Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Neurosci., 06 January 2021
Sec. Perception Science
This article is part of the Research Topic Body Representation and Interoceptive Awareness: Cognitive, Affective, and Social Implications View all 15 articles

Gender Brain Structural Differences and Interoception

\r\nMariachiara LongarzoMariachiara LongarzoGiulia Mele*Giulia Mele*Vincenzo AlfanoVincenzo AlfanoMarco SalvatoreMarco SalvatoreCarlo Cavaliere\r\nCarlo Cavaliere
  • IRCCS SDN, Naples, Italy

Interoception, the ability to perceive inner body sensations, has been demonstrated to be different among genders, with a stronger female attention toward interoceptive information. No study correlated this capability with brain differences between males and females. This study aims to detect behavioral variances and structural neuroimaging interoception correlates in a sample of healthy volunteers matched for age. Seventy-three participants (37 females, mean age 43.5; 36 males, mean age 37.4) completed the Self-Awareness Questionnaire (SAQ) for interoceptive sensibility and underwent a structural MRI session. A t test corrected for Bonferroni multiple comparisons was performed to compare brain morphological parameters (cortical thickness and parcel volume) in both groups. A multivariate analysis of variance was performed to assess the effect of gender on scores obtained on the SAQ. A moderation model through multiple linear regression analysis was performed between gray matter volumes or parcels, cortical thickness, and the interoception score. Group analysis showed significant differences in morphometric brain data between males and females, both for cortical and subcortical volumes, but not for cortical thickness analyses. MANOVA underlined a significant difference in SAQ scores between males and females with higher values for the second ones. Moreover, a significant correlation between the interoception scores and gray matter volumes of the two groups has been detected, with a sharp prevalence for the female gender in the left insula with F1, F2, and SAQ interoception scores (R2 = 0.41, p < 0.001). Our results demonstrated that in the female group, a stronger predisposition was found toward interoceptive sensations, and that multiple brain areas were correlated with interoceptive measure. These data sustain a female advantage in the attention toward this process and support the idea that interoception in females is a process more shared across several regions that participate in creating the sense of self.

Introduction

Interoception is a multifaceted construct, reflecting the capability in perceiving inner body signals. The self-evaluation of subjective interoceptive sensations returns the interoceptive sensibility (Garfinkel et al., 2015). Substantial sex-related variations in paying attention to one’s bodily states have been demonstrated in several independent samples (Longarzo et al., 2020), where women exhibit higher attention to internal states and somatic complaints but reduced objective interoceptive accuracy (Grabauskaitë et al., 2017), understood as the objective accuracy to detect bodily sensations (Garfinkel et al., 2015). Murphy et al. (2019) in their review argued about the known sex differences in interoception, whereby women with respect to men report heightened attention to internal signals, and they hypothesized that interoceptive differences might be due to the amount of physical and hormonal changes experienced by women through life due to experiences of menstruation, pregnancy, and menopause. Interoception is closely related with visceral sensations; in fact, it is also a factor of the Self-Awareness Questionnaire (SAQ), a questionnaire focused on interoceptive stimuli (Longarzo et al., 2015). Gender seems to cover a role in abdominal pathologies such as irritable bowel syndrome: several authors such as Chang and Heitkemper (2002) reported that women experienced more constipation, nausea, bloating, and extraintestinal symptoms than men. As a brain correlate, it has been found that neural networks respond differently to visceral stimuli in men and women with irritable bowel syndrome (IBS).

However, such difference has not been related to possible differences between males and females in brain structures involved in interoception capability.

Convergent results from neuroimaging studies support the existence of significant differences between males and females in brain cytoarchitecture. At a global level, Gur et al. (1999) found differences between males and females in gray and white matter, where females have more gray matter, whereas males have more white matter and cerebrospinal fluid. It has been reported that the brain of males is 10% larger than that of females (Goldstein et al., 2001) even after adjustment (Cosgrove et al., 2007). Frontal and medial paralimbic brain regions are larger in women, whereas the hypothalamus, amygdala, and angular gyrus seem to be larger in men (Rezzani et al., 2019). In females, the volume of the corpus callosum and temporal and parietal regions (surrounding the Sylvian fissure) engaged in language processing is comparatively larger, whereas males have larger parietal cortical area associated with visual–spatial function (Grabowska, 2017).

It is largely recognized that insula and cingulate cortices are related to interoceptive stimuli processing. The anterior insular cortex is a station of encoding and representing interoceptive information and has been defined as an interoceptive cortex (Critchley et al., 2004; Craig, 2009). The insula has reciprocal projections with the anterior cingulate cortex that guides attention on physiological information and provides autonomic responses. However, little is known about sex-related differences in the engagement of brain areas related to interoceptive stimuli.

The present work aims to explore interoception among males and females at different levels and to determine whether there are differences in interoceptive awareness as a function of gender. Studying sex-related differences in the interoceptive process and how it relates to morphological brain aspects allows researchers to target specific populations in order to understand what makes them more susceptible. Firstly, we compared brain morphological parameters in the two groups to identify both global and specific differences. Next, differences between genders in attention toward interoceptive information have been investigated through an interoceptive sensibility measure. Finally, we investigated how behavioral results were related to brain regions in the two samples.

Materials and Methods

Participants

The study sample consisting of 73 healthy subjects (37 females, mean age 43.5 ± 14.6; 36 males, mean age 37.4 ± 12.5) participated in a research protocol conducted at the IRCCS SDN that included a clinical evaluation and a magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) protocol. All participants were recruited if they met the following criteria: (i) lack of current or past history of alcohol or drug abuse; (ii) lack of current or past history of major psychiatric illnesses; (iii) lack of history of brain injury, stroke, or any other major clinical condition; and (iv) lack of current or past use of psychoactive medications. The eligibility criteria were assessed through a brief clinical interview performed by an expert psychologist.

Each participant provided written informed consent approved by the local Ethics Committee of IRCCS Pascale and performed according to the ethical standards laid down in the 1964 Helsinki Declaration and its later amendments. All individuals were naive to the scope of the study and gave their written informed consent to participate without any reward.

Neuropsychological Assessment

All participants in the study completed the SAQ, a self-report tool devised to evaluate the perception of a wide range of bodily sensations and, in particular, investigate the frequency with which volunteers perceive signals from their own body (Longarzo et al., 2015).

The SAQ consisted of 28 items to be rated on a five-point Likert scale (0 = never; 1 = sometimes; 2 = often; 3 = very often; 4 = always). The total score ranges 0–112 with higher scores meaning higher interoceptive awareness. The SAQ proved to have a bifactorial structure: the first factor (F1) comprises items related to visceral sensations, whereas the second one (F2) is related to somatosensory sensations.

For the present study, the multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) was performed to assess the effect of gender on demographics and scores obtained on SAQ. Statistical analyses were conducted using SPSS (IBM Corp. Released 2016. IBM SPSS Statistics, Version 24.0).

MRI Scanning and Brain Morphometry

Structural MRIs were collected from a 3-Tesla PET-MR Siemens Biograph mMR unit (housed at IRCCS SDN in Naples) using a 12-channel head coil with an axial structural 3D-T1-weighted sequence (TR = 2,400 ms, TE = 2.25 ms, flip angle = 8°, voxel size 0.8 × 0.8 × 0.8 mm, matrix 256 × 256, field of view 214 × 214). Structural images were also reviewed for incidental brain abnormalities by an experienced neuroradiologist (CC). The parcellations of morphological T1-weighted 3D images of subjects were processed with FreeSurfer v5.1 toolkit (Dale et al., 1999; Alfano et al., 2020). Briefly, this processing includes spatial inhomogeneity correction, non-linear noise reduction, skull-stripping, subcortical segmentation, intensity normalization, surface generation, topology correction, surface inflation, registration to a spherical atlas, and thickness calculation (Fischl and Dale, 2000). To map all subjects’ brains to a common space, reconstructed surfaces were registered to the Desikan-Killiany atlas using a non-linear procedure that optimally aligned sulcal and gyral features across subjects. Brain morphological parameters, including cortical and subcortical volume and cortical thickness, were calculated using processed and segmented FreeSurfer data. Then, they were normalized by the ratio with the estimated total intracranial volume (eTIV). A moderation model through multiple linear regression analysis was performed between gray matter volumes or parcels, cortical thickness, and the interoception score in both female and male groups using gender as the moderator. Furthermore, a type I and type III sum of squares was performed with the interoception score (F1, F2, SAQ total) in order to indicate whether a variable brings significant information or not, once all the other variables are already included in the model. Then, for the model estimation, the adjusted R2 was used to consider the number of predictors in the model. Finally, a two-tailed two-sample t test, corrected for Bonferroni multiple comparisons (significant p value < 0.0004), was performed to compare brain morphological parameters in both groups.

Results

MANOVA revealed significant differences between males and females (Wilks’ Lambda = 0.89; F = 4.5). The analyses on single measures showed significant differences between males and females on SAQ total score (p = 0.008) and on both factors, F1 (p = 0.004) related to visceral sensations and F2 (p = 0.049) related to somatosensory sensations. Statistical analysis of behavioral data underlined significant differences in interoceptive awareness between genders. Mean age did not differ between the two groups (Table 1).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Demographics and neuropsychological characteristics.

Regarding brain morphometry, group analysis showed significant differences in the male subjects compared to the female group: brain parcels that survived at multiple comparisons test are resumed in Table 2. Multiple linear regression analysis showed a significant model with correlations between gray matter brain parcels and F1, F2, and total SAQ score (female group: left inferior parietal R2 = 0.17, p = 0.04; left posterior cingulate R2 = 0.17, p = 0.04; left precentral R2 = 0.21, p = 0.02; left insula R2 = 0.41, p < 0.001; left parahippocampal R2 = 0.31, p = 0.002; right pars opercularis R2 = 0.18, p = 0.04; right post central R2 = 0.27, p = 0.005; right posterior cingulate R2 = 0.18, p = 0.03; right supramarginal R2 = 0.17, p = 0.04. Male group: left precuneus R2 = 0.20, p = 0.03) (Figure 1). In both groups, different values of F1, F2, and total SAQ score allow us to explain an amount of the variability of the brain parcels (Table 3); no correlation was found with cortical thickness parcels.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Morphological differences of normalized brain parcels’ volume between the male (M) and female (F) subject groups.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Left insula cortex representation with positive correlation of the left insular volume and the interoceptive scores: F1, F2, and SAQ in the female group, from multiple linear regression analysis.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Multiple linear regression model in the female and male groups between gray matter volumes and interoception scores (F1, F2, and SAQ total score).

Discussion

In the present study, we aimed to investigate interoceptive capability and interoceptive brain correlates in a sample of healthy males and females. The SAQ was used to measure subjective interoceptive sensibility, and similar to previous reports, women obtained scores significantly higher with respect to men on the SAQ total score and on both factors of the SAQ, related to visceral sensations and somatosensory sensations. Firstly, we investigated morphological differences in brain volumes among the two groups. Previous studies have demonstrated that dimorphism exists between males and females for volume and surface area, and reported that men display global larger brain volume. Similarly, we found that male subjects reported larger volume in several areas across the brain, even if the female group reported major volume in some parcels on both brain sides, such as the insula and central areas (Wierenga et al., 2014).

Moreover, and for the first time, behavioral results on interoception have been correlated with brain areas among males and females. Positive correlations have been found between interoception scores and several brain region volumes. In the male group, total score and both F1 and F2 factors of the SAQ correlated with the precuneus, a major association area that may subserve a variety of behavioral functions. Converging evidences from functional imaging studies in healthy subjects indicate that the precuneus covers a role in the internal mentation processes of self-consciousness (Cavanna and Trimble, 2006), but to date, no specific correlations were demonstrated between the precuneus and interoception, and it was not specifically investigated among genders.

The precuneus has reciprocal connections with many cortical areas, afferent to the parietal lobe such as the operculum, and extra parietal zones such as the cingulate cortex, which is primarily involved in interoceptive processing and which, in our sample, is significantly related with interoceptive scores in the female group. Furthermore, the connection of the precuneus with the temporo-parieto-occipital cortex functionally responds to the integration of somatosensory information.

Kircher et al. (2000) demonstrated that self-descriptive traits activate a network comprising the bilateral precuneus, superior parietal lobe, prefrontal cortex, and cingulate cortex. Also, Gusnard et al. (2001) attributed to the precuneus, cingulate, and medial prefrontal areas the role of engaging continuous information and representation of the self when a person is awake and alert (Cavanna and Trimble, 2006). In the present sample, we found that besides the precuneus, in the female group, also the parietal and temporal areas display a strong connection with our interoceptive measure, sustaining the hypothesis that this network, also if not directly implied in, probably cooperates to interoceptive monitoring and self-processing. The interaction between the precuneus and prefrontal cortex has been postulated in a state of consciousness characterized by a high level of reflective self-consciousness (Kjaer et al., 2002).

The interconnected medial prefrontal regions and the posterior medial parietal represent a network through which personal identity forms, permitting to build the self-awareness. Wang et al. (2019) found activation of subsequent brain regions involved in interoceptive attention: the inferior parietal lobule, post central, and supramarginal. One possibility is that all of these regions concur to create a personal perspective of the proper bodily status and that females have a stronger predisposition to achieve the contents of proper mind about bodily condition.

In the female group, the correlation we found between the insula and interoceptive sensitivity measure is noteworthy since wide neuroimaging evidences identify it as the brain site of interoceptive processes. The existence of several subdivisions of the insular cortex is important when considering the mechanisms of learning and memory that involve the insula (Von Bernhardi et al., 2017). In particular, the anterior one is involved in emotional awareness; the mid insula influences one’s physical self-perception, promotes goal-directed cognition, and is active during external and internal stimuli integration; the posterior part is involved in somatosensory functions and contributes to interoceptive processing. The latter one is a site of convergence of interoceptive and limbic systems inputs (Klabunde et al., 2016). Relevant to the central networks involved in learning are the neural connections of the rostral agranular insular cortex with the amygdala and hippocampal formation, which, in the present study, has demonstrated to be correlated with the interoceptive measure. Basing on the close relationship between the insula and hippocampus, we could speculate that its correlation with the SAQ may reflect the “proper corporeal memory” useful for building a quite stable interoceptive identity, with which one can compare the physical state of the moment in order to catch personal variations that allow answering the question “how do you feel?.” Another region closely related with the insula is the cingulate cortex, even if previous studies have reported a larger volume of the cingulate cortex in women (Mann et al., 2011). In the present study, this finding was not replicated, but a significant correlation was found between both the anterior and posterior cingulate cortex with the interoceptive measure, in the women subgroup. The cingulate is the area that, together with the insula, most of all contributes to elaborate the interceptive information, particularly in choosing the most appropriate response to the perceived stimuli. Also, from a functional perspective, the cingulum showed gender differences, mainly in studies on emotional processes, closely related to interoception (Mann et al., 2011).

The carelessness of males in paying attention toward proper bodily signs could be explained based on the data by Sun et al. (2015), who demonstrated a higher global efficiency of males, suggesting a predilection for global information integration rather than for detailed information. Considering this data, we could speculate that interoception entails more detailed information, not globally but at a specific level, that needs more attention to be captured.

Our results demonstrated that in the female group, a stronger correlation of multiple areas with interoceptive measure was found. These data sustain a female advantage in the attention toward this process and support the idea that interoception in females is a process more shared across several regions that participate in creating the sense of self.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Ethics Committee of IRCCS Pascale, Naples. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

ML conceptualized the study and wrote the manuscript. CC conceptualized the study and critically revised the manuscript. VA and GM collected the data and performed the behavioral and neuroimaging analyses. MS revised and approved final version of manuscript. All authors approved the submitted version.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Alfano, V., Longarzo, M., Aiello, M., Soricelli, A., and Cavaliere, C. (2020). Cerebral microstructural abnormalities in impulsivity: a magnetic resonance study. Brain Imag. Behav. doi: 10.1007/s11682-020-00261-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cavanna, A. E., and Trimble, M. R. (2006). The precuneus: a review of its functional anatomy and behavioural correlates. Brain 129(Pt 3), 564–583. doi: 10.1093/brain/awl004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chang, L., and Heitkemper, M. M. (2002). Gender differences in irritable bowel syndrome. Gastroenterology 123, 1686–1701. doi: 10.1053/gast.2002.36603

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cosgrove, K. P., Mazure, C. M., and Staley, J. K. (2007). Evolving knowledge of sex differences in brain structure, function, and chemistry. Biol. Psychiatry 62, 847–855. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.03.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Craig, A. D. (2009). How do you feel-now? The anterior insula and human awareness. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 10, 59–70. doi: 10.1038/nrn2555

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Critchley, H. D., Wiens, S., Rotshtein, P., Ohman, A., and Dolan, R. J. (2004). Neural systems supporting interoceptive awareness. Nat. Neurosci. 7, 189–195. doi: 10.1038/nn1176

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dale, A. M., Fischl, B., and Sereno, M. I. (1999). Cortical surface-based analysis. I. Segmentation and surface reconstruction. Neuroimage 9, 179–194. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1998.0395

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fischl, B., and Dale, A. M. (2000). Measuring the thickness of the human cerebral cortex from magnetic resonance images. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 97, 11050–11055. doi: 10.1073/pnas.200033797

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Garfinkel, S. N., Seth, A. K., Barrett, A. B., Suzuki, K., and Critchley, H. D. (2015). Knowing your own heart: distinguishing interoceptive accuracy from interoceptive awareness. Biol. Psychol. 104, 65–74. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2014.11.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goldstein, J. M., Seidman, L. J., Horton, N. J., Makris, N. K., Kennedy, D. N., Caviness, V. S. Jr., et al. (2001). Normal sexual dimorphism of the adult human brain assessed by in vivo magnetic resonance imaging. Cereb. Cortex 11, 490–497. doi: 10.1093/cercor/11.6.490

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grabauskaitë, A., Baranauskas, M., and Griškova-Bulanova, I. (2017). Interoception and gender: what aspects should we pay attention to? Conscious. Cogn. 48, 129–137. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2016.11.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grabowska, A. (2017). Sex on the brain: are gender-dependent structural and functional differences associated with behavior? J. Neurosci. Res. 95, 200–212. doi: 10.1002/jnr.23953

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gur, R. C., Turetsky, B. I., Matsui, M., Yan, M., Bilker, W., Hughett, P., et al. (1999). Sex differences in brain gray and white matter in healthy young adults: correlations with cognitive performance. J. Neurosci. 19, 4065–4072. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-10-04065.1999

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gusnard, D. A., Akbudak, E., Shulman, G. L., and Raichle, M. E. (2001). Medial prefrontal cortex and self-referential mental activity: relation to a default mode of brain function. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 98, 4259–4264. doi: 10.1073/pnas.071043098

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kircher, T. T., Senior, C., Phillips, M. L., Benson, P. J., Bullmore, E. T., Brammer, M., et al. (2000). Towards a functional neuroanatomy of self processing: effects of faces and words. Brain Res. 10, 133–144. doi: 10.1016/s0926-6410(00)00036-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kjaer, T. W., Nowak, M., and Lou, H. C. (2002). Reflective self-awareness and conscious states: PET evidence for a common midline parietofrontal core. Neuroimage 17, 1080–1086. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2002.1230

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klabunde, M., Weems, C. F., Raman, M., and Carrion, V. G. (2016). The moderating effects of sex on insula subdivision structure in youth with posttraumatic stress symptoms. Depress Anxiety 34, 51–58. doi: 10.1002/da.22577

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Longarzo, M., D’Olimpio, F., Chiavazzo, A., Santangelo, G., Trojano, L., and Grossi, D. (2015). The relationships between interoception and alexithymic trait. the self-awareness questionnaire in healthy subjects. Front. Psychol. 6:1149. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01149

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Longarzo, M., Grossi, D., and Trojano, L. (2020). Perception of interoception among genders. Percept. Conscious. Cogn. 48, 129–137.

Google Scholar

Mann, S. L., Hazlett, E. A., Byne, W., Hof, P. R., Buchsbaum, M. S., Cohen, B. H., et al. (2011). Anterior and posterior cingulate cortex volume in healthy adults: effects of aging and gender differences. Brain Res. 1401, 18–29. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2011.05.050

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Murphy, J., Viding, E., and Bird, G. (2019). Does atypical interoception following physical change contribute to sex differences in mental illness? Psychol. Rev. 126, 787–789. doi: 10.1037/rev0000158

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rezzani, R., Franco, C., and Rodella, L. F. (2019). Sex differences of brain and their implications for personalized therapy. Pharmacol. Res. 141, 429–442. doi: 10.1016/j.phrs.2019.01.030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sun, Y., Lee, R., Chen, Y., Collinson, S., Thakor, N., Bezerianos, A., et al. (2015). Progressive gender differences of structural brain networks in healthy adults: a longitudinal, diffusion tensor imaging study. PLoS One 10:e0118857. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0118857

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Von Bernhardi, R., Eugenín, J., and Muller, K. J. (2017). The Plastic Brain. Berlin: Springer International Publishing.

Google Scholar

Wang, X., Wu, Q., Egan, L., Gu, X., Liu, P., Gu, H., et al. (2019). Anterior insular cortex plays a critical role in interoceptive attention. eLife 8:e42265. doi: 10.7554/eLife.42265

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wierenga, L. M., Langen, M., Oranje, B., and Durston, S. (2014). Unique developmental trajectories of cortical thickness and surface area. Neuroimage 87, 120–126. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.11.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: interoception, sex differences, structural MRI, gray matter volume, SAQ score

Citation: Longarzo M, Mele G, Alfano V, Salvatore M and Cavaliere C (2021) Gender Brain Structural Differences and Interoception. Front. Neurosci. 14:586860. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2020.586860

Received: 24 July 2020; Accepted: 27 November 2020;
Published: 06 January 2021.

Edited by:

Matteo Martini, University of East London, United Kingdom

Reviewed by:

Taiyong Bi, Zunyi Medical University, China
Yang Sun, Shenyang Normal University, China

Copyright © 2021 Longarzo, Mele, Alfano, Salvatore and Cavaliere. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Giulia Mele, Giulia.mele@synlab.it

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.