- 1Postgraduate Program in Psychobiology, Federal University of Rio Grande do Norte, Natal, Brazil
- 2Institute of Neurobiology, Bulgarian Academy of Sciences, Sofia, Bulgaria
- 3Department of Psychology and Postgraduate Program in Psychobiology, Federal University of Rio Grande do Norte, Natal, Brazil
Objective: Cognitive impairment due to sleep deprivation (SD) is an important global health concern as part of the growing rates of sleep disorders and sleep deprivation worldwide. Amongst the affected cognitive processes, the effects of SD on the executive functions (EFs) show diverse methods and inconclusive or contradictory results, highlighting the importance of further research in this field. Considering this scenario, we evaluate one of the most used methods for objectively evaluating EFs on SD: the event-related potential (ERP) P300.
Methods: Our study provides a comprehensive review on the use of P300 for evaluating executive functions in sleep alterations on subjects of all ages, as well as an analysis on the efficiency of P300 as an assessment method for executive functions compared to traditional neurocognitive batteries. We review the benefits of P300 application for multiple sleep/wake alterations, whether evoked in laboratory or as part of pre-existing sleep disorders.
Results: We assess the diverse protocols used to elicit and complement P300, the most identified alterations in amplitude and latency, and suggest new lines of study that could benefit from P300 within the field.
Conclusion: We conclude that P300 is a valuable asset for evaluating executive dysfunction under sleep deprivation both as a standalone protocol and in conjunction with subjective methods, with consistently significant results in assessing executive dysfunction in a diversity of subjects and etiologies.
Introduction
Sleep alterations
Sleep alterations can be described as disturbances and abnormalities on individual sleep patterns, such as difficulty in initiating sleep, sleep fragmentation, or non-restorative sleep. Those alterations may appear as part of a clinical condition (Zisapel, 2007), a sleep disorder (Tufik et al., 2010), a psychiatric disorder (Borbély and Wirz-Justice, 1982), or as lifestyle consequences (Reynolds and Banks, 2010; Shochat, 2012), and generally result in sleep deprivation (Åkerstedt, 2007).
Sleep deprivation can be divided in two subtypes: total sleep deprivation, characterized by episodes of vigilance equal or above 24 h and absence of 90% of REM and non-REM; and chronic sleep restriction, characterized by a progressive reduction of the total sleep hours over a period of time (Philip et al., 2012). Sleep deprivation has been discovered to negatively affect several cognitive processes, such as short-term memory, selective attention, and executive functioning (Krause et al., 2017), and previous literature indicates that each type is associated to different manners of cognitive impairment. Total sleep deprivation is associated with increased sleepiness, impaired memory consolidation, lapses of vigilant attention (Trošt Bobić et al., 2016), and impaired decision making (Castro and de Almondes, 2018), while chronic sleep restriction is associated with slower processing speed, selective attention lapses, and increased errors of omission (Gradisar et al., 2008; Tucker et al., 2010; Hudson et al., 2020).
Amongst the processes affected by displaced sleep, the executive functions present an intriguing range of significant, yet inconclusive findings (Gradisar et al., 2008; Tucker et al., 2010; Leite Ferreira and Moraes de Almondes, 2014; Lo et al., 2016; Honn et al., 2019; Hudson et al., 2020), with the mechanisms behind sleep-related executive dysfunction not fully understood. To better understand this difficulty and the theoretic attempts to explain it, we must first approach the different perspectives through which those functions can be explored.
Executive functions
Friedman and Miyake (2017) describe the executive functions as “interdependent cognitive processes related to the activation of the prefrontal cortex, which are fundamental for self-regulating and goal-directed behaviors” (Friedman and Miyake, 2017). As higher-level cognitive processes, the executive functions exercise a top-down effect on behavior that allows for planning, strategizing, and working toward objectives (Miyake et al., 2000; Diamond, 2013; Friedman and Miyake, 2017), as well as inhibiting unwanted responses and ineffective strategies (Miyake et al., 2000; Friedman and Miyake, 2017). Despite their importance, there is considerable discussion in literature regarding what processes to classify as executive functions and their interactions, with different models suggested to explain their functioning (Karr et al., 2018).
Considering this on-going discussion, we draw attention to a specific model as a theoretical framework to the current review: that of Miyake et al. (2000) and Diamond (2013). The model is composed of three core processes: cognitive flexibility, working memory, and inhibitory control (which would include impulse control and selective attention) (Diamond, 2013), all with robust supporting evidence of application, measurement, and interrelation both in clinical and research trials (Friedman and Miyake, 2017). From the core functions, other complex regulating functions such as planning, creativity and decision-making would develop, thus creating a complex interrelated system of top-down control over behavior, attention, emotions, and thoughts (Diamond, 2013). Besides these core processes, other cognitive elements fundamental to proper executive functioning would be selective attention, which is directly influenced by inhibitory control and correlates with working memory, and information processing, which correlates with working memory during both development and aging (Diamond, 2013).
Two main hypotheses attempt to explain the relationship between sleep alterations and impairment of the executive functions: The Hypothesis of vigilance or lapses and waking instability (Williams et al., 1959; Doran et al., 2001) and the Frontal Lobe Hypothesis or the vulnerability of the prefrontal cortex (Horne, 1993).
The Hypothesis of Vigilance proposes that interactions between homeostatic sleep pressure and circadian propensity to sleep, both part of the Two Factor Sleep Model by Borbély and Achemann (2000) and Borbély et al. (2016), generate fluctuations in alertness while awake, destabilizing cognitive and neural performance. Such fluctuations would result in attentional lapses, temporary moments during which individuals would exhibit slower responses and deterioration of general processing during tasks that require reaction speed and surveillance (Almondes, 2019). Authors such as Durmer and Dinges (2005) and Lim and Dinges (2010) defend that attention lapses and the subsequent impairment of selective attention would relate to deficits in more complex cognitive processes, like the executive functions. However, studies relating to the three core executive functions and to short-term memory show contradictory results that this hypothesis does not fully explain.
The second hypothesis, that of Vulnerability of the Prefrontal Cortex, argues that sleep alterations affect the neurophysiological functioning of the prefrontal cortex, which relates to impairment in executive functions (Babkoff et al., 2005). The main support for this hypothesis comes from studies grounded in neuroimaging techniques, which show cortical metabolic dysfunctions associated with cognitive impairment of executive functions (Thomas et al., 2000; Killgore et al., 2006, 2008). However, other neuroimaging studies show several other areas affected by sleep alterations beyond the prefrontal cortex; thus, it is not certain if this structure is the single neurophysiological correlate to executive dysfunction (Chee and Choo, 2004; Scullin, 2017; Javaheipour et al., 2019). In addition to this, performance in more complex executive tasks, such as verbal working memory information manipulation tasks, tends to be preserved even when cortical metabolic alterations are assessed (Chee and Choo, 2004; Tucker et al., 2010). To explain these contradictory results, authors such as Staffen et al. (2002) have proposed the existence of cognitive compensatory mechanisms that could be triggered in response to increased task difficulty—which is called the Compensatory Adaptation hypothesis (Drummond and Brown, 2001; Staffen et al., 2002).
Considering the above discussion, objective studies that assess any of the three aforementioned hypothesis can have great value for our understanding of the neurophysiological and neurocognitive mechanisms behind executive dysfunction. Besides neuroimaging exams and neuropsychological tests, other possible resources in this scenario are Event-Related Potentials (ERPs), which we will detail on the following session.
Event-related potentials: P300
Event-Related Potentials are endogenous electromagnetic wave patterns that can be measured through electroencephalography, with different patterns that can be directly correlated to vigilance, consciousness, and exposure to stimuli (Jiang et al., 2015). Another common reading and interpretation to the ERP acronym is Evoked Response Potential (Legatt, 2014), in reference to patterns that can be reliably invoked in research settings through appropriate stimuli. Of the recorded ERPs in literature, one of the most studied regarding executive functioning is the peak positive wave known as P3 or P300, named as it usually occurs on a range around 300 ms after the presentation of a stimulus (Polich, 2007).
This P300 wave is assessed through latency, which relates to the time needed to process information, and through amplitude, which is related to allocation of attention by the subject (Polich, 2007). As such, P300 can be a useful resource in research concerning cognitive processes that rely heavily on attention and information processing, including the executive functions. The construct itself has been sufficiently correlated with core executive functions such as inhibitory control and working memory (Legatt, 2014; Kim et al., 2017). There is also evidence to suggest that P300 is connected to individual variations on reaction time (Ramchurn et al., 2014), which in turn connects to information processing speed; as such, this variable is also frequently considered in studies that analyze P300.
There are two subcomponents for P300 that have been identified in literature: P3a and P3b. P3b is the oldest discovered P300 subtype, associated with sustained attention and with its main source located around the parietal region. As expected in being the first discovered subtype, P3b's highest peak is at around 300 ms after the stimulus (Polich, 2007). P3a, being a later discovery, peaks a few milliseconds before P3b, at 250–280 ms. Its main source is situated on the frontal and central areas of the brain, and it is mostly associated with the recognition of recently introduced, novelty stimulus, such as a rare stimulus in an oddball paradigm task (Polich, 2007). It may also be an observed component of ERP reaction to non-target stimuli when novelty distractions are introduced, such as a dog bark amidst a number sequency (Kim et al., 2014). As the novelty stimulus becomes known, P3a amplitude is reduced (Polich, 2007). This contrasts with the so-called “NoGo P300,” where a P300 pattern is found in response exclusively to target stimuli and absent during already known distractions (Polich, 2007).
The most used method to access both P300 subtypes is through the oddball paradigm, in which the subject is instructed to discriminate a rare stimulus amidst more frequent, irrelevant stimuli. This paradigm can be administered either in auditory or visual form, and both successfully evoke P300. Between those, auditory stimulation is more commonly used and deemed more effective than visual stimulation (Polich, 2007). Alternative paradigms that can also evoke P300 include the N-back task, the Go/No-Go task, the Sternberg Working Memory Task, and the Stroop Task.
Considering the growing prevalence of sleep alterations and sleep disorders worldwide (Jahrami et al., 2021), as well as the cognitive impairment related to them, the present study intends to review pertaining literature regarding the use and efficiency of P300 to assess impairment of the executive functions under sleep deprivation on subjects of all ages. The executive functions are here understood through the model of Miyake et al. (2000) and Diamond (2013), and the term sleep alteration is here used to englobe alterations in sleep patterns brought both by sleep disorders of all etiologies and by sleep deprivation whether total or partial. We aim to summarize the previous findings on this subject throughout the years for different populations, assess the effectiveness of P300 as a tool for this objective in addition to and compared to traditional neuropsychological assessment, and propose new possibilities for studies in this developing area.
Methodology
Guidelines for integrative reviews, inclusion and exclusion criteria
This is an integrative review of literature, following the stage process for integrative reviews detailed by Whittemore and Knafl (2005) in their epidemiological study. The inclusion criteria for our review are the following: studies regarding sleep alterations (such as sleep deprivation, sleep restriction, and sleep disorders) that used P3a and/or P3b with the objective to assess executive functions on subjects of any age or gender. The exclusion criteria are the following: studies that did not include P3a/P3b in any form; studies that did not evaluate any of the executive functions; studies that did not evaluate sleep alterations; studies that evaluated sleep alterations as symptoms or risk factors of neurodegenerative diseases and/or other conditions that affect the executive functioning independently; and studies that were not in English.
For this study, we did not delimitate publication dates as exclusion criteria, as one of our goals is for a comprehensive review of all published data on the subject throughout the years. However, given that the present review was conducted solely through electronic databases, the access to older articles was limited, as not all of them have been digitalized.
Given that we aim to assess the effectiveness of P300 in multiple contexts and with different populations, we did not exclude articles based on the age of the participants. Although this limits the comparisons that can be made between subjects of different ages, we find this limitation necessary to conduct a review as in-depth on P300 application as possible.
Search strategy
Five different electronic databases were selected for review: PubMed, PsychInfo, Scopus, Cochrane, and Web of Science. The keywords and descriptors selected were “sleep” OR “sleep disorder” OR “sleep alteration” OR “sleep deprivation” + “executive functions” OR “executive function” + “evoked potential” OR “p300” OR “evoked cognitive potential” OR “auditory evoked potential” OR “event related potential” OR “p3”. No further filters or descriptors were used in any of the databases.
Study selection
The literature review for this article was conducted between May 2019 and May 2020. Studies were initially selected for further analysis by a reviewer per their abstracts and keywords, as detailed on the Search Strategy section. One hundred and twenty-seven results fitting those criteria were selected between the five databases, with the highest number of matches for all keywords being on Scopus (67 articles total). Amongst those 127 results, 19 articles were too old to be fully indexed on the databases (with only the abstracts available) or had broken links; we then attempted to contact the respective authors, or to find otherwise viable links through other indexing sites and/or the respective journal's websites. Of the 19 missing articles, 3 were excluded due to lack of viable links or contact of the authors, lowering the total number of selected articles from 127 to 124.
Of the remaining 124 studies selected, 39 were excluded for being duplicates posted in multiple databases, and 2 were excluded for not being in English, totalizing 83 articles apt for the next stage.
In the reading stage, the 83 articles were fully read through by a reviewer and searched further for the same keywords used during in the initial selection stage. In this stage, 54 studies were excluded for not addressing the research questions or properly fulfilling the inclusion criteria. The remaining 29 articles were according to the criteria and selected for the review itself.
Each step of the review and the number of papers appraised can be seen in detail on Figure 1.
Data collection and analysis
The 29 articles selected for the review stage had all relevant information extracted to a spreadsheet in Microsoft Excel, classified according to the following the categories: Authors and country of origin, objectives of the study, participants, study design (including EFs assessed, as we identified whether each design focused on working memory, inhibitory control, cognitive flexibility or general, non-specific executive performance), use of P300, and conclusion. Besides the information from each study, a blank space was available on the spreadsheet for further commentary on each article from the reviewer, such as clarification of any doubts or criticism of the methodology used. Any quantitative data was presented on the spreadsheet in frequency, percentage, and mean according to what was used in the original study.
Results
Characterization of selected studies
Twenty-nine cross-sectional studies were included in this review. Of those, 14 were case control studies, 11 were randomized control trials, and 4 were controlled clinical trials. The studies were conducted in 12 different countries, with South Korea alone being responsible for 5 articles. The United States, Turkey, and China contributed with 4 articles each. Canada produced 3 articles, Australia and India produced 2 each, and Germany, Sweden, Egypt, and Taiwan each had 1 selected article.
Considering the conditions explored, 16 studies focused on sleep disorders: 8 articles focused on Obstructive Sleep Apnea Syndrome (OSA or OSAS), including 1 article that discussed Sleep Disordered Breathing (SDB) in children; 4 articles discussed Restless Legs Syndrome (RLS), with 1 being a clinical trial for the effects of pramipexole on RSL patients; 2 articles discussed Shift Work Disorder (SWD) in regards to night shift workers; 1 article investigated inhibitory control deficits in Insomnia Disorder (ID) and another discussed the effects of modafinil on Idiopathic Hypersomnia (IH) patients.
Another 13 studies focused on both types of sleep deprivation and sleep alterations. Eight articles discussed Sleep Deprivation, with 7 employing Total Sleep Deprivation (TSD) protocols, and 1 investigating Chronic Partial Sleep Deprivation/Restriction (CPSD) in healthy subjects, while 5 articles discussed Sleep Fragmentation (SF) and subsequent SD caused by auditory stimuli.
Regarding the core EFs assessed, 11 studies discussed inhibitory control, 15 assessed working memory, and 4 explored cognitive flexibility, with 10 exploring more than one of the core EFs, or all three. Related processes, such as selective attention, were considered in 9 studies, while reaction time, a variable connected both to P300 and EFs, was considered in 7 studies. Approximately 8 studies did not disclose in detail which EFs were assessed, referring only to the general concept of executive functioning, not its underlying processes. It is important to note that there were theoretical disagreements on the concept of EFs across the selected papers: studies such as Cha et al. (2017) did not classify working memory as an executive process, but as mnesic. Others, such as Schapkin et al. (2006), described working memory as a fundamentally executive process. Such divergencies are part of the diversity of models for cognitive and executive functions in literature, and as such are to be expected and taken into consideration during this review.
Role of P300 across selected studies
Considering the role of P300 across studies, 50% used it as an electrophysiological correlation to neurocognitive deficits, with supporting neuropsychological tests to assess those deficits. At least one study focused on comparing the efficiency of both methods as indicators of cognitive impairment Yerlikaya et al. (2018). The other 50% used only P300 components to assess impairment of the executive functions, without any supporting neuropsychological evaluation. For the type of stimulus used to elicit P300 on subjects, most studies (18 total) used visual stimulation, such as a visual Go/No Go, Visual Oddball paradigm, or N-Back task. The other 13 studies used auditory stimulation, with the great majority of those (11 total) using the classic auditory oddball paradigm. Only 2 studies opted for an auditory stop-signal task.
It is important to note the greater variability of visual methods used to elicit P300 when compared to auditory methods: a total of 8 different visual tasks vs. 2 main choices for auditory designs.
Most studies identified P3a as the third positive peak between the expected latencies of 200–300 ms, and P3b as the third positive peak between 400 and 600 ms. This is congruent with the most frequent definitions in literature (Polich, 2007). Some, such as Ko et al. (2015), considered the third peak up to 700 ms depending on the age and health of participants.
There was significant variation on average P300 latency and amplitude values between articles, which is likely related to the diversity of age and gender of participants, the presence and etiology of sleep alterations, and the methods used to evoke P300 across studies (Polich and Corey-Bloom, 2005). Most studies selected did not discriminate P300 based on sex, age, or other demographic information of the participants. Despite previous evidence in literature that there may be significant sex-related variations in P300 elicited through visual paradigms (Steffensen et al., 2008), this difference was not assessed nor provided by any of the reviewed articles. About 8 studies used single-sex samples, but even amongst those, the methodological and etiological differences between subjects were too high to properly assess differences between the samples.
Normative values of latency and amplitude of P300 during sleep alterations could not be identified due to the wide variety of etiological and methodological characteristics amongst studies, such as whether the alterations related to a sleep disorder or were caused by a sleep deprivation protocol, the number and location of electrodes, and the different stimulation procedures.
Regardless of etiology, only one study did not report any variation of P300 components related to sleep alterations: that of Cote et al. (2003). All others reported either prolonged latency (Tufik et al., 2010), reduced amplitude (Leite Ferreira and Moraes de Almondes, 2014), or both alterations (Tucker et al., 2010), either when comparing results between groups or the same participants before experimental intervention. Those same variations of P300 components were associated with impairment of the executive functions, especially inhibitory control (Renn and Cote, 2013; Zhang et al., 2014), with results on working memory variating according to etiology (Gelir et al., 2014; Jung et al., 2015). Other significant processes to the executive functioning, such as sustained attention, vigilance, and information processing also had their impairment correlated to the P300 components (Lee et al., 2003; Kusztor et al., 2019). Overall, the assorted data indicates that P300 can be a useful electrophysiological indicator of deficits to all those processes.
The most used P300 subtype was the classic P3b, with most articles (24 total) making little distinction between both, or mentioning P3a only in the introduction. Of the remaining articles, 3 defined and explored both P3a and P3b in their studies. Only 2, those of Macdonald et al. (2008) and Zhao et al. (2018) dealt exclusively with P3a. Some, such as Schapkin et al. (2007) and Qi et al. (2010) referred to P3a as P3-NoGo, for its association to the recognition of stimuli before inhibition in the No Go step of the Go/No Go task.
Further detail on all assessed studies can be seen on Table 1, including methodology and P300 application.
Table 1. Details of selected articles, according to following categories: authors and country of origin, objectives of the study, participants, study design, use of P300, and conclusion.
Discussion
General results
At least two of the articles conclude that P300 may, in fact, be a more accurate measurement of executive functions impairment than neurocognitive testing (Kim et al., 2014; Yerlikaya et al., 2018), as it can be more sensitive to initial deficits that neurocognitive or neuropsychological batteries do not identify. However, these initial deficits did not associate with impaired functioning of the research subjects, which complicates the clinical application of P300 as a preventive method of screening for executive dysfunction outside of a research context. The absence of impaired functioning limits the potential of field professionals, such as doctors, to recognize the possibility of cognitive impairment until its onset. It is important to note as well that P300 does not discriminate impairment to a single executive process, such as solely working memory or inhibitory control, or even solely selective attention. Thus, utilizing only P300 as basis for clinical evaluation could prove limited in that regard.
Despite the above points, we consider that P300 could be a valuable tool in early diagnosis of executive dysfunction, especially if parallel to neuropsychological evaluation for a more detailed analysis of executive impairment. Considering the lower cost of EEG compared to complex neuroimaging techniques, P300 could be especially useful for the purpose of early-sign evaluation in lower income contexts.
A few of the reviewed studies seem to agree that the executive deficits evaluated would be related to lower alertness and increased processing time, rather than impairment to the mechanisms behind the executive processes themselves (Lee et al., 2003). This proposition fits with the previously mentioned Hypothesis of vigilance or lapses and waking instability, as a strong correlation was found between reduced P300 amplitude and delayed latency to attention deficits in several of the studies, which would point toward increased attention lapses that could affect executive functioning (Trošt Bobić et al., 2016; Castro and de Almondes, 2018). On the other hand, other studies found evidence of executive dysfunction in different etiologies being associated with abnormal functioning of frontal cortex structures (Jung et al., 2015; Cha et al., 2017), which is more in line with the frontal lobe hypothesis or the vulnerability of the prefrontal cortex, proposed by Horne (1993). These studies suggest that sleep deprivation directly affects the efficiency of the prefrontal cortex, resulting in alterations of the brain metabolism that can be visualized through EEG patterns, and thus could affect executive functioning independently of the attention pathways.
It is possible such differences could happen regarding the methodology employed by each design, as tasks that are more difficult would necessitate higher cognitive processes and automatic tasks would be more vulnerable to attention lapses due to monotony. However, this supposition would not explain all results, as discussed further below.
Both the hypothesis of vigilance and the frontal lobe hypothesis attempt to explain the executive function deficits caused by sleep alterations. However, the results pertaining to both remain mixed and inconsistent, which highlights the necessity of studies designed to further explore and test their applications.
To further discuss the results of this review, we divided the selected articles into three sessions: the first two focus on P300 and EFs in the context of sleep disorders, such as Obstructive Sleep Apnea (OSAS), Restless Legs Syndrome, Insomnia Disorder, and others, with a division between OSAS studies and studies dedicated to other disorders. A category solely for OSAS was necessary due to the sheer number of papers dedicated to this condition. The third session focuses on P300 and EFs in regard to sleep alterations, which included sleep deprivation protocols, sleep fragmentation, and sleep restriction.
This division is merely for easier understanding and discussion of P300 application for each condition by grouping similar subjects together.
P300 and executive functions in sleep disorders
Obstructive sleep apnea
Obstructive Sleep Apnea Syndrome (OSAS) is a sleep disorder characterized by partial or total interruption of the upper airways during sleep, causing sleep alterations such as sleep fragmentation, morning drowsiness, and reduced vigilance. It is associated with, amongst other health issues, increased risks of cardiovascular diseases (Gonzaga et al., 2015), diabetes (Reutrakul and Mokhlesi, 2017), and psychiatric disorders (Gupta and Simpson, 2015), as well as cognitive dysfunctions. The results regarding cognitive impairment in OSAS show deficits on vigilance, attention, executive functions, psychomotor processes, and memory associated with this sleep disorder (Lal et al., 2012; Seda and Han, 2020), with considerable individual variation.
In this review, reduced amplitude was associated with executive impairment in 37.5% of studies regarding OSAS. Twenty-five found alteration in both P300 components, presenting increased latency and reduced amplitude values. One study, that of Barnes et al. (2012), explored only the increased latency.
Lower amplitude values were correlated with executive impairment even in very early stages (Wong et al., 2006; Gelir et al., 2014; Yerlikaya et al., 2018) as well as with impaired attention (Vakulin et al., 2014). El-Gharib and Sharsher (2017) found increased executive performance, as well as shorter P300 latency and increased amplitude, after 3 months of regular CPAP use by OSAS patients, though their results remained below the scores of healthy controls. Ak et al. (2017) found a similar result when comparing OSAS patients' score post-treatment with their pre-CPAP scores, and when comparing both pre- and post-CPAP scores to age and gender matched controls. Gelir et al. (2014) correlated increased latency and lower amplitude with impairments in attention and executive functions on OSAS subjects, but found no significant relations to either working or procedural memory. Interestingly, the author does not seem to consider working memory as one of the executive functions, instead classifying it only as a memory component.
Barnes et al. (2012) identified reduced latency and a lower performance in neurocognitive batteries on OSAS child patients compared to controls, as well as a reverse activation pattern on P300. Their results suggest that OSAS-affected children process information for more complex tasks in regions associated with simpler processing, such as the amygdala or temporal lobe, while healthy children process information in areas such as the precuneus, correlated with more complex processes (Zhong et al., 2013). Their results are interesting to consider in the light of the compensatory adaptation hypothesis, as it may point to the precuneus as a potential compensatory point in this stage of development.
Important to note that an executive impairment does not necessarily relate to impaired functioning. Despite most of the articles presenting significant results comparing P300 components to control groups or neuropsychological tests, not all data was significant when correlated to functions of the patient's daily life, such as driving. Vakulin et al. (2014) investigated the effects of OSAS on driving capability, as previous literature identified that OSAS patients are more at risk for traffic accidents related to morning drowsiness (George, 2007). Their assessment showed that about 60% of OSAS patients showed no significant difference from the performance of healthy controls, under either sleep deprivation or alcohol. Such results may be due to individual sensitivity to the effects of OSAS-induced sleep fragmentation, as discussed further in the P300 and Executive Functions in Sleep Deprivation session.
Regarding interesting resources for further OSAS research, Wong et al. (2006) showed the efficiency of an algorithm that could, based on demographic factors and OSAS symptoms, be a tool to select a group of individuals manifesting likely OSAS-related deficits in information processing and executive function, while Yerlikaya et al. (2018) concluded that ERPs such as P300 are more effective in detecting initial executive dysfunction in OSAS patients than neuropsychological testing.
Overall, the tendency of the surveyed articles points to P300 being an excellent resource for evaluation of OSAS patients in the research field. However, as expressed above, there are certain difficulties in implementing P300 as a clinical tool in OSAS treatment centers, as more information needs to be assessed on the reliability of P300 results for this population.
P300 and executive functions in other sleep disorders
Considering Shift Work Disorder (SWD), both Belcher et al. (2015) and Choudhary et al. (2016) found results associating increased P300 latency and reduced amplitude to increased attention lapses and delayed reaction time in night turn workers. In line with the hypothesis of vigilance, Belcher et al. (2015) suggests that impairments to executive performance, especially in relation to shift work, likely relate to this attentional dysfunction rather than direct impairment to more complex processes such as working memory. It is important to note, however, the limitations of generalizing their results when their executive assessment relayed on the Endicott Work Productivity Scale, which is a self-application instrument not designed for in-depth cognitive evaluation (Endicott and Nee, 1997). Further research with stricter EF assessment methodology would be useful in expanding the reliability and generalization of their results.
In addition to the already discussed data, both Belcher et al. (2015) and Choudhary et al. (2016) point to the effectiveness of P300 as a tool to investigate executive dysfunction in Shift Work Disorder. However, both utilized only P300 as an assessment tool for executive dysfunction, which limits the possibilities of more detailed correlations to each of the core EFs: as P300 can be employed as correlate to either working memory or inhibitory control, for example, it is not possible to compare how each function is affected exclusively through this method. Further research comparing how P300 would correlate to accompanying neuropsychological assessment in Shift Work Disorder could be useful in this regard.
A few studies utilized P300 as an instrument while investigating cognitive deficits on patients with Restless Legs Syndrome (RLS). RLS is characterized by a compelling urge to move the legs, accompanied by an unpleasant tingling sensation, with both symptoms more common during the night (Klingelhoefer et al., 2016). Despite the motor nature of the disorder, cognitive impairments have been associated with it, especially in older patients (Klingelhoefer et al., 2016).
The P300 values on most reviewed studies show prolonged latency and reduced amplitude on RLS patients compared to control group, suggestive of cognitive impairment, as well as indication of abnormal activation of the frontal and cortical regions (Jung et al., 2011, 2015; Kim et al., 2014; Cha et al., 2017), which is relevant in the light of the vulnerability of the frontal cortex/compensatory adaptation hypothesis. In particular, Kim et al. (2014) found that impaired P300 amplitude correlated with sleep and neurocognitive results that suggest patients with severe RLS have pronounced working memory impairment that relates to such cortical dysfunctions. In another study following similar results, Jung et al. (2015) concluded that nightly doses of pramipexole before sleep significantly increased P300 amplitude post-treatment, pointing to this substance as a possible future treatment in mitigating working memory impairment for this population.
Overall, P300 is shown to be a useful resource in investigating possible frontal effects of RLS episodes in patients, as well as the effectiveness of possible treatments. Once again, more detailed studies with neuropsychological comparisons would be useful in comprehending which domains are most affected and how, especially in relation to the cortical dysfunction assessed in most of the reviewed studies. Similarly, neuroimaging studies that could trace parallels between the EEG patterns and specific area activations in this population could be useful for a better understanding of its metabolically functioning, as well as providing an easier, cheaper tool in clinical settings for diagnosis and treatment options for this group.
Regarding insomnia disorder, there was only one study eligible for this review, that of Zhao et al. (2018), which investigated whether ID patients present inhibitory control deficits and possible neural correlates. The findings suggest that individuals with ID show impairment to inhibitory control, as well as being the first study to identify an electrophysiological correlate of those deficits through P3a. A single study does not allow us to evaluate how P300 could aid in assessment and treatment of insomnia disorder. However, the results found by Ray et al. (2012) are promising and show the possibility of further applications of this methodology in future studies with this population.
Yaman et al. (2015) investigated the effects of modafinil use on P300 components of a population with Idiopathic Hypersomnia, a chronic sleep disorder that is characterized by hypersomnolence and debilitating sleep inertia (Billiard and Sonka, 2016). Their results point to a decrease in P300 latency post-treatment (Yaman et al., 2015). Similarly to ID, a single study is not enough for us to assess the effectiveness of P300 in clinical or research contexts with IH patients. However, it does suggest further possibilities for more studies with this population in the future, such as comparing baseline IH P300 latency to that of healthy controls or patients of other sleep alterations.
P300 and executive functions in sleep deprivation
Considering the type of sleep deprivation discussed, 4 of the selected studies utilized a total sleep deprivation protocol, while 1 utilized a partial sleep deprivation protocol. Overall, 62.5% of those studies found associations between reduced P300 amplitudes and executive dysfunction under sleep deprivation, a considerably higher percentage than those that found this association for OSAS patients. This reduced amplitude correlated significantly with impairments to sustained attention (Kusztor et al., 2019), vigilance (Lee et al., 2003), and information processing (Liu et al., 2015), similarly to the results found in OSAS patients. Amongst the executive functions, inhibitory control had the most significant results (Renn and Cote, 2013; Zhang et al., 2014), with suggestions of impairment to the behavioral control mechanisms and impulse inhibition during total sleep deprivation. Results suggest that the reduced amplitude is more prominent in P3a than in P3b, at least in Go/No Go tasks (Qi et al., 2010), which would suggest more impact of total deprivation on selective attention.
Amongst selected studies, 37.5% identified longer P300 latency when compared to the participant's own performance before TSD, while one found no changes in latency compared to the control group (Renn and Cote, 2013). Kusztor et al. (2019) suggests that cognitive processes that demand more of the brain processing capacity are more affected by sleep deprivation than automatic processes, which would explain a prolonged latency. This possibility would go against the compensatory adaptation hypothesis for impairment of executive functions, as it would point to a better performance in easier tasks. Similarly, Lee et al. (2003) points that P300 alterations during total sleep deprivation can be correlated to a decrease in vigilance and a longer reaction time, which goes in line with increased attentional lapses of the hypothesis of vigilance.
On the other hand, Liu et al. (2015) found evidence that total sleep deprivation influences information processing of affected individuals, to a further extent than even extreme conditions such as complete social isolation. The authors theorize that the use of a simpler task such as Go/No-Go prevented the activation of compensatory mechanisms and allowed for a clearer understanding of the impairment, in line with the compensatory adaptation hypothesis.
Ray et al. (2012) and Zhang et al. (2014) investigated the efficacy of two substances as possible countermeasures for the effects of sleep deprivation induced cognitive impairment: modafinil and zaleplon. Drug efficiency studies such as these are useful in observing the degree of precision P300 can have in detecting executive and attentional dysfunctions in a research setting, especially concerning pharmaceutical innovation, in which such precision can be vital. They are equally useful in adding to the repertoire of successful P300 applications in provoked sleep deprivation protocols, as such protocols are intrinsically necessary in verifying drug efficiency.
Overall, the above results point to P300 being a useful tool for executive function evaluation in sleep deprivation research protocol designs, as it provides an especially sensitive objective assessment that can be reapplied and compared throughout the experimental period without the additional expenses or machinery necessary for more complex neuroimaging exams.
About partial sleep deprivation, Molfese et al. (2013) was the only article to study the impacts of sleep restriction, by sleep depriving children for 1 h before their usual bedtime for 1 week. Similar to what was found in adults, the children showed reduced P300 amplitude associated with executive dysfunction. The results suggest that minor sleep restriction already affects children's neurocognitive functioning, which is according to previous literature (Vriend et al., 2015). Again, a single paper is not enough to assess the importance of P300 as a tool in sleep restriction study designs. However, this experience highlights the successful use of P300 even in non-laboratory sleep restriction conditions, as the participants of this study returned to their own homes during the sleep restriction period instead of sleeping in a controlled environment. Despite not allowing more precise comparisons as a laboratory study would, the use of P300 in this design still permitted correlation to concurrent neuropsychological testing, thus resulting in a more in-depth, detailed evaluation of possible executive dysfunction in a wider variety of scenarios.
Another important sleep alteration related to sleep deprivation is sleep fragmentation, a sleep period intercut by brief arousals, with or without consciousness or cortical activation. While one of the core symptoms for OSAS, sleep fragmentation can be caused by a myriad of other factors and etiologies.
Schapkin and collaborators (Schapkin et al., 2006, 2007) found evidence that sleep fragmentation due to auditory interference may impact and modulate inhibitory processes, as well as that motivation variables can modulate this executive control and stimulus-response during sleep. Their results suggest that individuals with more sensitivity to sleep fragmentation show an increase in latency and reduced amplitude that would correlate to impairment of inhibitory control, especially in more difficult tasks. Such results are interesting in the light of reduced amplitude shown in studies on conditions associated with sleep fragmentation, such as OSAS (Wong et al., 2006; Gelir et al., 2014; Yerlikaya et al., 2018), and sensitivity to sleep fragmentation-related impairment could potentially explain some of the variations found amongst OSAS patients. Similarly, in Schapkin et al. (2006, 2007) no noticeable loss of functioning nor performance as assessed by neurocognitive tasks was found after sleep fragmentation episodes, with impairment shown only through ERP data, comparable to what was found by Vakulin et al. (2014) about the performance of OSAS drivers.
Macdonald et al. (2008) found comparable results to those of Schapkin et al. (2006) and Schapkin et al. (2007), in which P3a showed reduced amplitude following fragmented sleep. This result is particularly interesting, as Macdonald et al. (2008) focused on investigating the impact of SF on executive functions during wakeness after REM sleep. The authors reason that this discovery could correlate to an interruption on central executive functioning during sleep, which would allow awareness of external stimuli and possibly lead to micro-awakenings, which are once again similar, but of a significantly different etiology, to the micro-awakenings of OSAS patients.
From a different standpoint than that of Macdonald et al. (2008) and Ko et al. (2015) investigated the effects of sleep fragmentation on error monitoring processes, related to the processes of inhibitory control and working memory. Their results were independent from the total number of sleep hours, suggesting that this executive impairment is related to the fragmentation itself, and not to overall deprivation caused by arousal episodes. Their findings contrast that of Gelir et al. (2014), who found no evidence of working memory deficits in OSAS patients.
Another article, that of Cote et al. (2003), investigated the brain physiology associated with sleep deprivation caused by auditory-induced sleep fragmentation. P300 components were consistent throughout the entire experiment, with no changes in latency nor amplitude compared to baseline conditions, though processing impairment was suggested by other ERP data. The authors reason that their method for inducing sleep fragmentation could influence the stable P300 data, though the low sample size of 8 individuals and the technical difficulties during the experiment may have also played a part in the results.
Conclusion
In conclusion, the reviewed scientific evidence suggests that there is a decrease in P300 amplitude and increase in latency under the effect of sleep alterations, both in sleep disorders and sleep deprivation. Reduced amplitude is related to executive dysfunction in OSAS patients and sleep deprived individuals; prolonged latency is also associated to impairment of executive functioning but with less conclusive evidence.
Overall, P300 has proven to be a useful tool for the evaluation of executive and attentional dysfunction in a wide variety of individuals of different ages, educational levels and socioeconomic conditions. It is shown to be an accurate measurement of executive dysfunction either alongside neurocognitive/neuropsychological tests or as the main executive assessment in a research protocol and has potential as a preventive or early sign assessment tool in clinical settings. However, unlike in traditional neuropsychological assessment, cognitive evaluation solely through P300 does not pinpoint any specific details in presentation and severity of impairment of different cognitive processes. For example, P300 testing is unable to discern executive dysfunction that affects working memory to a greater extent than inhibitory control, which could be assessed through a neuropsychological battery. This should be considered in future study designs.
This review identifies the necessity of further studies exploring the hypotheses of vigilance and frontal/compensatory adaptation in executive impairment under sleep alterations, as mixed results are associated with both throughout the assessed articles. Methodological reasons could partially explain such mixed results: across the different studies, P300 was utilized as correlate to a variety of processes such as working memory, inhibitory control, general executive functioning, and selective attention. Although there is a previous body of work to substantiate this use (Kim et al., 2014), there are no standard parameters on how to apply P300 as a correlate to each function, and no consensus amongst authors on how to evaluate and to interpret results for each of those functions. While there are attempts to adapt P300 application protocols to the focus of the study design, such as using a task like N-back to evoke P300 for a working memory assessment, the lack of consensus amongst authors results in difficulty to examine how P300 can be used to assess reliably and differentially such closely related processes.
Previous literature points to P300 as a correlate to information discrimination and working memory (Polich, 2007; Kim et al., 2017), with robust frontal peaks associated with P3a (Polich, 2007), and this interpretation leans toward the frontal cortex hypothesis. However, there is also strong evidence that P300 is modulated by attention, as more attentional resources on a task result in smaller P3 amplitude (Polich, 2007), which can be further related to the hypothesis of vigilance. Once more, the importance of a standard practice and consensus on the use of P300 for different cognitive processes would facilitate comparisons for both hypotheses.
There is also need for more longitudinal studies on P300's efficiency at identifying evolving executive impairment, since all studies included were cross-sectional.
We suggest that P300 may be a useful asset on studies about other related factors to sleep alterations, such as exposure to light, melatonin levels, and technology use. It could as well be useful in investigating executive dysfunctions in partial sleep deprivation, or in other conditions not contemplated here: for example, narcolepsy, in which sleep fragmentation is a common complaint (Scammell, 2015).
As limitations for this review, we can note the limited access to older studies due to the limitations of virtual databases, as well as the small number of studies included resulting in a smaller sample size and varying methodology that limits further statistical analysis. The small pool of articles also limits the results we can assess on each core EF, as not all papers discriminated between them nor detailed sufficiently their theoretical basis.
Author contributions
NL: research, review of articles, manuscript writing, and elaborating tables and figures. KA and RK: revision and corrections. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.
Funding
This study was supported by Coordenação de Aperfeiçoamento de Pessoal de Nível Superior - Brasil (CAPES) - Finance Code 001.
Conflict of interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Publisher's note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
References
Åkerstedt, T. (2007). Altered sleep/wake patterns and mental performance. Physiol. Behav. 90, 209–218. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2006.09.007
Ak, A. K., Sari, U. S., Oktan, B., Korkmaz, T., Horasan, G. D., Selcuki, D., et al. (2017). Evaluation of cognitive function using objective and subjective tests in the obstructive sleep apnea syndrome. J. Turkish Sleep Med. 4, 76–84. doi: 10.4274/jtsm.88597
Babkoff, H., Zukerman, G. I. L., Fostick, L., and Ben-Artzi, E. (2005). Effect of the diurnal rhythm and 24 h of sleep deprivation on dichotic temporal order judgment. J. Sleep Res. 14, 7–15. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2869.2004.00423.x
Barnes, M. E., Gozal, D., and Molfese, D. L. (2012). Attention in children with obstructive sleep apnoea: an event-related potentials study. Sleep Med. 13, 368–377. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2011.06.007
Belcher, R., Gumenyuk, V., and Roth, T. (2015). Insomnia in shift work disorder relates to occupational and neurophysiological impairment. J. Clin. Sleep Med. 11, 457–465. doi: 10.5664/jcsm.4606
Billiard, M., and Sonka, K. (2016). Idiopathic hypersomnia. Sleep Med. Rev. 29, 23–33. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2015.08.007
Borbély, A. A., and Achemann, P. (2000). “Sleep homeostasis and models of sleep regulation,” in Principles and Practice of Sleep Medicine, eds M. H. Kryger, T. Roth, and W. C. Dement (Stanford, CA: W. B. Saunders Company), 377–390.
Borbély, A. A., Daan, S., Wirz-Justice, A., and Deboer, T. (2016). The two-process model of sleep regulation: a reappraisal. J. Sleep Res. 25, 131–143. doi: 10.1111/jsr.12371
Borbély, A. A., and Wirz-Justice, A. (1982). Sleep, sleep deprivation and depression. A hypothesis derived from a model of sleep regulation. Human Neurobiol. 1, 205–210.
Castro, E. D. A. S., and de Almondes, K. M. (2018). Sleep pattern and decision-making in physicians from mobile emergency care service with 12-h work schedules. Int. J. Neurosci. 128, 530–539. doi: 10.1080/00207454.2017.1400970
Cha, K. S., Choi, J. W., Jung, K. Y., and Kim, K. H. (2017). Frontal dysfunction in patients with restless legs syndrome performing a visual oddball task: an event-related potential source imaging study. Sleep Med. 36, 48–54. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2017.04.017
Chee, M. W., and Choo, W. C. (2004). Functional imaging of working memory after 24 hr of total sleep deprivation. J. Neurosci. 24, 4560–4567. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0007-04.2004
Choudhary, A. K., Kishanrao, S. S., Dhanvijay, A. K. D., and Alam, T. (2016). Sleep restriction may lead to disruption in physiological attention and reaction time. Sleep Science 9, 207–211. doi: 10.1016/j.slsci.2016.09.001
Cote, K. A., Milner, C. E., Osip, S. L., Ray, L. B., and Baxter, K. D. (2003). Waking quantitative electroencephalogram and auditory event-related potentials following experimentally induced sleep fragmentation. Sleep 26, 687–694. doi: 10.1093/sleep/26.6.687
Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 64, 135–168 doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750
Doran, S. M., Van Dongen, H. P., and Dinges, D. F. (2001). Sustained attention performance during sleep deprivation: evidence of state instability. Arch. Italian Biol. 139, 253–267. doi: 10.4449/aib.v139i3.503
Drummond, S. P., and Brown, G. G. (2001). The effects of total sleep deprivation on cerebral responses to cognitive performance. Neuropsychopharmacology 25, S68–S73. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(01)00325-6
Durmer, J. S., and Dinges, D. F. (2005). Neurocognitive consequences of sleep deprivation. Semi. Neurol. 25, 117–129. doi: 10.1055/s-2005-867080
El-Gharib, A. M., and Sharsher, R. S. (2017). Effect of nasal continuous positive airway pressure (CPAP) on auditory P300 in obstructive sleep apnea syndrome (OSAS). Egypt. J. Ear Nose Throat Allied Sci. 18, 55–59. doi: 10.1016/j.ejenta.2016.10.001
Endicott, J., and Nee, J. (1997). Endicott Work Productivity Scale (EWPS): a new measure to assess treatment effects. Psychopharmacol. Bull. 33, 13–16.
Friedman, N. P., and Miyake, A. (2017). Unity and diversity of executive functions: individual differences as a window on cognitive structure. Cortex 86, 186–204. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2016.04.023
Gelir, E., Başaran, C., Bayrak, S., Yagcioglu, S., Budak, M. T., Firat, H., et al. (2014). Electrophysiological assessment of the effects of obstructive sleep apnea on cognition. PLoS ONE 9, e90647. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0090647
George, C. F. (2007). Sleep apnea, alertness, and motor vehicle crashes. Am. J. Respir. Crit. Care Med. 176, 954–956. doi: 10.1164/rccm.200605-629PP
Gonzaga, C., Bertolami, A., Bertolami, M., Amodeo, C., and Calhoun, D. (2015). Obstructive sleep apnea, hypertension and cardiovascular diseases. J. Hum. Hypertens. 29, 705–712. doi: 10.1038/jhh.2015.15
Gradisar, M., Terrill, G., Johnston, A., and Douglas, P. (2008). Adolescent sleep and working memory performance. Sleep Biol. Rhythms 6, 146–154. doi: 10.1111/j.1479-8425.2008.00353.x
Gupta, M. A., and Simpson, F. C. (2015). Obstructive sleep apnea and psychiatric disorders: a systematic review. J. Clin. Sleep Med. 11, 165–175. doi: 10.5664/jcsm.4466
Honn, K. A., Hinson, J. M., Whitney, P., and Van Dongen, H. P. A. (2019). Cognitive flexibility: a distinct element of performance impairment due to sleep deprivation. Accident Anal. Prev. 126, 191–197. doi: 10.1016/j.aap.2018.02.013
Horne, J. A. (1993). Human sleep, sleep loss and behaviour: implications for the prefrontal cortex and psychiatric disorder. Br. J. Psychiatry 162, 413–419. doi: 10.1192/bjp.162.3.413
Hudson, A. N., Van Dongen, H. P., and Honn, K. A. (2020). Sleep deprivation, vigilant attention, and brain function: a review. Neuropsychopharmacology 45, 21–30. doi: 10.1038/s41386-019-0432-6
Jahrami, H., BaHammam, A. S., Bragazzi, N. L., Saif, Z., Faris, M., and Vitiello, M. V. (2021). Sleep problems during the COVID-19 pandemic by population: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J. Clin. Sleep Med. 17, 299–313. doi: 10.5664/jcsm.8930
Javaheipour, N., Shahdipour, N., Noori, K., Zarei, M., Camilleri, J., Laird, A., et al. (2019). Functional brain alterations in acute sleep deprivation: an activation likelihood estimation meta-analysis. Sleep Med. Rev. 46, 64–73. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2019.03.008
Jiang, S., Qu, C., Wang, F., Liu, Y., Qiao, Z., Qiu, X., et al. (2015). Using event-related potential P300 as an electrophysiological marker for differential diagnosis and to predict the progression of mild cognitive impairment: a meta-analysis. Neurol. Sci. 36, 1105–1112. doi: 10.1007/s10072-015-2099-z
Jung, K. Y., Kim, S. M., Song, J. Y., Lee, B. U., Lee, C., Lee, S. K., et al. (2015). Sternberg working memory performance following treatment with pramipexole in patients with moderate-to-severe restless legs syndrome. Sleep Med. 16, 703–708. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2014.10.025
Jung, K. Y., Koo, Y. S., Kim, B. J., Ko, D., Lee, G. T., Kim, K. H., et al. (2011). Electrophysiologic disturbances during daytime in patients with restless legs syndrome: further evidence of cognitive dysfunction?. Sleep Med. 12, 416–421. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2010.08.018
Karr, J. E., Areshenkoff, C. N., Rast, P., Hofer, S. M., Iverson, G. L., and Garcia-Barrera, M. A. (2018). The unity and diversity of executive functions: a systematic review and re-analysis of latent variable studies. Psychol. Bull. 144, 1147–1185. doi: 10.1037/bul0000160
Killgore, W. D., Balkin, T. J., and Wesensten, N. J. (2006). Impaired decision making following 49 h of sleep deprivation. J. Sleep Res. 15, 7–13. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2869.2006.00487.x
Killgore, W. D., Kahn-Greene, E. T., Lipizzi, E. L., Newman, R. A., Kamimori, G. H., and Balkin, T. J. (2008). Sleep deprivation reduces perceived emotional intelligence and constructive thinking skills. Sleep Med. 9, 517–526. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2007.07.003
Kim, N. Y., Wittenberg, E., and Nam, C. S. (2017). Behavioral and neural correlates of executive function: interplay between inhibition and updating processes. Front. Neurosci. 11, 378. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2017.00378
Kim, S. M., Choi, J. W., Lee, C., Lee, B. U., Koo, Y. S., Kim, K. H., et al. (2014). Working memory deficit in patients with restless legs syndrome: an event-related potential study. Sleep Med. 15, 808–815. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2014.03.010
Klingelhoefer, L., Bhattacharya, K., and Reichmann, H. (2016). Restless legs syndrome. Clin. Med. 16, 379–82. doi: 10.7861/clinmedicine.16-4-379
Ko, C. H., Fang, Y. W., Tsai, L. L., and Hsieh, S. (2015). The effect of experimental sleep fragmentation on error monitoring. Biol. Psychol. 104, 163–172. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2014.12.008
Krause, A. J., Simon, E. B., Mander, B. A., Greer, S. M., Saletin, J. M., Goldstein-Piekarski, A. N., et al. (2017). The sleep-deprived human brain. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 18, 404–418. doi: 10.1038/nrn.2017.55
Kusztor, A., Raud, L., Juel, B. E., Nilsen, A. S., Storm, J. F., and Huster, R. J. (2019). Sleep deprivation differentially affects subcomponents of cognitive control. Sleep 42, zsz016. doi: 10.1093/sleep/zsz016
Lal, C., Strange, C., and Bachman, D. (2012). Neurocognitive impairment in obstructive sleep apnea. Chest 141, 1601–1610. doi: 10.1378/chest.11-2214
Lee, H. J., Kim, L., and Suh, K. Y. (2003). Cognitive deterioration and changes of P300 during total sleep deprivation. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 57, 490–496. doi: 10.1046/j.1440-1819.2003.01153.x
Legatt, A. D. (2014). “Evoked potentials,” in Encyclopedia of the Neurological Sciences, 228–231. doi: 10.1016/b978-0-12-385157-4.00529-7
Leite Ferreira, O. D., and Moraes de Almondes, K. (2014). The executive functions in primary insomniacs: literature review. Perspectivas en Psicología Revista de Psicología y Ciencias Afines 11, 1–9. Available online at: https://www.redalyc.org/articulo.oa?id=483547666001
Lim, J., and Dinges, D. F. (2010). A meta-analysis of the impact of short-term sleep deprivation on cognitive variables. Psychol. Bull. 136, 375–389. doi: 10.1037/a0018883
Liu, Q., Zhou, R., Liu, L., and Zhao, X. (2015). Effects of 72 hours total sleep deprivation on male astronauts' executive functions and emotion. Compr. Psychiatry 61, 28–35. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2015.05.015
Lo, J. C., Ong, J. L., Leong, R. L., Gooley, J. J., and Chee, M. W. (2016). Cognitive performance, sleepiness, and mood in partially sleep deprived adolescents: the need for sleep study. Sleep 39, 687–698. doi: 10.5665/sleep.5552
Macdonald, M., Jamshidi, P., and Campbell, K. (2008). Infrequent increases in stimulus intensity may interrupt central executive functioning during Rapid Eye Movement sleep. Neuroreport 19, 309–313. doi: 10.1097/WNR.0b013e3282f4ede8
Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., and Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: a latent variable analysis. Cogn. Psychol. 41, 49–100. doi: 10.1006/cogp.1999.0734
Molfese, D. L., Ivanenko, A., Key, A. F., Roman, A., Molfese, V. J., O'Brien, L. M., et al. (2013). A one-hour sleep restriction impacts brain processing in young children across tasks: evidence from event-related potentials. Dev. Neuropsychol. 38, 317–336. doi: 10.1080/87565641.2013.799169
Philip, P., Sagaspe, P., Prague, M., Tassi, P., Capelli, A., Bioulac, B., et al. (2012). Acute versus chronic partial sleep deprivation in middle-aged people: differential effect on performance and sleepiness. Sleep 35, 997–1002. doi: 10.5665/sleep.1968
Polich, J. (2007). Updating P300: an integrative theory of P3a and P3b. Clin. Neurophysiol. 118, 2128–2148. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2007.04.019
Polich, J., and Corey-Bloom, J. (2005). Alzheimer's disease and P300: review and evaluation of task and modality. Curr. Alzheimer Res. 2, 515–525. doi: 10.2174/156720505774932214
Qi, J. L., Shao, Y. C., Miao, D., Fan, M., Bi, G. H., and Yang, Z. (2010). The effects of 43 hours of sleep deprivation on executive control functions: event-related potentials in a visual go/no go task. Soc. Behav. Personal. Int. J. 38, 29–42. doi: 10.2224/sbp.2010.38.1.29
Quan, S. F., Archbold, K., Gevins, A. S., and Goodwin, J. L. (2013). Long-term neurophysiologic impact of childhood sleep disordered breathing on neurocognitive performance. Southwest J. Pulm. Crit. Care 7, 165–175. doi: 10.13175/swjpcc110-13
Ramchurn, A., de Fockert, J. W., Mason, L., Darling, S., and Bunce, D. (2014). Intraindividual reaction time variability affects P300 amplitude rather than latency. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8, 557. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00557
Ray, K., Chatterjee, A., Panjwani, U., Kumar, S., Sahu, S., Ghosh, S., et al. (2012). Modafinil improves event related potentials P300 and contingent negative variation after 24 h sleep deprivation. Life Sci. 91, 94–99. doi: 10.1016/j.lfs.2012.06.012
Renn, R. P., and Cote, K. A. (2013). Performance monitoring following total sleep deprivation: Effects of task type and error rate. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 88, 64–73. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2013.01.013
Reutrakul, S., and Mokhlesi, B. (2017). Obstructive sleep apnea and diabetes: a state of the art review. Chest 152, 1070–1086. doi: 10.1016/j.chest.2017.05.009
Reynolds, A. C., and Banks, S. (2010). Total sleep deprivation, chronic sleep restriction and sleep disruption. Prog. Brain Res. 185, 91–103. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-444-53702-7.00006-3
Schapkin, S. A., Falkenstein, M., Marks, A., and Griefahn, B. (2006). Executive brain functions after exposure to nocturnal traffic noise: effects of task difficulty and sleep quality. Eur. J. Appl. Physiol. 96, 693–702. doi: 10.1007/s00421-005-0049-9
Schapkin, S. A., Falkenstein, M., Marks, A., and Griefahn, B. (2007). Noise aftereffects and brain processes: mediating role of achievement motivation. J. Psychophysiol. 21, 1–8. doi: 10.1027/0269-8803.21.1.1
Scullin, M. K. (2017). Do older adults need sleep? A review of neuroimaging, sleep, and aging studies. Curr. Sleep Med. Rep. 3, 204–214. doi: 10.1007/s40675-017-0086-z
Seda, G., and Han, T. S. (2020). Effect of obstructive sleep apnea on neurocognitive performance. Sleep Med. Clin. 15, 77–85. doi: 10.1016/j.jsmc.2019.10.001
Shochat, T. (2012). Impact of lifestyle and technology developments on sleep. Nat. Sci. Sleep 4, 19–31. doi: 10.2147/NSS.S18891
Staffen, W., Mair, A., Zauner, H., Unterrainer, J., Niederhofer, H., Kutzelnigg, A., et al. (2002). Cognitive function and fMRI in patients with multiple sclerosis: evidence for compensatory cortical activation during an attention task. Brain 125, 1275–1282. doi: 10.1093/brain/awf125
Steffensen, S. C., Ohran, A. J., Shipp, D. N., Hales, K., Stobbs, S. H., and Fleming, D. E. (2008). Gender-selective effects of the P300 and N400 components of the visual evoked potential. Vision Res. 48, 917–925. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2008.01.005
Thomas, M., Sing, H., Belenky, G., Holcomb, H., Mayberg, H., Dannals, R., et al. (2000). Neural basis of alertness and cognitive performance impairments during sleepiness. I. Effects of 24 h of sleep deprivation on waking human regional brain activity. J. Sleep Res. 9, 335–352. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2869.2000.00225.x
Trošt Bobić, T., Šečić, A., Zavoreo, I., Matijević, V., Filipović, B., Kolak, Ž., et al. (2016). The impact of sleep deprivation on the brain. Acta Clin. Croat. 55, 469–473. doi: 10.20471/acc.2016.55.03.17
Tucker, A. M., Whitney, P., Belenky, G., Hinson, J. M., and Van Dongen, H. P. (2010). Effects of sleep deprivation on dissociated components of executive functioning. Sleep. 33, 47–57. doi: 10.1093/sleep/33.1.47
Tufik, S., Santos-Silva, R., Taddei, J. A., and Bittencourt, L. R. A. (2010). Obstructive sleep apnea syndrome in the São Paulo epidemiologic sleep study. Sleep Med. 11, 441–446. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2009.10.005
Vakulin, A., Catcheside, P. G., Baulk, S. D., Antic, N. A., Banks, S., Dorrian, J., et al. (2014). Individual variability and predictors of driving simulator impairment in patients with obstructive sleep apnea. J. Clin. Sleep Med. 10, 647–655. doi: 10.5664/jcsm.3792
Vriend, J., Davidson, F., Rusak, B., and Corkum, P. (2015). Emotional and cognitive impact of sleep restriction in children. Sleep Med. Clin. 10, 107–115. doi: 10.1016/j.jsmc.2015.02.009
Walsh, P., Kane, N., and Butler, S. (2005). The clinical role of evoked potentials. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 76(suppl 2), ii16–ii22. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.2005.068130
Whittemore, R., and Knafl, K. (2005). The integrative review: updated methodology. J. Adv. Nurs. 52, 546–553. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2648.2005.03621.x
Williams, H. L., Lubin, A., and Goodnow, J. J. (1959). Impaired performance with acute sleep loss. Psychol. Monogr. General Appl. 73, 1–26.
Wong, K. K., Grunstein, R. R., Bartlett, D. J., and Gordon, E. (2006). Brain function in obstructive sleep apnea: results from the Brain Resource International Database. J. Integr. Neurosci. 5, 111–121. doi: 10.1142/s0219635206001033
Yaman, M., Karakaya, F., Aydin, T., Mayda, H., Güzel, H. I., and Kayaalp, D. (2015). Evaluation of the effect of modafinil on cognitive functions in patients with idiopathic hypersomnia with P300. Med. Sci. Monit. Int. Med. J. Exp. Clin. Res. 21, 1850–1855. doi: 10.1142/S0219635206001033
Yerlikaya, D., Emek-Savaş, D. D., Kurşun, B. B., Öztura, I., and Yener, G. G. (2018). Electrophysiological and neuropsychological outcomes of severe obstructive sleep apnea: effects of hypoxemia on cognitive performance. Cogn. Neurodyn. 12, 471–480. doi: 10.1007/s11571-018-9487-z
Zhang, Q., Liao, Y., Qi, J., Zhao, Y., Zhu, T., Liu, Z., et al. (2014). A visual ERP study of impulse inhibition following a zaleplon-induced nap after sleep deprivation. PLoS ONE 9, e95653. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0095653
Zhao, W., Gao, D., Yue, F., Wang, Y., Mao, D., Chen, X., et al. (2018). Response inhibition deficits in insomnia disorder: an event-related potential study with the stop-signal task. Front. Neurol. 9, 610. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2018.00610
Zhong, J., Rifkin-Graboi, A., Ta, A. T., Yap, K. L., Chuang, K. H., Meaney, M. J., et al. (2013). Functional networks in parallel with cortical development associate with executive functions in children. Cereb. Cortex 24, 1937–1947. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bht051
Keywords: P300, cognition, sleep, sleep deprivation, executive function, working memory, Obstructive Sleep Apnea Syndrome (OSAS), Event-Related Potentials (ERP)
Citation: Lima NC, Kirov R and de Almondes KM (2022) Impairment of executive functions due to sleep alterations: An integrative review on the use of P300. Front. Neurosci. 16:906492. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2022.906492
Received: 28 March 2022; Accepted: 30 June 2022;
Published: 22 July 2022.
Edited by:
Yi-Ping Hou, Lanzhou University, ChinaReviewed by:
Ivana Rosenzweig, King's College London, United KingdomYolanda Del Río-Portilla, National Autonomous University of Mexico, Mexico
Copyright © 2022 Lima, Kirov and de Almondes. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Nathalya Chrispim Lima, nathalya_lima@outlook.com