Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Neurosci., 04 July 2022
Sec. Neurodegeneration

Brain Amyloid Index as a Probable Marker Bridging Between Subjective Memory Complaint and Objective Cognitive Performance

\r\nYoung Min Choe,Young Min Choe1,2Guk-Hee Suh,Guk-Hee Suh1,2Boung Chul Lee,Boung Chul Lee2,3Ihn-Geun Choi,Ihn-Geun Choi2,4Jun Ho LeeJun Ho Lee5Hyun Soo KimHyun Soo Kim6Jaeuk HwangJaeuk Hwang7Jee Wook Kim,*Jee Wook Kim1,2*
  • 1Department of Neuropsychiatry, Hallym University Dongtan Sacred Heart Hospital, Hwaseong, South Korea
  • 2Department of Psychiatry, Hallym University College of Medicine, Chuncheon, South Korea
  • 3Department of Neuropsychiatry, Hallym University Hangang Sacred Heart Hospital, Seoul, South Korea
  • 4Department of Psychiatry, Seoul W Psychiatric Office, Seoul, South Korea
  • 5Department of Neuropsychiatry, Seoul National University Hospital, Seoul, South Korea
  • 6Department of Laboratory Medicine, Hallym University Dongtan Sacred Heart Hospital, Hwaseong, South Korea
  • 7Department of Psychiatry, Soonchunhyang University Hospital, Seoul, South Korea

Background: The association between types of subjective memory complaint (SMC), poor objective cognitive performance, and brain Aβ deposition have been poorly understood. We investigated the association between types of SMC and objective global cognitive performance, then assessed whether this association is mediated by the brain amyloid prediction index (API).

Methods: In total, 173 non-demented older adults [63 cognitively normal (CN) and 110 mild cognitive impairment (MCI)] underwent comprehensive clinical assessments. Objective global cognitive performance and brain amyloid index were measured using the total score (TS) of the Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer’s Disease neuropsychological battery and API, respectively. In total, four items of SMC from the subjective memory complaints questionnaire (SMCQ) (SMCQ1: a feeling of memory problem; SMCQ2: the feeling of worse memory than 10 years ago; SMCQ3: the feeling of worse memory than others of similar age; or SMCQ4: the feeling of difficulty in everyday life) in global memory function were assessed.

Results: In non-demented and participants with MCI, SMCQ3-positive and SMCQ4-positive groups were associated with decreased TS. In participants with MCI, the SMCQ3-positive group was associated with increased API, and API was associated with decreased TS, but the SMCQ4-positive group did not. In addition, the association between the SMCQ3-positive group and poor TS disappeared when API was controlled as a covariate, indicating that API has a mediation effect.

Conclusion: The present findings suggest that SMC, a feeling of worse memory performance than others in a similar age group, in the older adults without dementia is associated with poor objective cognitive performance via increased brain amyloid index.

Introduction

Subjective memory complaint (SMC) is common in older adults (Jonker et al., 2000; Ramlall et al., 2013), and its prevalence varies from 25 to 50% in community-based studies (O’Connor et al., 1990; Gagnon et al., 1994; Tobiansky et al., 1995; Jonker et al., 1996; Blazer et al., 1997). SMC is associated with several biological and environmental markers including depression (Geerlings et al., 1999; Stewart et al., 2001; Minett et al., 2005; Jessen et al., 2014b), old age (Geerlings et al., 1999; Jessen et al., 2014b), sex (Gillett et al., 2003; Tomita et al., 2014), education (van Oijen et al., 2007; Alagoa Joao et al., 2016), vascular risk (Paradise et al., 2011; Sajjad et al., 2015), and apolipoprotein E (APOE) ε4 (Stewart et al., 2001; Rowe et al., 2010; Jessen et al., 2014b). More importantly, SMC makes older adults worry a lot about Alzheimer’s disease (AD), and there seems to be a clear link between SMC and AD or related cognitive decline.

A growing body of evidence suggests that SMC is a risk factor for AD or related cognitive impairment (Commissaris et al., 1994, 1998; Winblad et al., 2004; Reid and Maclullich, 2006; Reisberg et al., 2008; Jessen et al., 2010, 2014a,2014b; Choe et al., 2018; Janssen et al., 2021; Tort-Merino et al., 2021). In particular, SMC in cognitive normal (CN) state, i.e., subjective cognitive decline (SCD), has been proposed as an early symptom of AD, prior to objective cognitive dysfunction (Winblad et al., 2004; Reid and Maclullich, 2006; Reisberg et al., 2008; Jessen et al., 2010, 2014a,2014b). As brain amyloid-beta (Aβ) deposition is the earliest key pathology of AD (Braak and Braak, 1991; Tobiansky et al., 1995; Jack et al., 2010), SCD may be the first symptomatic manifestation of brain Aβ deposition. Unlike SMC in normal cognition, SMC is an essential component in mild cognitive impairment (MCI), classified as a transitional state between normal aging and dementia (Petersen et al., 1999). As MCI is an obvious risk factor for AD due to the high risk of annual conversion from MCI to AD (Manly et al., 2008; Petersen et al., 2009), MCI has become a critical pre-AD stage for understanding the pathology of AD.

Despite the clinical significance of SMC in preclinical and prodromal AD, its clinical application had been limited in that SMC was not standardized and varied depending on the SMC types. An international working group on the subjective decline in cognition addressed this limitation and presented the characteristics that increase the risk of AD based on the evidence of previous studies (Jessen et al., 2014a). These include a subjective decline in memory, rather than other domains of cognition; concerns (worries) associated with subjective decline (Jessen et al., 2010); and the feeling of worse performance than others of the same age group (Amariglio et al., 2012; Perrotin et al., 2012). However, given the diversity of SMC (Geerlings et al., 1999; Youn et al., 2009; Jessen et al., 2010, 2014a; Amariglio et al., 2012; Perrotin et al., 2012; Choe et al., 2018; Kim et al., 2019), it is not known which types of SMC are associated with AD-related cognitive decline, and whether Aβ deposition mediate the association between SMC and objective cognitive impairment. Taken together, the association between SMC types, poor objective cognitive performance, and brain Aβ deposition have been poorly understood.

Cerebral Aβ deposition is the hallmark pathologic biomarker in AD, and amyloid positron emission tomography (PET) imaging has made it possible to detect cerebral Aβ pathology in living human brains (Klunk et al., 2004). However, due to the high cost and concern about radiation hazards, the application of amyloid PET imaging is still limited in clinical and research settings. Therefore, we developed amyloid prediction index (API) (Lee et al., 2018) derived from commonly available variables including age, sex, education, hypertension, apolipoprotein ε4 (APOE4) positivity, and memory recall score, in memory clinic practice using the data from the Korean Brain Ageing Study for the Early diagnosis and prediction of Alzheimer’s disease (KBASE) cohort (Byun et al., 2017). API showed excellent capability (area under the curve = 0.873) in screening cognitively impaired individuals with cerebral amyloid positivity. API was also externally validated using an independent dataset from the Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative 2 (ADNI-2) (Petersen et al., 2010), supporting the general applicability of the API.

We first examined the associations of SMC types with objective cognitive performance in non-demented older adults. Regarding SMC types, we selected four items of SMCQ (having a memory problem, memory worsening than 10 years ago, memory worsening than that of others of a similar age, and difficulty in everyday life due to memory decline) designed to assess global memory function from Subjective Memory Complaints Questionnaire (SMCQ) (Youn et al., 2009) because subjective metacognition about global memory function would reflect an early pathological process of AD rather than those items of specific everyday dysfunction. In addition, we examined whether the association of the SMC type(s) with objective cognitive performance is affected by brain amyloid index, i.e., API, which is significantly correlated with SMC type(s) in older adults with MCI.

Materials and Methods

Participants

This study was performed as part of the General Lifestyle and AD (GLAD) study, an ongoing prospective cohort study that began in 2020. As of February 2022, 173 individuals (63 adults with CN, and 110 adults with MCI), between 65 and 90 years of age have been enrolled in the GLAD study. Participants were recruited from the community and one recruitment site near Hwaseong, South Korea. Potentially eligible individuals who participated in a dementia screening program at the memory clinic of one university hospital (Hallym University Dongtan Sacred Heart Hospital, Hwaseong, South Korea) were informed about study participation; individuals who volunteered were invited for an assessment of eligibility. In addition, volunteers from the community were recruited through recommendations by other participants, family members, friends, or acquaintances.

For the GLAD study, the cognitive normal group consisted of participants with a Clinical Dementia Rating (Morris, 1993) score of 0 and no diagnosis of MCI or dementia. All participants with MCI met the current consensus criteria for amnestic MCI: memory complaints confirmed by an informant; objective memory impairments; preservation of global cognitive function; independence in functional activities; and absence of dementia. Regarding objective memory impairment, the age-, education-, and sex-adjusted z-score was < –1.0 for at least one of four episodic memory tests: Word List Memory, Word List Recall, Word List Recognition, and Constructional Recall tests included in the Korean version of the Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer’s Disease Assessment Packet (CERAD-K) neuropsychological battery (Morris et al., 1989; Lee et al., 2002, 2004). All participants with MCI had a Clinical Dementia Rating score of 0.5. The exclusion criteria were as follows: presence of a major psychiatric illness and/or significant neurological or medical condition or comorbidity that could affect mental functioning; illiteracy; the presence of significant visual/hearing difficulties and/or severe communication or behavioral problems that would make clinical examinations difficult; and, use of an investigational drug.

This study protocol was approved by the institutional review board of the Hallym University Dongtan Sacred Heart Hospital, it was conducted in accordance with the recommendations of the Declaration of Helsinki. All the participants or their legal representatives provided written informed consent.

Clinical Assessments

All the participants underwent standardized clinical assessments by trained psychiatrists based on the GLAD study clinical assessment protocol, which incorporated the Korean version of the CERAD-K (Morris et al., 1989; Lee et al., 2002). A CERAD total score (TS) was generated by summing the scores of six tests in the CERAD neuropsychological battery including the Verbal Fluency, modified Boston Naming Test, Word List Memory, Constructional Praxis, Word List Recall (WLR), and Word List Recognition tests (Seo et al., 2010). The TS was selected as a measure of objective global cognitive function. In addition, TS–WLR was calculated by subtracting the WLR score from TS in order to exclude the effect of WLR.

Assessment of Subjective Memory Complaint

Four items of SMCs for each participant were assessed based on the answer to the following question on global memory function from the Subjective Memory Complaints Questionnaire (Youn et al., 2009) assessment: SMCQ1: “Do you think that you have a memory problem?”; SMCQ2: “Do you think that your memory is worse than 10 years ago?”; SMCQ3: “Do you think that your memory is worse than that of other people of a similar age?” (Choe et al., 2018); SMCQ4: “Do you feel that your everyday life is difficult due to memory decline?”. The response was recorded as “yes” or “no” to enhance the feasibility and reliability of the item for older adults.

Measurement of the Amyloid Prediction Index

We measured the API (Lee et al., 2018) for each participant with MCI with commonly available variables, i.e., age, sex, years of education, history of hypertension, APOE4 positivity, and WLR score, in memory clinic practice. The API was defined as the probability for a participant to be amyloid-positive, calculated using the logit value derived from the multivariate logistic regression model for each participant; the details of the calculation on API were described previously (Lee et al., 2018). API derived from this model was proven to be valid across the two cohorts. The area under the curve was 0.873 (95% CI = 0.815–0.918) in the KBASE cohort (Byun et al., 2017), it was 0.808 (95% CI = 0.769–0.842) in the ADNI-2 cohort (Petersen et al., 2010).

Assessment of Potential Confounders

All participants were evaluated to identify potential confounders, such as depression, vascular risk, body mass index, APOE4 positivity, serum fasting glucose, alcohol intake, and smoking. We used the Geriatric Depression Scale (GDS) (Yesavage et al., 1982; Kim et al., 2008) to measure the severity of depressive symptoms. The comorbidity of vascular risks (e.g., hypertension and diabetes mellitus) was assessed based on data collected by a trained researcher during systematic interviews of participants and their informants. The body mass index was calculated by dividing the weight in kilograms by the square of the height in meters. Lifetime alcohol intake status (never/former/drinker) and smoking status (never/ex-smoker/smoker) were also evaluated through trained researcher interviews and medical record reviews. To acquire accurate information, reliable informants were interviewed. After an overnight fast, blood samples were obtained by venipuncture in the morning (8–9 a.m.). Glucose levels were measured using a COBAS c702 analyzer and dedicated reagents (Roche Diagnostics, Germany). Apolipoprotein E was genotyped using the Seeplex ApoE ACE genotyping kit (Seegene, South Korea). APOE4-positivity was defined as the presence of at least one ε4 allele.

Statistical Analyses

To examine the associations between SMC and TS, we performed multiple linear regression analyses. For these analyses, we used SMC groups (based on every single SMC item) as the independent variable. The association between SMC and TS may be influenced by various factors. Therefore, we systematically evaluated all the participants to identify potential confounders, such as age, sex, education, depression, APOE4, clinical diagnosis, hypertension, diabetes mellitus, body mass index status, alcohol intake, smoking, and serum fasting glucose. We tested three models to control the covariates in a stepwise manner. The first model included age, sex, education, and GDS status as covariates; the second model included the covariates in the first model plus APOE4, clinical diagnosis, hypertension, and diabetes mellitus; the third model included the covariates in the second model plus body mass index status, alcohol intake, smoking, and serum fasting glucose. We investigated the mediation effects of API on the associations between SMC groups and TS. We first conducted the same regression analyses for the associations between SMC groups and API, then for the associations between SMC groups and TS. We also analyzed the associations between SMC groups and TS using the same regression model while controlling for API as an additional covariate. Referring to the issues of API including objective performance, we performed the same additional analyses in which WLR was used as an additional covariate and in which TS was replaced by TS–WLR in order to exclude the effect of WLR as a component overlaps with TS and API. Referring to the issues of multiple comparisons, the Bonferroni correction method was used for the association of SMC types with TS or API by using a p-value < 0.05/the number of SMC types (= 4) as a threshold for statistical significance. P-values < 0.05 were considered statistically significant when not otherwise specified. All statistical analyses were performed using IBM SPSS Statistics 27 software (IBM, Armonk, NY, United States).

Results

Participant Characteristics

Table 1 presents the demographic and clinical characteristics of the overall participants. Among all participants, 131 (75.72%), 148 (85.55%), 84 (48.55%), and 46 (26.29%) were categorized into the SMCQ1-positive, SMCQ2-positive, SMCQ3-positive, and SMCQ4-positive groups, respectively. Supplementary Tables S1–S3 summarized the demographic and clinical characteristics of overall, participants with MCI and CN according to the SMCQ3 groups, respectively.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Overall participant characteristics.

Association Between Subjective Memory Complaint and Total Score

In non-demented and participants with MCI, SMCQ3- and SMCQ4-positive groups were significantly associated with worse TS compared with those negative groups, respectively (Table 2 and Figures 1A,B). In contrast, no group differences were observed in TS between SMCQ1 and SMCQ2 groups (Table 2).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Results of multiple linear regression analyses for the associations of SMC with objective cognitive performance in CN, MCI, and non-demented older adults.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Plots of the associations among SMC (i.e., SMCQ3), API, and TS: (A) SMC vs. TS for non-demented participants, (B) SMC vs. TS for participants with MCI, (C) SMC vs. API for participants with MCI, and (D) API vs. TS for participants with MCI. SMC, subjective memory complaint; API, amyloid prediction index; TS, total score of the Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer’s Disease neuropsychological battery; MCI, mild cognitive impairment. For panels (A–C), box plot controlling for all potential covariates. For panel (D), partial regression plot controlling for all potential covariates are presented.

Association Subjective Memory Complaint, Amyloid Prediction Index, and Total Score

Subjective memory complaints questionnaire 3 positive group alone was significantly associated with increased API in the participants with MCI compared with the SMCQ3-negative group (Table 3 and Figure 1C). In contrast, no group differences were observed in API between the SMCQ1, SMCQ2, and SMCQ4 groups (Table 3). API was significantly associated with decreased TS in the participants with MCI (Table 4 and Figure 1D).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Results of multiple linear regression analyses for the associations of SMC with objective cognitive performance and API in older adults with MCI.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Results of multiple linear regression analyses for the associations of API with objective cognitive performance in older adults with MCI.

Effects of Amyloid Prediction Index on the Association Between Subjective Memory Complaint and Total Score

The association between the SMCQ3-positive group and poor TS disappeared in participants with MCI when API was controlled as a covariate, indicating that API has a mediation effect (Table 5). In contrast, there was no mediating effect of API on the association between other SMCQ-positive groups and TS (Table 5).

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Results of multiple linear regression analyses for the associations of SMC with objective cognitive performance and API in older adults with MCI.

Association Among Subjective Memory Complaint, Amyloid Prediction Index, and Total Score With the Independent Effect of Word List Recall

Assuming that the API mediates the association SMCQ3-positive group and TS, it is necessary to exclude the effect of WLR as a component overlaps with TS and API. To demonstrate the independent effect of WLR, we performed the same additional analyses in which WLR was used as an additional covariate and obtained similar results (Supplementary Tables S4–S6). In addition, we performed the same additional analyses in which TS was replaced by TS–WLR and obtained similar results (Supplementary Tables S7–S9).

Discussion

The result of this study revealed that SMC, defined as a feeling of worse memory performance than others in a similar age group, was significantly associated with a worse objective cognitive performance or brain amyloid index (which are measures of API). In particular, the brain amyloid index had a significant mediation effect on the association between SMC and worse objective cognitive performance. To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study to reveal the mediation effect of brain amyloid index on the association between SMC and objective cognitive dysfunction.

The present study found that SMCQ3 among four SMCQ items is associated with objective global cognitive performance, i.e., TS, via increased brain amyloid index, but other SMCQ items did not show the association between subjective complaints, API, and global cognitive decline. As mentioned above, we first assumed that subjective metacognition about global memory function would reflect an early pathological process of AD rather than those items of specific everyday dysfunction, so selected four items of SMCQ were designed to assess global memory function. Among the four SMCQ items, the SMCQ1, “Do you think that you have a memory problem?” did not yield any significant results because of ambiguity and uncertainty, and the SMCQ2, “Do you think that your memory is worse than 10 years ago?”, did not yield any significant results due to the possibility of false positive, being confused with the normal aging process. The SMCQ4, “Do you feel that your everyday life is difficult due to memory decline?”, did not show the association due to its association with impairment of activities of daily living (ADL) in AD rather than MCI. Finally, given less likelihood to confuse ADL impairment or concerns for false positive, we speculated that SMCQ3 would be a more suitable item to provide the information on SMC in the present study. In addition, the international SCD working group supported our finding that SMCQ3 was more informative predictor for objective cognitive performance via increased brain amyloid index. They acknowledged the diversity of SMC and suggested AD-related key characteristics of subjective decline (Jessen et al., 2014a), such as subjective memory decline and the feeling of worse performance than others of the same age group (Amariglio et al., 2012; Perrotin et al., 2012), which is similar to the SMCQ3 in the present study.

The finding concerning the association between SMC and objective cognitive performance is consistent with the results of three cohort studies involving non-demented subjects: the Paquid study (Gagnon et al., 1994), the Eastern Baltimore Mental Health Survey (Bassett and Folstein, 1993), and the Berkeley Aging Cohort Study (Vogel et al., 2017). The Paquid cohort (Gagnon et al., 1994) showed that participants with SMC had lower objective cognitive performance than did non-complainers. The Eastern Baltimore Mental Health Survey (Bassett and Folstein, 1993) indicated that 29% of subjective complainers were cognitively impaired, compared with 15% of the non-complainers. The Berkeley Aging Cohort Study (Vogel et al., 2017) revealed that SMC predicted a faster decline in memory performance independently of amyloid-positivity status and interacted with amyloid-positivity status to predict change in memory and global cognition over 4 years. However, the Hughes Hall Project (O’Connor et al., 1990) found no association between SMC and objective performance. These conflicting findings have led to difficulty assessing the association between SMC and objective performance; differences among studies are presumably related to their different types of dementia and distinct definitions of cognitive impairment (O’Connor et al., 1990; Reid and Maclullich, 2006).

Our finding concerning the association between SMC and brain amyloid index is consistent with the results of several studies on SMC–Aβ association using amyloid PET imaging or plasma Aβ measurements in non-demented individuals (Amariglio et al., 2012; Perrotin et al., 2012; Kryscio et al., 2014; Cantero et al., 2016; Kim et al., 2019; Hong et al., 2021; Janssen et al., 2021; Tort-Merino et al., 2021). Although some studies using amyloid PET imaging did not identify a significant SMC–Aβ association (Chetelat et al., 2010; Rodda et al., 2010), these reported a trend toward an SMC–Aβ association (Chetelat et al., 2010; Rodda et al., 2010). These findings suggests that SMC is a risk factor for AD or related cognitive impairment (Commissaris et al., 1994, 1998; Winblad et al., 2004; Reid and Maclullich, 2006; Reisberg et al., 2008; Jessen et al., 2010, 2014a,2014b; Choe et al., 2018; Janssen et al., 2021; Tort-Merino et al., 2021) because Aβ pathology is the earliest key pathology of AD (Braak and Braak, 1991; Tobiansky et al., 1995; Jack et al., 2010). In addition, Aβ may show a mediation effect on the association between SMC and AD or related cognitive impairment.

Strengths and Limitations

The present study provides the following novel findings. First, we investigated the mediation effect of brain amyloid index and the association between SMC and objective cognitive performance in older adults with MCI. In particular, few studies have investigated their association in older adults with MCI, rather than in CN older adults. Second, to draw a more general conclusion about the association between these factors, we used TS as an indicator of objective cognitive performance, instead of individual cognitive tests that reflect only specific aspects of cognitive performance. Third, we extensively evaluated potential confounders and controlled for them in our statistical models to ensure clarity in our investigations of associations between SMC and objective cognitive performance or brain amyloid index. Nevertheless, this study has some limitations. First, it was a cross-sectional study, which led to difficulty in establishing causal relationships on the base of our findings. To elucidate the nature of the associations among SMC, objective cognitive performance, and brain amyloid deposition, further longitudinal follow-up studies are necessary. Second, we evaluated the API in older adults with MCI using a probability model, instead of brain amyloid PET images. As cognitively intact older adults do not show cognitive impairment and are less likely to have brain amyloid deposition compared with cognitively impaired older adults, it may be difficult to find the association among SMC, API, and cognition. Therefore, this finding is relatively useful for older adults with MCI but not CN. In addition, according to the previous study (Lee et al., 2018), API was defined to be used in cognitively impaired older adults, not in cognitively intact older adults, so it was applied only to MCI older adults in the present study. To exclude the effect of WLR as a component overlaps with TS and API, we performed the same additional analyses in which WLR was used as an additional covariate (Supplementary Tables S3–S5) and in which TS was replaced by TS–WLR (Supplementary Tables S6–S8). These additional analyses showed similar results, respectively, and support that our findings on the association between SMC, API, and TS remain independent of the effect of WLR overlapped with API and TS. Nevertheless, complete elimination of the effects of objective cognitive performance on the association between API and TS was limited because the association could be in part driven by the fact that both include objective cognitive performance. Therefore, the present findings could proceed with the research involving direct assessment of brain Aβ deposition, which has been difficult to perform due to high cost or concern about radiation hazards and will clarify the mechanisms underlying the association between SMC and objective cognitive performance. At last, there was a possibility of a crossover between the SMC types. In the broad framework of SMC, cross-over between each SMC type may be bound to be high. This possibility may call into question the classification approach. It may be necessary to classify the SMC types by fully considering these issues in future studies.

Conclusion

The present findings suggest that SMC, a feeling of worse memory performance than others in a similar age group, in older adults without dementia is associated with poor objective cognitive performance via increased brain amyloid index.

Data Availability Statement

The study data are not freely accessible because the IRB of the Hallym University Dongtan Sacred Heart Hospital prevents public sharing of such data for privacy reasons. However, the data are available on reasonable request after IRB approval. Requests for data access can be submitted to an independent administrative coordinator by e-mail eW9vbjQ2NDVAZ21haWwuY29t.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Hallym University Dongtan Sacred Heart Hospital. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

JK conceived and designed the study. YC, G-HS, BL, I-GC, JL, HK, JH, and JK were involved in the acquisition, or analysis, and interpretation of the data and helped to draft the manuscript. JK, YC, G-HS, BL, I-GC, JL, HK, and JH were major contributors to writing the manuscript and critically revising the manuscript for intellectual content. JK served as principal investigator and supervised the study. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Funding

This study was supported by grants from the Hallym University Research Fund (grant no. HURF-2020-56), the Whanin Pharm. Co., Ltd. (grant no. 7S200401366S000100), and the Ministry of Science and ICT, South Korea (grant no. NRF-2020R1G1A1099652). The funding sources had no role in the study design, data collection, data analysis, data interpretation, writing of the manuscript, or decision to submit it for publication.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Acknowledgments

We thank the GLAD study participants and their caregivers.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnins.2022.912891/full#supplementary-material

References

Alagoa Joao, A., Maroco, J., Gino, S., Mendes, T., de Mendonca, A., and Martins, I. P. (2016). Education modifies the type of subjective memory complaints in older people. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 31, 153–160. doi: 10.1002/gps.4305

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Amariglio, R. E., Becker, J. A., Carmasin, J., Wadsworth, L. P., Lorius, N., Sullivan, C., et al. (2012). Subjective cognitive complaints and amyloid burden in cognitively normal older individuals. Neuropsychologia 50, 2880–2886. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.08.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bassett, S. S., and Folstein, M. F. (1993). Memory complaint, memory performance, and psychiatric diagnosis: a community study. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry Neurol. 6, 105–111. doi: 10.1177/089198879300600207

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blazer, D. G., Hays, J. C., Fillenbaum, G. G., and Gold, D. T. (1997). Memory complaint as a predictor of cognitive decline: a comparison of African American and White elders. J. Aging Health 9, 171–184. doi: 10.1177/089826439700900202

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Braak, H., and Braak, E. (1991). Neuropathological stageing of Alzheimer-related changes. Acta Neuropathol. 82, 239–259. doi: 10.1007/BF00308809

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Byun, M. S., Yi, D., Lee, J. H., Choe, Y. M., Sohn, B. K., Lee, J. Y., et al. (2017). Korean Brain Aging Study for the Early Diagnosis and Prediction of Alzheimer’s Disease: methodology and baseline sample characteristics. Psychiatry Investig. 14, 851–863. doi: 10.4306/pi.2017.14.6.851

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cantero, J. L., Iglesias, J. E., Van Leemput, K., and Atienza, M. (2016). Regional Hippocampal Atrophy and Higher Levels of Plasma Amyloid-Beta Are Associated With Subjective Memory Complaints in Nondemented Elderly Subjects. J. Gerontol. Ser. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 71, 1210–1215. doi: 10.1093/gerona/glw022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chetelat, G., Villemagne, V. L., Bourgeat, P., Pike, K. E., Jones, G., Ames, D., et al. (2010). Relationship between atrophy and beta-amyloid deposition in Alzheimer disease. Ann. Neurol. 67, 317–324.

Google Scholar

Choe, Y. M., Byun, M. S., Lee, J. H., Sohn, B. K., Lee, D. Y., and Kim, J. W. (2018). Subjective memory complaint as a useful tool for the early detection of Alzheimer’s disease. Neuropsychiatr. Dis. Treat. 14, 2451–2460. doi: 10.2147/NDT.S174517

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Commissaris, C. J., Ponds, R. W., and Jolles, J. (1998). Subjective forgetfulness in a normal Dutch population: possibilities for health education and other interventions. Patient Educ. Couns. 34, 25–32. doi: 10.1016/s0738-3991(98)00040-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Commissaris, C. J., Verhey, F. R. Jr., Ponds, R. W., Jolles, J., and Kok, G. J. (1994). Public education about normal forgetfulness and dementia: importance and effects. Patient Educ. Couns. 24, 109–115. doi: 10.1016/0738-3991(94)90004-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gagnon, M., Dartigues, J. F., Mazaux, J. M., Dequae, L., Letenneur, L., Giroire, J. M., et al. (1994). Self-reported memory complaints and memory performance in elderly French community residents: results of the PAQUID Research Program. Neuroepidemiology 13, 145–154. doi: 10.1159/000110373

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Geerlings, M. I., Jonker, C., Bouter, L. M., Ader, H. J., and Schmand, B. (1999). Association between memory complaints and incident Alzheimer’s disease in elderly people with normal baseline cognition. Am. J. Psychiatry 156, 531–537.

Google Scholar

Gillett, M. J., Martins, R. N., Clarnette, R. M., Chubb, S. A., Bruce, D. G., and Yeap, B. B. (2003). Relationship between testosterone, sex hormone binding globulin and plasma amyloid beta peptide 40 in older men with subjective memory loss or dementia. J. Alzheimers Dis. 5, 267–269. doi: 10.3233/JAD-2003-5401

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hong, Y. J., Park, J. W., Lee, S. B., Kim, S.-H., Kim, Y., Ryu, D.-W., et al. (2021). The Influence of Amyloid Burden on Cognitive Decline over 2 years in Older Adults with Subjective Cognitive Decline: a Prospective Cohort Study. Dement. Geriatr. Cogn. Disord. 50, 437–445. doi: 10.1159/000519766

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jack, C. R. Jr., Knopman, D. S., Jagust, W. J., Shaw, L. M., Aisen, P. S., Weiner, M. W., et al. (2010). Hypothetical model of dynamic biomarkers of the Alzheimer’s pathological cascade. Lancet Neurol. 9, 119–128. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(09)70299-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Janssen, O., Jansen, W. J., Vos, S. J. B., Boada, M., Parnetti, L., Gabryelewicz, T., et al. (2021). Characteristics of subjective cognitive decline associated with amyloid positivity. Alzheimers Dement. [Epub ahead of print]. doi: 10.1002/alz.12512

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jessen, F., Wolfsgruber, S., Wiese, B., Bickel, H., Mösch, E., Kaduszkiewicz, H., et al. (2014b). AD dementia risk in late MCI, in early MCI, and in subjective memory impairment. Alzheimers Dement. 10, 76–83. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2012.09.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jessen, F., Amariglio, R. E., van Boxtel, M., Breteler, M., Ceccaldi, M., Chételat, G., et al. (2014a). A conceptual framework for research on subjective cognitive decline in preclinical Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement. 10, 844–852. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2014.01.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jessen, F., Wiese, B., Bachmann, C., Eifflaender-Gorfer, S., Haller, F., Kölsch, H., et al. (2010). Prediction of dementia by subjective memory impairment: effects of severity and temporal association with cognitive impairment. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 67, 414–422. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2010.30

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jonker, C., Geerlings, M. I., and Schmand, B. (2000). Are memory complaints predictive for dementia? A review of clinical and population-based studies. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 15, 983–991. doi: 10.1002/1099-1166(200011)15:11<983::AID-GPS238>3.0.CO;2-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jonker, C., Launer, L. J., Hooijer, C., and Lindeboom, J. (1996). Memory complaints and memory impairment in older individuals. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 44, 44–49. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.1996.tb05636.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, J. W., Byun, M. S., Yi, D., Lee, J. H., Ko, K., Jung, G., et al. (2019). Vascular risk modulates the relationship between cerebral amyloid deposition and subjective memory complaints. Neuropsychiatr. Dis. Treat. 15, 637–645. doi: 10.2147/NDT.S192231

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, J. Y., Park, J. H., Lee, J. J., Huh, Y., Lee, S. B., Han, S. K., et al. (2008). Standardization of the Korean version of the geriatric depression scale: reliability, validity, and factor structure. Psychiatry Investig. 5, 232–238. doi: 10.4306/pi.2008.5.4.232

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klunk, W. E., Engler, H., Nordberg, A., Wang, Y., Blomqvist, G., Holt, D. P., et al. (2004). Imaging brain amyloid in Alzheimer’s disease with Pittsburgh Compound-B. Ann. Neurol. 55, 306–319. doi: 10.1002/ana.20009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kryscio, R. J., Abner, E. L., Cooper, G. E., Fardo, D. W., Jicha, G. A., Nelson, P. T., et al. (2014). Self-reported memory complaints: implications from a longitudinal cohort with autopsies. Neurology 83, 1359–1365. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000000856

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, D. Y., Lee, K. U., Lee, J. H., Kim, K. W., Jhoo, J. H., Kim, S. Y., et al. (2004). A normative study of the CERAD neuropsychological assessment battery in the Korean elderly. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 10, 72–81. doi: 10.1017/S1355617704101094

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, J. H., Byun, M. S., Yi, D., Sohn, B. K., Jeon, S. Y., Lee, Y., et al. (2018). Prediction of cerebral amyloid with common information obtained from memory clinic practice. Front. Aging Neurosci. 10:309. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2018.00309

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, J. H., Lee, K. U., Lee, D. Y., Kim, K. W., Jhoo, J. H., Kim, J. H., et al. (2002). Development of the Korean version of the Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer’s Disease Assessment Packet (CERAD-K): clinical and neuropsychological assessment batteries. J. Gerontol. Ser. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 57, 47–53. doi: 10.1093/geronb/57.1.p47

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Manly, J. J., Tang, M. X., Schupf, N., Stern, Y., Vonsattel, J. P., and Mayeux, R. (2008). Frequency and course of mild cognitive impairment in a multiethnic community. Ann. Neurol. 63, 494–506. doi: 10.1002/ana.21326

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Minett, T. S., Dean, J. L., Firbank, M., English, P., and O’Brien, J. T. (2005). Subjective memory complaints, white-matter lesions, depressive symptoms, and cognition in elderly patients. Am. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 13, 665–671. doi: 10.1097/00019442-200508000-00005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morris, J. C. (1993). The Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR): current version and scoring rules. Neurology 43, 2412–2414. doi: 10.1212/wnl.43.11.2412-a

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morris, J. C., Heyman, A., Mohs, R. C., Hughes, J. P., van Belle, G., Fillenbaum, G., et al. (1989). The Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer’s Disease (CERAD). Part I. Clinical and neuropsychological assessment of Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology 39, 1159–1165. doi: 10.1212/wnl.39.9.1159

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

O’Connor, D. W., Pollitt, P. A., Roth, M., Brook, P. B., and Reiss, B. B. (1990). Memory complaints and impairment in normal, depressed, and demented elderly persons identified in a community survey. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 47, 224–227. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1990.01810150024005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paradise, M. B., Glozier, N. S., Naismith, S. L., Davenport, T. A., and Hickie, I. B. (2011). Subjective memory complaints, vascular risk factors and psychological distress in the middle-aged: a cross-sectional study. BMC Psychiatry 11:108. doi: 10.1186/1471-244X-11-108

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Perrotin, A., Mormino, E. C., Madison, C. M., Hayenga, A. O., and Jagust, W. J. (2012). Subjective cognition and amyloid deposition imaging: a Pittsburgh Compound B positron emission tomography study in normal elderly individuals. Arch. Neurol. 69, 223–229. doi: 10.1001/archneurol.2011.666

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Petersen, R. C., Aisen, P. S., Beckett, L. A., Donohue, M. C., Gamst, A. C., Harvey, D. J., et al. (2010). Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative (ADNI): clinical characterization. Neurology 74, 201–209. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3181cb3e25

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Petersen, R. C., Roberts, R. O., Knopman, D. S., Boeve, B. F., Geda, Y. E., Ivnik, R. J., et al. (2009). Mild cognitive impairment: ten years later. Arch. Neurol. 66, 1447–1455. doi: 10.1001/archneurol.2009.266

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Petersen, R. C., Smith, G. E., Waring, S. C., Ivnik, R. J., Tangalos, E. G., and Kokmen, E. (1999). Mild cognitive impairment: clinical characterization and outcome. Arch. Neurol. 56, 303–308. doi: 10.1001/archneur.56.3.303

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ramlall, S., Chipps, J., Bhigjee, A. I., and Pillay, B. J. (2013). The sensitivity and specificity of subjective memory complaints and the subjective memory rating scale, deterioration cognitive observee, mini-mental state examination, six-item screener and clock drawing test in dementia screening. Dement. Geriatr. Cogn. Disord. 36, 119–135. doi: 10.1159/000350768

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reid, L. M., and Maclullich, A. M. (2006). Subjective memory complaints and cognitive impairment in older people. Dement. Geriatr. Cogn. Disord. 22, 471–485. doi: 10.1159/000096295

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reisberg, B., Prichep, L., Mosconi, L., John, E. R., Glodzik-Sobanska, L., Boksay, I., et al. (2008). The pre-mild cognitive impairment, subjective cognitive impairment stage of Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement. 4, S98–S108. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2007.11.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rodda, J., Okello, A., Edison, P., Dannhauser, T., Brooks, D. J., and Walker, Z. (2010). (11)C-PIB PET in subjective cognitive impairment. Eur Psychiatry 25, 123–125. doi: 10.1016/j.eurpsy.2009.07.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rowe, C. C., Ellis, K. A., Rimajova, M., Bourgeat, P., Pike, K. E., Jones, G., et al. (2010). Amyloid imaging results from the Australian Imaging, Biomarkers and Lifestyle (AIBL) study of aging. Neurobiol. Aging 31, 1275–1283. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2010.04.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sajjad, A., Mirza, S. S., Portegies, M. L., Bos, M. J., Hofman, A., Koudstaal, P. J., et al. (2015). Subjective memory complaints and the risk of stroke. Stroke 46, 170–175. doi: 10.1161/STROKEAHA.114.006616

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seo, E. H., Lee, D. Y., Lee, J. H., Choo, I. H., Kim, J. W., Kim, S. G., et al. (2010). Total scores of the CERAD neuropsychological assessment battery: validation for mild cognitive impairment and dementia patients with diverse etiologies. Am. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 18, 801–809. doi: 10.1097/JGP.0b013e3181cab764

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stewart, R., Russ, C., Richards, M., Brayne, C., Lovestone, S., and Mann, A. (2001). Depression, APOE genotype and subjective memory impairment: a cross-sectional study in an African-Caribbean population. Psychol. Med. 31, 431–440. doi: 10.1017/S0033291701003257

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tobiansky, R., Blizard, R., Livingston, G., and Mann, A. (1995). The Gospel Oak Study stage IV: the clinical relevance of subjective memory impairment in older people. Psychol. Med. 25, 779–786. doi: 10.1017/s0033291700035029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tomita, T., Sugawara, N., Kaneda, A., Okubo, N., Iwane, K., Takahashi, I., et al. (2014). Sex-specific effects of subjective memory complaints with respect to cognitive impairment or depressive symptoms. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 68, 176–181. doi: 10.1111/pcn.12102

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tort-Merino, A., Valech, N., Laine, M., Olives, J., León, M., Ecay-Torres, M., et al. (2021). Accelerated long-term forgetting in individuals with subjective cognitive decline and amyloid-beta positivity. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 36, 1037–1049. doi: 10.1002/gps.5539

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Oijen, M., de Jong, F. J., Hofman, A., Koudstaal, P. J., and Breteler, M. M. (2007). Subjective memory complaints, education, and risk of Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement. 3, 92–97. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2007.01.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vogel, J. W., Varga Dolezalova, M., La Joie, R., Marks, S. M., Schwimmer, H. D., Landau, S. M., et al. (2017). Subjective cognitive decline and beta-amyloid burden predict cognitive change in healthy elderly. Neurology 89, 2002–2009. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000004627

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Winblad, B., Palmer, K., Kivipelto, M., Jelic, V., Fratiglioni, L., Wahlund, L. O., et al. (2004). Mild cognitive impairment–beyond controversies, towards a consensus: report of the International Working Group on Mild Cognitive Impairment. J. Internal Med. 256, 240–246. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2796.2004.01380.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yesavage, J. A., Brink, T. L., Rose, T. L., Lum, O., Huang, V., Adey, M., et al. (1982). Development and validation of a geriatric depression screening scale: a preliminary report. J. Psychiatr. Res. 17, 37–49. doi: 10.1016/0022-3956(82)90033-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Youn, J. C., Kim, K. W., Lee, D. Y., Jhoo, J. H., Lee, S. B., Park, J. H., et al. (2009). Development of the subjective memory complaints questionnaire. Dement. Geriatr. Cogn. Disord. 27, 310–317.

Google Scholar

Keywords: subjective memory complaint, cognition, Alzheimer’s disease, amyloid prediction index, mild cognitive impairment

Citation: Choe YM, Suh G-H, Lee BC, Choi I-G, Lee JH, Kim HS, Hwang J and Kim JW (2022) Brain Amyloid Index as a Probable Marker Bridging Between Subjective Memory Complaint and Objective Cognitive Performance. Front. Neurosci. 16:912891. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2022.912891

Received: 05 April 2022; Accepted: 10 June 2022;
Published: 04 July 2022.

Edited by:

Paul Newhouse, Vanderbilt University, United States

Reviewed by:

Shizuko Morimoto, The University of Utah, United States
Katie Gifford, Vanderbilt University Medical Center, United States
Kimberly Albert, Vanderbilt University Medical Center, United States

Copyright © 2022 Choe, Suh, Lee, Choi, Lee, Kim, Hwang and Kim. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Jee Wook Kim, Mzg1NDlAaGFsbHltLmFjLmty

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.