosom. 85, 357–365. doi: 10.1159/000447671

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Santarnecchi, E., Bossini, L., Vatti, G., Fagiolini, A., Porta, P., La, Di Lorenzo, G., et al. (2019). Psychological and brain connectivity changes following trauma-focused CBT and EMDR treatment in single-episode PTSD patients. Front. Psychol. 10, 129. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00129

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shapiro, F. (2001). Eye Movement Desensitization and Reprocessing—Basic Principles, Protocols, and Procedures. New York, NY: Guilford.

Google Scholar

Sherin, J. E., and Nemeroff, C. B. (2011). Post-traumatic stress disorder: the neurobiological impact of psychological trauma. Dialogues Clin. Neurosci. 13, 263–278. doi: 10.31887/DCNS.2011.13.2/jsherin

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Silva, V. F., da, Ribeiro, A. P., Santos, V. A., dos, Nardi, A. E., King, A. L. S., and Calomeni, M. R. (2015). Stimulation by light and sound: therapeutics effects in humans. Systematic review. Clin. Pract. Epidemiol. Ment. Health 11, 150. doi: 10.2174/1745017901511010150

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stern, D. N. (1990). Diary of a Baby. What Your Child Sees, Feels, and Experiences. New York, NY: Basic Books.

Google Scholar

Tecchio, F., Bertoli, M., Gianni, E., L'Abbate, T., Paulon, L., and Zappasodi, F. (2020). To be is to become. Fractal neurodynamics of the body-brain control system. Front. Physiol. 11, 609768. doi: 10.3389/fphys.2020.609768

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tecchio, F., Cancelli, A., Cottone, C., Zito, G., Pasqualetti, P., Ghazaryan, A., et al. (2014). Multiple sclerosis fatigue relief by bilateral somatosensory cortex neuromodulation. J. Neurol. 261, 1552–1558. doi: 10.1007/s00415-014-7377-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Den Hout, M. A., Engelhard, I. M., Beetsma, D., Slofstra, C., Hornsveld, H., Houtveen, J., et al. (2011). EMDR and mindfulness. Eye movements and attentional breathing tax working memory and reduce vividness and emotionality of aversive ideation. J. Behav. Ther. Exp. Psychiatry 42, 423–431. doi: 10.1016/j.jbtep.2011.03.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van den Hout, M. A., Rijkeboer, M. M., Engelhard, I. M., Klugkist, I., Hornsveld, H., Toffolo, M. J. B., et al. (2012). Tones inferior to eye movements in the EMDR treatment of PTSD. Behav. Res. Ther. 50, 275–279. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2012.02.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Ommeren, M. (2013). Guidelines for the Management of Conditions Specifically Related to Stress i WHO Library Cataloguing-in-Publication Data. Available online at: www.who.int (accessed November 11, 2021).

Google Scholar

Vance, M. C., Kovachy, B., Dong, M., and Bui, E. (2018). Peritraumatic distress: a review and synthesis of 15 years of research. J. Clin. Psychol. 74, 1457–1484. doi: 10.1002/jclp.22612

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vanderwolf, C. H. (1969). Hippocampal electrical activity and voluntary movement in the rat. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 26, 407–418. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(69)90092-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vermetten, E., and Lanius, R. A. (2012). “Biological and clinical framework for posttraumatic stress disorder,” in Handbook of Clinical Neurology, eds M. J. Aminoff, F. Boller, and D. F. Swaab (London: Elsevier), 291–342. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-444-52002-9.00018-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, X. J. (1999). Synaptic basis of cortical persistent activity: the importance of NMDA receptors to working memory. J. Neurosci. 19, 9587–9603. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-21-09587.1999

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wehr, M., and Zador, A. M. (2003). Balanced inhibition underlies tuning and sharpens spike timing in auditory cortex. Nature 426, 442–446. doi: 10.1038/nature02116

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Winnicot, D. W. (2005). Playing and Reality. London: Routledge.

World Health Organization. (2013). Guidelines for the Management of Conditions Specifically Related to Stress. Geneva: WHO Library Cataloguing-in-Publication Data.

Google Scholar

Yang, C. C. S., Lieberman, J. S., and Hong, C. Z. (1989). Early smooth horizontal eye movement: A favorable prognostic sign in patients with locked-in syndrome. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 70, 230–232. doi: 10.1016/S0003-9993(21)01692-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yarbus, A. L. (1967). Eye Movements and Vision. Boston, MA: Springer US. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4899-5379-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD), visual system, sensorimotor interaction, feedback-synchrony-plasticity (FeeSyCy), eye movement desensitization and reprocessing (EMDR)

Citation: Persichilli G, Grifoni J, Pagani M, Bertoli M, Gianni E, L'Abbate T, Cerniglia L, Bevacqua G, Paulon L and Tecchio F (2022) Sensorimotor Interaction Against Trauma. Front. Neurosci. 16:913410. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2022.913410

Received: 05 April 2022; Accepted: 18 May 2022;
Published: 14 June 2022.

Edited by:

Michela Chiappalone, Italian Institute of Technology (IIT), Italy

Reviewed by:

Vinícius Rosa Cota, Italian Institute of Technology (IIT), Italy

Copyright © 2022 Persichilli, Grifoni, Pagani, Bertoli, Gianni, L'Abbate, Cerniglia, Bevacqua, Paulon and Tecchio. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Franca Tecchio, franca.tecchio@cnr.it; orcid.org/0000-0002-1325-5059

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.