Skip to main content

OPINION article

Front. Neurosci., 20 August 2019
Sec. Sleep and Circadian Rhythms
This article is part of the Research Topic Local Aspects of Sleep and Wakefulness View all 15 articles

Local Sleep Oscillations: Implications for Memory Consolidation

  • 1Department of Neurosurgery, University of California, Los Angeles, Los Angeles, CA, United States
  • 2Sagol School of Neuroscience, Tel Aviv University, Tel Aviv, Israel
  • 3Department of Physiology and Pharmacology, Sackler School of Medicine, Tel Aviv University, Tel Aviv, Israel
  • 4Functional Neurophysiology and Sleep Research Lab, Tel-Aviv Sourasky Medical Center, Tel Aviv, Israel

Introduction

Accumulating evidence suggests that sleep is important for plasticity and memory consolidation (Maquet, 2001; Walker and Stickgold, 2004; Datta and Maclean, 2007; Diekelmann and Born, 2010; Tononi and Cirelli, 2014; Dudai et al., 2015)—the transformation of new labile memories encoded in wakefulness into stable representations that integrate into long-term memory networks. A central model accounting for memory consolidation during sleep is that of coupling between hippocampal (HC) and neocortical networks (Buzsáki, 1996). According to this two-stage model of memory formation [also termed the hippocampal—neocortical dialogue model (Buzsáki, 1989)], the dominant direction of information flow across the brain differs between wake and sleep periods. During wakefulness, acquisition of sensory information mainly drives signal propagation from cortex to hippocampus (HC) (Buzsáki, 1998; Mormann et al., 2008). By contrast, during subsequent non-rapid eye movement (NREM) sleep, this model suggests a central role for information flow from HC to cortex especially around sharp-wave ripples (SWRs) events (Buzsáki, 1998). Accordingly, slow waves that originate in the neocortex repeatedly reactivate the newly encoded HC information when SWRs occur, driving subsequent activity in select cortical circuits (Siapas and Wilson, 1998). However, it is clear that information flow is not strictly unidirectional (Wagner et al., 2010) and may involve complex loops (Rothschild et al., 2017). HC reactivation tends to co-occur with sleep spindles that optimize plasticity (Seibt et al., 2017), resulting in long-term modification of synaptic efficacy. Thus, hippocampal–neocortical coupling requires interregional cross-frequency coordination between sleep oscillations, including slow waves and sleep spindles in thalamo-cortical circuits as well as HC ripples.

The underlying prevalent assumption is that sleep oscillations (slow waves in particular) are global events that co-occur nearly simultaneously across different brain regions. But in fact, they have been described as traveling waves propagating from anterior-to-posterior cortex (Massimini et al., 2004), and they typically occur out of phase across different cortical sites (Nir et al., 2011; Vyazovskiy et al., 2011; Malerba et al., 2019). How can we reconcile models requiring co-occurrence of sleep oscillations with accumulating evidence of non-uniform timing of oscillations across the brain? In this article, we first review the current data that sheds light on this question, and highlight recent studies that link regional coupling of sleep oscillations with consolidation of specific memories. Then, we highlight the gap between sleep and memory theory and experimental evidence. Based on studies that monitor and manipulate specific cortical circuits, we propose that coupling can occur between sleep oscillations in general, and between HC and cortex specifically, but that such coupling likely involves different brain regions at each point in time, contributing to memory consolidation in select circuits.

Slow Waves, Spindles, and Their Regional Modulation Following Learning

Slow waves and sleep spindles constitute electroencephalographic (EEG) hallmarks of NREM sleep (Gibbs and Gibbs, 1950; Steriade, 2003). These robust oscillations are easily identified using non-invasive EEG and form the main criterion for sleep stage definition across mammalian species (Iber et al., 2007). Both oscillations are implicated in memory consolidation as we review below. While EEG represents summed activity across large cortical territories (Nunez, 1995), we will focus here on accumulating evidence that characterizes slow waves and spindles as local phenomena.

Neocortical slow waves reflect slow (1–4 Hz) alternations of cellular active (up-) and inactive (down-) states of neuronal activity (Steriade et al., 2001; Nir et al., 2011). Although not perfectly coherent, these oscillations represent the most synchronous event in the healthy brain, and traveling waves across large cortical territories may mediate diverse sleep functions including downregulation of synaptic strengths (Vyazovskiy et al., 2008; Norimoto et al., 2018), maintenance of cellular homeostasis (Tononi and Cirelli, 2014), and mediation of memory consolidation and synaptic plasticity (Diekelmann and Born, 2010).

Slow waves are thought to provide a temporal frame for a dialogue between the neocortex and subcortical structures, which is necessary for redistributing memories for long-term storage (Sirota et al., 2003; Sirota and Buzsáki, 2005; Marshall and Born, 2007): On a global scale, a strong increase in EEG coherence is observed during NREM sleep following learning in humans (Mölle et al., 2004, 2009). On a local scale, changes in sleep oscillations occur in specific cortical regions that were involved in encoding, both in rodents (Vyazovskiy et al., 2000; Hanlon et al., 2009) and in humans (Huber et al., 2004, 2006; Mölle et al., 2009). Although very commonly regarded as a global event occurring near-simultaneously across the cortex, cortical up-states are typically ignited locally in prefrontal cortex and spread to other cortical areas over tens to a few 100 ms (Massimini et al., 2004). Neural recordings in rodents were able to pinpoint the ignition source to layer 5 cells of cortex (Luczak et al., 2007; Chauvette et al., 2010; Beltramo et al., 2013). Intracranial recordings from epilepsy patients reveal that most slow waves, and the underlying active and inactive neuronal states, occur locally (Nir et al., 2011). This observation goes beyond potential confounds of epilepsy, since it is readily observed also in rodents and in cats (Chauvette et al., 2011; Vyazovskiy et al., 2011). Especially during late sleep, circumscribed slow waves are also detected via EEG recordings (Siclari et al., 2014; Bernardi et al., 2018).

Sleep spindles are classically defined as waxing-and-waning 10–16 Hz oscillations lasting 0.5–2 s (Gibbs and Gibbs, 1950). Sleep spindles are implicated in plasticity and trigger synaptic long-term potentiation via calcium transients that are believed to prime cortical networks for the long-term storage of memory representations (Timofeev et al., 2002; Rosanova and Ulrich, 2005; Ulrich, 2016; Niethard et al., 2018). On a global scale, increased spindle activity is observed during NREM sleep following learning of both declarative tasks and procedural motor skills (Gais et al., 2002; Eschenko et al., 2006; Fogel and Smith, 2006; Morin et al., 2008; Mölle et al., 2009). On a local scale, regional spindle activity correlates with offline improvement in consolidation of motor memories (Nishida and Walker, 2007). Importantly, despite the fact that spindles engage thalamo-cortical “loops,” they are also mostly a local phenomenon occurring in select circuits at a time (Rasch and Born, 2013). Even when observed near-simultaneously across regions, their precise timings varies across cortical locations (Nir et al., 2011; Muller et al., 2016). Accordingly, learning different types of memories changes the properties of spindles in different topographically-restricted regions (Bergmann et al., 2012; Cox et al., 2014).

Not only are slow waves and sleep spindles each related to memory consolidation separately, recent evidence suggests that their precise interaction may play a role. For example, many sleep spindles tend to be “nested” in the “up” phase of the slow oscillation as revealed by phase-amplitude coupling (PAC) analysis (Diekelmann and Born, 2010; Staresina et al., 2015). However, slow wave and spindle oscillations behave as traveling waves at a whole-brain scale [for an extensive review see (Muller et al., 2018)], which translates to a delay of up to hundreds of milliseconds between oscillation peaks across different cortical areas. Thus, the temporal relationship between sleep oscillations across cortical regions varies substantially. Locally, within each brain region, the coupling of sleep spindles to slow wave up-states occurs in a topographically restricted fashion (Cox et al., 2014) and local slow waves coordinate spindle activity at virtually every cortical site (Cox et al., 2018). In contrast, coupling between distant brain regions does not necessarily occur regularly. For example, while parietal spindles are coupled to parietal slow waves, they are not necessarily coupled with frontal slow waves (Figure 1). Along this line, the strength of slow wave-spindle coupling differs between global and local slow waves, as well as between cortical locations (Malerba et al., 2019), highlighting the complexity of cross-frequency coupling between sleep oscillations across different brain regions.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Local sleep oscillations and memory consolidation: theory vs. experimental findings. (Top) Theory suggests that the nesting of hippocampal ripples (green) to sleep spindle troughs (orange), which in turn are nested in slow wave up-phase (orange), is critical for memory consolidation during sleep. (Bottom) Experimental data indicates that timing of both slow waves and spindles (as well as spindle nesting phase; Andrillon et al., 2011) varies across cortical regions (purple and blue), such that the nesting of each ripple (green) inevitably corresponds to different cortical locations (dashed vertical lines). Thus, each hippocampal ripple occurring at a specific time is associated with hippocampal-cortical coupling in different circuits, likely supporting memory consolidation related to that circuit.

Interregional Coupling Between Hippocampus and Specific Cortical Regions During Sleep, and Its Role in Successful Memory Consolidation

During NREM sleep, hippocampal (HC) activity is concentrated in sharp wave ripple (SWR) events, which correspond to a summed synchronous depolarization of a large fraction of the neurons in the CA1 sub region of the hippocampus (O'keefe and Nadel, 1978; Buzsáki et al., 1983; Buzsáki, 1986). Extensive animal research established a tight link between HC SWRs and memory consolidation in both wakefulness and sleep: SWRs accompany the sleep-associated re-activation of HC neuron ensembles that were active during the preceding awake learning experience (Nadasdy et al., 1999; Eschenko et al., 2008; Peyrache et al., 2009). SWRs occurrence increases in previously potentiated synaptic circuits (Behrens et al., 2005), and may further modulate synaptic strength (Buzsáki et al., 1987; King et al., 1999; Norimoto et al., 2018). Finally, selective manipulation of SWRs through electrical or optogenetic stimulation in HC modulates memory consolidation (Girardeau et al., 2009; Ego-Stengel and Wilson, 2010; Fernandez-Ruiz et al., 2019). Thus, SWRs represent important time epochs for offline HC activity, and their occurrence in NREM sleep carries a privileged role in plasticity and memory consolidation.

In deep layers of medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC), where most of the HC fibers make contacts, pyramidal cells respond phasically to SWRs (Siapas and Wilson, 1998; Mölle et al., 2006; Peyrache et al., 2011). Conversely, the occurrence of SWRs is modulated by neocortical inputs (Isomura et al., 2006), revealing bidirectional interactions between HC and cortex. Multiple studies revealed the fine temporal relationship between SWRs and neocortical sleep oscillations (Sirota et al., 2003; Sirota and Buzsáki, 2005; Staresina et al., 2015; Wang and Ikemoto, 2016), in which SWRs tend to be phase-locked to cortical spindle troughs, which in turn are phase-locked to slow wave up-states. Human studies are typically limited in SWR detection, as non-invasive EEG cannot reliably monitor local high-frequency activities in deep brain structures. Nevertheless, sleep studies in epilepsy patients implanted with intracranial electrodes support the notion that SWRs during sleep preferentially occur at specific times in relation to neocortical slow waves and spindles (Clemens et al., 2007, 2011; Nir et al., 2011; Staresina et al., 2015), extending the temporal tuning finding from rodents to human sleep. Given that spindles are mostly a local phenomenon, and their precise timing varies across cortical locations (Nir et al., 2011; Muller et al., 2016), temporal tuning between one cortical area and HC during a specific spindle does not necessarily imply temporal tuning between other cortical areas to HC at that time (Figure 1).

At present, a gap exists between theory on how hippocampal-cortical coupling supports memory consolidation (usually considering the entire cortex as a uniform entity) and the available experimental evidence highlighting that slow waves, spindles, and SWRs occur at different times in different regions.

Coupling of Sleep Oscillations in Select Brain Regions and the Consolidation of Specific Memories

A potential way to transcend this discrepancy is to consider that coupling between sleep oscillations may occur, but may involve select circuits at each given time—supporting memory consolidation in specific associated tasks. We illustrate this by considering two recent studies in rodents that causally link the coupling of sleep oscillations across specific regions to the consolidation of specific memories. A recent study (Maingret et al., 2016) established that co-occurrence of HC ripples and medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) slow waves and spindles correlates with memory consolidation in a spatial learning task. Boosting this coupling by delivering SWR-triggered electrical stimulation to deep cortical layers causally improved memory performance on this hippocampus-dependent task (Maingret et al., 2016). Another study used a different closed-loop stimulation protocol to improve memory performance in a hippocampal dependent task: frontal slow waves triggered optogenetic stimulation of the thalamic reticular nucleus during sleep, resulting in time-locked frontal sleep spindles, and HC SWRs (Latchoumane et al., 2017). Notably, these experiments, as well as studies selectively manipulating SWRs, report changes in coupling between SWRs in a specific hippocampal (HC) sub-field [mostly CA1 (Girardeau et al., 2009; Ego-Stengel and Wilson, 2010; Maingret et al., 2016)], and spindles in specific regions [either mPFC (Siapas and Wilson, 1998) or anterior cingulate cortex (Wang and Ikemoto, 2016)]. Thus, these findings demonstrate that although each SWR may be coupled with slow waves and spindle oscillations in different brain regions (Figure 1), HC-cortical coupling in select circuits may support memory consolidation in specific tasks.

Although the majority of sleep and memory experiments focus on temporal coupling between HC and cortex, several studies also demonstrate the importance of coherence between specific cortical regions. Miyamoto and colleagues demonstrated that coordinating slow wave activity between layer-5 primary somatosensory cortex and secondary motor cortex via synchronous optogenetic stimulation at 2 Hz enhances memory consolidation of a newly learned non-declarative skill. Asynchronous stimulation of these two regions (using opposite phases) reduced performance relative to the no-intervention controls (Miyamoto et al., 2016).

These experiments (Maingret et al., 2016; Miyamoto et al., 2016; Latchoumane et al., 2017) highlight the importance of both temporal and anatomical specificity of interventions designed to boost the coupling between sleep oscillations across two brain regions. Accordingly, a brief delay in stimulation timing was enough to abolish the memory enhancement that is observed when locking stimulation accurately to HC SWRs (Maingret et al., 2016).

How Can We Improve Causal Interventions in Humans Linking Sleep Oscillations to Learning and Memory?

Over the last decade, several studies have gone beyond demonstrating the existence of correlation between sleep oscillations (slow waves, spindles) and subsequent memory recall (e.g., Gais et al., 2002; Huber et al., 2004, 2006; Mölle et al., 2009; Fogel and Smith, 2011; Van Der Helm et al., 2011; Tamminen et al., 2013), to interventions that link an experimentally-induced increase in the amplitude of a sleep oscillation to human learning (Marshall et al., 2006; Ngo et al., 2013; Ladenbauer et al., 2017, but also see Bueno-Lopez et al., 2019). A recent study demonstrated that causal interventions affecting memory consolidation may also be applied locally. Unilateral olfactory stimulation induced “local targeted memory reactivation” and elicited both behavioral and EEG effects that were largely lateralized to one hemisphere (see preprint at - Bar et al., 2019). Such lateralization seems more difficult to demonstrate in the auditory modality (Simor et al., 2018), possibly because cortical auditory processing is less lateralized compared to vision and olfaction (Schnupp et al., 2011).

One line of causal interventions during sleep employs a temporally tuned approach, to perform “closed-loop” stimulation, phase-locked to endogenous sleep oscillations. For example, auditory stimulation in phase with slow wave up-states (as measured with scalp EEG) enhances slow wave activity and slow wave-spindle coupling, and improves the consolidation of declarative memory (Ngo et al., 2013; Lafon et al., 2017; Ketz et al., 2018; Goldi et al., 2019). Given that the timing of sleep oscillations differs across cortical regions, choosing a specific EEG channel to trigger stimulation, phase-locks the intervention to the timing of a specific cortical region. An elegant human study that took this into consideration shows degradation of learning efficiency following focal perturbation of slow wave activity over the motor cortex (Fattinger et al., 2017). Importantly, the perturbation was ineffective when targeting temporo-parietal cortex slow waves (Fattinger et al., 2017). Such an experimental approach draws our attention to the role of local sleep oscillations in specific cortical areas for consolidation of different types of memory tasks. The exact timing of intervention is critical for enhancing memory consolidation, and changing the stimulation phase may abolish memory effects completely (Ngo et al., 2013; Goldi et al., 2019).

Though impossible to directly compare, memory enhancement in humans appears to be modest and less pronounced compared to memory enhancement following interventions manipulating spindles and SWRs in rodents (Maingret et al., 2016; Latchoumane et al., 2017). We suggest that the precise timing of the intervention is critical for memory enhancement and may constitute an obstacle we need to overcome to obtain larger effects in human subjects. At present, human interventions typically rely on scalp EEG summating neuronal activity across wide regions, whereas animal studies track activity of specific neural populations in deep brain areas.

When studying coherence of EEG sleep oscillations between different cortical sites in humans, an important consideration is the tight and often underappreciated relation between (i) the amplitude of a sleep oscillation (e.g., slow wave or sleep spindle) as recorded with scalp EEG or intracranially, and (ii) its coherent occurrence across neuronal ensembles. Put simply, high-amplitude oscillations often reflect high synchronization between neuronal populations. Indeed we have shown, based on local iEEG recordings, that the amplitude of each slow wave recorded on the scalp is tightly correlated with the number of distant brain regions where this wave occurs near-simultaneously, such that high-amplitude slow waves are global (Nir et al., 2011). In the case of sleep spindles, high-amplitude events in scalp EEG likely reflect a precise coordination among neurons in cortex, thalamus, and reticular thalamic nucleus (Nunez, 1995). This means that many findings that link EEG slow wave or spindle amplitude/power in a given region to learning and memory may in fact imply stronger coherence within relevant neuronal circuits. Notwithstanding this, other factors also influence the amplitude of EEG sleep oscillations, as asynchronous local generators can also produce an unexpectedly large scalp signal (Von Ellenrieder et al., 2016). Further research is needed in order to separate the contribution of high oscillatory power vs. high coherence between specific areas to memory consolidation.

Future Outlooks

Technological advances should allow accurate mapping of the roles of specific spatially-circumscribed cortical sleep events in the consolidation of long term memory in humans, and separate them from other functions carried out by events that travel and encompass the whole cortex. We expect that maturation of novel electrophysiology tools will improve both spatial and temporal resolutions of monitoring human brain activity in real-time (Khodagholy et al., 2017; Liu et al., 2018), thereby allowing accurate experimental interventions in humans and improving their electrophysiological and cognitive effects. For such advances to make an impact on basic scientific understanding and create genuine clinical utility, it is imperative that theory is fine-tuned according to the available data, and that we go beyond considering the sleeping brain as a uniform coherent entity.

Author Contributions

All authors listed have made a substantial, direct and intellectual contribution to the work, and approved it for publication.

Funding

This study was supported by Sagol School of Neuroscience and The Naomi Foundation/GRTF Program at Tel Aviv University, The Rothschild Foundation (MG-S), Adelis Foundation, and the US-Israel Binational Science Foundation (BSF) grant # 2017628 (YN).

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We thank Anat Arzi and Aaron Krom for discussions and comments on the manuscript, and Sharon Tsach for graphics assistance.

References

Andrillon, T., Nir, Y., Staba, R. J., Ferrarelli, F., Cirelli, C., Tononi, G., et al. (2011). Sleep spindles in humans: insights from intracranial EEG and unit recordings. J. Neurosci. 31, 17821–17834. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2604-11.2011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bar, E., Arzi, A., Perl, O., Livne, E., Sobel, N., Dudai, Y., et al. (2019). Local targeted memory reactivation in human sleep. bioRxiv [Preprint]. doi: 10.1101/539114

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Behrens, C. J., Van Den Boom, L. P., De Hoz, L., Friedman, A., and Heinemann, U. (2005). Induction of sharp wave-ripple complexes in vitro and reorganization of hippocampal networks. Nat. Neurosci. 8, 1560–1567. doi: 10.1038/nn1571

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beltramo, R., D'urso, G., Dal Maschio, M., Farisello, P., Bovetti, S., Clovis, Y., et al. (2013). Layer-specific excitatory circuits differentially control recurrent network dynamics in the neocortex. Nat. Neurosci. 16, 227–234. doi: 10.1038/nn.3306

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bergmann, T. O., Mölle, M., Diedrichs, J., Born, J., and Siebner, H. R. (2012). Sleep spindle-related reactivation of category-specific cortical regions after learning face-scene associations. Neuroimage 59, 2733–2742. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.10.036

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bernardi, G., Siclari, F., Handjaras, G., Riedner, B. A., and Tononi, G. (2018). Local and widespread slow waves in stable NREM sleep: evidence for distinct regulation mechanisms. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 12:248. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2018.00248

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bueno-Lopez, A., Eggert, T., Dorn, H., and Danker-Hopfe, H. (2019). Slow oscillatory transcranial direct current stimulation (so-tDCS) during slow wave sleep has no effects on declarative memory in healthy young subjects. Brain Stimul. 12, 948–958. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2019.02.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buzsáki, G. (1986). Hippocampal sharp waves: their origin and significance. Brain Res. 398, 242–252. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(86)91483-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buzsáki, G. (1989). Two-stage model of memory trace formation: a role for “noisy” brain states. Neuroscience 31, 551–570. doi: 10.1016/0306-4522(89)90423-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buzsáki, G. (1996). The hippocampo-neocortical dialogue. Cereb. Cortex 6, 81–92. doi: 10.1093/cercor/6.2.81

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buzsáki, G. (1998). Memory consolidation during sleep: a neurophysiological perspective. J. Sleep Res. 7(Suppl. 1), 17–23. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2869.7.s1.3.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buzsáki, G., Haas, H. L., and Anderson, E. G. (1987). Long-term potentiation induced by physiologically relevant stimulus patterns. Brain Res. 435, 331–333. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(87)91618-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buzsáki, G., Leung, L. W., and Vanderwolf, C. H. (1983). Cellular bases of hippocampal EEG in the behaving rat. Brain Res. 287, 139–171. doi: 10.1016/0165-0173(83)90037-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chauvette, S., Crochet, S., Volgushev, M., and Timofeev, I. (2011). Properties of slow oscillation during slow-wave sleep and anesthesia in cats. J. Neurosci. 31, 14998–15008. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2339-11.2011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chauvette, S., Volgushev, M., and Timofeev, I. (2010). Origin of active states in local neocortical networks during slow sleep oscillation. Cereb. Cortex 20, 2660–2674. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhq009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clemens, Z., Mölle, M., Eross, L., Barsi, P., Halasz, P., and Born, J. (2007). Temporal coupling of parahippocampal ripples, sleep spindles and slow oscillations in humans. Brain 130, 2868–2878. doi: 10.1093/brain/awm146

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clemens, Z., Mölle, M., Eross, L., Jakus, R., Rasonyi, G., Halasz, P., et al. (2011). Fine-tuned coupling between human parahippocampal ripples and sleep spindles. Eur. J. Neurosci. 33, 511–520. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2010.07505.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cox, R., Hofman, W. F., De Boer, M., and Talamini, L. M. (2014). Local sleep spindle modulations in relation to specific memory cues. Neuroimage 99, 103–110. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.05.028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cox, R., Mylonas, D. S., Manoach, D. S., and Stickgold, R. (2018). Large-scale structure and individual fingerprints of locally coupled sleep oscillations. Sleep 41:zsy175. doi: 10.1093/sleep/zsy175

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Datta, S., and Maclean, R. R. (2007). Neurobiological mechanisms for the regulation of mammalian sleep-wake behavior: reinterpretation of historical evidence and inclusion of contemporary cellular and molecular evidence. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 31, 775–824. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2007.02.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diekelmann, S., and Born, J. (2010). The memory function of sleep. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 11, 114–126. doi: 10.1038/nrn2762

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dudai, Y., Karni, A., and Born, J. (2015). The consolidation and transformation of memory. Neuron 88, 20–32. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.09.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ego-Stengel, V., and Wilson, M. A. (2010). Disruption of ripple-associated hippocampal activity during rest impairs spatial learning in the rat. Hippocampus 20, 1–10. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20707

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eschenko, O., Mölle, M., Born, J., and Sara, S. J. (2006). Elevated sleep spindle density after learning or after retrieval in rats. J. Neurosci. 26, 12914–12920. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3175-06.2006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eschenko, O., Ramadan, W., Mölle, M., Born, J., and Sara, S. J. (2008). Sustained increase in hippocampal sharp-wave ripple activity during slow-wave sleep after learning. Learn. Mem. 15, 222–228. doi: 10.1101/lm.726008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fattinger, S., De Beukelaar, T. T., Ruddy, K. L., Volk, C., Heyse, N. C., Herbst, J. A., et al. (2017). Deep sleep maintains learning efficiency of the human brain. Nat. Commun. 8:15405. doi: 10.1038/ncomms15405

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fernandez-Ruiz, A., Oliva, A., Fermino De Oliveira, E., Rocha-Almeida, F., Tingley, D., and Buzsáki, G. (2019). Long-duration hippocampal sharp wave ripples improve memory. Science 364, 1082–1086. doi: 10.1126/science.aax0758

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fogel, S. M., and Smith, C. T. (2006). Learning-dependent changes in sleep spindles and Stage 2 sleep. J. Sleep Res. 15, 250–255. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2869.2006.00522.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fogel, S. M., and Smith, C. T. (2011). The function of the sleep spindle: a physiological index of intelligence and a mechanism for sleep-dependent memory consolidation. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 35, 1154–1165. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.12.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gais, S., Mölle, M., Helms, K., and Born, J. (2002). Learning-dependent increases in sleep spindle density. J. Neurosci. 22, 6830–6834. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-15-06830.2002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gibbs, F. A., and Gibbs, E. L. (1950). Atlas of Electroencephalography. Cambridge, MA: Addison-Wesley Press.

Google Scholar

Girardeau, G., Benchenane, K., Wiener, S. I., Buzsáki, G., and Zugaro, M. B. (2009). Selective suppression of hippocampal ripples impairs spatial memory. Nat. Neurosci. 12, 1222–1223. doi: 10.1038/nn.2384

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goldi, M., Van Poppel, E. A. M., Rasch, B., and Schreiner, T. (2019). Increased neuronal signatures of targeted memory reactivation during slow-wave up states. Sci. Rep. 9:2715. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-39178-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hanlon, E. C., Faraguna, U., Vyazovskiy, V. V., Tononi, G., and Cirelli, C. (2009). Effects of skilled training on sleep slow wave activity and cortical gene expression in the rat. Sleep 32, 719–729. doi: 10.1093/sleep/32.6.719

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huber, R., Ghilardi, M. F., Massimini, M., Ferrarelli, F., Riedner, B. A., Peterson, M. J., et al. (2006). Arm immobilization causes cortical plastic changes and locally decreases sleep slow wave activity. Nat. Neurosci. 9, 1169–1176. doi: 10.1038/nn1758

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huber, R., Ghilardi, M. F., Massimini, M., and Tononi, G. (2004). Local sleep and learning. Nature 430, 78–81. doi: 10.1038/nature02663

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Iber, C., Ancoli-Israel, S., Chesson, A. L., and Quan, S. F. (2007). AASM Manual for the Scoring of Sleep and Associate Events. Rules, Terminology and Technical Specifications. Westchester, IL: American Association of Sleep Medicine.

Google Scholar

Isomura, Y., Sirota, A., Ozen, S., Montgomery, S., Mizuseki, K., Henze, D. A., et al. (2006). Integration and segregation of activity in entorhinal-hippocampal subregions by neocortical slow oscillations. Neuron 52, 871–882. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.10.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ketz, N., Jones, A. P., Bryant, N. B., Clark, V. P., and Pilly, P. K. (2018). Closed-loop slow-wave tacs improves sleep-dependent long-term memory generalization by modulating endogenous oscillations. J. Neurosci. 38, 7314–7326. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0273-18.2018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Khodagholy, D., Gelinas, J. N., and Buzsáki, G. (2017). Learning-enhanced coupling between ripple oscillations in association cortices and hippocampus. Science 358, 369–372. doi: 10.1126/science.aan6203

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

King, C., Henze, D. A., Leinekugel, X., and Buzsáki, G. (1999). Hebbian modification of a hippocampal population pattern in the rat. J. Physiol. 521(Pt 1), 159–167. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7793.1999.00159.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ladenbauer, J., Ladenbauer, J., Kulzow, N., De Boor, R., Avramova, E., Grittner, U., et al. (2017). Promoting sleep oscillations and their functional coupling by transcranial stimulation enhances memory consolidation in mild cognitive impairment. J. Neurosci. 37, 7111–7124. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0260-17.2017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lafon, B., Henin, S., Huang, Y., Friedman, D., Melloni, L., Thesen, T., et al. (2017). Low frequency transcranial electrical stimulation does not entrain sleep rhythms measured by human intracranial recordings. Nat. Commun. 8:1199. doi: 10.1038/s41467-017-01045-x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Latchoumane, C. V., Ngo, H. V., Born, J., and Shin, H. S. (2017). Thalamic spindles promote memory formation during sleep through triple phase-locking of cortical, thalamic, and hippocampal rhythms. Neuron 95, 424–35 e426. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2017.06.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, X., Lu, Y., and Kuzum, D. (2018). High-density porous graphene arrays enable detection and analysis of propagating cortical waves and spirals. Sci. Rep. 8:17089. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-35613-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Luczak, A., Bartho, P., Marguet, S. L., Buzsáki, G., and Harris, K. D. (2007). Sequential structure of neocortical spontaneous activity in vivo. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 104, 347–352. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0605643104

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maingret, N., Girardeau, G., Todorova, R., Goutierre, M., and Zugaro, M. (2016). Hippocampo-cortical coupling mediates memory consolidation during sleep. Nat. Neurosci. 19, 959–964. doi: 10.1038/nn.4304

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Malerba, P., Whitehurst, L. N., Simons, S. B., and Mednick, S. C. (2019). Spatio-temporal structure of sleep slow oscillations on the electrode manifold and its relation to spindles. Sleep 42:zsy197. doi: 10.1093/sleep/zsy197

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maquet, P. (2001). The role of sleep in learning and memory. Science 294, 1048–1052. doi: 10.1126/science.1062856

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marshall, L., and Born, J. (2007). The contribution of sleep to hippocampus-dependent memory consolidation. Trends Cogn. Sci. 11, 442–450. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2007.09.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marshall, L., Helgadottir, H., Mölle, M., and Born, J. (2006). Boosting slow oscillations during sleep potentiates memory. Nature 444, 610–613. doi: 10.1038/nature05278

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Massimini, M., Huber, R., Ferrarelli, F., Hill, S., and Tononi, G. (2004). The sleep slow oscillation as a traveling wave. J. Neurosci. 24, 6862–6870. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1318-04.2004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miyamoto, D., Hirai, D., Fung, C. C., Inutsuka, A., Odagawa, M., Suzuki, T., et al. (2016). Top-down cortical input during NREM sleep consolidates perceptual memory. Science 352, 1315–1318. doi: 10.1126/science.aaf0902

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mölle, M., Eschenko, O., Gais, S., Sara, S. J., and Born, J. (2009). The influence of learning on sleep slow oscillations and associated spindles and ripples in humans and rats. Eur. J. Neurosci. 29, 1071–1081. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2009.06654.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mölle, M., Marshall, L., Gais, S., and Born, J. (2004). Learning increases human electroencephalographic coherence during subsequent slow sleep oscillations. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 101, 13963–13968. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0402820101

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mölle, M., Yeshenko, O., Marshall, L., Sara, S. J., and Born, J. (2006). Hippocampal sharp wave-ripples linked to slow oscillations in rat slow-wave sleep. J. Neurophysiol. 96, 62–70. doi: 10.1152/jn.00014.2006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morin, A., Doyon, J., Dostie, V., Barakat, M., Hadj Tahar, A., Korman, M., et al. (2008). Motor sequence learning increases sleep spindles and fast frequencies in post-training sleep. Sleep 31, 1149–1156.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Mormann, F., Kornblith, S., Quiroga, R. Q., Kraskov, A., Cerf, M., Fried, I., et al. (2008). Latency and selectivity of single neurons indicate hierarchical processing in the human medial temporal lobe. J. Neurosci. 28, 8865–8872. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1640-08.2008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Muller, L., Chavane, F., Reynolds, J., and Sejnowski, T. J. (2018). Cortical travelling waves: mechanisms and computational principles. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 19, 255–268. doi: 10.1038/nrn.2018.20

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Muller, L., Piantoni, G., Koller, D., Cash, S. S., Halgren, E., and Sejnowski, T. J. (2016). Rotating waves during human sleep spindles organize global patterns of activity that repeat precisely through the night. Elife 5:e17267. doi: 10.7554/eLife.17267

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nadasdy, Z., Hirase, H., Czurko, A., Csicsvari, J., and Buzsáki, G. (1999). Replay and time compression of recurring spike sequences in the hippocampus. J. Neurosci. 19, 9497–9507. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-21-09497.1999

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ngo, H. V., Martinetz, T., Born, J., and Mölle, M. (2013). Auditory closed-loop stimulation of the sleep slow oscillation enhances memory. Neuron 78, 545–553. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.03.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Niethard, N., Ngo, H. V., Ehrlich, I., and Born, J. (2018). Cortical circuit activity underlying sleep slow oscillations and spindles. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 115, E9220–E9229. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1805517115

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nir, Y., Staba, R. J., Andrillon, T., Vyazovskiy, V. V., Cirelli, C., Fried, I., et al. (2011). Regional slow waves and spindles in human sleep. Neuron 70, 153–169. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.02.043

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nishida, M., and Walker, M. P. (2007). Daytime naps, motor memory consolidation and regionally specific sleep spindles. PLoS ONE 2:e341. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0000341

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Norimoto, H., Makino, K., Gao, M., Shikano, Y., Okamoto, K., Ishikawa, T., et al. (2018). Hippocampal ripples down-regulate synapses. Science 359, 1524–1527. doi: 10.1126/science.aao0702

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nunez, P. L. (1995). Neocortical Dynamics and Human EEG Rhythms. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

O'keefe, J., and Nadel, L. (1978). The Hippocampus as a Cognitive Map. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Peyrache, A., Battaglia, F. P., and Destexhe, A. (2011). Inhibition recruitment in prefrontal cortex during sleep spindles and gating of hippocampal inputs. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 108, 17207–17212. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1103612108

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peyrache, A., Khamassi, M., Benchenane, K., Wiener, S. I., and Battaglia, F. P. (2009). Replay of rule-learning related neural patterns in the prefrontal cortex during sleep. Nat. Neurosci. 12, 919–926. doi: 10.1038/nn.2337

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rasch, B., and Born, J. (2013). About sleep's role in memory. Physiol. Rev. 93, 681–766. doi: 10.1152/physrev.00032.2012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rosanova, M., and Ulrich, D. (2005). Pattern-specific associative long-term potentiation induced by a sleep spindle-related spike train. J. Neurosci. 25, 9398–9405. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2149-05.2005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rothschild, G., Eban, E., and Frank, L. M. (2017). A cortical-hippocampal-cortical loop of information processing during memory consolidation. Nat. Neurosci. 20, 251–259. doi: 10.1038/nn.4457

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schnupp, J., Nelken, I., and King, A. (2011). Auditory Neuroscience : Making Sense of Sound. Cambridge: MIT Press. doi: 10.7551/mitpress/7942.001.0001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seibt, J., Richard, C. J., Sigl-Glockner, J., Takahashi, N., Kaplan, D. I., Doron, G., et al. (2017). Cortical dendritic activity correlates with spindle-rich oscillations during sleep in rodents. Nat. Commun. 8:684. doi: 10.1038/s41467-017-00735-w

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Siapas, A. G., and Wilson, M. A. (1998). Coordinated interactions between hippocampal ripples and cortical spindles during slow-wave sleep. Neuron 21, 1123–1128. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(00)80629-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Siclari, F., Bernardi, G., Riedner, B. A., Larocque, J. J., Benca, R. M., and Tononi, G. (2014). Two distinct synchronization processes in the transition to sleep: a high-density electroencephalographic study. Sleep 37, 1621–1637. doi: 10.5665/sleep.4070

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Simor, P., Steinbach, E., Nagy, T., Gilson, M., Farthouat, J., Schmitz, R., et al. (2018). Lateralized rhythmic acoustic stimulation during daytime NREM sleep enhances slow waves. Sleep 41:zsy176. doi: 10.1093/sleep/zsy176

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sirota, A., and Buzsáki, G. (2005). Interaction between neocortical and hippocampal networks via slow oscillations. Thalamus Relat. Syst. 3, 245–259. doi: 10.1017/S1472928807000258

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sirota, A., Csicsvari, J., Buhl, D., and Buzsáki, G. (2003). Communication between neocortex and hippocampus during sleep in rodents. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 100, 2065–2069. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0437938100

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Staresina, B. P., Bergmann, T. O., Bonnefond, M., Van Der Meij, R., Jensen, O., Deuker, L., et al. (2015). Hierarchical nesting of slow oscillations, spindles and ripples in the human hippocampus during sleep. Nat. Neurosci. 18, 1679–1686. doi: 10.1038/nn.4119

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steriade, M. (2003). Neuronal Substrates of Sleep and Epilepsy. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press. doi: 10.1017/CBO9780511541711

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steriade, M., Timofeev, I., and Grenier, F. (2001). Natural waking and sleep states: a view from inside neocortical neurons. J. Neurophysiol. 85, 1969–1985. doi: 10.1152/jn.2001.85.5.1969

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tamminen, J., Lambon Ralph, M. A., and Lewis, P. A. (2013). The role of sleep spindles and slow-wave activity in integrating new information in semantic memory. J. Neurosci. 33, 15376–15381. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5093-12.2013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Timofeev, I., Grenier, F., Bazhenov, M., Houweling, A. R., Sejnowski, T. J., et al. (2002). Short- and medium-term plasticity associated with augmenting responses in cortical slabs and spindles in intact cortex of cats in vivo. J. Physiol. 542, 583–598. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2001.013479

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tononi, G., and Cirelli, C. (2014). Sleep and the price of plasticity: from synaptic and cellular homeostasis to memory consolidation and integration. Neuron 81, 12–34. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.12.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ulrich, D. (2016). Sleep spindles as facilitators of memory formation and learning. Neural Plast. 2016:1796715. doi: 10.1155/2016/1796715

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Der Helm, E., Gujar, N., Nishida, M., and Walker, M. P. (2011). Sleep-dependent facilitation of episodic memory details. PLoS ONE 6:e27421. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0027421

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Von Ellenrieder, N., Dan, J., Frauscher, B., and Gotman, J. (2016). Sparse asynchronous cortical generators can produce measurable scalp EEG signals. Neuroimage 138, 123–133. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.05.067

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vyazovskiy, V., Borbely, A. A., and Tobler, I. (2000). Unilateral vibrissae stimulation during waking induces interhemispheric EEG asymmetry during subsequent sleep in the rat. J. Sleep Res. 9, 367–371. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2869.2000.00230.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vyazovskiy, V. V., Cirelli, C., Pfister-Genskow, M., Faraguna, U., and Tononi, G. (2008). Molecular and electrophysiological evidence for net synaptic potentiation in wake and depression in sleep. Nat. Neurosci. 11, 200–208. doi: 10.1038/nn2035

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vyazovskiy, V. V., Olcese, U., Hanlon, E. C., Nir, Y., Cirelli, C., and Tononi, G. (2011). Local sleep in awake rats. Nature 472, 443–447. doi: 10.1038/nature10009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wagner, T., Axmacher, N., Lehnertz, K., Elger, C. E., and Fell, J. (2010). Sleep-dependent directional coupling between human neocortex and hippocampus. Cortex 46, 256–263. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2009.05.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Walker, M. P., and Stickgold, R. (2004). Sleep-dependent learning and memory consolidation. Neuron 44, 121–133. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2004.08.031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, D. V., and Ikemoto, S. (2016). Coordinated interaction between hippocampal sharp-wave ripples and anterior cingulate unit activity. J. Neurosci. 36, 10663–10672. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1042-16.2016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: sleep, memory, spindles, ripples, coherence, regional

Citation: Geva-Sagiv M and Nir Y (2019) Local Sleep Oscillations: Implications for Memory Consolidation. Front. Neurosci. 13:813. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2019.00813

Received: 29 May 2019; Accepted: 22 July 2019;
Published: 20 August 2019.

Edited by:

Giulio Bernardi, IMT School for Advanced Studies Lucca, Italy

Reviewed by:

Roy Cox, University of Bonn, Germany
Nicola Cellini, University of Padova, Italy

Copyright © 2019 Geva-Sagiv and Nir. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Maya Geva-Sagiv, bWF5YWdldmFzYWdpdiYjeDAwMDQwO2dtYWlsLmNvbQ==; Yuval Nir, eXV2YWxuaXIudGF1JiN4MDAwNDA7Z21haWwuY29t

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.