Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Neurosci., 19 April 2022
Sec. Auditory Cognitive Neuroscience

Atypical Functional Connectivity During Unfamiliar Music Listening in Children With Autism

\r\nCarina Freitas,*Carina Freitas1,2*Benjamin A. E. Hunt,Benjamin A. E. Hunt3,4Simeon M. Wong,Simeon M. Wong3,4Leanne RisticLeanne Ristic2Susan FragiadakisSusan Fragiadakis2Stephanie ChowStephanie Chow2Alana IaboniAlana Iaboni2Jessica Brian,Jessica Brian2,5Latha SooryaLatha Soorya6Joyce L. ChenJoyce L. Chen7Russell SchacharRussell Schachar8Benjamin T. Dunkley,Benjamin T. Dunkley3,4Margot J. Taylor,,,Margot J. Taylor1,3,4,9Jason P. Lerch,,Jason P. Lerch4,10,11Evdokia Anagnostou,,,\r\nEvdokia Anagnostou1,2,4,5
  • 1Institute of Medical Science, Faculty of Medicine, University of Toronto, Toronto, ON, Canada
  • 2Bloorview Research Institute, Holland Bloorview Kids Rehabilitation Hospital, Toronto, ON, Canada
  • 3Department of Diagnostic Imaging, Hospital for Sick Children, Toronto, ON, Canada
  • 4Neuroscience and Mental Health Program, Hospital for Sick Children Research Institute, Toronto, ON, Canada
  • 5Department of Paediatrics, University of Toronto, Toronto, ON, Canada
  • 6Department of Psychiatry, Rush University Medical Center, Chicago, IL, United States
  • 7Faculty of Kinesiology and Physical Education and Rehabilitation Sciences Institute, University of Toronto, Toronto, ON, Canada
  • 8Department of Psychiatry Research, Hospital for Sick Children, Toronto, ON, Canada
  • 9Departments of Psychology and Medical Imaging, University of Toronto, Toronto, ON, Canada
  • 10Department of Medical Biophysics, University of Toronto, Toronto, ON, Canada
  • 11Wellcome Centre for Integrative Neuroimaging, University of Oxford, Oxford, United Kingdom

Background: Atypical processing of unfamiliar, but less so familiar, stimuli has been described in Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD), in particular in relation to face processing. We examined the construct of familiarity in ASD using familiar and unfamiliar songs, to investigate the link between familiarity and autism symptoms, such as repetitive behavior.

Methods: Forty-eight children, 24 with ASD (21 males, mean age = 9.96 years ± 1.54) and 24 typically developing (TD) controls (21 males, mean age = 10.17 ± 1.90) completed a music familiarity task using individually identified familiar compared to unfamiliar songs, while magnetoencephalography (MEG) was recorded. Each song was presented for 30 s. We used both amplitude envelope correlation (AEC) and the weighted phase lag index (wPLI) to assess functional connectivity between specific regions of interest (ROI) and non-ROI parcels, as well as at the whole brain level, to understand what is preserved and what is impaired in familiar music listening in this population.

Results: Increased wPLI synchronization for familiar vs. unfamiliar music was found for typically developing children in the gamma frequency. There were no significant differences within the ASD group for this comparison. During the processing of unfamiliar music, we demonstrated left lateralized increased theta and beta band connectivity in children with ASD compared to controls. An interaction effect found greater alpha band connectivity in the TD group compared to ASD to unfamiliar music only, anchored in the left insula.

Conclusion: Our results revealed atypical processing of unfamiliar songs in children with ASD, consistent with previous studies in other modalities reporting that processing novelty is a challenge for ASD. Relatively typical processing of familiar stimuli may represent a strength and may be of interest to strength-based intervention planning.

Introduction

Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is a neurodevelopmental disorder characterized by persistent difficulties in social interaction and social communication, and restricted, repetitive patterns of behaviors, interests and activities. In the first description of autism, Kanner described key features present in all of his 11 original cases of children; these were extreme aloneness, lack of interaction, inability to form close emotional ties with others and “an anxious desire for the maintenance of sameness.” This need for sameness often presents as rigorous adherence to particular routines or rituals and resistance to change in the surroundings (such as placement of toys). This subgroup of restricted and repetitive behaviors (RRBs) is currently referred as insistence on sameness (IS) (Szatmari et al., 2006; Bishop et al., 2013).

Both social communication and repetitive behavior domains have been linked to novelty processing. Munson et al. (2008) demonstrated that memory and preference for novelty were predictive of social and communication growth in pre-schoolers with ASD between 4 and 6 1/2 years of age. In contrast, the desire for sameness and resistance to change may highlight a need for preservation and adherence to familiar environments and routines (Szatmari et al., 2006) and a heightened sensitivity to novel stimuli may underlie preference for sameness (Baron-Cohen and Belmonte, 2005).

The effect of familiarity has been investigated in the ASD population in only a few experimental tasks. Most of these studies were on face processing, as understanding socio-emotional difficulties is relevant in this disorder. Faces are salient stimuli critical for social interaction and communication (Jack and Schyns, 2015). Atypical face recognition processing in the ASD population has been reported in different neuroimaging modalities, including event-related potentials (ERPs) (Dawson et al., 2002; Webb et al., 2006, 2010; Batty et al., 2011), magnetoencephalography (MEG) (Leung et al., 2014, 2018; Safar et al., 2018) and fMRI (Pierce et al., 2004; Grelotti et al., 2005; Pierce and Redcay, 2008). Compared to controls, ASD individuals show face recognition impairments in the processing of unfamiliar but not familiar faces (Simmons et al., 2009), although some authors suggest delayed development of the processing of familiar faces (Batty et al., 2011; Webb et al., 2011). Nevertheless, unfamiliar faces have been the preferred stimuli to investigate the altered emotional face processing in ASD. Familiarity has also been related to the need for sameness and repetitive behaviors in ASD (Gomot et al., 2008; Chen et al., 2009; Boyd et al., 2010), suggesting it has a valuable role in helping understand these classic symptoms.

Music is invaluable to the study of human cognition, emotion and underlying brain networks, including familiarity (Koelsch, 2005a,b). Unlike faces, music is an auditory stimulus that interests and motivates many children with ASD (Kanner, 1943). Despite a 50 years history of music therapy in autism (Reschke-Hernández, 2011) and many anecdotal reports of its importance to ASD children (Sacks, 2008), there has been only a handful of fMRI studies on songs or music processing in ASD. These studies examined either emotional processing of music in adults with ASD (Caria et al., 2011; Gebauer et al., 2014) or compared music and language processing in autistic children (Lai et al., 2012; Sharda et al., 2015), given that both music and language share perceptual and neural mechanisms (Fedorenko et al., 2009; Schön et al., 2010; Rogalsky et al., 2011). These studies used different neuroimaging methods, such as fMRI, diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) or multimodal approaches, with a passive listening paradigm, but the familiarity effect was not investigated.

Even though familiar and unfamiliar songs were used separately in the above reports, to our knowledge, there is no neuroimaging study that compared familiar and unfamiliar music listening in individuals with ASD. Understanding the neural mechanisms underlying music familiarity processing is important to understanding the neurobiological substrates of familiarity processing in ASD, but also to inform therapeutics, especially given the potential impact of the construct of familiarity on both social communication and insistence on sameness.

Neuroimaging studies focusing on the processing of familiarity in music listening have been completed in healthy adults (Plailly et al., 2007; Watanabe et al., 2008; Janata, 2009; Pereira et al., 2011; Sikka et al., 2015), in adults with Alzheimer’s disease (Yang et al., 2015; King et al., 2018) and Down syndrome (Virji-Babul et al., 2013). In a recent neuroimaging meta-analysis on familiarity in music listening in healthy adults (Freitas et al., 2018), we found that listening to familiar music demonstrated the highest likelihood of activation in left motor and premotor areas, suggesting an audio-motor synchronization to familiar tunes, anticipating music elements (melody, rhythm harmonic progression, lyrics) in one’s mind. On the other hand, unfamiliar music activated the left insula and the right anterior cingulate cortex, important structures for evaluative judgments (Brattico and Pearce, 2013), potentially deciding whether a song is familiar or novel. Surprisingly, the brain regions related to emotion and reward were not amongst the most consistently activated while listening to familiar songs, as we had expected.

Thus, familiarity and novelty processing relate to core ASD symptoms (social communication deficits and repetitive behaviors) and understanding deficits and strengths in familiarity processing may provide insights into the nature of ASD. Understanding what is impaired and what is preserved in familiarity processing across different sensory modalities will provide important guidance for the development of therapeutic strategies in this population.

With the present research we fill this existing literature gap on music familiarity in ASD and address the following questions: (i) What patterns of functional connectivity are associated with familiarity music processing in typically developing children? We define familiarity as the “feeling of knowing a song,” as described in the literature (Groussard et al., 2010). (ii) Do children with autism differ from typically developing children when neurally processing the songs during familiar or unfamiliar music? (iii) Will atypical network connectivity relate to the insistence on sameness or social communication?

The MEG analyses of this study were guided by the results of the our previous systematic and fMRI neuroimaging meta-analysis (Freitas et al., 2018) which identified the most consistently active brain areas (nodes) when listening to familiar and unfamiliar songs. These areas included the left superior frontal gyrus, SMA, left ventral lateral nucleus of the thalamus, left insula, right cingulate, among others. We selected the top 8 nodes in each condition (familiar and unfamiliar) as our regions of interest (ROI) for connectivity analyses. We chose to analyze two distinct connectivity approaches to MEG data, as one, AEC, reflects coupled fluctuations of signal envelope in neural oscillations and the other, wPLI, measures quick, transient activity related to rapid processes in the brain. These two complementary approaches provide information on different aspects of the dynamics of ongoing neural activity (for a review, please see Engel et al., 2013).

Materials and Methods

Participants

Forty-eight youth were included in the study. Twenty-four children and adolescents with ASD (21 males; range: 7 to 14 years, mean = 9.96 years ± 1.54, 20 right handed) sex-matched with 24 typically developing controls (21 males; range 7 to 14 years old, mean = 10.17 ± 1.90, 20 right handed) were recruited from the Province of Ontario Neurodevelopmental Disorders (POND) Network dataset. Exclusion criteria included a history of brain injury and major psychiatric illness for children with ASD, and comorbid psychiatry disorder and first-degree family history of neurodevelopmental disorder for the control group. In addition, for both groups uncorrected vision, blindness, deafness, IQ < 70 and ferromagnetic dental work or metallic implants were also exclusion factors. The study was approved by The Holland Bloorview Kids Rehabilitation Hospital and The Hospital for Sick Children Research Ethics Boards. All participants and their parents gave informed written assent and consent.

Clinical Evaluation

Participants with ASD had been diagnosed by a registered medical professional and clinical psychologists according to the DSM-5 criteria. Diagnoses were confirmed by the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS-General or ADOS-2) and the Autism Diagnostic Interview- Revised (ADI-R) (Lord et al., 1994). All participants underwent cognitive testing using the Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence—Second Edition (WASI-II) (Wechsler, 2002). Parents were interviewed and completed the Social Communication Questionnaire (SCQ) (Rutter et al., 2003) and the Repetitive Behavior Scale Revised (RBS-R) (Lam and Aman, 2007). Participants were also asked about their musical training (if they had formal musical education). Baseline demographic information and performance on cognitive/behavioral measures are presented in Table 1.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Demographic, diagnostic and cognitive-behavioral assessment measures for ASD and control participants.

Study Design

There were two visits for this study. In the first visit, participants were asked to bring a set of 10 familiar liked and 10 disliked songs, rating their likability using a Likert scale—ranging from 1 (least liked or most disliked) to 5 (most liked). They also completed a music questionnaire (assessing music preferences, years of training and type instrument played) (Bhatara et al., 2013) and a pitch discrimination test (Stanutz et al., 2014) (see Supplementary Figure 1). The complete list of all familiar songs used in this study is in Supplementary Table 1. During the second visit they completed a hearing screening and the MEG task.

Music Stimuli

Our music task paradigm was adapted from an fMRI study on music familiarity in healthy adults described by Pereira et al. (2011). Using songs that are listened to and known by the participants, instead of tones, increases ecological value of the experiment. After the first study visit, we selected 8 familiar most-liked songs and 8 familiar most-disliked songs for each participant from their self-selected music list. This selection was based on participants’ likability ratings: songs rated in the extreme positions of the rating scale. Then, we selected the first 30 s of each song, using Audacity 2.1.0 music software program for editing these music excerpts, due to its relevance on listeners’ attention (Brandon Miler, 2016; Crane, 2017; Léveillé Gauvin, 2018). Subsequently, we extracted three musical features (tempo, mode and dissonance) from the 30 s excerpts, using the Matlab Toolbox for Music Information Retrieval (MIR Toolbox) (Lartillot et al., 2008) version 1.6, running on Matlab 2017b (The MathWorks Inc., Natick, MA, United States). This program provided us with an objective measure of the musical features. Two other musical features, genre and presence or absence of lyrics, were classified by auditory inspection. We matched the familiar songs with unfamiliar ones on the following musical characteristics: tempo [(slow (40 to 72 bpm); moderate (72 to 120 bpm) and fast (120 to 208 bpm)]; mode (minor or major); genre (i.e., classic, pop, rock) and presence or absence of lyrics (vocal or instrumental). For all songs with lyrics (503/551 songs), we matched the language of the lyrics. In total we had songs in 5 languages (English, French, Spanish, Portuguese and Hebrew) which were matched for this variable with unfamiliar songs from our data base. The unfamiliar songs were selected from a database of European music, mostly from the Eurovision Song Contest. European music was chosen as, like most North American popular music, it shares the rules and regularities of Western tonal music but is less likely to be familiar to North American participants. In total, for each participant 16 extracts (30 s each) of familiar songs (liked and disliked) were matched with 24 unfamiliar songs. The list of all unfamiliar songs is available in the Supplementary Table 2.

Choice of Magnetoencephalography Paradigm

The stimulus duration was 30 s consistent with other studies (Janata, 2009; Demorest et al., 2010; Pereira et al., 2011); this choice was related to the uniqueness of our question. Listening to music excerpts and recognizing if that excerpt is familiar or not is a cognitive task that requires time, especially in children. Recognition is a process that develops gradually while the melody unfolds over time (Dalla Bella et al., 2003).

Magnetoencephalography Task

During the MEG task, stimuli were delivered using Presentation software (Version 18.1, Neurobehavioral Systems, Berkeley, CA, United States). A unique set of 40 song extracts of 30 s each was prepared for each participant. Before entering the MEG scanner, each participant was trained to complete the task with familiar and unfamiliar songs not used in the task experiment. Inside the magnetically shielded room, participants were positioned supine and instructed to maintain visual fixation on an X within a circle projected on the screen, situated ∼70 cm from the participant’s eyes. The whole task consisted of 6 runs: two resting state scans of 3 min each (before and after the music task) and four runs of the music task which included 10 music excerpts in each run (Figure 1). The songs presented were of three different conditions: familiar liked (FL), familiar disliked (FD), and unfamiliar (UF). After hearing each song extract for 30 s through MEG compatible earphones, participants responded to two questions, by pressing left (Yes) or right (No) buttons. The questions were “Do you know this song?” and “Do you like this music?” Participants were instructed to wait until the end of the 30 s before answering, to avoid MEG contamination by the motor response. Ratings took approximately 10 s each. The familiarity rating done during the scanning session was used in the analysis. This task produced a total of 40 trials for each participant.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. The MEG experimental paradigm.

Neuroimaging Data Acquisition

The MEG data were obtained using a 151-channel whole-head system with axial gradiometers (CTF MEG International Services LP Coquitlam, Canada). Data were recorded at a 600 Hz sampling rate with a band-pass filter of 1–150 Hz. Localization coils (fiducial markers) positioned at the nasion and left and right pre-auricular points were used to monitor the head position continuously during the task. Trials with greater than 10 mm of head motion were not included (Pang, 2011). The mean head displacement during the task, for the 45 min recording, did not differ between groups (ASD mean = 5.9 mm ± 2.6; TD mean = 4.32 mm ± 1.98; p = 0.54). There were also no group differences in motion during the two resting state recordings (ASD mean = 2.6 mm ± 1.3; TD mean = 1.7 mm ± 1.2; p = 0.964 and ASD mean = 3.0 mm ± 1.4; TD mean = 2.6 mm ± 1.7; p = 0.164). After the MEG session, for co-registration purposes, fiducial coils were replaced by MRI radio-opaque markers. Structural brain MRIs were obtained in all children. In five children a whole brain T1-weighted MRI sequence in a 3.0 T MR scanner (MAGNETOM Tim Trio, Siemens AG, Erlanger, Germany) with a 12-channel head coil was completed. Their brain images were obtained using a high-resolution 3D SAG MPRAGE sequence (PAT, GRAPPA = 2, TR/TE/FA = 2,300/2.96 ms/90, FOV = 28.8 × 19.2 cm, 256 × 256 matrix, 192 slices, slice thickness = 1.0 mm isotropic voxels). The remaining 43 participants were scanned using a 3.0 T MR (PrismaFIT, Siemens Healthineers, Germany) with a head and neck 20-channel coil. Images were obtained using a similar protocol but with 0.8 mm isotropic voxels.

Neuroimaging Data Preprocessing

The 30 s music trials were epoched into chunks of 10 s (10 + 10 + 10) to avoid rejection of the whole trial due to head movement. This epoching convention was preserved for all analyses. The data epochs for familiar (FL and FD) and unfamiliar (UL and UD) music conditions were selected according to participant’s responses during the MEG task. As a result of variability in each participant’s likeability ratings and the higher number of unfamiliar stimuli in the MEG paradigm, the final number of trials in each music condition differed. The average number of trials for each condition in the ASD group was FL:10.1; FD:5.4; UL:8.9; UD: 15,5; and in the TD group this average was: FL: 9.1; FD: 6.1; UL: 6.2. and UD: 18.4; these numbers of trials did not differ between groups (all ps > 0.10). To be considered for data analysis, participants had to have a minimum of 3 trials in each condition (> 90s of data per participant and condition). As the numbers were variable between liked and disliked trials, these two categories were collapsed into familiar and unfamiliar conditions. There were no significant differences in the number of trials included for familiar vs. unfamiliar trials between the two groups. For the TD group, 33.1 ± 8.7 trials were included in analysis, while for the ASD 28.8 ± 9.7 trials were included; there was no significant difference between groups in number of trials in analyses (t = −1.5, p = 0.12). Independent Component Analysis (ICA) was used to identify the components that reflected ocular artifacts, generated by eye movement and non-ocular artifacts such as cardiac and muscle activity on a trial-by-trial basis for each participant and condition. A maximum of 60 components per participant were visually inspected and artifacts were removed (Muthukumaraswamy, 2013). In the TD group an average of 3.3 ± 0.9 components were dropped, while in the ASD group the average was 3.5 ± 1.0 components dropped. There was no significant difference between groups in number of components dropped (t = 1.38, p = 0.17).

Atlas-Guided Source Reconstruction

For MEG data processing we used the FieldTrip software toolbox (Oostenveld et al., 2011) implemented in MATLAB R2017b (The MathWorks Inc., Natick, MA, United States). A single shell head model based on initial fiducial positions using each individual’s MRI scan was constructed and normalized into standard MNI space (ICBM 152; (Fonov et al., 2009, 2011). A total of 92 source (seed) locations were then selected for time-series to be extracted and analyzed. We used the coordinates of 90 sources representing the center of mass of all cortical and subcortical parcels in the Automated Anatomical Labeling (AAL) atlas (Tzourio-Mazoyer et al., 2002) as well as 2 additional coordinates for the accumbens nuclei defined in the Yale BioImage Suite Package1. All 92 seeds were projected from standard space into subject space. A linearly constrained minimum variance (LCMV) beamformer (Van Veen et al., 1997) was used to reconstruct broadband time series (“virtual sensors”) for each source location and trial for each subject representing the activity of each of the 92 sources. Beamformers are a type of spatial filter used to estimate activity at a given brain location while suppressing activity from other locations (Van Veen et al., 1997). Our contrasts were binaurally presented and did not detect correlated auditory sources. This could have been due to large analysis windows used with a fairly varied auditory stimuli. Beamformer leakage was corrected using the MEG-ROI-nets toolbox2 using the closest method (Colclough et al., 2015) prior to computing connectivity analysis using amplitude envelope correlation. No leakage correction was applied prior to estimation of wPLI, as wPLI is resistant to signal leakage (Vinck et al., 2011). The beamformer projections were computed at the centroid of each AAL region, rather than on a grid.

The broadband time series were filtered using a two-pass FIR filter into five frequency bands for further analysis: theta (4–7 Hz), alpha (8–14 Hz), beta (15–29 Hz), and gamma (30–80 Hz). The gamma frequency was split into gamma 1 (30–55 Hz) and gamma 2 (65–80 Hz). A notch filter was applied at 60 Hz to remove power-line interference (Chimeno and Pallàs-Areny, 2000).

Assessing Functional Connectivity

We used both amplitude envelope correlation (AEC; Cohen, 2014) and the weighted phase lag index (wPLI; Vinck et al., 2011) to assess connectivity in the music familiarity task amongst a priori defined 13 regions of interest (ROI) (from Freitas et al., 2018) and at the whole brain level (AAL—92 regions). The wPLI estimates the degree of phase synchronization based on the magnitude of the imaginary component of the cross- spectrum (Vinck et al., 2011; Lau et al., 2012) and measures quick, transient brain activity (frequency is between 1 to 150 Hz). The amplitude envelope reflects fluctuations in the envelope of spontaneous neural oscillations (Cohen, 2014), and AEC is the correlation over time between seed regions. The AEC has a low frequency (below 0.1 Hz) (but still indexes activity within frequency bands that are invisible to fMRI) and correlates with fMRI activity. These two measures offer complementary information about neural interactions and functional coupling across distinct areas of the brain (Engel et al., 2013). The seed selection was based on our previous meta-analysis on the neural correlates of familiarity in music listening (Freitas et al., 2018; see Supplementary Tables 3, 4). For both ROI and whole-brain analyses, the Hilbert transform was used to obtain time series of the instantaneous phase and amplitude envelope for each source, frequency band and condition. Both AEC and wPLI were calculated within trials.

For the specified ROI analysis, both amplitude and phase-based metrics were used to assess connectivity between thirteen AAL nodes and each of the other 92 AAL nodes, representing the rest of the brain. A 92 × 13 adjacency matrix was created for each trial, frequency band, condition and participant. Following seed analysis, we performed a whole brain analysis, which generated a 92 × 92 adjacency matrix. In both cases, after calculating adjacency matrices, similar subsequent analyses were performed to produce baselined estimates of functional connectivity revealing task-dependent connectomic effects.

For each frequency, condition (familiar, unfamiliar) and subject, functional connectivity matrices were averaged over the music trials. This trial-average was baselined by subtracting the average of 30 s epochs of resting state recording: generating a single functional connectivity matrix per condition, frequency and subject for wPLI and AEC.

Statistical Analyses of Networks Dynamics

Non-parametric Network-Based Statistics (NBS) (Zalesky et al., 2010) were used for statistical comparison of amplitude and phase connectivity differences within and between-groups while controlling for family wise error rate (FWER) (Zalesky et al., 2010, 2012). This method performs multiple univariate tests (t-tests) on all 92 edges (each element of the adjacency matrix) or 92 × 13 for the ROI seed analysis. This yields a t-value for each connection in the matrix, the t-values are then thresholded by a primary component-forming threshold and those that exceeded this cut-off were identified and subjected to permutation tests at the network level (5000 permutation in the present study). We set the primary component forming threshold for between and within group comparisons to t = 2.75, 3.0, 3.5 for AEC metric and t = 3.0 for wPLI measures. These values of the thresholds (around 3.0–3.5) were selected as they are robust levels for significance and hence, commonly reported in the literature. Also, they represent significant, relatively strict, thresholds: t = 2.75 corresponds to ∼p < 0.005; t = 3.00 corresponds to ∼p < 0.001. Furthermore, the networks were stable across these thresholds. Statistical significance was assigned at the level of the connectivity component as a whole, defining clusters of functionally integrated nodes that significantly differed between groups or conditions. Statistical correction controlling for false positives due to multiple comparisons was performed within each frequency band for the two types of analysis using Bonferroni correction (p < 0.0125 and p < 0.025 for between and within groups’ differences, respectively). We corrected only within metric, as the metrics are distinct and look at two quite different aspects of the signal (i.e., amplitude envelopes versus phase). The results obtained using NBS were plotted using the Brain Net Viewer toolbox (Xia et al., 2013).

Brain-Behavioral Analyses

We performed a correlation analysis across ASD and TD groups between mean network strength (edge weights summed for each individual subject) of significant group difference networks and the scores of the Social Communication Questionnaire (SCQ) scores (Rutter et al., 2003), as well as the ritualistic/sameness subscale of the Repetitive Behavior Scale Revised (RBS-R) (Lam and Aman, 2007) using SPSS 25.0 software (IBM Corp. Released, 2017. IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, Version 25.0. Armonk, NY: IBM Corp) to explore whether differences seen on connectivity during familiar/unfamiliar music listening were related to core symptom domains of ASD.

Results

Behavioral results (Supplementary Table 1 and Supplementary Figure 1).

Magnetoencephalography Results

Connectivity Within Groups: Familiar vs. Unfamiliar

Regions of Interest Analyses

For the within-group analysis, we compared familiar to unfamiliar music for both ASD and control groups, separately, in both amplitude and phase measures. Increased wPLI synchronization was noted for control children in the gamma 1 frequency band for familiar music > unfamiliar music. The network comprised seven edges and eight nodes (pcorr = 0.007), involving connections in the left hemisphere, the superior and inferior opercular frontal gyri, the putamen, the middle orbital gyrus, the insula and the precuneus, and also the right middle temporal pole and the right putamen (Figure 2). There were no significant differences within the ASD group. For the opposite contrast (unfamiliar > familiar music) there were no significant differences in connectivity in either group.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Within TD group contrast: familiar > unfamiliar music. Increased gamma 1 band (30–55 Hz) wPLI phase synchronization during processing of familiar music in TD children.

Whole-Brain Analyses

There were no significant differences at the whole brain level in either amplitude or wPLI metrics for any of the 5 frequencies bands tested for either ASD or control group, for the same contrasts.

Regions of Interest and whole brain connectivity within groups comparing the active window with the baseline for ASD and control groups, separately, for familiar and unfamiliar music conditions are reported in Supplementary Tables 58.

Connectivity Between Groups

Regions of Interest Analyses

We also conducted between-group contrasts for each frequency band and music condition (i.e., familiar and unfamiliar). No main effects for group or condition were found. A difference emerged for the processing of unfamiliar music, where children with ASD showed increased amplitude connectivity in the theta and beta frequency bands compared to controls (pcorr = 0.018 and pcorr = 0.0064, respectively). Increased theta connectivity involved four edges and six connected nodes: the right middle frontal and right post-central gyri, the left superior occipital gyrus, the left mid-cingulum and the left superior and inferior temporal gyri (Figure 3A1). Increased beta connectivity involved six edges and seven connected nodes, all left lateralized: the superior frontal gyrus, the opercular frontal, the insula, the fusiform gyrus, the inferior parietal gyrus, the angular gyrus and the middle occipital gyrus (Figure 3A2). No significant differences emerged for the between-group contrasts in the familiar music condition. There were no significant differences in other frequencies bands.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Between-group analyses. ROI analyses in theta frequency band (A1) and beta frequency band (A2). Whole-brain analyses in theta frequency band (B1) and beta frequency band (B2). All significant results indicated increased connectivity in the ASD group compared to TD during the presentation of unfamiliar music, measured using amplitude envelope connectivity.

Whole Brain Analyses

We also performed between-group contrasts for each frequency band and music condition (i.e., familiar and unfamiliar) using a whole-brain analysis. No significant group or condition main effects were seen. Again, a difference emerged only for the processing of unfamiliar music. Not in theta frequency band (Figure 3B1), as in ROI analysis, but in beta frequency band. ASD children showed increased amplitude connectivity in the beta frequency band compared to controls (pcorr = 0.010). Increased beta connectivity involved three edges and four connected nodes, all in the left hemisphere: the opercular part of the inferior frontal gyrus, the postcentral gyrus, the fusiform gyrus and the precuneus (Figure 3B2). No other significant differences emerged for the between-group contrasts in other frequencies bands.

Interaction Effects in Regions of Interest Analysis

We also explored interaction effects using a 2 (group: ASD, TD) × 2 (music condition: familiar, unfamiliar) mixed design ANOVA using NBS, in all five frequency bands, using both amplitude and wPLI metrics. The primary threshold was set to F = 7. Significant results were found using the wPLI metric in the alpha frequency band. The mean network connectivity was different for ASD and TD groups depending on music familiarity (p = 0.023). The TD group had greater connectivity in the unfamiliar condition. No effect was seen in the ASD group (Figure 4A). A significant interaction for TD children is represented by a network consisting of 15 edges and 12 nodes (Figure 4B). This network was anchored in the left insula and putamen, but with nodes in both hemispheres. No significant results were found using the amplitude metric.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. (A) Interaction effects between music condition and group; (B) Network representing the significant interaction in the alpha frequency band, using the wPLI metric (p = 0.023).

Brain-Behavior Relations

We had three statistically significant networks emerge from between group differences, all in the unfamiliar music condition (network 1 = ROI _theta, network 2 = ROI_ beta, and network 3 = WB _theta). We selected the global strength (average of the strengths of all nodes) of these networks as one of the most fundamental measures of brain topology assessed by graph theory (Mijalkov et al., 2017). First, we correlated these network connectivity strengths with the ADOS calibrated severity score within the ASD group, but did not find any significant correlations (network 1: r = 0.008, p = 0.975; network 2: r = 0.334, p = 0.191; and network 3: r = 0.147, p = 0.574; uncorrected). We also explored if RBS-R-subscale IV (sameness) scores in ASD participants were correlated with network connectivity strength, but no significant results were found in any of the three networks (network 1: r = −0.037, p = 0.889; network 2: r = 0.031, p = 0.906 and network 3: r = −0.307, p = 0.230). Lastly, no significant correlations were noted between SCQ and RBS and network connectivity strength within either group (Supplementary Table 9).

Discussion

In this study, we examined the neural correlates of the processing of familiarity in music listening in typically developing and autistic children using MEG, at the macro-scale level to fill an existing literature gap. For this, we performed connectivity analyses within and between groups using wPLI and AEC measures, at both ROI and whole-brain level. In the following sections we will discuss our findings with respect to these two groups.

Typically Developing Children

We demonstrated increased gamma wPLI synchronization in typically developing children for familiar music > unfamiliar music. Listening to familiar music increased phase connectivity in a network that consisted of frontal, parietal, temporal and subcortical areas. The function of these regions has been associated with expressive language (the left inferior opercular frontal gyrus), memory (left superior frontal gyrus), emotional processing (right temporal pole and left insula) (Olson et al., 2007), mental imagery, memory recollection, information integration and visuo-spatial imagery (precuneus) (Cavanna and Trimble, 2006). The motor areas activated here (i.e., the left and right putamen and left caudate) can reflect auditory-motor synchronization to music elements (Chen et al., 2008; Freitas et al., 2018). More specifically, Grahn (2009) demonstrated that the basal ganglia are crucial for rhythm processing, with a role linked to internal generation of the beat. It was not surprising that auditory areas were not included in our network, as the sound was delivered bilaterally, and was equated for familiar and unfamiliar stimuli, so in the contrasts no auditory cortex effects would be expected. Our resulting network for control children was very similar to the results of music familiarity processing in the typical adult neuroimaging meta-analysis (Freitas et al., 2018).

Our connectivity findings in the TD group were found in gamma band frequency only. The gamma band oscillations (30–100 Hz) have been shown to be correlated with various cognitive processes (Engel et al., 2001), the most prominent of which is memory (encoding and performance) (Sederberg et al., 2003) and perceptual binding (Rodriguez et al., 1999). As recognizing a familiar song involves linking and integrating memory processes with auditory percepts, our results align with this explanation that gamma synchrony mediates the coupling of functionally specialized regions involved in music listening.

Children With Autism Spectrum Disorder

In the children with ASD there were no differences in the MEG metrics between the processing of familiar and unfamiliar music. However, between ASD and TD groups our results demonstrated consistent, significant differences in the unfamiliar music condition at both ROI and whole-brain analyses, assessed by amplitude envelope connectivity (AEC). ASD children showed increased connectivity compared to controls, while the processing of familiar music was similar in the two groups.

This is the first neuroimaging study focusing on understanding familiarity in music in ASD; thus, there is no comparative study available. Nevertheless, neuroimaging literature on the processing of familiarity in faces in ASD individuals has shown atypicalities. It has been suggested that there may be impaired processing of unfamiliar faces, with no deficit or delayed development of familiar faces (Simmons et al., 2009). Pierce and Redcay (2008) reported a selective deficit in fusiform function in response to adult stranger faces, but no atypicalities in the fusiform in response to familiar faces. One possible explanation for this finding was either reduced or enhanced attention or motivation to attend to unfamiliar and familiar faces, respectively. Our findings of atypical processing of unfamiliar music in ASD children could be consistent with this interpretation. Our task (listening to songs for 30 s and deciding if familiar or not) required sustained attention. In addition, repetition increases perceptual fluency (Alter and Oppenheimer, 2009; Joye et al., 2016), or the ease of processing stimuli. As such, we could interpret that unfamiliar songs would require more effort to process. As an example, Pernet et al. (2003) demonstrated in the visual recognition domain using ERPs that the more familiar an object is, the fewer cognitive resources are required. Another interpretation is that ASD children showed reduced neural adaptation-like effects (also referred as habituation or repetition suppression) to unfamiliar music. Neural adaptation is defined by decreasing sensitivity to a repeatedly presented stimulus, whereby the sensory system codes for the derivative or change in the stimulus, priming the individual for changes in the environment: for example, no longer being consciously aware of clothes on the skin over time. In ASD neural adaptation is attenuated in sensory domains, at multiple levels in time and space, from short time scales to long, and in multiple sensory pathways, from the level of neurons, through to the macroscopic BOLD signal, with reduced cortical adaptation effects to sensory stimuli (Turi et al., 2015; Noel et al., 2017), including the auditory domain (Millin et al., 2018). In other words, the ASD brain processes novelty differently compared with the typical brain, driven by possibly impaired neural adaptation and deficits in the dynamic range of neural responsiveness.

Consistent with this, 90% of individuals with autism present atypical sensory experiences: These atypicalities affect every sensory modality and also deficits in multisensory integration (Tomchek and Dunn, 2007; Robertson and Baron-Cohen, 2017). This raises the question if the absence of processing differences between familiar and unfamiliar music results from atypical modulation of sensory processing by high-order cognitive mechanisms. In ASD, several learning challenges, including stimulus overselectivity, have been related to the processing of novel stimuli (Kelly et al., 2015). Stimulus overselectivity could be due to either attentional or performance deficits. The theoretical perspective of “attention deficit” posits that there is an attention abnormality (a narrowness of attention or a hyper-attentiveness to selective stimuli) (Dube, 2009). In our study because of exposure to the same category of stimuli, children with ASD could have given more attention to their familiar music. Regarding the performance deficits theory, it attributes performance problems to an “over-sensitive” comparator mechanism (Reed, 2011). This comparator mechanism is over-sensitive to small differences in importance between stimuli and will respond to a narrow set of stimuli. Under this theory, our children with ASD directed their behavior to familiar music at the expense of unfamiliar music.

Another theory suggests that ASD individuals present deficits in Bayesian prediction processes in information processing (Pellicano and Burr, 2012). This Bayesian framework posits that these individuals see the world more accurately because they lack the modulation by prior experience (top-down mechanisms). The “hypo-prior” (attenuated Bayesian priors) may lead to difficulties in using information from the remote past to match with incoming stimuli. In this case, we could speculate that increased (atypical) connectivity during processing of unfamiliar songs, compared to controls, could be due to a lack of priors (i.e., the children with ASD cannot relate the current experiences to previous experience).

Another consideration is whether music training would have affected familiarity processing. Dalla Bella et al. (2003) reported that music training affects familiarity judgments, and musicians recognize familiar songs in fewer notes than non-musicians. However, our participants did not show group differences in music training (Table 1) or pitch discrimination abilities (an ability correlated with music training) (Supplementary Figure 1), meaning that this was not a selection bias that could be a potential confounding factor.

In this study the differences in the unfamiliar condition in ASD compared to controls were in theta and beta frequencies. Theta oscillations are associated with long-range communication between brain areas implicated in various cognitive processes, such as imitation, language acquisition, working memory, attention, cognitive control and emotional arousal (Engel and Fries, 2010; Kikuchi et al., 2015). Consistent with the existing literature, these theta-band dependent alterations were identified across nodes involving long distance connections that comprise large-scale networks, integrating and coordinating information between frontal, parietal and occipital brain regions. Synchrony in beta oscillations has also been found in long-range cortical interactions related to sensorimotor function, primary-motor integration and switching from the “status quo” (Engel and Fries, 2010; Siegel et al., 2012). We found increased beta band synchrony in frontal-temporal, fronto-occipital and parieto-temporal connections with a left lateralization. More specifically, both networks (see Figures 3A2,B2) included part of the Broca’s area, the pars opercularis of the inferior frontal gyrus (IFG; Brodmann area 44). This area is a component of the motor articulatory network involved in speech production, phonological (Snell, 2010) and semantic processing (Roskies et al., 2001). Moreover, the pars opercularis has been found to be one of the human mirror neurons regions. It is active in motor imagery, imitation and action observation of distal hand and mouth actions (Rajmohan and Mohandas, 2007). Listening to songs requires processing verbal (lyrics) and musical (tunes) components, as well as motor preparation to sing along, dance or tap to the beat. One interpretation could be that children with ASD, compared to controls, showed impaired processing of these left-hemisphere brain functions when listening to unknown songs. Many fMRI studies have reported that ASD individuals lack left lateralization in structure and function of brain areas involved in language (Boddaert et al., 2003; Knaus et al., 2010; Nielsen et al., 2014) which can support this interpretation.

Our findings of differences in theta and beta frequencies are of particular interest in the context of music processing. Previous MEG studies in the auditory domain have implicated theta as well as beta-band activity in the detection of pitch changes (Florin et al., 2017). Also, theta oscillations are important for temporal integration and for the detection of sounds (Ng et al., 2012; Florin et al., 2017). One potential explanation could be that children with ASD would have atypical processing of pitch compared to TD children, even though we did not find behavioral differences between groups in pitch discrimination ability. In addition, beta band is implicated in motor functions, and motor atypicalities are frequently reported in individuals with ASD (Williams et al., 2004; Fournier et al., 2010; Buard et al., 2018), implicating the mμ and beta rhythms during a fine motor imitation task in ASD adolescents. Listening to music is a sensorimotor experience in which we recognize a pulse of a rhythm pattern and naturally synchronize to that beat (through foot tapping or clapping). One could hypothesize that ASD children lack typical motor synchronization and entrainment to unfamiliar songs, but this requires further investigation.

The between group differences in theta and beta bands were seen in amplitude envelope connectivity. This measure, which correlates fluctuations in regional neuronal activity, is much slower than wPLI, and networks defined by AEC closely align with resting state networks seen with fMRI (Biswal et al., 1995; Brookes et al., 2011). The slow timescale (<0.1 Hz) of AEC can provide complementary insights of local signal power that regulates the activation of neural populations underlying large scale cortical interactions (Siegel et al., 2012).

When we explored the interaction between familiarity and groups, comparing the effects of connectivity (network strength) in familiar versus unfamiliar music in ASD versus TD children (using wPLI), we saw a pattern of increased connectivity strength in the unfamiliar condition compared to familiar stimuli in TD in the alpha frequency, but no effects in the ASD group. The resulting network is important for processing music familiarity in TD and comprised nodes of the limbic system, anchored in the left insula and putamen, and including the right amygdala, parahippocampus, hippocampus, mid and post cingulum, motor areas (caudate and putamen) and fusiform gyrus. The insulae are important in processing stimuli which are salient, related to the salience network, and atypical insular function has been reported in ASD (Uddin et al., 2013). The fact that the main hub of this network was the left insula suggests that for the TD children, the familiar songs had far more salience for them, capturing their attention, but that this was not the case for the children with ASD. This is also consistent with the work by Odriozola et al. (2016) and Leung et al. (2014), who showed that for faces the right insula activity was atypical in ASD.

It was also interesting to find brain areas related to face processing in this interaction network when processing music familiarity, as recognizing a song also implies recognizing the singer’s voice. There is emerging evidence showing that when we hear a familiar voice, even without seeing the face, our visual face processing brain areas become active (Von Kriegstein et al., 2005; Von Kriegstein and Giraud, 2006; Blank et al., 2011). This cross-modal activation is fast, automatic and well supported by neural circuits, which have been shaped by multisensory stimuli. For example, in an MEG study with typically adults, Schall et al. (2013) investigated early auditory processing of familiar voices and showed that it is facilitated by visual mechanisms.

Alpha wPLI synchronization, as reported in our interaction effect, has been implicated in visual perception in TD individuals (Freunberger et al., 2008; Palva and Palva, 2011) and also plays a key role in cognitive functions, coordinating neuronal processing (Palva and Palva, 2007, 2011). The effect we see here in TD but not in ASD children suggests that individuals with ASD process novel songs differently due to deficits in neural adaptation-like effects.

Lastly, the lack of association between differences in connectivity and measures of core symptom domains was disappointing. This may be partially explained by the fact that processing of familiar stimuli was relatively preserved and as such unlikely to be associated with down-stream behavioral effects. Another consideration is heterogeneity in both the recognition of familiarity, in the ASD symptoms measures and in the ASD population in general.

Limitations and Future Work

There are some potential limitations of the present study. The first one relates to the sample size. Forty eight participants (24 per group), although aligned with previous MEG studies in autistic children (Gandal et al., 2010; Roberts et al., 2011; Safar et al., 2018; Yuk et al., 2018), is relatively small in the current climate of big data. However, the complexity of the study, with the multiple visits and assessments was a limiting factor. The second limitation is that our clinical and control groups were not matched on IQ (Table 1), which was significantly lower in our clinical group yet still in the average range. This is, however, not atypical in studies with those with ASD, as on average there is an IQ difference. Equating for IQ then makes the ASD group less representative of what is found generally and the results less generalizable.

A final limitation is the exclusion of the cerebellum from our analysis plan due to technical and methodological reasons. The cerebellum is important for sensorimotor, cognitive and emotional processing (Buckner, 2013) and plays a key role in rhythm and timing processes (Schmahmann, 2004; Nozaradan et al., 2017), but MEG signals from the cerebellum are difficult to record and subject to considerable artifact and distortion (Ioannides, 2005; Muthukumaraswamy, 2013). It would also be important for future studies to assess the rhythm abilities of participants as successful recognition of familiar songs builds on melody (pitch) and rhythm identification (Peretz and Coltheart, 2003; Volkova et al., 2014), and differences in these skills may impact the findings.

Our results on relatively preserved processing of familiar songs in ASD cannot address whether this is specific to music networks, as it could be domain-general to all familiar auditory stimuli. Future research could compare the processing of familiar music to the processing of familiar speech or familiar environmental sounds to shed light on this matter.

Conclusion

This study provides the first evidence of brain connectivity patterns involved in familiarity in music listening in both typically developing and autistic children. Our results revealed atypical processing of unfamiliar songs in children with ASD. During the processing of unfamiliar music, we demonstrated increased theta and beta band task-dependent connectivity in children with ASD compared to controls. The effects of connectivity (network strength) between familiarity and groups showed increased alpha connectivity strength in unfamiliar condition compared to familiar stimuli in TD but no effect was seen in the ASD group. These results, in addition to adding valuable information to the growing literature on atypical brain connectivity in the ASD population, inform future research in the field of learning and on the neurobiological correlates of music familiarity in autism that may guide the development of music-based interventions. Future work is needed to replicate and expand our findings throughout development, both in typical development and children with ASD.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Research Ethics Board of both Holland Bloorview Kids Rehabilitation Hospital and Hospital for Sick Children. Written informed consent to participate in this study was provided by the participants’ legal guardian/next of kin.

Author Contributions

CF participated in the task design, data recruitment, data collection, data analysis, results interpretation, and manuscript writing. SW participated in the data analysis. BH participated in the data analysis and interpretation of the results. AI, LR, SF, and SC participated in the data recruitment and data collection. JC, RS, BD, MT, JL, and EA participated in the task design, data analysis, results interpretation, and manuscript editing. JB and LS participated in discussion of the results and this manuscript. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This research was supported by the Province of Ontario Neurodevelopmental Disorders (POND) Network (PI: EA), in partnership with the Ontario Brain Institute (OBI) and Bloorview Research Institute and also by a doctoral scholarship (reference SFRH/BD/116213/2016) attributed to CF from the Portuguese Foundation for Science and Technology (FCT).

Conflict of Interest

RS is on the Professional Advisory Board of Ironshore Pharmaceutical and CHADD. EA has received consultation fees from Roche and Quadrant; industry funding from Roche, SynapDx and Sanofi-Aventis; in kind industry support from AMO Pharma and CRR, royalties from APPI and Springer International Publishing; editorial honoraria from Wiley.

The remaining authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank Olivia Dell’Unto, Julie Lu, Amanda Robertson, and Marc Lalancette for their help in the data collection, and thank and acknowledge the dedication of all the families that participated in this study.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnins.2022.829415/full#supplementary-material

Footnotes

  1. ^ https://medicine.yale.edu/bioimaging/suite/mni2tal/
  2. ^ https://github.com/OHBA-analysis/MEG-ROI-nets

References

Alter, A. L., and Oppenheimer, D. M. (2009). Uniting the tribes of fluency to form a metacognitive nation. Per. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 13, 219–235. doi: 10.1177/1088868309341564

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baron-Cohen, S., and Belmonte, M. (2005). Autism: a window onto the development of the social and the analytic brain. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 28, 109–126. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.27.070203.144137

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Batty, M., Meaux, E., Wittemeyer, K., Rogé, B., and Taylor, M. J. (2011). Early processing of emotional faces in children with autism: an event-related potential study. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 109, 430–444. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2011.02.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bhatara, A., Quintin, E.-M., Fombonne, E., and Levitin, D. J. (2013). Early sensitivity to sound and musical preferences and enjoyment in adolescents with autism spectrum disorders. Psychomusicol. Music. Mind Brain 23, 100–108. doi: 10.1037/a0033754

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bishop, S. L., Hus, V., Duncan, A., Huerta, M., Gotham, K., Pickles, A., et al. (2013). Subcategories of restricted and repetitive behaviors in children with autism spectrum disorders. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 43, 1287–1297. doi: 10.1007/s10803-012-1671-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Biswal, B., Yetkin, F. Z., Haughton, V. M., and Hyde, J. S. (1995). Functional connectivity in the motor cortex of resting human brain using echo-planar MRI. Magn. Reson. Med. 34, 537–541. doi: 10.1002/mrm.1910340409

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blank, H., Anwander, A., and von Kriegstein, K. (2011). Direct structural connections between voice- and face-recognition areas. J. Neurosci. 31, 12906–12915. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.2091-11.2011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boddaert, N., Belin, P., Chabane, N., Poline, J. B., Barthélémy, C., Mouren-Simeoni, M. C., et al. (2003). Perception of complex sounds: abnormal pattern of cortical activation in autism. Am. J. Psychiatry 160, 2057–2060. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.11.2057

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boyd, B. A., Baranek, G. T., Sideris, J., Poe, M. D., Watson, L. R., Patten, E., et al. (2010). Sensory features and repetitive behaviors in children with autism and developmental delays. Autism Res. 3, 78–87. doi: 10.1002/aur.124

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brandon Miler (2016). The First 30 Seconds. Available online at: https://imusician.pro/en/resources/blog/the-first-30-seconds

Google Scholar

Brattico, E., and Pearce, M. (2013). The neuroaesthetics of music. Psychol. Aesthetics Creat. Arts 7, 48–61. doi: 10.1037/a0031624

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brookes, M. J., Hale, J. R., Zumer, J. M., Stevenson, C. M., Francis, S. T., Barnes, G. R., et al. (2011). Measuring functional connectivity using MEG: Methodology and comparison with fcMRI. Neuroimage 56, 1082–1104. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.02.054

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buard, I., Kronberg, E., Steinmetz, S., Hepburn, S., and Rojas, D. C. (2018). Neuromagnetic beta-band oscillations during motor imitation in youth with autism. Autism Res. Treat. 2018:9035793. doi: 10.1155/2018/9035793

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buckner, R. L. (2013). The cerebellum and cognitive function: 25 years of insight from anatomy and neuroimaging. Neuron 80, 807–815. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.10.044

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Caria, A., Venuti, P., and De Falco, S. (2011). Functional and dysfunctional brain circuits underlying emotional processing of music in autism spectrum disorders. Cereb. Cortex 21, 2838–2849. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhr084

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cavanna, A. E., and Trimble, M. R. (2006). The precuneus: a review of its functional anatomy and behavioural correlates. Brain 129, 564–583. doi: 10.1093/brain/awl004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, J. L., Penhune, V. B., and Zatorre, R. J. (2008). Moving on time: brain network for auditory-motor synchronization is modulated by rhythm complexity and musical training. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 20, 226–239. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2008.20018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, Y. H., Rodgers, J., and McConachie, H. (2009). Restricted and repetitive behaviours, sensory processing and cognitive style in children with autism spectrum disorders. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 39, 635–642. doi: 10.1007/s10803-008-0663-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chimeno, M. F., and Pallàs-Areny, R. (2000). A comprehensive model for power line interference in biopotential measurements. IEEE Trans. Instrum. Meas. 49, 535–540. doi: 10.1109/19.850390

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen, M. X. (2014). Analyzing Neural time Series data: Theory and Practice. Armonk, NY: MIT Press.

Google Scholar

Colclough, G. L., Brookes, M. J., Smith, S. M., and Woolrich, M. W. (2015). NeuroImage a symmetric multivariate leakage correction for MEG connectomes. Neuroimage 117, 439–448. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.03.071

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crane, M. (2017). Has Music Streaming Killed the Instrumental Intro?. Columbus, OH: Ohio State Univ, 3–7.

Google Scholar

Dalla Bella, S., Peretz, I., and Aronoff, N. (2003). Time course of melody recognition: a gating paradigm study. Percept. Psychophys. 65, 1019–1028. doi: 10.3758/BF03194831

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dawson, G., Carver, L., Meltzoff, A. N., Panagiotides, H., Webb, S. J., Development, C., et al. (2002). Neural correlates of face and object recognition in young children with autism spectrum disorder, developmental delay, and typical development. Child Dev. 73, 700–717. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00433

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Demorest, S. M., Morrison, S. J., Stambaugh, L. A., Beken, M., Richards, T. L., and Johnson, C. (2010). An fMRI investigation of the cultural specificity of music memory. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 5, 282–291. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsp048

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dube, W. V. (2009). “Stimulus overselectivity in discrimination learning,” in Behavioral Theories and Interventions for Autism, ed. P. Reed (New York, NY: Nova Science Publishers), 23–46.

Google Scholar

Engel, A. K., and Fries, P. (2010). Beta-band oscillations–signalling the status quo? Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 20, 156–165. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2010.02.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Engel, A. K., Fries, P., and Singer, W. (2001). Dynamic predictions: oscillations and synchrony in top-down processing. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2, 704–716. doi: 10.1038/35094565

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Engel, A. K., Gerloff, C., Hilgetag, C. C., and Nolte, G. (2013). intrinsic coupling modes: multiscale interactions in ongoing brain activity. Neuron 80, 867–886. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.09.038

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fedorenko, E., Patel, A., Casasanto, D., Winawer, J., and Gibson, E. (2009). Structural integration in language and music: evidence for a shared system. Mem. Cogn. 37, 1–9. doi: 10.3758/MC.37.1.1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Florin, E., Vuvan, D., Peretz, I., and Baillet, S. (2017). Pre-target neural oscillations predict variability in the detection of small pitch changes. PLoS One 12:e0177836. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0177836

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fonov, V., Almli, C., Evans, A., Collins, D., and McKinstry, R. (2009). Unbiased nonlinear average age-appropriate brain templates from birth to adulthood. Neuroimage 47:S102. doi: 10.1016/s1053-8119(09)70884-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fonov, V., McKinstry, R. C., Collins, D. L., Evans, A. C., Botteron, K., and Almli, C. R. (2011). Unbiased average age-appropriate atlases for pediatric studies. Neuroimage 54, 313–327. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.07.033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fournier, K. A., Hass, C. J., Naik, S. K., Lodha, N., and Cauraugh, J. H. (2010). Motor coordination in autism spectrum disorders: a synthesis and meta-analysis. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 40, 1227–1240. doi: 10.1007/s10803-010-0981-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Freitas, C., Manzato, E., Burini, A., Taylor, M. J., Lerch, J. P., and Anagnostou, E. (2018). neural correlates of familiarity in music listening : a systematic review and a neuroimaging. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 12:686. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2018.00686

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Freunberger, R., Klimesch, W., Griesmayr, B., Sauseng, P., and Gruber, W. (2008). Alpha phase coupling reflects object recognition. Neuroimage 42, 928–935. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.05.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gandal, M. J., Edgar, J. C., Ehrlichman, R. S., Mehta, M., Roberts, T. P. L., and Siegel, S. J. (2010). Validating gamma oscillations and delayed auditory responses as translational biomarkers of autism. Biol. Psychiatry 68, 1100–1106. doi: 10.1002/cncr.27633.Percutaneous

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gebauer, L., Skewes, J., Westphael, G., Heaton, P., Vuust, P., Altenmüller, E., et al. (2014). Intact brain processing of musical emotions in autism spectrum disorder, but more cognitive load and arousal in happy vs. sad music. Front. Neurosci. 8:192. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2014.00192

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gomot, M., Belmonte, M. K., Bullmore, E. T., Bernard, F. A., and Baron-Cohen, S. (2008). Brain hyper-reactivity to auditory novel targets in children with high-functioning autism. Brain 131, 2479–2488. doi: 10.1093/brain/awn172

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grahn, J. A. (2009). The role of the basal ganglia in beat perception: neuroimaging and neuropsychological investigations. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1169, 35–45. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.04553.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grelotti, D. J., Klin, A. J., Gauthier, I., Skudlarski, P., Cohen, D. J., Gore, J. C., et al. (2005). fMRI activation of the fusiform gyrus and amygdala to cartoon characters but not to faces in a boy with autism. Neuropsychologia 43, 373–385. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.06.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Groussard, M., Rauchs, G., Landeau, B., Viader, F., Desgranges, B., Eustache, F., et al. (2010). The neural substrates of musical memory revealed by fMRI and two semantic tasks. Neuroimage 53, 1301–1309. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.07.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

IBM Corp. Released (2017). IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, Version 25.0. Armonk, NY: IBM Corp.

Google Scholar

Ioannides, A. A. (2005). Widely distributed magnetoencephalography spikes related to the planning and execution of human saccades. J. Neurosci. 25, 7950–7967. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.1091-05.2005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jack, R. E., and Schyns, P. G. (2015). The human face as a dynamic tool for social communication. Curr. Biol. 25, R621–R634. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2015.05.052

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Janata, P. (2009). The neural architecture of music-evoked autobiographical memories. Cereb. Cortex 19, 2579–2594. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhp008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Joye, Y., Steg, L., Ünal, A. B., and Pals, R. (2016). When complex is easy on the mind: Internal repetition of visual information in complex objects is a source of perceptual fluency. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 42, 103–114. doi: 10.1037/xhp0000105

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kanner, L. (1943). Autistic disturbances of affective contact. Nerv. Child 2, 217–250. doi: 10.1105/tpc.11.5.949

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kelly, M. P., Leader, G., and Reed, P. (2015). Stimulus over-selectivity and extinction-induced recovery of performance as a product of intellectual impairment and autism severity. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 45, 3098–3106. doi: 10.1007/s10803-015-2466-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kikuchi, M., Yoshimura, Y., Hiraishi, H., Munesue, T., Hashimoto, T., Tsubokawa, T., et al. (2015). Reduced long-range functional connectivity in young children with autism spectrum disorder. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 10, 248–254. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsu049

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

King, J., Jones, K., Goldberg, E., Rollins, M., MacNamee, K., Moffit, C., et al. (2018). increased functional connectivity after listening to favored music in adults with Alzheimer dementia. J. Prev. Alzheimers Dis. 6, 56–62. doi: 10.14283/jpad.2018.19

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Knaus, T. A., Silver, A. M., Kennedy, M., Lindgren, K. A., Kelli, C., Siegel, J., et al. (2010). Language laterality in autism spectrum disorder and typical controls: a functional, volumetric, and diffusion tensor MRI study. Brain Lang. 112, 1–21. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2009.11.005.Language

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koelsch, S. (2005a). Investigating emotion with music: neuroscientific approaches. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1060, 412–418. doi: 10.1196/annals.1360.034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koelsch, S. (2005b). Neural substrates of processing syntax and semantics in music. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 15, 207–212. doi: 10.1007/978-3-211-75121-3_9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lai, G., Pantazatos, S. P., Schneider, H., and Hirsch, J. (2012). Neural systems for speech and song in autism. Brain 135(Pt 3), 961–975. doi: 10.1093/brain/awr335

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lam, K. S. L., and Aman, M. G. (2007). The repetitive behaviour scale-revised: Independent validation in individuals with autism spectrum disorders. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 37, 855–866. doi: 10.1007/s10803-006-0213-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lartillot, O., Toiviainen, P., and Eerola, T. (2008). “A matlab toolbox for music information retrieval,” in Data Analysis, Machine Learning and Applications, Studies in Classification, Data Analysis and Knowledge Organization, eds C. Preisach, H. Burkhardt, L. Schmidt-Thieme, and R. Decker (Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer-Verlag). doi: 10.1007/978-3-540-78246-9_31

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lau, T. M., Gwin, J. T., McDowell, K. G., and Ferris, D. P. (2012). Weighted phase lag index stability as an artifact resistant measure to detect cognitive EEG activity during locomotion. J. Neuroeng. Rehabil. 9:47. doi: 10.1186/1743-0003-9-47

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leung, R. C., Pang, E. W., Anagnostou, E., and Taylor, M. J. (2018). Young adults with autism spectrum disorder show early atypical neural activity during emotional face processing. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 12:57. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2018.00057

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leung, R. C., Ye, A. X., Wong, S. M., Taylor, M. J., and Doesburg, S. M. (2014). Reduced beta connectivity during emotional face processing in adolescents with autism. Mol. Autism 5, 1–13. doi: 10.1186/2040-2392-5-51

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Léveillé Gauvin, H. (2018). Drawing listener attention in popular music: Testing five musical features arising from the theory of attention economy. Music. Sci. 22, 291–304. doi: 10.1177/1029864917698010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lord, C., Rutter, M., and Le Couteur, A. (1994). Autism diagnostic interview-revised: a revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 24, 659–685. doi: 10.1007/BF02172145

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mijalkov, M., Kakaei, E., Pereira, J. B., Westman, E., and Volpe, G. (2017). BRAPH: a graph theory software for the analysis of brain connectivity. PLoS One 12:e0178798. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0178798

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Millin, R., Kolodny, T., Flevaris, A. V., Kale, A. M., Schallmo, M. P., Gerdts, J., et al. (2018). Reduced auditory cortical adaptation in autism spectrum disorder. Elife 7, 1–15. doi: 10.7554/eLife.36493

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Munson, J., Faja, S., Meltzoff, A., and Abbott, R. (2008). Neurocognitive predictors of social and communicative developmental trajectories in preschoolers with autism spectrum disorders. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 14, 956–966. doi: 10.1017/S1355617708081393.Neurocognitive

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Muthukumaraswamy, S. D. (2013). High-frequency brain activity and muscle artifacts in MEG/EEG: a review and recommendations. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:138. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00138

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ng, B., Schroeder, T., and Kayser, C. (2012). A precluding but not ensuring role of entrained low-frequency oscillations for auditory perception. J. Neurosci. 32, 12268–12276. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1877-12.2012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nielsen, J. A., Zielinski, B. A., Fletcher, P. T., Alexander, A. L., Lange, N., Bigler, E. D., et al. (2014). Abnormal lateralization of functional connectivity between language and default mode regions in autism. Mol. Autism 5, 1–11. doi: 10.1186/2040-2392-5-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Noel, J. P., De Niear, M. A., Stevenson, R., Alais, D., and Wallace, M. T. (2017). Atypical rapid audio-visual temporal recalibration in autism spectrum disorders. Autism Res. 10, 121–129. doi: 10.1002/aur.1633

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nozaradan, S., Schwartze, M., Obermeier, C., and Kotz, S. A. (2017). Specific contributions of basal ganglia and cerebellum to the neural tracking of rhythm. Cortex 95, 156–168. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2017.08.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Odriozola, P., Uddin, L. Q., Lynch, C. J., Kochalka, J., Chen, T., and Menon, V. (2016). Insula response and connectivity during social and non-social attention in children with autism. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 11, 433–444. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsv126

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Olson, I. R., Plotzker, A., and Ezzyat, Y. (2007). The enigmatic temporal pole: a review of findings on social and emotional processing. Brain 130, 1718–1731. doi: 10.1093/brain/awm052

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oostenveld, R., Fries, P., Maris, E., and Schoffelen, J. M. (2011). FieldTrip: open source software for advanced analysis of MEG, EEG, and invasive electrophysiological data. Comput. Intell. Neurosci. 2011:156869. doi: 10.1155/2011/156869

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Palva, S., and Palva, J. M. (2007). New vistas for α-frequency band oscillations. Trends Neurosci. 30, 150–158. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2007.02.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Palva, S., and Palva, J. M. (2011). Functional roles of alpha-band phase synchronization in local and large-scale cortical networks. Front. Psychol. 2:204. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00204

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pang, E. W. (2011). Practical aspects of running developmental studies in the MEG. Brain Topogr. 24, 253–260. doi: 10.1007/s10548-011-0175-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pellicano, E., and Burr, D. (2012). When the world becomes ‘ too real ’: a Bayesian explanation of autistic perception. Cell Press 16, 504–510. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2012.08.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pereira, C. S., Teixeira, J., Figueiredo, P., Xavier, J., Castro, S. L., and Brattico, E. (2011). Music and emotions in the brain: familiarity matters. PLoS One 6:e27241. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0027241

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peretz, I., and Coltheart, M. (2003). Modularity of music processing. Nat. Neurosci. 6, 688–691. doi: 10.1038/nn1083

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pernet, C., Cardebat, D., Basan, S., Demonet, J.-F., Doyon, B., and Celsis, P. (2003). Neural timing of visual implicit categorization. Cogn. Brain Res. 17, 327–338. doi: 10.1016/s0926-6410(03)00134-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pierce, K., Haist, F., Sedaghat, F., and Courchesne, E. (2004). The brain response to personally familiar faces in autism: findings of fusiform activity and beyond. Brain 127, 2703–2716. doi: 10.1093/brain/awh289

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pierce, K., and Redcay, E. (2008). fusiform function in children with an autism spectrum disorder is a matter of “Who.”. Biol. Psychiatry 64, 552–560. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.05.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Plailly, J., Tillmann, B., and Royet, J. P. (2007). The feeling of familiarity of music and odors: The same neural signature? Cereb. Cortex 17, 2650–2658. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhl173

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rajmohan, V., and Mohandas, E. (2007). Mirror neuron system. Indian J. Psychiatry. 49, 66–69.

Google Scholar

Reed, P. (2011). “Comparator mechanisms and autistic spectrum conditions,” in Associative Learning and Conditioning: Human and Animal Applications, eds T. R. Schachtman and S. R. Reilly (Oxford: Oxford University Press).

Google Scholar

Reschke-Hernández, A. E. (2011). History of music therapy treatment interventions for children with autism. J. Music Ther. 48, 169–207. doi: 10.1093/jmt/48.2.169

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roberts, T. P. L., Cannon, K. M., Kambiz Tavabi, L., Blaskeya, L., Khana, S. Y., Monroea, J. F., et al. (2011). Auditory magnetic mismatch field latency: a biomarker for language impairment in autism. Biol. Psychiatry 70, 263–269. doi: 10.1038/mp.2011.182

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Robertson, C. E., and Baron-Cohen, S. (2017). Sensory perception in autism. Nat. Publ. Gr. 18, 671–684. doi: 10.1038/nrn.2017.112

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rodriguez, E., George, N., Lachaux, J., Martinerie, J., Renault, B., and Varela, F. (1999). Perception’s shadow: long-distance synchronization of human brain activity. Nature 397, 430–433. doi: 10.1038/17120

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rogalsky, C., Rong, F., Saberi, K., and Hickok, G. (2011). Functional anatomy of language and music perception: temporal and structural factors investigated using functional magnetic resonance imaging. J. Neurosci. 31, 3843–3852. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.4515-10.2011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roskies, A. L., Fiez, J. A., Balota, D. A., Raichle, M. E., and Petersen, S. E. (2001). Task-dependent modulation of regions in the left inferior frontal cortex during semantic processing. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 13, 829–843. doi: 10.1162/08989290152541485

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rutter, M., Bailey, A., and Lord, C. (2003). The Social Communication Questionnaire (SCQ). Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services.

Google Scholar

Sacks, O. (2008). Musicophilia:Tales of Music and the Brain. New York, NY: Alfred A.

Google Scholar

Safar, K., Wong, S. M., Leung, R. C., Dunkley, B. T., and Taylor, M. J. (2018). Increased functional connectivity during emotional face processing in children with autism spectrum disorder. Front. Hum. Neurosci 12:408. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2018.00408

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schall, S., Kiebel, S. J., Maess, B., and Von Kriegstein, K. (2013). Early auditory sensory processing of voices is facilitated by visual mechanisms. Neuroimage 77, 237–245. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.03.043

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmahmann, J. D. (2004). Disorders of the cerebellum: ataxia, dysmetria of thought, and the cerebellar affective syndrome. J. Neuropsychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 16, 367–378. doi: 10.1176/jnp.16.3.367

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schön, D., Gordon, R., Campagne, A., Magne, C., Astésano, C., Anton, J. L., et al. (2010). Similar cerebral networks in language, music and song perception. Neuroimage 51, 450–461. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.02.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sederberg, P. B., Kahana, M. J., Howard, M. W., Donner, E. J., and Madsen, J. R. (2003). Theta and gamma oscillations during encoding predict subsequent recall. J. Neurosci 23, 10809–10814. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.23-34-10809.2003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sharda, M., Midha, R., Malik, S., Mukerji, S., and Singh, N. C. (2015). Fronto-temporal connectivity is preserved during sung but not spoken word listening, across the autism spectrum. Autism Res. 8, 174–186. doi: 10.1002/aur.1437

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Siegel, M., Donner, T. H., and Engel, A. K. (2012). Spectral fingerprints of large-scale neuronal interactions. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 13, 121–134. doi: 10.1038/nrn3137

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sikka, R., Cuddy, L. L., Johnsrude, I. S., and Vanstone, A. D. (2015). An fMRI comparison of neural activity associated with recognition of familiar melodies in younger and older adults. Front. Neurosci. 9:356. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2015.00356

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Simmons, D. R., Robertson, A. E., McKay, L. S., Toal, E., McAleer, P., and Pollick, F. E. (2009). Vision in autism spectrum disorders. Vision Res. 49, 2705–2739. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2009.08.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Snell, R. S. (2010). Clinical Neuroanatomy. Philadelphia, PA: Wolters Kluwer-Lippincott, Williams & Wilkins.

Google Scholar

Stanutz, S., Wapnick, J., and Burack, J. A. (2014). Pitch discrimination and melodic memory in children with autism spectrum disorders. Autism 18, 137–147. doi: 10.1177/1362361312462905

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Szatmari, P., Georgiades, S., Bryson, S., Zwaigenbaum, L., Roberts, W., Mahoney, W., et al. (2006). Investigating the structure of the restricted, repetitive behaviours and interests domain of autism. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry Allied Discip. 47, 582–590. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2005.01537.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tomchek, S. D., and Dunn, W. (2007). Sensory processing in children with and without autism: a comparative study using the short sensory profile. Am. J. Occup. Ther. 61, 190–200. doi: 10.5014/ajot.61.2.190

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Turi, M., Burr, D. C., Igliozzi, R., Aagten-Murphy, D., Muratori, F., and Pellicano, E. (2015). Children with autism spectrum disorder show reduced adaptation to number. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 112, 7868–7872. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1504099112

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tzourio-Mazoyer, N., Landeau, B., Papathanassiou, D., Crivello, F., Etard, O., Delcroix, N., et al. (2002). Automated anatomical labeling of activations in SPM using a macroscopic anatomical parcellation of the MNI MRI single-subject brain. Neuroimage 15, 273–289. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0978

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Uddin, L. Q., Supekar, K., Lynch, C. J., Khouzam, A., Phillips, J., Feinstein, C., et al. (2013). Salience network-based classification and prediction of symptom severity in children with autism. JAMA Psychiatry 70, 869–879. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2013.104

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Veen, B. D., Van Drongelen, W., Yuchtman, M., and Suzuki, A. (1997). Localization of brain electrical activity via linearly constrained minimum variance spatial filtering. IEEE Trans. Biomed. Eng. 44, 867–880. doi: 10.1109/10.623056

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vinck, M., Oostenveld, R., Van Wingerden, M., Battaglia, F., and Pennartz, C. M. A. (2011). An improved index of phase-synchronization for electrophysiological data in the presence of volume-conduction, noise and sample-size bias. Neuroimage 55, 1548–1565. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.01.055

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Virji-Babul, N., Moiseev, A., Sun, W., Feng, T., Moiseeva, N., Watt, K. J., et al. (2013). Neural correlates of music recognition in down syndrome. Brain Cogn. 81, 256–262. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2012.11.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Volkova, A., Trehub, S. E., Schellenberg, E. G., Papsin, B. C., and Gordon, K. A. (2014). Children’s identification of familiar songs from pitch and timing cues. Front. Psychol. 5:863. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00863

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Von Kriegstein, K., and Giraud, A. L. (2006). Implicit multisensory associations influence voice recognition. PLoS Biol. 4:e326. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0040326

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Von Kriegstein, K., Kleinschmidt, A., Sterzer, P., and Giraud, A. (2005). Interaction of face and voice areas during speaker recognition. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 17, 367–376. doi: 10.1162/0898929053279577

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Watanabe, T., Yagishita, S., and Kikyo, H. (2008). Memory of music: roles of right hippocampus and left inferior frontal gyrus. Neuroimage 39, 483–491. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.08.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Webb, S. J., Dawson, G., Bernier, R., and Panagiotides, H. (2006). ERP evidence of atypical face processing in young children with autism. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 36, 881–890. doi: 10.1007/s10803-006-0126-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Webb, S. J., Jones, E. J. H., Merkle, K., Murias, M., Greenson, J., Richards, T., et al. (2010). Response to familiar faces, newly familiar faces, and novel faces as assessed by ERPs is intact in adults with autism spectrum disorders. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 77, 106–117. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2010.04.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Webb, S. J., Jones, E. J. H., Merkle, K., Venema, K., Greenson, J., Murias, M., et al. (2011). Developmental change in the erp responses to familiar faces in toddlers with autism spectrum disorders versus typical development. Child Dev. 82, 1868–1886. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01656.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wechsler, D. (2002). Wechsler Abbreviated Scales of Intelligence. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.

Google Scholar

Williams, J. H. G., Whiten, A., and Singh, T. (2004). A systematic review of action imitation in autistic spectrum disorder. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 34, 285–299. doi: 10.1023/B:JADD.0000029551.56735.3a

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Xia, M., Wang, J., and He, Y. (2013). Brainnet viewer: a network visualization tool for human brain connectomics. PLoS One 8:e68910 doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0068910

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yang, L. M. J., Shahinfard, E., Slack, P., Jacova, C., Kirkland, K., and Boyd, L. (2015). Patients with Alzheimer disease respond differently to familiar and unfamiliar music: An fMRI study Alzheimers Dement. 11:424

Google Scholar

Yuk, V., Urbain, C., Pang, E. W., Anagnostou, E., Buchsbaum, D., and Taylor, M. J. (2018). Do you know what I’m thinking? Temporal and spatial brain activity during a theory-of-mind task in children with autism. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 34, 139–147. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2018.08.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zalesky, A., Cocchi, L., Fornito, A., Murray, M. M., and Bullmore, E. (2012). Connectivity differences in brain networks. Neuroimage 60, 1055–1062. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.01.068

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zalesky, A., Fornito, A., and Bullmore, E. T. (2010). Network-based statistic: Identifying differences in brain networks. Neuroimage 53, 1197–1207. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.06.041

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: autism spectrum disorder, music, familiarity processing, magnetoencephalography, neural oscillation, functional connectivity

Citation: Freitas C, Hunt BAE, Wong SM, Ristic L, Fragiadakis S, Chow S, Iaboni A, Brian J, Soorya L, Chen JL, Schachar R, Dunkley BT, Taylor MJ, Lerch JP and Anagnostou E (2022) Atypical Functional Connectivity During Unfamiliar Music Listening in Children With Autism. Front. Neurosci. 16:829415. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2022.829415

Received: 05 December 2021; Accepted: 10 March 2022;
Published: 19 April 2022.

Edited by:

Stephen J. Gotts, National Institute of Mental Health (NIH), United States

Reviewed by:

Jessica R. Gilbert, National Institute of Mental Health (NIH), United States
Kyle M. Jasmin, University College London, United Kingdom

Copyright © 2022 Freitas, Hunt, Wong, Ristic, Fragiadakis, Chow, Iaboni, Brian, Soorya, Chen, Schachar, Dunkley, Taylor, Lerch and Anagnostou. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Carina Freitas, carina.debarrosfreitas@mail.utoronto.ca

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.