- Computational Medicine Laboratory, Department of Electrical and Computer Engineering, University of Houston, Houston, TX, United States
Markers from local field potentials, neurochemicals, skin conductance, and hormone concentrations have been proposed as a means of closing the loop in Deep Brain Stimulation (DBS) therapy for treating neuropsychiatric and movement disorders. Developing a closed-loop DBS controller based on peripheral signals would require: (i) the recovery of a biomarker from the source neural stimuli underlying the peripheral signal variations; (ii) the estimation of an unobserved brain or central nervous system related state variable from the biomarker. The state variable is application-specific. It is emotion-related in the case of depression or post-traumatic stress disorder, and movement-related for Parkinson's or essential tremor. We present a method for closing the DBS loop in neuropsychiatric disorders based on the estimation of sympathetic arousal from skin conductance measurements. We deconvolve skin conductance via an optimization formulation utilizing sparse recovery and obtain neural impulses from sympathetic nerve fibers stimulating the sweat glands. We perform this deconvolution via a two-step coordinate descent procedure that recovers the sparse neural stimuli and estimates physiological system parameters simultaneously. We next relate an unobserved sympathetic arousal state to the probability that these neural impulses occur and use Bayesian filtering within an Expectation-Maximization framework for estimation. We evaluate our method on a publicly available data-set examining the effect of different types of stress on peripheral signal changes including body temperature, skin conductance and heart rate. A high degree of arousal is estimated during cognitive tasks, as are much lower levels during relaxation. The results demonstrate the ability to decode psychological arousal from neural activity underlying skin conductance signal variations. The complete pipeline from recovering neural stimuli to decoding an emotion-related brain state using skin conductance presents a promising methodology for the ultimate realization of a closed-loop DBS controller. Closed-loop DBS treatment would additionally help reduce unnecessary power consumption and improve therapeutic gains.
Introduction
Deep Brain Stimulation (DBS) is a type of therapy involving the application of high frequency electrical stimulation, usually at ~130 Hz, to specific anatomical structures deep within the brain (Oluigbo et al., 2012; Carron et al., 2013). While the precise mechanics of the therapy are yet to be fully understood, it has been hypothesized that DBS mimics the effect of ablative lesions without causing any tissue damage (Dostrovsky and Lozano, 2002). A second hypothesis suggests that stimulation from the implanted electrodes modulates electrical circuit activity within dysfunctional brain regions (Oluigbo et al., 2012; Cleary et al., 2015). DBS has been approved by the Food and Drug Administration (FDA) for the treatment of Parkinson's disease and essential tremor in the United States. Humanitarian device exemptions have also been granted by the FDA for the use of DBS in the treatment of severe obsessive compulsive disorder and dystonia (Grahn et al., 2014). Meanwhile, the therapy has also been investigated as a treatment option for a host of other medical conditions including major depression (Puigdemont et al., 2012; Merkl et al., 2013), chronic pain (Boccard et al., 2015; Lempka et al., 2017), drug-resistant epilepsy (Vesper et al., 2007; Fisher et al., 2010), anorexia nervosa (Lipsman et al., 2013; Wu et al., 2013), and substance abuse (Zhou et al., 2011; Müller et al., 2013).
Commercially-available DBS systems currently function in an open loop manner. In open-loop DBS, stimulation is delivered continuously until manually re-adjusted. In contrast, a closed-loop DBS (CLDBS) system automatically adjusts stimulation parameters based on sensor feedback recorded from the patient (Herron et al., 2017). The feedback signal is usually based on a symptom-related biomarker (Bouthour et al., 2019). Open-loop systems can require multiple post-operative visits in the months following surgery (Grahn et al., 2014). During visits, different parameters of the electrical stimulation including frequency, amplitude, and pulse width are adjusted for improving therapeutic benefit (Bronstein et al., 2011). Manual adjustment of the parameters in a trial-and-error fashion is time consuming. It is also challenging to explore the complete stimulation parameter space during brief patient visits. Moreover, open-loop DBS systems apply stimulation even if not strictly required. Consider, for instance, two common movement disorders—essential tremor and Parkinson's disease. Motor symptoms for both disorders include rhythmic involuntary movements (tremors). In essential tremor, the tremors occur during volitional movement (Plumb and Bain, 2007) and stimulation may be unnecessary when a patient is not using an affected limb (Herron et al., 2017). The tremors occur at rest in Parkinson's (Chou et al., 2011). However, motor symptoms can fluctuate continually (Rosin et al., 2011; Little et al., 2013). Evidence suggests that local field potential (LFP) β-band oscillations in the subthalamic nucleus correlate with motor impairment in Parkinson's (Little and Brown, 2012). CLDBS systems switching on control based on LFP threshold crossings were shown to have superior performance in treating Parkinson's patients and had substantial gains in reducing stimulation time (Little et al., 2013, 2016). The effectiveness of CLDBS over an open-loop stimulation in Parskinson's was also shown in non-human primates (Rosin et al., 2011). CLDBS systems have thus arisen gradually to eliminate part of the inefficiencies of their open-loop predecessors.
In a recent work describing a theoretical framework for the design of a CLDBS system for treating chronic pain, Shirvalkar et al. (2018) point out two important elements of closing the loop: (i) the extraction of an accurate, relevant, and timely biomarker of the underlying state variable of interest; (ii) a control-theoretic (e.g., state-space) representation of the system relating the biomarker to the unobserved state variable. In their specific application, they suggest using LFPs from the somatosensory cortex, the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex and the orbitofrontal cortex for tracking a multidimensional pain state. Others have similarly suggested neurochemical biomarkers, skin conductance features, and hormone concentrations as a means of feedback for treating a broad range of neuropsychiatric disorders (Grahn et al., 2014; Bina and Langevin, 2018). Following the suggestion of Shirvalkar et al. (2018), we present a proof-of-principle state-space framework that can be used for CLDBS therapy.
DBS has recently emerged as a potentially successful treatment option for patients diagnosed with post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) (Koek et al., 2014; Langevin et al., 2016). PTSD is a type of psychiatric disorder that can occur in patients who have experienced traumatic or stressful events in the past. Distressing memories or dreams often persist long after the event (Jetly et al., 2015). Symptoms of PTSD include changes in psychological arousal, reactivity and mood, and are evidenced by factors such as hypervigilance and exaggerated startle responses (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). This state of hyperarousal or hypervigilance in PTSD has been noted in multiple studies (Woodward et al., 2000; Risser et al., 2006; Hellmuth et al., 2012). While the method we present here could find broader applicability to a range of neuropsychiatric disorders, it seems particularly suited to address PTSD with its hyperarousal symptoms.
Bina and Langevin (2018) suggest the possibility of monitoring skin conductance changes as a potential biomarker in a CLDBS system for treating PTSD. Sympathetic nerve fibers innervate the sweat glands (Low, 2012). Consequently, changes in the conductivity of the skin owing to perspiration provide a measure of sympathetic drive or arousal (Critchley et al., 2002). Heightened responsivity in terms of skin conductance has been noted in PTSD patients compared to controls (Orr and Roth, 2000). In a three-group study of Vietnam combat veterans with PTSD, psychiatric Vietnam combat veterans, and psychiatric non-combat Vietnam-era veterans, McNally et al. (1987) reported that PTSD subjects had the largest skin conductance responses (SCRs) in response to combat-related words. In a similar study comprising of Vietnam combat veterans with PTSD, Vietnam combat veterans, and non-combat controls, Goldfinger et al. (1998) reported that PTSD veterans had the highest baseline skin conductance levels. In an affect-toned Rorschach test conducted in the same study, arousal, as measured by skin conductance, was highest in the PTSD group as well. Pole (2007) also noted higher skin conductance baselines, larger SCRs and slower skin conductance habituation in startle and trauma-cue studies in PTSD patients in a meta-analysis study of adults with and without PTSD. Skin conductance changes also occur in depression. Ward and Doerr (1986) measured skin conductance in patients with depression, parents of firstborn one- to three-month old infants and control subjects, and found that depressed patients had significantly lower skin conductance levels than the other groups. Lin et al. (2011) examined the effects of stress and depression using a series of physiological measures. Participants were first categorized into the normal, low-risk and high-risk depression groups and were assigned to one of two stress treatments. Percentage change in skin conductance between baseline and during the stress treatment periods were significantly dependent on and correlated positively with depression. Both PTSD and depression are potential candidates for DBS therapy when other treatment options have been exhausted. Skin conductance additionally has the advantage of being easily measured with wearable devices such as the Empatica E4 (Koskimäki et al., 2017). Wearable devices afford convenience, seamless integration into clothing and do not involve the risks of surgically implanted sensors.
We develop a state-space model to track an unobserved sympathetic arousal state from skin conductance measurements. The relationship between arousal and skin conductance has been attested to in multiple studies (Boucsein, 2012). Individual SCRs are a notable feature in a skin conductance signal. SCRs accompany psychologically arousing stimuli as the skin's conductivity increases momentarily. We relate arousal to the rate at which SCRs occur. Current methods for detecting SCRs in a skin conductance signal rely on detecting peaks above a threshold set between 0.01 and 0.05 μS (Benedek and Kaernbach, 2010). Inter-subject variability in skin conductance signals is a known phenomena (Dawson et al., 2007). We therefore use a deconvolution strategy for extracting the physiological parameters related to sweat secretion for each individual and detect neural impulses to the eccrine sweat glands that generate the SCRs rather than relying on heuristic peak detection. The following section describes our two-part methodology. We first describe the deconvolution approach that utilizes its own state-space formulation for detecting neural impulses based on sweat diffusion and evaporation dynamics. We next describe the state-space formulation for the CLDBS system that relates the probability of neural impulses to a latent sympathetic arousal state. We present our results thereafter and finally conclude with a discussion of our results, and how our methodology could be used in an experimental CLDBS prototype (e.g., such as in the conceptual architecture depicted in Figure 1).
Figure 1. A CLDBS System based on Skin Conductance Measurements. A conceptual CLDBS architecture for treating neuropsychiatric disorders based on tracking a neural state from peripheral skin conductance measurements. Koek et al. (2014) and Langevin et al. (2016) applied stimulation to the amygdala in treatment-refractory PTSD patients. The amygdala plays an important role in emotion processing. In particular, the basolateral nucleus of the amygdala contains cells that are responsive to both fear acquisition and fear extinction (Lüthi and Lüscher, 2014). Koek et al. (2014) and Langevin et al. (2016) targeted this area owing to the partially dysfunctional fear extinction mechanism for trauma-related cues in PTSD patients.
1. Materials and Methods
1.1. Data
We use the Non-EEG Dataset for Assessment of Neurological Status (Birjandtalab et al., 2016). The data is publicly available through the PhysioNet database (Goldberger et al., 2000). The data-set contains skin conductance recordings from 20 healthy college students who were exposed to physical, emotional, and cognitive stress during three different time periods. Skin conductance was recorded using the wrist-worn Affectiva Q Curve device. Skin conductance can be contaminated by noise sources such as motion artifacts, range saturation and amplification factor changes (Boucsein, 2012). Many of the signals had to be discarded owing to motion artifact contamination and noise due to bad skin contact. Hence, we only used the data from six subjects. We re-labeled the original subject numbers with corresponding participant numbers (Table 1). The physical, cognitive, and emotional stress periods each lasted 5 min and were interspersed by 5 min intervals of relaxation. Subjects were made to stand, walk and jog during the physical stress part of the experiment. We excluded data from this portion of the experiment and focus only on the psychological aspects. The cognitive stress portion consisted of two separate tasks. In the first task, subjects had to count backwards in 7's beginning at 2,485 for 3 min and then perform the Stroop test for a further 2 min. In a Stroop test, a subject is shown a word denoting a color and is asked to read it out. However, the color in which the text is written may not necessarily correspond to what it means. A buzzer notified subjects of any errors they made. Emotional stress was induced by means of a horror movie clip. The authors of the data-set noted that many of the volunteers participating in the experiment showed a stress response that was visible to the experiment administrator while they were just being given instructions regarding the cognitive tasks. Hence, they categorized the 40 s interval just prior to the counting task and the Stroop test as a stress period.
1.2. Skin Conductance Deconvolution Using Compressed Sensing
1.2.1. Skin Conductance Model Formulation
A skin conductance signal ySC(t) consists of two distinct parts. The comparatively slow varying part, also known as the tonic level, is primarily related to thermoregulation and is a function of ambient temperature and humidity. The other part, also known as the phasic component, fluctuates much faster and is generated by sympathetic nerve fibers stimulating the sweat glands. Therefore,
where y(t) and yT(t) represent the constituent phasic and tonic components, respectively.
The phasic component y(t) can be extracted from ySC(t) using an algorithm such as cvxEDA (Greco et al., 2016). The physiology leading to the generation of the phasic component—namely the diffusion of sweat from the sweat duct to the stratum corneum, and its subsequent evaporation thereafter—can be modeled using first order dynamics (Alexander et al., 2005; Benedek and Kaernbach, 2010; Boucsein, 2012), and mathematically expressed via the following pair of differential equations:
where x1(t) is an internal variable, x2(t) is the phasic component, and u(t) is the neural stimuli to the sweat glands. x1(t) is related to the amount of sweat and pressure within the sweat duct. The phasic component consists of a series of SCRs, each of which results from a single neural impulse burst. τr and τd are the rise and decay times of a single SCR.
The number of SCRs in a phasic skin conductance signal is typically much smaller than the total number of acquired data samples. Consequently, the number of underlying neural impulse bursts causing the SCRs is also small. This enables us to employ a sparsity constraint when solving for u(t). We model u(t) as a finite sum of weighted, shifted delta functions
where ui represents the amplitude of an impulse occurring at Δi, and N is the number of samples in the neural stimuli signal. N is proportional to the recording duration Td and the input sampling frequency fu (N = Td · fu). Δi = iTu where . ui is positive if there is an impulse at time instance Δi and 0 otherwise. The continuous-time phasic skin conductance y(t) contaminated by measurement noise ν(t) is
If the signal is periodically sampled at Ty intervals to yield a total of M measurements, we can define the equivalent discrete-time observation yk as
where νk is Gaussian noise. Given all the discrete measurements yk for k = 1, 2, …, M, we would like to recover u(t) and estimate τr and τd. We take x1(0) = 0 as an initial condition assuming that the sweat duct is empty at the beginning. The state-space solution for x2(kTy) leads us to (Faghih et al., 2015b)
where and represents a sparse vector containing all the neural stimuli over the entire signal duration (i.e., very few of the ui's are non-zero). Concatenating all the measurements into a single vector we have,
where , , and y0 is the initial condition of the phasic skin conductance signal. Here, Ty is an integer multiple of Tu.
1.2.2. Deconvolution
We set the sampling interval for the phasic skin conductance signal and neural stimuli to Ty = 0.5 s and Tu = 0.25 s, respectively. Equation (8) has M < N and represents an ill-posed problem with multiple solutions. The sparsity constraint on u however, makes it possible to solve the equation via compressed sensing. An l1-norm penalization term is typically added to the objective function to impose sparsity (Faghih, 2018). We consider lp-norm penalization in this particular formulation. We further constrain the rise and decay times to 0.1 ≤ τr ≤ 1.4 and 1.5 ≤ τd ≤ 6 similar to Amin and Faghih (2018, 2019). We impose these constraints based on prior work in the literature to ensure that the solution is identifiable and physiologically plausible (Alexander et al., 2005; Benedek and Kaernbach, 2010; Greco et al., 2016). Letting , we formulate the following constrained optimization problem based on Equation (8) to estimate τ and u
where , b = [−0.1 1.4 −1.5 6]⊤and λ is the lp-norm regularization parameter for imposing sparsity on u. λ is chosen to provide a balance between exploiting sparsity and accounting for signal fluctuations (Faghih, 2018). This optimization problem is challenging. We therefore decouple it into two sub-problems. A coordinate descent approach can be formulated similar to Faghih (2014, 2018), Faghih et al. (2014, 2015a,b) by solving the following sub-problems iteratively (for l = 0, 1, 2, ··· ) until convergence:
The first step represents a sparse recovery problem with a constrained convex optimization formulation. Many different approaches exist to solve this. One of the popular approaches is the iterative re-weighted least squares (IRLS) method. We solve this sparse recovery problem using two IRLS methods called FOCUSS+ (Murray, 2005) and GCV-FOCUSS+ (Zdunek and Cichocki, 2008). FOCUSS+ uses a heuristic approach for increasing λ at each IRLS step. We use FOCUSS+ for obtaining a suitable initialization of u. GCV-FOCUSS+ uses the Generalized Cross-Validation (GCV) technique to update λ at each step (Golub et al., 1979). We initialize GCV-FOCUSS+ with the result from FOCUSS+ and then run the IRLS until convergence. We finally constrain the minimum amplitude of any detected neural impulse to be 0.01 to reduce noisy detections. The second step in the coordinate descent approach represents a system identification problem with a constrained non-convex optimization formulation. We use the interior point method to solve this step.
The overall deconvolution algorithm begins by extracting the phasic skin conductance component using cvxEDA (Greco et al., 2016), then randomly initializing τ and performing the initialization step for u using FOCUSS+. Thereafter, we proceed with coordinate descent using GCV-FOCUSS+ and the interior point method. We perform the deconvolution on a small 3 min segment (taken from close to the opening portion of the experimental data we consider) of the signal to obtain the rise and decay times. Once we obtain these parameters, we use them to perform sparse recovery with GCV-FOCUSS+ on the entire skin conductance signal.
1.3. Sympathetic Arousal State Estimation
The autonomic nervous system contains both a sympathetic and a parasympathetic branch. The sympathetic branch mediates the body's “fight or flight" response and causes increases in blood pressure, perspiration and heart rate (Silverthorn, 2009). As pointed out earlier, sympathetic nerve fibers innervate the sweat glands (Low, 2012) and consequently skin conductance provides an index of sympathetic arousal (Critchley et al., 2002). Multiple skin conductance features such as tonic levels, rates of SCR appearance, SCR amplitudes and decay rates have been examined in the context of various behavioral interventions (Dawson et al., 2007; Boucsein, 2012). The rate at which SCRs occur has been shown to be related to cognitive task load (Jennings, 1986; Munro et al., 1987) and is thus a useful biomarker of autonomic arousal (Aikins et al., 2009). Here, we describe our approach of estimating an unobserved sympathetic arousal state based on the appearance of neural impulses underlying SCR generation.
We develop a state-space model relating arousal to the probability that the neural impulses occur (Wickramasuriya et al., 2018). The model is inspired by an earlier work relating a sequence of binary response variables to a latent cognitive learning state (Smith et al., 2004). We first divide the time-axis into bins of Tu duration indexed over j and assign sj = 1 or sj = 0 based on whether or not a neural impulse occurs at the jth time instance. Similar to Smith et al. (2004), we assume that sympathetic arousal zj follows a random walk with time,
The appearance of neural impulses sj is a Bernoulli distributed random variable with probability pj and is taken to be related to zj via a sigmoid function (Smith et al., 2004),
The choice of the sigmoid function follows from the theory of generalized linear models (McCullagh and Nelder, 1989). Such logarithmic or exponential transformations are frequently encountered in count or frequency type data. Assuming that a subject's sympathetic arousal state z0 ≈ 0 at the outset of the experiment, can be calculated by taking p0 as the probability that a neural impulse occurs randomly in a time bin for each individual (Smith et al., 2004; Wickramasuriya et al., 2018).
Given the observations S1 : J = {s1, s2, …, sJ} we wish to estimate zj ∀ j. We use Bayesian filtering and Expectation-Maximization (EM) for estimating the arousal states zj and recovering the unknown model parameters z0 and . The algorithm iterates between the E-step and M-step until convergence.
1.3.1. Expectation Step
The E-step consists of two parts—a forward filter and a backward smoother. The filter first calculates a state estimate zj|j using the observations S1 : j available up to the jth time index. The backward smoother determines a second estimate zj|J given all the available observations S1 : J. A Gaussian approximation is made at the filter formulation step and leads to the following equations at the lth EM iteration (Smith et al., 2004):
Predict:
Update:
One should note that zj|j appears on both sides of Equation (14) and therefore is numerically solved using Newton's method. We next obtain the smoothed state and variance estimates zj|J and as follows (Mendel, 1995):
1.3.2. Maximization Step
We maximize the complete data likelihood at the M-step to estimate the two unknown model parameters and z0. The parameter updates for the (l + 1)th iteration are as follows (Smith et al., 2004):
Following a criteria similar to Smith et al. (2004), we take the parameters to have converged once their absolute difference between consecutive iterations does not exceed 10−8.
1.3.3. High Arousal Index
Similar to Smith et al. (2004), we calculate the probability that sympathetic arousal state zj exceeds a specific threshold. We name this the High Arousal Index (HAI). HAI helps express how aroused a person is above a certain baseline. After the EM algorithm has converged, the state zj at each time instance is taken to be Gaussian distributed and we define HAI as follows:
where the threshold zT is set to each subject's median state value across the whole experiment. Recall that the experiment acquired data from subjects during episodes of both stress and relaxation. The high stress induced during the experiment corresponds to a state of high arousal and the relaxation corresponds to low arousal. Therefore, we selected zT as the median value as an approximation of normal arousal in between the two extremes.
2. Results
2.1. Skin Conductance Deconvolution
Figure 2 shows the skin conductance signals and deconvolution results for the selected participants during the backward counting task. In each sub-figure, the upper sub-panel shows the separation of the tonic and phasic components using cvxEDA (Greco et al., 2016). The lower sub-panel in each sub-figure shows the corresponding neural stimuli recovered using our deconvolution approach along with the reconstructed signal. It is the timings of these neural impulses that are used for estimating sympathetic arousal. Our method detects all significant impulses though it misses a few small ones that are comparable to noise. The number of detected impulses and the estimated SCR rise and decay times τr and τd are given in Table 2. Recall that these numbers are calculated based on data acquired during the backward counting task of the experiment. The τr and τd values estimated from this portion of the experiment are finally used to solve for the neural impulses over the entire signal. Also given in Table 2 are the squared multiple correlation coefficients R2 for the participants. R2 is an indication of goodness-of-fit and expresses how much of the variance of the data is captured by the model. R2 is above 0.93 for everyone indicating a good fit to the data. The number of impulses varies considerably from person to person. This is likely due to the fact that each participant responds to stress uniquely, despite being exposed to the same type of external stressor.
Figure 2. Estimated deconvolution of the experimental skin conductance data. For each of the participants, (i) the upper sub-panel depicts the raw skin conductance data (blue curve) and the separated tonic component of the skin conductance data using cvxEDA (green curve); (ii) the lower sub-panel depicts the separated phasic component (blue stars), the estimated reconstructed signal (red curve), the estimated neural stimuli timings and amplitudes (black vertical lines).
To further validate our deconvolution approach, we generated a second set of synthetic data using the τ and u already estimated for each participant. We added 25 dB SNR Gaussian noise to corrupt this new simulated phasic skin conductance data and performed deconvolution yet again. Figure 3 shows the results along with the ground truth. Table 3 shows the estimated parameters and their errors. Again, all R2 values are above 0.98 indicating a very good fit to the data.
Figure 3. Estimated deconvolution of the simulated phasic skin conductance data in six participants. For each of the participants, (i) the upper sub-panel depicts the simulated phasic component of the skin conductance data samples with 25 dB SNR Gaussian noise (blue stars), the estimated reconstructed signal (red curve); (ii) the lower sub-panel depicts the estimated neural stimuli timings and amplitudes (black vertical lines) and the ground truth of the neural stimuli (red vertical lines) for each of the participants.
2.2. Sympathetic Arousal State Estimation
Figure 4 shows the sympathetic arousal state estimation results. For participant 1, arousal as measured by HAI remains above 90% during the cognitive tasks and reduces significantly during relaxation. HAI then increases around the start of emotional stress. Participant 2 has a similar response although the increase in arousal at the start of emotional stress is much less. There is also a notable, though not significantly high, increase right in the middle of the relaxation period. The arousal profile for participant 3 is almost identical to that of participant 1 with a high level at the start, a significant drop during relaxation and a moderate increase at the starting point of emotional stress.
Figure 4. Sympathetic arousal state estimation. For each of the participants, (i) the top sub-panel depicts the skin conductance signal; (ii) the 2nd sub-panel depicts the recovered neural impulses; (iii) the 3rd sub-panel depicts the smoothed sympathetic arousal state zj|J and its confidence intervals; (iv) the 4th sub-panel depicts the smoothed impulse occurrence probability pj|J and its confidence intervals; (v) the lower sub-panel depicts the high arousal index (HAI) with the region above 90% probability highlighted in red. The color-coded backgrounds correspond to the instruction period for the cognitive tasks (red), the backward counting task (green), the Stroop test (cyan) (both the counting task and the Stroop test make up the cognitive stress portion), relaxation (light brown) and emotional stress (violet). Small green rectangles above 3rd and 4th sub-panels depict neural impulse location timings.
The HAIs are somewhat different for the remaining participants. For participant 4, arousal increases up to or above the 90% threshold a few times during the cognitive tasks, but does not remain high continuously. There is a significant drop during relaxation and an increase above 90% when the horror movie begins. HAI for participant 5 remains high during the cognitive tasks and then drops during relaxation. There are several notable increases during emotional stress, though none of them increase above the 90% threshold. None of the increases however, exceed 90% in this period. Participant 6 is closest to participant 4 although HAI remains more consistently above 90% during cognitive stress with only a slight drop in the middle. Arousal thereafter drops during relaxation and increases above 90% for a brief period at the start of emotional stress. The estimated arousal states zj|J also follow the general trend of the corresponding HAIs for all participants.
3. Discussion
3.1. Skin Conductance Deconvolution
Our method successfully recovers neural impulses associated with phasic SCRs. Between-subject variability in the estimated rise and decay times and the number of impulses is to be noted. The tonic levels also show considerable variations from person to person. These variations clearly highlight the need for determining the physiological parameters τr and τd on an individual basis rather than relying on fixed values for everyone.
Deconvolution using simulated data (Figure 3 and Table 3) also shows that most impulses are accurately recovered. Only impulses that are comparable to noise peaks are missed. The error percentages in estimating τr and τd are less than 10% for five of the participants. Participant 4 has a much higher percentage error. Recall that our problem formulation for estimating the rise and decay times is not convex. Consequently, there exists the possibility of stagnating at a local minimum. While we attempt to mitigate this problem through multiple initializations, and then taking the solution with the smallest squared error, there still exists a finite possibility of stagnating at a location other than the global minimum. The number of neural impulses for participant 4 is also much lower than for the others. Consequently, there are less SCRs to fit to and the result is more error-prone.
Each SCR, resulting from a single neural impulse, is mathematically modeled as a bi-exponential function. Estimating the rise and decay times of an SCR in the presence of noise is challenging due to this sensitive exponential nature. More than one pair of rise and decay times exist that can closely approximate an experimental SCR shape. Results are also heavily dependent on the removal of the tonic part. cvxEDA (Greco et al., 2016) models the tonic part with cubic B-spline basis functions with a 10 s knot size. Greco et al. (2016) used l2-norm penalization on the cubic spline basis function coefficients to avoid overfitting. They selected the regularization parameter to the penalization term based on prior data they had analyzed. This parameter depends on how the data is scaled in reality and how much of the tonic part it contains. Here, we set the regularization parameter related to the smoothness of the tonic component in cvxEDA at 0.001 instead of the 0.01 default to obtain a better separation of the tonic and phasic components.
Our current implementation of skin conductance deconvolution comprises of the two-step coordinate descent algorithm described earlier. Although it performs well in terms of accuracy, a faster implementation is necessary for a real-time CLDBS system. The physiological parameters τr and τd usually remain stable over a prolonged period of time. Therefore, we can perform only the sparse recovery procedure on windows of incoming data after an initial parameter estimation is complete, and thereafter estimate τr and τd in the background from time-to-time. A faster implementation of the sparse recovery step (e.g., using Greedy algorithms or Bayesian approaches) could further help improve time complexity.
3.2. Sympathetic Arousal State Estimation
A general trend is to be observed in the participant arousal levels in Figure 4. In the case of cognitive stress, the subjects' arousal states and HAI remain almost constantly high with the exception of participant 4. In contrast, there is only a moderate increase that dies down at the start of the emotional stress phase. The cognitive tasks required active engagement, i.e., mathematical calculations and active concentration, on the part of the participants. Meanwhile, the emotional stress period only involved passive engagement—the subjects just had to watch a horror movie clip without any significant cognitive effort. The varying level of cognitive activity is a likely reason for the difference in arousal between cognitive and emotional stress. Birjandtalab et al. (2016) used a clip from the horror movie entitled the “The Horde" to generate emotional stress. It is also possible that the movie was insufficient to generate significant emotional stress. The desired stress-generating effect may not have been realized if, for instance, a participant had already watched the movie. A visual inspection of the sub-panels depicting skin conductance in Figure 4 does not show a significant number of SCRs during emotional stress. It is likely PTSD patients may experience more emotional rather than cognitive stress. However, the emotions they experience may not necessarily be those evoked in healthy subjects for the very same stimuli. For instance, scenes of blood and dead bodies in a horror movie may evoke traumatic memories in PTSD patients leading to higher levels of stress. Further experimentation with a patient population would help validate our methods in detecting elevated levels of arousal in PTSD.
Sympathetic arousal information is not only encoded in how frequently neural impulses to the sweat glands occur, but also in the skin conductance signal amplitudes. Consequently, the amplitude of individual SCRs are taken as indicators of arousal (Bach et al., 2010). The tonic skin conductance level also contains emotion-related information (Braithwaite et al., 2013). Our current state-space formulation only considers the rate at which neural impulses (i.e., binary events) occur. Future work would incorporate the additional amplitude features for estimating sympathetic arousal using augmented state-space models that include both binary and continuous observations (Prerau et al., 2009; Coleman et al., 2011). The addition of heart rate could also help obtain an improved sympathetic arousal estimate (Wickramasuriya and Faghih, 2019).
The current EM approach is also offline and therefore requires modification if it is to be used in real-time in an experimental CLDBS prototype. We suggest running the forward filter in the E-step continuously and performing the full EM procedure in the background from time to time. This is very similar to the approach proposed for deconvolution when adapting to the needs of real-time computation. This may also permit the model parameters to change in adaption to disease progression and changing environmental conditions over time. The steps could also be run in parallel in a multicore processor. Several smartphones are now enabled with multicore processors and one option could be to perform the CLDBS computations on a wearer's phone. Another option would be to stream the data to the internet and perform computations in the cloud. Developing a custom hardware device to accompany the CLDBS implant is yet another option for performing skin conductance deconvolution and arousal state estimation in real-time.
3.3. Study Limitations
As noted earlier, many of the skin conductance recordings were contaminated with noise and had to be discarded. In-band motion artifacts such as those seen in the data-set usually contaminate a signal nonlinearly. Adaptive filtering (Mathews, 1991; Zaknich, 2005) and multi-level wavelet-based thresholding (Chen et al., 2015) are some of the options for suppressing motion artifacts. It is likely that skin conductance will indeed be contaminated with such artifacts in a real-world setting. We leave the development of an accelerometer-based adaptive filter for removing motion artifacts in skin conductance for future work. In another work, Amin and Faghih (2018) illustrated a way of performing concurrent deconvolution from multi-channel skin conductance data to obtain an estimate from noisy data. They included weights in different channels based on the standard deviation of noise in each channel while estimating the neural stimuli. They showed that the multi-channel approach could be more reliable for noisy skin conductance data. Multi-channel approach could potentially be used to achieve a more reliable CLDBS system. Moreover, factors other than sympathetic arousal can also influence skin conductance (e.g., body temperature, hydration, physical activity, and electrolytes). Variations induced by these factors may thus confound sympathetic arousal estimates; this is a limiting factor of using skin conductance alone. Such factors could be taken into account in a real-world setting and their effect canceled to obtain an improved arousal estimate. This would require an extended state-space model incorporating body temperature and hydration levels, for instance, as additional observations.
The present work is a proof-of-principle framework for using skin conductance in closing the DBS loop. As skin conductance relates to sympathetic arousal, and moreover as PTSD patients frequently show symptoms of hyperarousal, we note the suitability of using skin conductance as a CLDBS biomarker. The data-set used here however, does not include any PTSD patients and is a limitation of this study. Further investigation is therefore necessary to validate the use of skin conductance in an experimental CLDBS prototype for PTSD patients. Recall that differences in skin conductance have been reported in the literature between individuals with and without PTSD. The coefficients of the differential equations (τ) in Equations (2) and (3) governing sweat secretion dynamics are determined on a per subject basis. Thus, even if τ were significantly different between healthy and patient populations, our deconvolution methodology would still adapt to each individual. Furthermore, we only track a single skin conductance feature—the occurrence of neural impulses. The α coefficient in Equation (11) is calculated from the baseline probability of impulses on a per subject basis. The process noise variance in Equation (10) is also estimated for each subject individually via the EM algorithm. Therefore, our Bayesian filter for sympathetic arousal estimation is also able to adapt to each individual. Although the present study did not include data from a patient population, we would however expect our methods to generalize to them nevertheless due to the ability of the framework to adapt to each individual.
3.4. Effect of DBS on Skin Conductance
The neural substrates underlying skin conductance have been examined in studies involving functional imaging, brain lesions and direct electrical stimulation (Critchley, 2002). Mangina and Beuzeron-Mangina (1996) applied electrical stimulation to the limbic structures of a group of subjects with intractable epilepsy and measured bilateral skin conductance. When the left sides of the amygdala, posterior hippocampus, anterior hippocampus and cingulate gyrus were stimulated, higher SCR amplitudes on the left hand were observed compared to the right. The reverse was also true when the right sides of the same interior structures were stimulated. They also reported that stimulation intensity increased SCR amplitudes. Lanteaume et al. (2006) examined the effect of electrical stimulation of the amygdala on self-reported emotions and SCRs in a group of patients with drug-resistant partial epilepsy. SCR amplitudes were larger when the stimulation caused a positive emotional change as opposed to a negative change or no change. Our methodology does not make use of the SCR amplitudes but rather the rates at which they occur. Further research would be necessary to quantify the effect on SCR rates when applying direct stimulation via a DBS implant to the limbic structures. A correction for the effect due to the stimulation could be incorporated into the state-space model in this case.
3.5. Closing the DBS Loop
Developing a complete CLDBS system is a challenge. Our approach demonstrates the ability to recover sympathetic arousal from skin conductance measurements using state-space methods. Neural stimuli to the sweat glands originating from the sympathetic nerve fibers encode emotion-related information in how frequently they fire. By relating the probability of neural impulse occurrence to sympathetic arousal through a state-space model, we are able to estimate a continuous state trajectory across different episodes of relaxation and stress.
Different biomarkers have been suggested for closing the loop in DBS therapy. Ideally, the biomarker should enable the real-time tracking of an unobserved brain state. Here, we investigate skin conductance as a viable alternative for a CLDBS as a means of treating neuropsychiatric disorders such as PTSD. While we have sought to address how to estimate an emotion-related state trajectory from peripheral skin conductance measurements, there remains the problem of determining the mapping from the state variable back into the CLDBS stimulation parameter space (i.e., the amplitude, frequency, and width of the electrical stimuli). Determining this mapping will enable sympathetic arousal to be controlled in realtime. Grahn et al. (2014) proposed a novel means of addressing this mapping problem in a CLDBS system they developed for maintaining stable dopamine levels in rodents. They first varied the frequency, amplitude, and pulse width of the electrical stimulation and measured the corresponding dopamine level responses for different parameter combinations. We too could similarly vary the electrical stimulation parameters of a DBS system while measuring skin conductance changes and estimate the corresponding arousal levels. Grahn et al. (2014) next characterized the dopamine responses using a combination of 7th order polynomials and 2nd order exponentials. This required a total of 12 model coefficients. They next trained a neural network having the frequency, amplitude and pulse width as the inputs and the 12 model coefficients as the outputs. Likewise, we could train a neural network mapping stimulation parameters to arousal responses. Thereafter, Grahn et al. (2014) trained a second neural network having the model coefficients as inputs and the frequency, amplitude and pulse width as the outputs. They used this inverse model for predicting the stimulation parameters necessary for maintaining specific extra-cellular dopamine levels. Therefore, a CLDBS system utilizing our method for arousal estimation could utilize a similar neural network characterizing the inverse relationship back into the stimulation parameter space in its feedback path.
A simpler option would be to use on/off control instead of adjusting the stimulation parameters in a continuous manner. Herron et al. (2017) developed an on/off CLDBS controller for a patient suffering from tremor. The controller applied electrical stimulation when β-band power recorded from invasively acquired electroencephalography (EEG) dropped below a certain (manually-tuned) threshold, and switched it off when the power exceeded yet another threshold. Preliminary on/off control could be applied in the case of neuropsychiatric disorders too, for instance when sudden angry outbursts or bouts of depression as detected by abnormally elevated or diminished arousal levels occur.
We wish to point out that not all elevated arousal levels need to be controlled (e.g. high arousal levels due to positive excitement). When proposing a CLDBS framework for the treatment of chronic pain, Shirvalkar et al. (2018) too noted that they did not wish to avoid all feelings of pain per se, as pain itself provides a warning of potential tissue damage. Therefore, a fully closed-loop system for the treatment of neuropsychiatric disorders should be able to recognize the positive–negative aspect of emotion as well. This positive–negative or pleasure–displeasure dimension is known by the term emotional valence (Russell, 1980). The CLDBS would ideally need to estimate a vector zj containing both arousal and valence. Emotional valence information can be decoded using physiological signals such as scalp EEG, heart rate, electromyography and skin conductance (Koelstra et al., 2012; Soleymani et al., 2012). Stimulation can then be turned on, for instance, only if high arousal is detected and valence is negative.
Finally a control algorithm is necessary in the CLDBS feedback loop when progressing beyond preliminary on/off stimulation. Our state estimation approach relies on a rate of neural firing. Azgomi et al. (2019) developed a fuzzy feedback controller based on the rate of SCR appearances for increasing arousal during relaxation and decreasing it during periods of cognitive stress. The controller was based on the model proposed in Wickramasuriya et al. (2018) and can directly be used with the method presented here. In a review of CLDBS therapy, Carron et al. (2013) mention two different other works, namely those by Grant and Lowery (2013) and Pasillas-Lépine et al. (2013), that propose a control mechanism based on the rate of neural firing for estimating a state variable. Pasillas-Lépine et al. (2013) proposed a proportional control based on the firing rates of the subthalamic nucleus and global pallidus. Grant and Lowery (2013) developed a similar CLDBS controller based on β-band oscillations in LFPs. An adaptation of one of these methods could also be used for developing a control law for regulating emotion based on peripheral skin conductance measurements.
3.6. Conclusion
DBS has met with success in treating a host of disease conditions where other therapeutic measures have been exhausted. CLDBS systems have been proposed as the future of DBS due to their inherent advantages over the previous generation of open-loop systems. Closing the DBS loop is a challenge. In this work, we present a method for estimating sympathetic arousal from skin conductance measurements as a potential mechanism that could be deployed within a CLDBS system for treating neuropsychiatric disorders. The methodology consists of two parts: (i) the deconvolution of phasic skin conductance to obtain the neural impulses that generate SCRs; (ii) a state-space model for tracking sympathetic arousal based on the frequency at which the SCRs appear. Results are demonstrated on a publicly available data-set. We finally discuss possibilities for developing a controller that could map the state estimates back into the stimulation parameter space for automated closed-loop control. While we mention PTSD as an example scenario here, our approach could be generalized to other disease conditions where any type of impulse-like or pulsatile signal is a biomarker. For instance, if neural spiking or pulsatile cortisol secretions are clinically-relevant features for a particular disease condition, then the Bayesian filter described here could also be used as part of closing the loop. Additionally, the methods presented here are personalized, i.e., the model parameters are estimated for each individual. Differences in skin conductance have been reported in the literature between healthy subjects and patients with anorexia nervosa (Tchanturia et al., 2007) and depression (Ward and Doerr, 1986). If skin conductance biomarkers can be determined for each of these conditions (based on SCR amplitudes, rates of SCR occurrence, skin conductance levels etc.), then an extended state-space model could be developed to track a symptom-related neural state that incorporates both binary and continuous-valued observations (Prerau et al., 2009; Coleman et al., 2011).
Data Availability
Publicly available datasets were analyzed in this study. This data can be found here: https://physionet.org/physiobank/database/noneeg/.
Author Contributions
RF conceived and designed the study. DW and MA analyzed data and wrote the manuscript. RF, DW, and MA developed the algorithms and analysis tools.
Funding
The work was supported in part by the National Science Foundation grant 1755780 CRII: CPS: Wearable Machine Interface Architectures.
Conflict of Interest Statement
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
References
Aikins, D. E., Johnson, D. C., Borelli, J. L., Klemanski, D. H., Morrissey, P. M., Benham, T. L., et al. (2009). Thought suppression failures in combat PTSD: a cognitive load hypothesis. Behav. Res. Ther. 47, 744–751. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2009.06.006
Alexander, D. M., Trengove, C., Johnston, P., Cooper, T., August, J. P., and Gordon, E. (2005). Separating individual skin conductance responses in a short interstimulus-interval paradigm. J. Neurosci. Methods 146, 116–123. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2005.02.001
American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th Edn. Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Association.
Amin, M. R., and Faghih, R. T. (2018). “Inferring autonomic nervous system stimulation from hand and foot skin conductance measurements,” in 52th Asilomar Conference on Signals, Systems and Computers. (Pacific Grove, CA).
Amin, M. R., and Faghih, R. T. (2019). Sparse deconvolution of electrodermal activity via continuous-time system identification. IEEE Trans. Biomed. Eng. doi: 10.1109/TBME.2019.2892352. [Epub ahead of print].
Azgomi, H. F., Wickramasuriya, D. S., and Faghih, R. T. (2019). “State-space modeling and fuzzy feedback control of cognitive stress,” in 41st Annual International Conference of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society (EMBC) (Berlin).
Bach, D. R., Flandin, G., Friston, K. J., and Dolan, R. J. (2010). Modelling event-related skin conductance responses. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 75, 349–356. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2010.01.005
Benedek, M., and Kaernbach, C. (2010). Decomposition of skin conductance data by means of nonnegative deconvolution. Psychophysiology 47, 647–658. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2009.00972.x
Bina, R. W., and Langevin, J.-P. (2018). Closed loop deep brain stimulation for PTSD, addiction, and disorders of affective facial interpretation: review and discussion of potential biomarkers and stimulation paradigms. Front. Neurosci. 12:300. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2018.00300
Birjandtalab, J., Cogan, D., Pouyan, M. B., and Nourani, M. (2016). “A non-EEG biosignals dataset for assessment and visualization of neurological status,” in 2016 IEEE International Workshop on Signal Processing Systems (SiPS) (Dallas, TX), 110–114.
Boccard, S. G., Pereira, E. A., and Aziz, T. Z. (2015). Deep brain stimulation for chronic pain. J. Clin. Neurosci. 22, 1537–1543. doi: 10.1016/j.jocn.2015.04.005
Boucsein, W. (2012). Electrodermal Activity. New York, NY: Springer Science + Business Media, LLC. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4614-1126-0
Bouthour, W., Mégevand, P., Donoghue, J., Lüscher, C., Birbaumer, N., and Krack, P. (2019). Biomarkers for closed-loop deep brain stimulation in Parkinson disease and beyond. Nat. Rev. Neurol. 15, 343–352. doi: 10.1038/s41582-019-0166-4
Braithwaite, J. J., Watson, D. G., Jones, R., and Rowe, M. (2013). A guide for analysing electrodermal activity (EDA) & skin conductance responses (SCRs) for psychological experiments. Psychophysiology 49, 1017–1034. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2012.01384.x
Bronstein, J. M., Tagliati, M., Alterman, R. L., Lozano, A. M., Volkmann, J., Stefani, A., et al. (2011). Deep brain stimulation for Parkinson disease: an expert consensus and review of key issues. Arch. Neurol. 68, 165–165. doi: 10.1001/archneurol.2010.260
Carron, R., Chaillet, A., Filipchuk, A., Pasillas-Lépine, W., and Hammond, C. (2013). Closing the loop of deep brain stimulation. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 7:112. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2013.00112
Chen, W., Jaques, N., Taylor, S., Sano, A., Fedor, S., and Picard, R. W. (2015). “Wavelet-based motion artifact removal for electrodermal activity,” in 37th Annual International Conference of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society (EMBC) (Milano), 6223–6226.
Chou, K. L., Grube, S., and Patil, P. G. (2011). Deep Brain Stimulation: A New Life for People With Parkinson's, Dystonia, and Essential Tremor. New York, NY: Oxford University Press; Demos Medical Publishing.
Cleary, D. R., Ozpinar, A., Raslan, A. M., and Ko, A. L. (2015). Deep brain stimulation for psychiatric disorders: where we are now. Neurosurg. Focus 38:E2. doi: 10.3171/2015.3.FOCUS1546
Coleman, T. P., Yanike, M., Suzuki, W. A., and Brown, E. N. (2011). “A mixed-filter algorithm for dynamically tracking learning from multiple behavioral and neurophysiological measures,” in The Dynamic Brain: An Exploration of Neuronal Variability and Its Functional Significance, eds M. Ding and D. L. Glanzman (New York, NY: Oxford University Press), 1–16.
Critchley, H. D. (2002). Electrodermal responses: what happens in the brain. Neuroscientist 8, 132–142. doi: 10.1177/107385840200800209
Critchley, H. D., Melmed, R. N., Featherstone, E., Mathias, C. J., and Dolan, R. J. (2002). Volitional control of autonomic arousal: a functional magnetic resonance study. Neuroimage 16, 909–919. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2002.1147
Dawson, M. E., Schell, A. M., and Filion, D. L. (2007). “The electrodermal system,” in The Handbook of Psychophysiology, eds J. T. Cacioppo, L. G. Tassinary, and G. G. Berntson (New York, NY: Cambridge University Press), 159–181.
Dostrovsky, J. O., and Lozano, A. M. (2002). Mechanisms of deep brain stimulation. Mov. Disord. 17, S63–S68. doi: 10.1002/mds.10143
Faghih, R. T. (2014). System identification of cortisol secretion: characterizing pulsatile dynamics (Ph.D. thesis). Massachusetts Institute of Technology: Cambridge, MA, United States.
Faghih, R. T. (2018). “From physiological signals to pulsatile dynamics: a sparse system identification approach,” in Dynamic Neuroscience, eds Z. Chen and S. V. Sarma (Cham: Springer), 239–265.
Faghih, R. T., Dahleh, M. A., Adler, G. K., Klerman, E. B., and Brown, E. N. (2014). Deconvolution of serum cortisol levels by using compressed sensing. PLoS ONE 9:e85204. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0085204
Faghih, R. T., Dahleh, M. A., Adler, G. K., Klerman, E. B., and Brown, E. N. (2015a). Quantifying pituitary-adrenal dynamics and deconvolution of concurrent cortisol and adrenocorticotropic hormone data by compressed sensing. IEEE Trans. Biomed. Eng. 62, 2379–2388. doi: 10.1109/TBME.2015.2427745
Faghih, R. T., Stokes, P. A., Marin, M.-F., Zsido, R. G., Zorowitz, S., Rosenbaum, B. L., et al. (2015b). “Characterization of fear conditioning and fear extinction by analysis of electrodermal activity,” in Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society (EMBC), 2015 37th Annual International Conference of the IEEE (Milano: IEEE), 7814–7818.
Fisher, R., Salanova, V., Witt, T., Worth, R., Henry, T., Gross, R., et al. (2010). Electrical stimulation of the anterior nucleus of thalamus for treatment of refractory epilepsy. Epilepsia 51, 899–908. doi: 10.1111/j.1528-1167.2010.02536.x
Goldberger, A. L., Amaral, L. A., Glass, L., Hausdorff, J. M., Ivanov, P. C., Mark, R. G., et al. (2000). PhysioBank, PhysioToolkit, and PhysioNet: components of a new research resource for complex physiologic signals. Circulation 101, e215–e220. doi: 10.1161/01.CIR.101.23.e215
Goldfinger, D. A., Amdur, R. L., and Liberzon, I. (1998). Psychophysiologic responses to the rorschach in ptsd patients, noncombat and combat controls. Depress. Anxiety 8, 112–120. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1520-6394(1998)8:3<112::AID-DA3>3.0.CO;2-U
Golub, G. H., Heath, M., and Wahba, G. (1979). Generalized cross-validation as a method for choosing a good ridge parameter. Technometrics 21, 215–223. doi: 10.1080/00401706.1979.10489751
Grahn, P. J., Mallory, G. W., Khurram, O. U., Berry, B. M., Hachmann, J. T., Bieber, A. J., et al. (2014). A neurochemical closed-loop controller for deep brain stimulation: toward individualized smart neuromodulation therapies. Front. Neurosci. 8:169. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2014.00169
Grant, P. F., and Lowery, M. M. (2013). Simulation of cortico-basal ganglia oscillations and their suppression by closed loop deep brain stimulation. IEEE Trans. Neural Syst. Rehabil. Eng. 21, 584–594. doi: 10.1109/TNSRE.2012.2202403
Greco, A., Valenza, G., Lanata, A., Scilingo, E. P., and Citi, L. (2016). cvxEDA: A convex optimization approach to electrodermal activity processing. IEEE Trans. Biomed. Eng. 63, 797–804. doi: 10.1109/TBME.2015.2474131
Hellmuth, J. C., Stappenbeck, C. A., Hoerster, K. D., and Jakupcak, M. (2012). Modeling PTSD symptom clusters, alcohol misuse, anger, and depression as they relate to aggression and suicidality in returning us veterans. J. Traumat. Stress 25, 527–534. doi: 10.1002/jts.21732
Herron, J. A., Thompson, M. C., Brown, T., Chizeck, H. J., Ojemann, J. G., and Ko, A. L. (2017). Cortical brain–computer interface for closed-loop deep brain stimulation. IEEE Trans. Neural Syst. Rehabil. Eng. 25, 2180–2187. doi: 10.1109/TNSRE.2017.2705661
Jennings, J. R. (1986). “Bodily changes during attending,” in Psychophysiology: Systems, Processes, and Applications, eds M. G. H. Coles, E. Donchin, and S. Porges (New York, NY: Guilford Press), 268–289.
Jetly, R., Heber, A., Fraser, G., and Boisvert, D. (2015). The efficacy of nabilone, a synthetic cannabinoid, in the treatment of PTSD-associated nightmares: a preliminary randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled cross-over design study. Psychoneuroendocrinology 51, 585–588. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2014.11.002
Koek, R. J., Langevin, J.-P., Krahl, S. E., Kosoyan, H. J., Schwartz, H. N., Chen, J. W., et al. (2014). Deep brain stimulation of the basolateral amygdala for treatment-refractory combat post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD): study protocol for a pilot randomized controlled trial with blinded, staggered onset of stimulation. Trials 15:356. doi: 10.1186/1745-6215-15-356
Koelstra, S., Muhl, C., Soleymani, M., Lee, J.-S., Yazdani, A., Ebrahimi, T., et al. (2012). DEAP: A database for emotion analysis; using physiological signals. IEEE Trans. Affect. Comput. 3, 18–31. doi: 10.1109/T-AFFC.2011.15
Koskimäki, H., Mönttinen, H., Siirtola, P., Huttunen, H.-L., Halonen, R., and Röning, J. (2017). “Early detection of migraine attacks based on wearable sensors: experiences of data collection using Empatica E4,” in Proceedings of the 2017 ACM International Joint Conference on Pervasive and Ubiquitous Computing and Proceedings of the 2017 ACM International Symposium on Wearable Computers (Maui, HI: ACM), 506–511.
Langevin, J.-P., Chen, J., Koek, R., Sultzer, D., Mandelkern, M., Schwartz, H., and Krahl, S. (2016). Deep brain stimulation of the basolateral amygdala: targeting technique and electrodiagnostic findings. Brain Sci. 6:28. doi: 10.3390/brainsci6030028
Lanteaume, L., Khalfa, S., Régis, J., Marquis, P., Chauvel, P., and Bartolomei, F. (2006). Emotion induction after direct intracerebral stimulations of human amygdala. Cereb. Cortex 17, 307–1313. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhl041
Lempka, S. F., Malone, D. A. Jr., Hu, B., Baker, K. B., Wyant, A., Ozinga, J. G. IV, et al. (2017). Randomized clinical trial of deep brain stimulation for poststroke pain. Ann. Neurol. 81, 653–663. doi: 10.1002/ana.24927
Lin, H.-P., Lin, H.-Y., Lin, W.-L., and Huang, A. C.-W. (2011). Effects of stress, depression, and their interaction on heart rate, skin conductance, finger temperature, and respiratory rate: sympathetic-parasympathetic hypothesis of stress and depression. J. Clin. Psychol. 67, 1080–1091. doi: 10.1002/jclp.20833
Lipsman, N., Woodside, D. B., Giacobbe, P., Hamani, C., Carter, J. C., Norwood, S. J., et al. (2013). Subcallosal cingulate deep brain stimulation for treatment-refractory anorexia nervosa: a phase 1 pilot trial. Lancet 381, 1361–1370. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(12)62188-6
Little, S., Beudel, M., Zrinzo, L., Foltynie, T., Limousin, P., Hariz, M., et al. (2016). Bilateral adaptive deep brain stimulation is effective in Parkinson's disease. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 87, 717–721. doi: 10.1136/jnnp-2015-310972
Little, S., and Brown, P. (2012). What brain signals are suitable for feedback control of deep brain stimulation in parkinson's disease? Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1265, 9–24. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2012.06650.x
Little, S., Pogosyan, A., Neal, S., Zavala, B., Zrinzo, L., Hariz, M., et al. (2013). Adaptive deep brain stimulation in advanced Parkinson disease. Ann. Neurol. 74, 449–457. doi: 10.1002/ana.23951
Low, P. A. (2012). “Chapter 51 - sweating,” in Primer on the Autonomic Nervous System, 3rd Edition, eds D. Robertson, I. Biaggioni, G. Burnstock, P. A. Low, and J. F. Paton (San Diego, CA: Academic Press), 249–251.
Lüthi, A., and Lüscher, C. (2014). Pathological circuit function underlying addiction and anxiety disorders. Nat. Neurosci. 17:1635. doi: 10.1038/nn.3849
Mangina, C. A., and Beuzeron-Mangina, J. H. (1996). Direct electrical stimulation of specific human brain structures and bilateral electrodermal activity. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 22, 1–8. doi: 10.1016/0167-8760(96)00022-0
McCullagh, P., and Nelder, J. A. (1989). Generalized Linear Models, Vol. 37. London; New York: Chapman and Hall; CRC Press.
McNally, R. J., Luedke, D. L., Besyner, J. K., Peterson, R. A., Bohm, K., and Lips, O. J. (1987). Sensitivity to stress-relevant stimuli in posttraumatic stress disorder. J. Anxiety Disord. 1, 105–116. doi: 10.1016/0887-6185(87)90001-6
Mendel, J. M. (1995). Lessons in Estimation Theory for Signal Processing, Communications, and Control. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall PTR; Pearson Education.
Merkl, A., Schneider, G.-H., Schönecker, T., Aust, S., Kühl, K.-P., Kupsch, A., et al. (2013). Antidepressant effects after short-term and chronic stimulation of the subgenual cingulate gyrus in treatment-resistant depression. Exp. Neurol. 249, 160–168. doi: 10.1016/j.expneurol.2013.08.017
Müller, U. J., Voges, J., Steiner, J., Galazky, I., Heinze, H.-J., Möller, M., et al. (2013). Deep brain stimulation of the nucleus accumbens for the treatment of addiction. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1282, 119–128. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2012.06834.x
Munro, L. L., Dawson, M. E., Schell, A. M., and Sakai, L. M. (1987). Electrodermal lability and rapid vigilance decrement in a degraded stimulus continuous performance task. J. Psychophysiol. 1, 249–257.
Murray, J. F. (2005). Visual recognition, inference and coding using learned sparse overcomplete representations (Ph.D. thesis). University of California, San Diego, CA, United States.
Oluigbo, C. O., Salma, A., and Rezai, A. R. (2012). Deep brain stimulation for neurological disorders. IEEE Rev. Biomed. Eng. 5, 88–99. doi: 10.1109/RBME.2012.2197745
Orr, S. P., and Roth, W. T. (2000). Psychophysiological assessment: clinical applications for PTSD. J. Affect. Disord. 61, 225–240. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0327(00)00340-2
Pasillas-Lépine, W., Haidar, I., Chaillet, A., and Panteley, E. (2013). “Closed-loop deep brain stimulation based on firing-rate regulation,” in 2013 6th International IEEE/EMBS Conference on Neural Engineering (NER) (San Diego, CA), 166–169.
Pole, N. (2007). The psychophysiology of posttraumatic stress disorder: a meta-analysis. Psychol. Bull. 133:725. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.133.5.725
Prerau, M. J., Smith, A. C., Eden, U. T., Kubota, Y., Yanike, M., Suzuki, W., et al. (2009). Characterizing learning by simultaneous analysis of continuous and binary measures of performance. J. Neurophysiol. 102, 3060–3072. doi: 10.1152/jn.91251.2008
Puigdemont, D., Pérez-Egea, R., Portella, M. J., Molet, J., de Diego-Adeliño, J., Gironell, A., et al. (2012). Deep brain stimulation of the subcallosal cingulate gyrus: further evidence in treatment-resistant major depression. Int. J. Neuropsychopharmacol. 15, 121–133. doi: 10.1017/S1461145711001088
Risser, H. J., Hetzel-Riggin, M. D., Thomsen, C. J., and McCanne, T. R. (2006). PTSD as a mediator of sexual revictimization: the role of reexperiencing, avoidance, and arousal symptoms. J. Traumat. Stress 19, 687–698. doi: 10.1002/jts.20156
Rosin, B., Slovik, M., Mitelman, R., Rivlin-Etzion, M., Haber, S. N., Israel, Z., et al. (2011). Closed-loop deep brain stimulation is superior in ameliorating parkinsonism. Neuron 72, 370–384. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.08.023
Russell, J. A. (1980). A circumplex model of affect. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 39:1161. doi: 10.1037/h0077714
Shirvalkar, P., Veuthey, T. L., Dawes, H. E., and Chang, E. F. (2018). Closed-loop deep brain stimulation for refractory chronic pain. Front. Comput. Neurosci. 12:18. doi: 10.3389/fncom.2018.00018
Silverthorn, D. U. (2009). Human Physiology: An Integrated Approach, 6th Edn. Illinois City, IL: Pearson Education, Inc.
Smith, A. C., Frank, L. M., Wirth, S., Yanike, M., Hu, D., Kubota, Y., et al. (2004). Dynamic analysis of learning in behavioral experiments. J. Neurosci. 24, 447–461. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2908-03.2004
Soleymani, M., Lichtenauer, J., Pun, T., and Pantic, M. (2012). A multimodal database for affect recognition and implicit tagging. IEEE Trans. Affect. Comput. 3, 42–55. doi: 10.1109/T-AFFC.2011.25
Tchanturia, K., Liao, P.-C., Uher, R., Lawrence, N., Treasure, J., and Campbell, I. C. (2007). An investigation of decision making in anorexia nervosa using the Iowa Gambling Task and skin conductance measurements. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 13, 635–641. doi: 10.1017/S1355617707070798
Vesper, J., Steinhoff, B., Rona, S., Wille, C., Bilic, S., Nikkhah, G., et al. (2007). Chronic high-frequency deep brain stimulation of the STN/SNr for progressive myoclonic epilepsy. Epilepsia 48, 84–1989. doi: 10.1111/j.1528-1167.2007.01166.x
Ward, N. G., and Doerr, H. O. (1986). Skin conductance: a potentially sensitive and specific marker for depression. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 174, 553–559.
Wickramasuriya, D. S., and Faghih, R. T. (2019). “A novel filter for tracking real-world cognitive stress using multi-time-scale point process observations,” in 41st Annual International Conference of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society (EMBC) (Berlin).
Wickramasuriya, D. S., Qi, C., and Faghih, R. T. (2018). “A state-space approach for detecting stress from electrodermal activity,” in 40th Annual International Conference of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society (EMBC) (Honolulu, HI), 3562–3567.
Woodward, S. H., Murburg, M. M., and Bliwise, D. L. (2000). PTSD-related hyperarousal assessed during sleep. Physiol. Behav. 70, 197–203. doi: 10.1016/S0031-9384(00)00271-7
Wu, H., Van Dyck-Lippens, P. J., Santegoeds, R., van Kuyck, K., Gabriëls, L., Lin, G., et al. (2013). Deep-brain stimulation for anorexia nervosa. World Neurosurg. 80, S29–se1. doi: 10.1016/j.wneu.2012.06.039
Zaknich, A. (2005). Principles of Adaptive Filters and Self-Learning Systems. London: Springer Science & Business Media.
Zdunek, R., and Cichocki, A. (2008). Improved M-FOCUSS algorithm with overlapping blocks for locally smooth sparse signals. IEEE Trans. Signal Process. 56, 4752–4761. doi: 10.1109/TSP.2008.928160
Keywords: skin conductance (SC), deep brain stimulation (DBS), deconvolution analysis, arousal, state-space (SS) representation
Citation: Wickramasuriya DS, Amin MR and Faghih RT (2019) Skin Conductance as a Viable Alternative for Closing the Deep Brain Stimulation Loop in Neuropsychiatric Disorders. Front. Neurosci. 13:780. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2019.00780
Received: 03 May 2019; Accepted: 11 July 2019;
Published: 07 August 2019.
Edited by:
Cuntai Guan, Nanyang Technological University, SingaporeReviewed by:
Julie G. Pilitsis, Albany Medical College, United StatesNicole Bentley, University of Alabama at Birmingham, United States
Copyright © 2019 Wickramasuriya, Amin and Faghih. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Rose T. Faghih, cnRmYWdoaWgmI3gwMDA0MDt1aC5lZHU=