Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Neurosci., 26 August 2022
Sec. Auditory Cognitive Neuroscience
This article is part of the Research Topic Crosstalk Between Intonation and Lexical Tones: Linguistic, Cognitive and Neuroscience Perspectives View all 10 articles

Brain hemispheres with right temporal lobe damage swap dominance in early auditory processing of lexical tones

  • 1Biomedical Engineering Center, School of Information Science and Technology, University of Science and Technology of China, Hefei, China
  • 2Department of Magnetic Resonance Imaging, The First Affiliated Hospital of Zhengzhou University, Zhengzhou, China
  • 3Department of Neurobiology and Biophysics, School of Life Sciences, University of Science and Technology of China, Hefei, China
  • 4Department of Otolaryngology-Head and Neck Surgery, The First Affiliated Hospital, University of Science and Technology of China, Hefei, China
  • 5Department of Rehabilitation Medicine, The First Affiliated Hospital, University of Science and Technology of China, Hefei, China
  • 6Department of Neurosurgery, The First Affiliated Hospital, University of Science and Technology of China, Hefei, China
  • 7Clinical Hearing Center, Affiliated Eye and ENT Hospital, Fudan University, Shanghai, China

Labor division of the two brain hemispheres refers to the dominant processing of input information on one side of the brain. At an early stage, or a preattentive stage, the right brain hemisphere is shown to dominate the auditory processing of tones, including lexical tones. However, little is known about the influence of brain damage on the labor division of the brain hemispheres for the auditory processing of linguistic tones. Here, we demonstrate swapped dominance of brain hemispheres at the preattentive stage of auditory processing of Chinese lexical tones after a stroke in the right temporal lobe (RTL). In this study, we frequently presented lexical tones to a group of patients with a stroke in the RTL and infrequently varied the tones to create an auditory contrast. The contrast evoked a mismatch negativity response, which indexes auditory processing at the preattentive stage. In the participants with a stroke in the RTL, the mismatch negativity response was lateralized to the left side, in contrast to the right lateralization pattern in the control participants. The swapped dominance of brain hemispheres indicates that the RTL is a core area for early-stage auditory tonal processing. Our study indicates the necessity of rehabilitating tonal processing functions for tonal language speakers who suffer an RTL injury.

Introduction

The hemispheric specialization of language has been evaluated in multiple studies over the years, and the general consensus is that the left hemisphere is specialized for the processing of speech (Broca, 1861; Wernicke, 1874), whereas the right hemisphere is specialized for the processing of pitch such as musical tones (Zatorre et al., 2002; Poeppel, 2003). As to the factors responsible for this labor division between the two hemispheres, the functional hypothesis claims that the division depends on the auditory cues that serve as input signals (Whalen and Liberman, 1987; Liberman and Whalen, 2000), whereas the acoustic hypothesis claims that the division depends on the acoustic properties of input signals. Thus, the functional hypothesis predicts that linguistic pitch such as lexical tones is preferentially processed in the left hemisphere, whereas the acoustic hypothesis predicts that pitch is preferentially processed in the right hemisphere (Zatorre and Belin, 2001; Zatorre et al., 2002; Albouy et al., 2020). Moreover, a recent study reported increased activation in the right hemisphere when comprehending noisy spoken sentences in Mandarin Chinese (Song et al., 2020). As a matter of fact, neither of these two competing hypotheses can account for the full range of experimental data (Shankweiler and Studdert-Kennedy, 1967, 1975; Shtyrov et al., 2000).

Tonal languages such as Mandarin Chinese deploy lexical tones together with consonants and vowels to define word meaning. Previous neuroimaging studies, including positron emission tomography (PET) and functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), have reported that the bilateral superior temporal gyri (STG), the left anterior insula cortex, and the left middle temporal gyrus, as well as the right lateralized cortical activations in the posterior inferior frontal gyrus, are activated during the processing of lexical tone in Mandarin Chinese (Klein et al., 2001; Wong et al., 2004; Liu et al., 2006; Xi et al., 2010; Chang et al., 2014). In our previous study, spectrograms of the syllable /bai/ pronounced in four lexical tones (bai1, bai2, bai3, and bai4) illustrated that the lexical tones are characterized by varying frequencies with time, and that lexical tones have minimal effects on the voice onset time of the consonant /b/; moreover, spectrograms of the syllables /bai, /dai, and /tai/ pronounced in a flat tone (bai1, dai1, and tai1) illustrated that the syllables show relatively unchanged frequencies with time and that the consonants in the upper syllables are characterized by temporal variations as reflected by the voice onset time. Therefore, lexical tones and consonants are ideal materials for testing the two hypotheses. In our previous study (Luo et al., 2006), we proposed that the processing of a lexical tone carrying semantic information is lateralized to the right hemisphere at an early stage, but to the left hemisphere at a late stage. This so-called two-stage model (Luo et al., 2006) claims that hemisphere labor division initially depends on the acoustic properties of input signals and then depends on the functional cues in the processing from sound to meaning. Thus, the acoustic hypothesis and the functional hypothesis are not mutually exclusive, with each representing a different temporal stage of processing (Ren et al., 2009; Zhou et al., 2021). Our two-stage model resolves the debate over the cues that are used by the brain for the processing of speech sound and tonal sound.

The role of the right hemisphere in speech comprehension (Gainotti et al., 1981; Posner and Petersen, 1989; Mitchell and Crow, 2005; Lam et al., 2016; Gajardo-Vidal et al., 2018) and the speech impairments in patients with right brain damage (Gandour et al., 1988; Hagoort et al., 1996; Mitchell and Crow, 2005; Kadyamusuma et al., 2011; Gajardo-Vidal et al., 2018) have been explored since the 1980s. However, issues related to brain labor division for linguistic processing became more complicated in clinical observations. Some studies have shown that patients with left brain damage show less left lateralization in early auditory processing of consonants (Becker and Reinvang, 2007) and impairments in tone tasks for tonal languages (Gandour et al., 1992, 1996). Other studies have shown impairments of tone identification and production (Kadyamusuma et al., 2011) as well as the acoustic pattern (Gandour et al., 1988) in patients with right brain lesions. Patients with brain lesions are ideal subjects for investigating these issues. However, previous studies were mostly conducted at a behavioral level, which reflects auditory processing at a late stage, not an early stage. Thus, the impairments in the processing of lexical tones at an early stage, or a preattentive stage, and the influence of injury on the labor division of the brain hemispheres for auditory tonal processing at the electrophysiological level remain unclear. We believe that the right hemisphere dominance in early auditory processing of lexical tones would be impaired at the electrophysiological level in patients with right brain lesions. Considering the critical role of the right temporal lobe (RTL) for lexical-tone processing (Ge et al., 2015; Si et al., 2017; Liang and Du, 2018), we predicted that the impairments would be apparent in patients with RTL lesions but not in those with right non-temporal lobe (RNTL) lesions.

In the present study, we explored the hemisphere dominance in early auditory processing of lexical tones by using whole-head electric recordings of mismatch negativity (MMN) obtained from native Mandarin Chinese-speaking patients with RTL or RNTL lesions under a passive auditory oddball paradigm (Picton et al., 2000). The MMN is an index of the brain's automatic processing at an early stage (Naatanen et al., 1978), and it has been used as a probe in several studies related to the realm of pitch and music, as well as language (Luo et al., 2006; Chobert et al., 2012; Wang et al., 2013). For the source localization of MMN, many neuroimaging studies such as PET, fMRI, and magneto/encephalography (M/EEG) have proposed that beyond the bilateral STG, the right inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) contributes to MMN generation (Rinne et al., 2000; Opitz et al., 2002; Dura-Bernal et al., 2012). For the purpose of comparison, we also measured the MMN evoked with pure tones with varied frequencies, which are non-speech stimuli and are known to be dominantly processed in the right brain (Schonwiesner et al., 2005). We used the shortened Mandarin Chinese version of the Token test (De Renzi and Faglioni, 1978) to measure whether right brain injury impairs the ability of speech comprehension for Mandarin Chinese.

Materials and methods

Informed consent was obtained from the participants in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. The research protocols used in this study were approved by the Ethics Committee of the First Affiliated Hospital, University of Science and Technology of China.

Participants

Patients participating in this study were recruited from the First Affiliated Hospital, University of Science and Technology of China and screened using the following criteria: (i) provided informed consent after the procedure had been fully explained; (ii) native speakers of Mandarin Chinese; (iii) lesions were restricted to the right hemisphere, i.e., RTL and RNTL; (iv) right-handed before stroke onset, and musically untrained; (v) no contraindications to magnetic resonance imaging (MRI); and (vi) no medical history of audiological, mental, or neurological problems before stroke. These criteria were met in 24 patients: 11 patients with stroke in the RTL (age, 37–76 years; mean age, 57 years; two females) and 13 patients with stroke in the RNTL (age, 37–63 years; mean age, 53 years; two females). Brain lesions in these 24 patients were caused by a stroke with cerebral infarction (10 RTL: 11 RNTL) or cerebral hemorrhage (one RTL: two RNTL). Demographic, clinical, and lesion data of each patient are shown in Supplementary Table 1. Fourteen healthy age- and sex-matched control participants (age, 42–64 years; mean age, 52 years; five females) with Mandarin Chinese as their native language also volunteered to participate in this study (Supplementary Table 1). These participants were not musically trained and did not have a medical history of audiological, mental, or neurological diseases. All participants were right-handed in the assessment performed with the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory (Oldfield, 1971). The hearing thresholds of the three groups were tested by pure-tone audiometry at 500, 1, 2, and 4 kHz, as in a previous study (Robson et al., 2014). We first performed a Shapiro–Wilk test to identify whether the hearing thresholds for each group were normally distributed, and the results showed that the hearing thresholds were not normally distributed for some parts of the groups. Then, we performed the Kruskal–Wallis H-test to determine whether the hearing thresholds among the groups were matched, and the results showed no significant difference among the groups (the left ear: X(2)2 = 5.40, P > 0.05; the right ear: X(2)2 = 3.63, P > 0.05). The data for the hearing thresholds of subject No. 8 in the RTL group and subject No. 3 in the RNTL group were not collected. The patients reported no hearing problems before stroke.

Lesion overlay map

Structural high-resolution MRI scans of 13 patients (four RTL: nine RNTL) and CT scans of 11 patients (seven RTL: four RNTL) were acquired. The MRI scans were acquired on a 3T Philips Achieva scanner and included good T2-weighted or DWI B0 images. The CT scans were acquired on a Siemens scanner or a Philips scanner. Lesions were manually delineated by an experienced neurologist in the axial plane on each slice of the T2-weighted (eight RNTL patients; slice thickness, 5 or 5.5 mm; in-plane resolution, 1 mm), DWI (four RTL and one RNTL patients; slice thickness, 5 or 5.5 mm; in-plane resolution, 2 mm), or CT images (slice thickness, 5 mm; in-plane resolution, ≤ 0.5 mm) by using MRIcron (Rorden and Brett, 2000). Lesion volume was computed by multiplying the damaged area on each delineated slice by the slice thickness. The T2, DWI, and CT images of patients were transformed into standard stereotactic space (MNI) by using a clinical toolbox (www.nitrc.org/projects/clinicaltbx/) and SPM12 (www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm). These images were resampled to yield the same voxel resolution, i.e., 1 mm3. The lesion overlay maps for the RTL (Figure 1A) and RNTL (Figure 1B) groups displayed a distributed profile at the group level. Inspection of the lesion overlay maps and individual MRI/CT scans indicated that the patients in the RTL group mainly had lesions in the RTL, the right insular, and the right frontal lobe (Supplementary Table 1 and Figure 1A), and those in the RNTL group mainly had lesions in the right periventricular white matter, the right basal ganglia, and the right occipital lobe (Supplementary Table 1 and Figure 1B).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Lesion overlay map and speech comprehension ability. (A,B) The lesion overlap map of patients with right temporal lobe (RTL) damage (A) and right non-temporal lobe (RNTL) damage (B). The heat map displays the number of patients with lesions in that respective area. Coordinates refer to MNI space. (C) Token test scores in each group. Token test scores of the RTL group were lower than that of the control group and that of the RNTL group (Left). The values are expressed as mean ± SE. *P < 0.05, **P < 0.01. Significantly negative correlation was found between lesion volume and Token test scores in the RTL group only (Central), but not the RNTL group (Right). rs represents the correlation coefficient. L, left; R, right.

Stimuli

Lexical tones and pure tones were used as stimuli in this study. Lexical-tone stimuli were obtained and slightly modified from those used in our previous study (Luo et al., 2006), in which the Mandarin consonant-vowel (CV) syllables /bai1/ and /bai4/ were employed and originally pronounced by an adult male Mandarin speaker (Sinica Corpus, Institute of Linguistics, Chinese Academy of Social Sciences, Beijing, China). Pure tones were generated by Audition 3.0 (Adobe Systems Inc., Mountain View, CA, USA). The duration of each lexical tone was normalized to 350 ms, the duration of each pure tone was 200 ms, and both included a 5-ms linear rise and fall time. The lexical-tone contrast was created by a sequence of /bai1/ frequently presented as the standard stimuli and /bai4/ infrequently presented as the deviant stimuli during the auditory stream. The pure-tone contrast was created by a sequence of pure tones frequently presented at 550 Hz as the standard stimuli and infrequently presented at 350 Hz as the deviant stimuli.

Procedure

Two blocks including the lexical-tone contrast and the pure-tone contrast were separately and randomly presented to the participant in one session with a 5-min break. Each block consisted of 800 trials. The participants were instructed to watch a silent movie and ignore the auditory stimuli they heard. The detection thresholds of lexical tones and pure tones were measured first, and all stimuli were then presented binaurally at 78 dB above the detection thresholds for each listener through headphones (TDH-39; Telephonics, Farmingdale, NY, United States) in an electrically shielded soundproof room. The standard stimuli were presented with a probability of 7/8 and the deviant stimuli were presented with a probability of 1/8. The stimulus order was pseudorandomized while maintaining a restriction that each deviant stimulus was separated by at least two standard stimuli. The inter-stimulus onset interval was 550 ms for the lexical-tone contrast and 500 ms for the pure-tone contrast.

Data collection and analysis

The EEGs were recorded with 17 Ag/AgCl electrodes (Brain Products GmbH, Munich, Germany) placed at the standard electrode sites (F3, Fz, F4, FC1, FC2, FC5, FC6, C3, Cz, C4, P3, Pz, P4, FCz, Fpz, left mastoid, and right mastoid) according to the extended international 10–20 system. Two electrodes were used to measure the vertical and horizontal electrooculograms (EOGs). The reference electrode was attached at FCz and the ground electrode was placed between Fpz and Fz. Current signals (0.1–100 Hz) were continuously recorded by BrainAmp DC amplifier and sampled at 500 Hz. Impedances were maintained at <5 kΩ for all electrodes. The EEG and EOG data were recorded online and digitized using Brain Vision Recorder software (Brain Products, Munich, Germany).

Data from the head recordings were processed offline using Brain Vision Analyzer software (Brain Products, Munich, Germany). The recording was rejected when it was evidently contaminated by the EMG signal. An automatic ocular correction was then performed. Data were re-referenced to the average of the left and right mastoids and filtered (1–30 Hz). Epochs obtained from the continuous data were 600 ms in length, including a 100-ms pre-stimulus baseline, and were rejected when fluctuations in amplitude were >100 μV. The event-related potentials evoked by the standard and the deviant stimuli were calculated by averaging individual trials. MMN was derived from a different wave by subtracting the event-related potential evoked by the standard stimuli from that evoked by the deviant stimuli (Picton et al., 2000; Naatanen et al., 2004). Scalp topographic maps were produced using Brain Electric Source Analysis (MEGIS Software GmbH, Munich, Germany).

Speech comprehension test

We measured the speech comprehension ability of Mandarin Chinese participants by using the shortened Mandarin Chinese version of the Token test (De Renzi and Faglioni, 1978). The materials consisted of tokens of different colors (white, blue, yellow, red, and green), shapes (squares and circles), and sizes (large and small). The examinee followed verbal instructions that increased in complexity from simple commands (e.g., “Touch a circle”; “Touch the red circle”) to more challenging commands such as “Before touching the yellow circle, pick up the red square.” The Token test scores were adjusted for years of education. Adjusted scores between 25 and 28 were regarded as an indicator of mild comprehension problems, those between 17 and 7 indicated moderate problems, and those below 17 indicated severe or very severe problems. The Token test scores of subject No. 3 in the RNTL group were not collected.

Statistical analysis

Two sets of electrodes on the left (F3, FC1, FC5, C3) and right (F4, FC2, FC6, C4) sides of the scalp were identified as the regions of interest (Doeller et al., 2003; Luo et al., 2006). The amplitudes of MMN recorded at the four electrodes on each side were averaged within a time window from 20 ms before the peak of MMN between 100 and 300 ms recorded from electrode Fz to 20 ms after that peak (as indicated by the gray bars in the left panels of Figures 2A,B) (Wang et al., 2013). The MMN amplitude would be set to zero for all subsequent analyses when the averaged value was positive (Robson et al., 2014). The MMN latency was measured between 100 and 300 ms at electrode Fz. Then, the lateralization index (LI) for each stimulus condition and each participant was calculated by using the MMN amplitudes of the left and right sides. The LI was calculated by the following formula:

LI = (left MMN amplitude  right MMN amplitude)/          (left MMN amplitude + right MMN amplitude).
FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. MMN responses recorded on the left and right sides of the scalp. (A) Grand average traces of MMN evoked by the lexical-tone contrast were recorded from one pair of electrodes on the left (F3, thick blue lines) and right (F4, thin red lines) sides in the control (Upper, n = 14), RTL (Central, n = 11), and RNTL (Lower, n = 13) groups (Left). Gray bars indicate the time window in which MMN amplitude was calculated. Scalp topographic maps constructed from grand average MMN evoked by the lexical-tone contrast are shown at the time point of MMN peak amplitude on electrode Fz (Right). (B) Grand average traces recorded from one pair of electrodes on the left and right sides (Left) and grand average scalp topographic maps (Right) of MMN evoked by the pure-tone contrast in the control (Upper, n = 14), RTL (Central, n =11), and RNTL (Lower, n = 13) groups. Gray bars indicate the time window in which MMN amplitude was calculated.

An index value of −1 indicates a lateralized response entirely in the right hemisphere and an index value of +1 indicates a lateralized response entirely in the left (Seghier, 2008). Supplementary Figure 1 shows the relationship between the LI for each stimulus condition (lexical-tone contrast and pure-tone contrast) and the time post-stroke onset in the RTL and RNTL groups. The findings showed no significant relationship.

A Shapiro–Wilk test was performed to check the normality of Token test scores, bilateral MMN amplitudes, and MMN latencies and LI values for each group. The results showed that Token test scores and MMN latencies were normally distributed in all groups, but bilateral MMN amplitudes and LI values were not normally distributed in some groups and stimulus conditions. Welch ANOVA was performed to test the significance of differences in Token test scores among groups, given that the Token test scores among groups (control, RTL, and RNTL) failed the assumption for homogeneity of variance tested using Levene's test. Post-hoc pairwise comparisons with the Games–Howell test were performed. For assessing possible lateralization effects, we performed a Wilcoxon signed-rank test between bilateral MMN amplitudes for each stimulus condition and each group. Moreover, the LI for each stimulus condition and each group were assessed by using a one-sample Wilcoxon signed-rank test and compared with the value 0. An LI index value significantly <0 indicates rightward lateralization and a value significantly >0 indicates leftward lateralization. We performed the Kruskal–Wallis H-test and used Dunn's test as a post-hoc test to determine whether the LI and unilateral MMN amplitudes showed significant differences among groups (control, RTL, RNTL) for each stimulus condition. For exploring the relationships among LIs obtained under different stimulus conditions, lesion volume, and speech-comprehension ability, we obtained the Spearman's correlation coefficients for the following correlations: (i) the correlations between lesion volume and Token test scores in the RTL and RNTL groups; (ii) the correlations between lesion volume and LI for each stimulus condition in the RTL and RNTL groups; (iii) the correlations between the Token test scores and LI for each stimulus condition and each group. The statistical difference was considered significant with the alpha-level set as P < 0.05 for all tests. One-way ANOVA was performed to test whether MMN latency showed significant differences among groups, and post-hoc pairwise comparisons with the Bonferroni test were performed. All data are expressed as mean ± SE or median ± minimum/maximum value. The correlation coefficients were marked as rs. SPSS V13 software (IBM, USA) and OriginPro V8 software (OriginLab Corp., USA) were used for statistical analysis and graph plotting.

Results

Speech comprehension ability for each group and its correlation with lesion volume

There was a significant difference in Token test scores among groups as determined by Welch ANOVA [F(2, 19.50) = 6.31, P < 0.01]. Post-hoc pairwise comparisons with Games–Howell's test showed that Token test scores in the RTL group were lower than those in the control group (95% confidence interval= −6.17, −0.93; P < 0.01) and those in the RNTL group (95% confidence interval= −6.05, −0.66; P < 0.05; Figure 1C, Left). Moreover, a significant negative correlation was found between lesion volume and Token test scores in the RTL group (rs = −0.84, P < 0.01; Figure 1C, Central); that is, the larger lesion volume, the worse speech comprehension ability of Mandarin Chinese. And no significant correlation was found between lesion volume and Token test scores in the RNTL group (rs = 0.27, P > 0.05; Figure 1C, Right).

MMN and LI for the left and right sides of the scalp in lexical and pure tone conditions

MMN waveforms were prominent in both lexical and pure tone conditions as illustrated by sample traces of grand average MMN in response to the lexical-tone contrast and to the pure-tone contrast (Figures 2A,B, left panels). The MMN in the control group under each stimulus condition and that in the RNTL group under the pure-tone contrast were stronger in magnitude when recorded on the right side of the scalp than those recorded on the left side. In contrast, MMN of the RTL group in response to the lexical-tone contrast demonstrated a swapped pattern: it was stronger in magnitude when recorded on the left side of the scalp than that recorded on the right side. More detailed latencies of the MMN between groups under each condition showed no significant difference across groups (Supplementary Figure 2). The right panels of Figures 2A,B show the scalp topographic maps constructed with grand average MMN in response to the lexical-tone contrast and the pure-tone contrast for the control (Upper), RTL (Central), and RNTL (Lower) groups. The MMN topographic maps were obviously lateralized in strength to the right side of the scalp in the control and RNTL groups under each stimulus condition, whereas they were obviously lateralized in strength to the left side of the scalp in the RTL group.

The analysis of MMN amplitudes calculated from four pairs of electrodes on the left (F3, FC1, FC5, C3) and right (F4, FC2, FC6, C4) sides of the scalp in the individual participant (Figure 3A) demonstrates the swapped hemispheric lateralization of the MMN responses to the lexical-tone contrast in the RTL group. Wilcoxon signed-rank test showed that the MMN response was significantly lateralized to the right side of the scalp in the control group for both conditions (lexical tone, Z = −2.48, P < 0.05; pure tone, Z = −3.30, P < 0.01) and in the RNTL group for the pure-tone contrast (Z = −2.20, P < 0.05), whereas the MMN response was significantly lateralized to the left side of the scalp in the RTL group for the lexical-tone contrast (Z = −2.13, P < 0.05; Figure 3A). No other significant results between bilateral MMNs were shown in the RTL or RNTL group.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. MMN amplitudes and lateralization index (LI) were recorded from four electrodes on the left (F3, FC1, FC5, C3) and four electrodes on the right (F4, FC2, FC6, C4) sides of the scalp. (A) MMN was significantly larger in amplitude on the right side of the scalp than on the left in the control group for the lexical-tone contrast and the pure-tone contrast and in the RNTL group for the pure-tone contrast but larger in amplitude on the left side of the scalp than on the right in the RTL group for the lexical-tone contrast. (B) The comparisons for LI within each group and among groups. The analysis within the group indicates that the LI was significantly less than zero (indicates right hemisphere lateralized response) in the control group for the lexical-tone contrast and the pure-tone contrast and in the RNTL group for the pure-tone contrast but greater than zero (indicates left hemisphere lateralized response) in the RTL group for the lexical-tone contrast. The analysis among groups indicates that the LI in the RTL group was larger than that in the control group and the RNTL group for each stimulus condition. Box plots depict medians with interquartile ranges and whiskers represent the minimum and maximum values. *P < 0.05, **P < 0.01.

The swapped pattern of hemisphere lateralization of MMN responses to the lexical-tone contrast was also revealed by the LI. A one-sample Wilcoxon signed-rank test showed that the LI was significantly less than zero (i.e., lateralized to the right hemisphere) in the control group under each stimulus condition (lexical tone: median value = −0.08, P < 0.05; pure tone: median value = −0.12, P < 0.01) and in the RNTL patients for the pure-tone contrast (median value = −0.09, P < 0.05), whereas the LI was significantly greater than zero (i.e., lateralized to the left hemisphere) in the RTL group under the lexical-tone contrast (median value = 0.24, P < 0.05; Figure 3B). The Kruskal–Wallis H test showed significant differences in LI among groups for both conditions (lexical tone: X(2)2 = 9.67, P < 0.01, with a mean rank LI score of 15.00 for the control group, 28.18 for the RTL group, and 17.00 for the RNTL group; pure tone: X(2)2 = 9.20, P < 0.05, with a mean rank LI score of 14.07 for the control group, 27.55 for the RTL group, and 18.54 for the RNTL group). Post-hoc pairwise comparisons with Dunn's test showed that LIs for both stimulus conditions in the RTL group were larger than those in the control (lexical tone: P < 0.01; pure tone: P < 0.01) and the RNTL groups (lexical tone: P < 0.01; pure tone: P < 0.05; Figure 3B). No significant LI difference was observed in either stimulus condition between the control and the RNTL groups.

To explore the causes of the swapped hemisphere dominance, we analyzed the differences in unilateral MMN amplitudes across groups. The Kruskal–Wallis H-test showed that the right MMN amplitudes across groups were significantly different for each stimulus condition (lexical tone: X(2)2 = 9.42, P < 0.01, with a mean rank MMN amplitude score of 17.29 for the control group, 28.00 for the RTL group, and 14.69 for the RNTL group; pure tone: X(2)2 = 11.76, P < 0.01, with a mean rank MMN amplitude score of 15.71 for the control group, 29.18 for the RTL group, and 15.38 for the RNTL group). Post-hoc pairwise comparisons with Dunn's test revealed that the MMN amplitude on the right scalp for each stimulus condition in the RTL group was lower than that in the control (lexical tone: P < 0.05; pure tone: P < 0.01) and RNTL groups (lexical tone: P < 0.01; pure tone: P < 0.01). However, MMN amplitudes on the right scalp between the control and RNTL groups were significantly different for neither the lexical-tone contrast (P > 0.05) nor the pure-tone contrast (P > 0.05). Moreover, MMN amplitudes on the left scalp among groups were significantly different for neither the lexical-tone contrast (X(2)2 = 3.20, P > 0.05) nor the pure-tone contrast (X(2)2 = 1.74, P > 0.05). The reduction in the MMN amplitude in the right hemisphere of the RTL group indicates a swapping of brain dominance.

Correlations between lesion volume and LI and those between LI and token test scores

Our findings showed significant correlations between lesion volume and LI, and between LI and Token test scores in the RTL group. Significant correlation between lesion volume and LI was only found in the RTL group for the lexical-tone contrast (rs = 0.72, P < 0.05; Figure 4A, Left), indicating that a larger lesion volume corresponds to less right hemisphere involvement. Significant correlation was also observed between LI and Token test scores in the RTL group for the lexical-tone contrast (rs = −0.74, P < 0.01; Figure 4B, Central), suggesting that less right hemisphere involvement corresponds to impaired speech comprehension ability for Mandarin Chinese. No other significant correlations were observed among lesion volume or Token test scores (Figure 4). The detailed results of correlation analysis between the LI for each stimulus condition (lexical-tone contrast and pure-tone contrast) and lesion volume/Token test scores are shown in Supplementary Table 2.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Correlations between lesion volume and lateralization index (LI), and those between LI and Token test scores. (A) Significant correlation between lesion volume and LI was only found in the RTL group for the lexical-tone contrast—more lesion volume corresponding to less right hemisphere involvement. (B) Significant correlation between LI and Token test scores was only found in the RTL group for the lexical-tone contrast—less right hemisphere involvement corresponding to worse speech comprehension ability of Mandarin Chinese.

Discussion

In the present study, we investigated how brain damage in stroke patients affects the labor division of the brain hemispheres for auditory processing of linguistic tones. Our study demonstrates that RTL injury results in swapped dominance of brain hemispheres in the preattentive auditory processing of Chinese lexical tones, suggesting that the RTL is a core area for early-stage auditory tonal processing.

Since the early auditory processing of lexical tones was lateralized to the right hemisphere for the control group and the left hemisphere for the RTL group (Figures 2, 3), the findings demonstrate swapped dominance of brain hemispheres in preattentive auditory processing of Chinese lexical tones after RTL stroke. Meta-analyses of lexical-tone processing have suggested significant activations in both temporal lobes in response to lexical tones (Kwok et al., 2017; Liang and Du, 2018), but non-tonal language studies only showed significant activations in the left temporal lobe (Kwok et al., 2017). Lexical-tone studies demonstrate more activations in the RTL than in the left (Liang and Du, 2018). Some researchers suggest that the appearance of language impairments in right-handed patients with right brain injury represents atypical language lateralization before stroke (Gajardo-Vidal et al., 2018). Therefore, the right lateralization in the control group and the impairment of right lateralization in the RTL group would not have been caused by atypical language lateralization. Moreover, the comparisons of LI for the lexical tone and the pure tone within and among groups showed that the RTL group exhibited left lateralization while the control and RNTL groups exhibited right lateralization at a preattentive stage (Figure 3B). Our findings support our hypothesis that RTL injury changes the right hemisphere dominance during the early auditory processing of lexical tones. Notably, both lexical and pure tones reflecting varied spectral information are dominantly processed in the right hemisphere (Zatorre and Belin, 2001; Schonwiesner et al., 2005), which is consistent with the acoustic hypothesis that the hemispheric specialization for processing auditory perception depends on the acoustic structure of the auditory input (Zatorre and Belin, 2001; Schonwiesner et al., 2005). Our results demonstrate that only RTL injury swaps the hemisphere dominance in early auditory processing of lexical tone that carries semantic information. In the RTL group, the reduced right hemisphere involvement for processing lexical tones at a preattentive stage corresponds to the worse speech-comprehension ability for Mandarin Chinese (Figure 4B, Central). Although left hemisphere dominance for processing language has been demonstrated since the 1870s (Broca, 1861; Wernicke, 1874; Tyler et al., 2010, 2011; Teki et al., 2013), the present study suggests that language disorders can occur after injury in the right hemisphere, especially in the RTL.

Although the RTL group exhibited left hemispheric lateralization for processing lexical tones at an early stage, this does not necessarily mean that the left hemisphere compensates for the impaired auditory function of the right hemisphere. The worse speech-comprehension ability for Mandarin Chinese in the RTL group was associated with a reduced right hemisphere-lateralized response for early auditory processing of lexical tones (Figure 4B, Central). This is consistent with a previous study in which the RTL was shown to correlate with speech comprehension (Walenski et al., 2019). Moreover, we found a significant association between lesion volume and LI for the lexical-tone contrast at a preattentive stage in the RTL group: the larger the lesion volume, the lower the right hemisphere-lateralized response (Figure 4A, Left). Similar associations between larger lesion volumes and lower functional improvement have been also found in rats after stroke (Sasaki et al., 2016). Since the RTL group mainly showed lesions in the RTL (Figure 1A), we think that the disappearance of right hemisphere dominance may be caused by the decreased neural activity involved in lexical-tone perception in the right hemisphere. We further suggest that RTL injury impairs the speech comprehension of tonal languages. A previous study demonstrated that the speech comprehension of non-tonal languages can be impaired by right brain damage and that the most frequently impaired language task is auditory sentence-to-picture matching (Gajardo-Vidal et al., 2018).

Notably, in RTL stroke patients, the neural activity revealed by the MMN amplitude on the right scalp was significantly lower than those in the control and RNTL groups, but the neural activity of the left hemisphere in the RTL group was not significantly different. This is in line with the findings of previous studies, which showed that patients with unilateral brain damage may show diminished response on the injured side (Alho et al., 1994; Deouell et al., 2000; Tyler et al., 2011). The swapped hemisphere dominance in early auditory processing of lexical tone in patients with RTL stroke is obviously caused by the decreased MMN amplitude of the right hemisphere. The previous studies indicated that the source generator of MMN originates from the left and right auditory regions (Naatanen et al., 1997; Kujala et al., 2002). Naatanen et al. (1997) demonstrated the source generators of MMN in the left and right auditory cortices for speech and non-speech sounds in their well-known MEG study. If the lesion is in the RTL, then the MMN activity on the right side would be reduced. The more extensive the damage is, the greater the reduction in MMN on the right side. The MMN amplitudes of the left hemisphere (uninjured side) showed no significant difference across groups (Figure 3A). This is inconsistent with the findings of previous studies in which decreased neural activity on the injured side was suggested to result in increased neural activity on the uninjured side in speech perception (Becker and Reinvang, 2007; Tyler et al., 2010, 2011; Teki et al., 2013). Similar findings have been reported in another study showing enhanced interactions between the hemispheres in patients with RTL epilepsy but not in control subjects or patients with RNTL epilepsy (He et al., 2018). Some of the possible reasons to explain why the left MMN amplitude in the RTL group was not increased can be summarized as follows: (i) patients in the RTL group were mostly in the acute stage (post-stroke onset <3 months, Supplementary Table 1), and stable compensation of the left hemisphere for lexical-tone perception may not yet have occurred; (ii) the spectral variation of lexical tones is a basal variation in acoustic patterns, which might be difficult for the left hemisphere to compensate for; and (iii) the lesion volume and the lesion area in RTL stroke patients varies substantially, and it might be difficult to form a stable pattern. Moreover, the decreased MMN amplitude of the right side and unchanged MMN amplitude of the left side in the RTL group may result in the patients not able to discriminate the lexical tones as well as the controls. Future research should add behavioral experiments to explore whether the RTL group has impairments in both MMN amplitude in lexical-tone contrast and the ability to distinguish the lexical tones.

Our present study shows that the RNTL injury also affects the hemisphere dominance in response to the lexical-tone contrast (Figure 3). The RNTL may be the facultative brain regions in early auditory processing of lexical tone, and the RTL may be the obligatory brain region. In addition to the RTL, other brain areas of the right hemisphere, such as the right white matter (Zhao et al., 2016), the right basal ganglia (Chang and Kuo, 2016), and the right visual cortex (Kwok et al., 2015) may also participate in lexical-tone perception. Patients with RNTL injuries mainly showed lesions in the right periventricular white matter, the right basal ganglia, and the right occipital lobe (Supplementary Table 1 and Figure 1B). These results may indicate the importance of the cooperation and connectivity of the multiple brain areas in the early auditory processing of lexical tones, and a special pattern might form when a specific brain region is damaged. Unlike the lexical-tone contrast, the RNTL group still showed right hemisphere dominance in response to the pure-tone contrast (Figures 2, 3). This is consistent with the results of previous studies reporting that the processing of spectral variation for non-speech stimuli mainly occurs in the RTL (Opitz et al., 2002; Schonwiesner et al., 2005). Therefore, early auditory processing of pure tones was not impaired in the RNTL group.

Several limitations should be noted when interpreting our findings. First, the sample size was relatively small. We spent 2 years and recruited 30 stroke patients and 14 healthy control participants. Among these patients, we excluded five stroke patients with severe hearing loss, and one patient withdrew consent. Therefore, the stroke patients were well-characterized. Second, although the difference in age across groups was not significant, participant age showed substantial variation, implying that the sample may not be representative of younger stroke sufferers. Third, since lexical tones and music have the same patterns (Nan and Friederici, 2013; Chen et al., 2018), detailed information about the musical experience of the participants should have been collected. Nevertheless, since the aim of our study was to investigate the effect of right brain injury on the hemispheric dominance of lexical tones, we did not consider the effects of musical training when recruiting subjects. Stroke patients were not always ready for recruitment, and their availability would have been reduced even further if the musical training-related factor had been applied. Fourth, our study lacked exact control between the two stimulus conditions. Because the recorded MMN response in the pure-tone contrast occurred ~150 ms after the onset of the stimuli under a passive auditory oddball paradigm (Aaltonen et al., 1993), we selected a 200-ms pure tone and a 500-ms inter-stimulus onset interval (ISI) for the pure-tone contrast (Wang et al., 2021). Notably, the MEG data for the neural basis of perceptual processing of lexical tones indicated a left hemispheric dominance for detecting large lexical-tone changes and small deviant contrasts involving less left hemispheric activation in the auditory cortex and greater activation in the right frontal cortex at a later time window (Hsu et al., 2014). The cross-category contrasts also revealed larger MMN responses than within-category contrasts in the left scalp, but not in the right scalp (Xi et al., 2010; Zhang et al., 2011). In addition, an MMN study investigating the effect of allophonic variation on the mental representation and neural processing of lexical tones suggested that activation of the allophonic tonal variants can lead to right-hemisphere-dominant processing of lexical tones, which are otherwise categorically processed via recruitment of both left and right hemispheres (Li and Chen, 2015).

We assessed the speech-comprehension ability for Mandarin Chinese (a tonal language) by the Token test, a language task involving auditory sentence-to-picture matching. The performance in the speech-comprehension task in the RTL group was worse than that in the RNTL and control groups (Figure 1C, Left). In the RTL group, the speech-comprehension ability for Mandarin Chinese negatively correlated with the lesion volume (Figure 1C, Central), indicating a causal role of the RTL in Mandarin speech perception. Considering the growing awareness that aphasia following a stroke can include deficits in other cognitive functions (Schumacher et al., 2019) and the importance of accurately representing lexical-tone information for hearing-impaired Mandarin speakers (Li et al., 2019; Chen et al., 2020), our study highlights the necessity of rehabilitating the language functions of tonal language speakers who suffer from RTL injury and applying formal lexical-tone-related communication tests in clinical assessment and rehabilitation for patients who are speakers of tonal languages and experience brain injury and communication disorders.

To summarize, our findings showed swapped dominance of lateralization from the right to the left hemisphere in patients with RTL injuries but not in those with RNTL injuries, indicating that the RTL is a core area for auditory tonal processing at an early stage or a preattentive stage. These findings indicate the necessity of rehabilitating language functions of tonal language speakers who experience RTL injury.

Data availability statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/Supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding authors.

Ethics statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Ethics Committee of the First Affiliated Hospital, University of Science and Technology of China. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

LC designed the research. YW and MW performed the research. YW, XL, XG, XW, YQ, and RA analyzed data. LC, XF, HL, BQ, MW, RQ, YW, and XL wrote the paper. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This work was supported by the National Natural Science Foundation of China (Grants 81970886, 81570915, and 81870723) and Anhui Provincial Natural Science Foundation (Grant 30973084).

Acknowledgments

Thanks are due to Xiaopeng Han for technical support and to Lihong Huang, Xinyi Lv, and Li Wang for their assistance with recruiting patients.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnins.2022.909796/full#supplementary-material

References

Aaltonen, O., Tuomainen, J., Laine, M., and Niemi, P. (1993). Cortical differences in tonal versus vowel processing as revealed by an ERP component called mismatch negativity (MMN). Brain Lang. 44, 139–152. doi: 10.1006/brln.1993.1009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Albouy, P., Benjamin, L., Morillon, B., and Zatorre, R. J. (2020). Distinct sensitivity to spectrotemporal modulation supports brain asymmetry for speech and melody. Science 367, 1043. doi: 10.1126/science.aaz3468

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Alho, K., Woods, D. L., Algazi, A., Knight, R. T., and Naatanen, R. (1994). Lesions of frontal cortex diminish the auditory mismatch negativity. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 91, 353–362. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(94)00173-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Becker, F., and Reinvang, I. (2007). Mismatch negativity elicited by tones and speech sounds: changed topographical distribution in aphasia. Brain Lang. 100, 69–78. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2006.09.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Broca, P. (1861). Sur le siège de la faculté du langage articulé avec deux observations d'aphémie. Bull. Soc. Anat. Paris 36, 330–357.

Google Scholar

Chang, C. H. C., and Kuo, W. J. (2016). The neural substrates underlying the implementation of phonological rule in lexical tone production: an fMRI study of the tone 3 sandhi phenomenon in Mandarin Chinese. PLoS ONE 11. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0159835

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chang, H. C., Lee, H. J., Tzeng, O. J., and Kuo, W. J. (2014). Implicit target substitution and sequencing for lexical tone production in Chinese: an FMRI study. PLoS ONE 9, e83126. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0083126

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, A., Peter, V., Wijnen, F., Schnack, H., and Burnham, D. (2018). Are lexical tones musical? Native language's influence on neural response to pitch in different domains. Brain Lang. 180–182, 31–41. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2018.04.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, Y., Wong, L. L. N., Qian, J., Kuehnel, V., Christina Voss, S., Chen, F., et al. (2020). The role of lexical tone information in the recognition of mandarin sentences in listeners with hearing aids. Ear Hear. 41, 532–538. doi: 10.1097/AUD.0000000000000774

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chobert, J., Francois, C., Velay, J. L., and Besson, M. (2012). Twelve months of active musical training in 8- to 10-year-old children enhances the preattentive processing of syllabic duration and voice onset time. Cereb Cortex. 24, 956–967. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs377

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Renzi, E., and Faglioni, P. (1978). Normative data and screening power of a shortened version of the Token test. Cortex 14, 41–49. doi: 10.1016/S0010-9452(78)80006-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Deouell, L. Y., Bentin, S., and Soroker, N. (2000). Electrophysiological evidence for an early (pre-attentive) information processing deficit in patients with right hemisphere damage and unilateral neglect. Brain 123, 353–365. doi: 10.1093/brain/123.2.353

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Doeller, C. F., Opitz, B., Mecklinger, A., Krick, C., Reith, W., Schroger, E., et al. (2003). Prefrontal cortex involvement in preattentive auditory deviance detection: neuroimaging and electrophysiological evidence. Neuroimage 20, 1270–1282. doi: 10.1016/S1053-8119(03)00389-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dura-Bernal, S., Wennekers, T., and Denham, S. L. (2012). Top-down feedback in an HMAX-like cortical model of object perception based on hierarchical Bayesian networks and belief propagation. PLoS ONE 7, e48216. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0048216

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gainotti, G., Caltagirone, C., Miceli, G., and Masullo, C. (1981). Selective semantic-lexical impairment of language comprehension in right-brain-damaged patients. Brain Lang. 13, 201–211. doi: 10.1016/0093-934X(81)90090-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gajardo-Vidal, A., Lorca-Puls, D. L., Hope, T. M. H., Jones, O. P., Seghier, M. L., Prejawa, S., et al. (2018). How right hemisphere damage after stroke can impair speech comprehension. Brain 141, 3389–3404. doi: 10.1093/brain/awy270

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gandour, J., Petty, S. H., and Dardarananda, R. (1988). Perception and production of tone in aphasia. Brain Lang. 35, 201–240. doi: 10.1016/0093-934X(88)90109-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gandour, J., Ponglorpisit, S., Khunadorn, F., Dechongkit, S., Boongird, P., Boonklam, R., et al. (1992). Lexical tones in Thai after unilateral brain damage. Brain Lang. 43, 275–307. doi: 10.1016/0093-934X(92)90131-W

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gandour, J., Potisuk, S., Ponglorpisit, S., Dechongkit, S., Khunadorn, F., Boongird, P., et al. (1996). Tonal coarticulation in Thai after unilateral brain damage. Brain Lang. 52, 505–535. doi: 10.1006/brln.1996.0027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ge, J., Peng, G., Lyu, B., Wang, Y., Zhuo, Y., Niu, Z., et al. (2015). Cross-language differences in the brain network subserving intelligible speech. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 112, 2972–2977. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1416000112

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hagoort, P., Brown, C. M., and Swaab, T. Y. (1996). Lexical-semantic event-related potential effects in patients with left hemisphere lesions and aphasia, and patients with right hemisphere lesions without aphasia. Brain 119, 627–649. doi: 10.1093/brain/119.2.627

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

He, X., Bassett, D. S., Chaitanya, G., Sperling, M. R., Kozlowski, L., Tracy, J. I., et al. (2018). Disrupted dynamic network reconfiguration of the language system in temporal lobe epilepsy. Brain 141, 1375–1389. doi: 10.1093/brain/awy042

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hsu, C. H., Lin, S. K., Hsu, Y. Y., and Lee, C. Y. (2014). The neural generators of the mismatch responses to Mandarin lexical tones: an MEG study. Brain Res. 1582, 154–166. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2014.07.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kadyamusuma, M. R., De Bleser, R., and Mayer, J. (2011). Lexical tone disruption in Shona after brain damage. Aphasiology 25, 1239–1260. doi: 10.1080/02687038.2011.590966

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klein, D., Zatorre, R. J., Milner, B., and Zhao, V. (2001). A cross-linguistic PET study of tone perception in Mandarin Chinese and English speakers. Neuroimage 13, 646–653. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2000.0738

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kujala, A., Alho, K., Valle, S., Sivonen, P., Ilmoniemi, R. J., Alku, P., et al. (2002). Context modulates processing of speech sounds in the right auditory cortex of human subjects. Neurosci. Lett. 331, 91–94. doi: 10.1016/S0304-3940(02)00843-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kwok, V. P., Wang, T., Chen, S., Yakpo, K., Zhu, L., Fox, P. T., et al. (2015). Neural signatures of lexical tone reading. Hum. Brain Mapp. 36, 304–312. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22629

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kwok, V. P. Y., Dan, G., Yakpo, K., Matthews, S., Fox, P. T., Li, P., et al. (2017). A meta-analytic study of the neural systems for auditory processing of lexical tones. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 11, 375. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2017.00375

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lam, N. H., Schoffelen, J. M., Udden, J., Hulten, A., and Hagoort, P. (2016). Neural activity during sentence processing as reflected in theta, alpha, beta, and gamma oscillations. Neuroimage. 15, 43–54. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.03.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, N., Wang, S., Wang, X., and Xu, L. (2019). Contributions of lexical tone to Mandarin sentence recognition in hearing-impaired listeners under noisy conditions. J Acoust Soc Am 146, EL99. doi: 10.1121/1.5120543

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, X., and Chen, Y. (2015). Representation and processing of lexical tone and tonal variants: evidence from the mismatch negativity. PLoS ONE 10, e0143097. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0143097

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liang, B., and Du, Y. (2018). The functional neuroanatomy of lexical tone perception: an activation likelihood estimation meta-analysis. Front. Neurosci. 12. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2018.00495

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liberman, A. M., and Whalen, D. H. (2000). On the relation of speech to language. Trends Cogn. Sci. 4, 187–196. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01471-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, L., Peng, D., Ding, G., Jin, Z., Zhang, L., Li, K., et al. (2006). Dissociation in the neural basis underlying Chinese tone and vowel production. Neuroimage 29, 515–523. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.07.046

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Luo, H., Ni, J. T., Li, Z. H., Li, X. O., Zhang, D. R., Zeng, F. G., et al. (2006). Opposite patterns of hemisphere dominance for early auditory processing of lexical tones and consonants. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 103, 19558–19563. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0607065104

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mitchell, R. L., and Crow, T. J. (2005). Right hemisphere language functions and schizophrenia: the forgotten hemisphere? Brain 128(Pt 5), 963–978. doi: 10.1093/brain/awh466

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Naatanen, R., Gaillard, A. W., and Mantysalo, S. (1978). Early selective-attention effect on evoked potential reinterpreted. Acta Psychol. 42, 313–329. doi: 10.1016/0001-6918(78)90006-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Naatanen, R., Lehtokoski, A., Lennes, M., Cheour, M., Huotilainen, M., Iivonen, A., et al. (1997). Language-specific phoneme representations revealed by electric and magnetic brain responses. Nature 385, 432–434. doi: 10.1038/385432a0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Naatanen, R., Pakarinen, S., Rinne, T., and Takegata, R. (2004). The mismatch negativity (MMN): towards the optimal paradigm. Clin. Neurophysiol. 115, 140–144. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2003.04.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nan, Y., and Friederici, A. D. (2013). Differential roles of right temporal cortex and broca's area in pitch processing: evidence from music and mandarin. Hum. Brain Mapp. 34, 2045–2054. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22046

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oldfield, R. C. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia 9, 97–113. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(71)90067-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Opitz, B., Rinne, T., Mecklinger, A., von Cramon, D. Y., and Schroger, E. (2002). Differential contribution of frontal and temporal cortices to auditory change detection: fMRI and ERP results. Neuroimage 15, 167–174. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0970

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Picton, T. W., Alain, C., Otten, L., Ritter, W., and Achim, A. (2000). Mismatch negativity: different water in the same river. Audiol. Neurootol. 5, 111–139. doi: 10.1159/000013875

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Poeppel, D. (2003). The analysis of speech in different temporal integration windows: cerebral lateralization as 'asymmetric sampling in time'. Speech Commun. 41, 245–255. doi: 10.1016/S0167-6393(02)00107-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Posner, M. I., and Petersen, S. E. (1989). The attention system of the human brain. Ann. Neurosci.503, 686–4939.

Google Scholar

Ren, G. Q., Yang, Y., and Li, X. (2009). Early cortical processing of linguistic pitch patterns as revealed by the mismatch negativity. Neuroscience 162, 87–95. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2009.04.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rinne, T., Alho, K., Ilmoniemi, R. J., Virtanen, J., and Naatanen, R. (2000). Separate time behaviors of the temporal and frontal mismatch negativity sources. Neuroimage 12, 14–19. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2000.0591

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Robson, H., Cloutman, L., Keidel, J. L., Sage, K., Drakesmith, M., Welbourne, S., et al. (2014). Mismatch negativity (MMN) reveals inefficient auditory ventral stream function in chronic auditory comprehension impairments. Cortex 59, 113–125. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2014.07.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rorden, C., and Brett, M. (2000). Stereotaxic display of brain lesions. Behav. Neurol. 12, 191–200. doi: 10.1155/2000/421719

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sasaki, Y., Sasaki, M., Kataoka-Sasaki, Y., Nakazaki, M., Nagahama, H., Suzuki, J., et al. (2016). Synergic effects of rehabilitation and intravenous infusion of mesenchymal stem cells after stroke in rats. Phys Ther. 96, 1791–1798. doi: 10.2522/ptj.20150504

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schonwiesner, M., Rubsamen, R., and von Cramon, D. Y. (2005). Hemispheric asymmetry for spectral and temporal processing in the human antero-lateral auditory belt cortex. Eur. J. Neurosci. 22, 1521–1528. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2005.04315.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schumacher, R., Halai, A. D., and Lambon Ralph, M. A. (2019). Assessing and mapping language, attention and executive multidimensional deficits in stroke aphasia. Brain 142, 3202–3216. doi: 10.1093/brain/awz258

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seghier, M. L. (2008). Laterality index in functional MRI: methodological issues. Magn. Reson. Imaging 26, 594–601. doi: 10.1016/j.mri.2007.10.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shankweiler, D., and Studdert-Kennedy, M. (1967). Identification of consonants and vowels presented to left and right ears. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 19, 59–63. doi: 10.1080/14640746708400069

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shankweiler, D., and Studdert-Kennedy, M. (1975). A continuum of lateralization for speech perception? Brain Lang. 2, 212–225. doi: 10.1016/S0093-934X(75)80065-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shtyrov, Y., Kujala, T., Palva, S., Ilmoniemi, R. J., and Naatanen, R. (2000). Discrimination of speech and of complex nonspeech sounds of different temporal structure in the left and right cerebral hemispheres. Neuroimage 12, 657–663. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2000.0646

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Si, X., Zhou, W., and Hong, B. (2017). Cooperative cortical network for categorical processing of Chinese lexical tone. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 114, 12303–12308. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1710752114

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Song, F., Zhan, Y., Ford, J. C., Cai, D. C., Fellows, A. M., Shan, F., et al. (2020). Increased right frontal brain activity during the mandarin hearing-in-noise test. Front. Neurosci. 14, 614012. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2020.614012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Teki, S., Barnes, G. R., Penny, W. D., Iverson, P., Woodhead, Z. V., Griffiths, T. D., et al. (2013). The right hemisphere supports but does not replace left hemisphere auditory function in patients with persisting aphasia. Brain 136(Pt 6), 1901–1912. doi: 10.1093/brain/awt087

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tyler, L. K., Marslen-Wilson, W. D., Randall, B., Wright, P., Devereux, B. J., Zhuang, J., et al. (2011). Left inferior frontal cortex and syntax: function, structure and behaviour in patients with left hemisphere damage. Brain 134(Pt 2), 415–431. doi: 10.1093/brain/awq369

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tyler, L. K., Wright, P., Randall, B., Marslen-Wilson, W. D., and Stamatakis, E. A. (2010). Reorganization of syntactic processing following left-hemisphere brain damage: does right-hemisphere activity preserve function? Brain 133, 3396–3408. doi: 10.1093/brain/awq262

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Walenski, M., Europa, E., Caplan, D., and Thompson, C. K. (2019). Neural networks for sentence comprehension and production: an ALE-based meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies. Hum. Brain Mapp. 40, 2275–2304. doi: 10.1002/hbm.24523

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, X. D., Wang, M., and Chen, L. (2013). Hemispheric lateralization for early auditory processing of lexical tones: dependence on pitch level and pitch contour. Neuropsychologia 51, 2238–2244. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.07.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, X. D., Xu, H., Yuan, Z., Luo, H., Wang, M., Li, H. W., et al. (2021). Brain hemispheres swap dominance for processing semantically meaningful pitch. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 15, 621677. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2021.621677

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wernicke, C. (1874). Der aphasische Symptomen complex: Enie Psychologische Studie auf Anatomischer basis. Breslau: Kohn und Weigert.

Google Scholar

Whalen, D. H., and Liberman, A. M. (1987). Speech perception takes precedence over nonspeech perception. Science 237, 169–171. doi: 10.1126/science.3603014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wong, P. C., Parsons, L. M., Martinez, M., and Diehl, R. L. (2004). The role of the insular cortex in pitch pattern perception: the effect of linguistic contexts. J. Neurosci. 24, 9153–9160. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2225-04.2004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Xi, J., Zhang, L., Shu, H., Zhang, Y., and Li, P. (2010). Categorical perception of lexical tones in Chinese revealed by mismatch negativity. Neuroscience 170, 223–231. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2010.06.077

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zatorre, R. J., and Belin, P. (2001). Spectral and temporal processing in human auditory cortex. Cerebral Cortex 11, 946–953. doi: 10.1093/cercor/11.10.946

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zatorre, R. J., Belin, P., and Penhune, V. B. (2002). Structure and function of auditory cortex: music and speech. Trends Cogn. Sci. 6, 37–46. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01816-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, L., Xi, J., Xu, G., Shu, H., Wang, X., Li, P., et al. (2011). Cortical dynamics of acoustic and phonological processing in speech perception. PLoS ONE6, e20963. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0020963

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhao, Y., Chen, X., Zhong, S., Cui, Z., Gong, G., Dong, Q., et al. (2016). Abnormal topological organization of the white matter network in Mandarin speakers with congenital amusia. Sci. Rep. 6, 26505. doi: 10.1038/srep26505

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhou, W. J., Wang, Z. Y., Wang, S. W., and Zhao, L. (2021). Processing neutral tone under the non-attentional condition: a mismatch negativity study. J. Integr. Neurosci. 20, 131–136. doi: 10.31083/j.jin.2021.01.301

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: hemisphere dominance, lexical tone, mismatch negativity, stroke, brain lesion

Citation: Wei Y, Liang X, Guo X, Wang X, Qi Y, Ali R, Wu M, Qian R, Wang M, Qiu B, Li H, Fu X and Chen L (2022) Brain hemispheres with right temporal lobe damage swap dominance in early auditory processing of lexical tones. Front. Neurosci. 16:909796. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2022.909796

Received: 31 March 2022; Accepted: 01 August 2022;
Published: 26 August 2022.

Edited by:

Fang Liu, University of Reading, United Kingdom

Reviewed by:

Jing Shao, Hong Kong Baptist University, Hong Kong SAR, China
Yang Zhang, University of Minnesota Health Twin Cities, United States

Copyright © 2022 Wei, Liang, Guo, Wang, Qi, Ali, Wu, Qian, Wang, Qiu, Li, Fu and Chen. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Lin Chen, linchen@ustc.edu.cn; Xianming Fu, fuxianmingah@163.com

These authors have contributed equally to this work and share first authorship

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.