Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Neurosci., 14 February 2019
Sec. Auditory Cognitive Neuroscience
This article is part of the Research Topic The Clinical Neuroscience of Music: Evidence Based Approaches and Neurologic Music Therapy View all 15 articles

Motor Synchronization to Rhythmic Auditory Stimulation (RAS) Attenuates Dopaminergic Responses in Ventral Striatum in Young Healthy Adults: [11C]-(+)-PHNO PET Study

\r\nYuko Koshimori,*Yuko Koshimori1,2*Antonio P. Strafella,,Antonio P. Strafella2,3,4Mikaeel Valli,Mikaeel Valli2,3Vivek Sharma,Vivek Sharma1,5Sang-soo Cho,Sang-soo Cho2,3Sylvain HouleSylvain Houle2Michael H. Thaut,Michael H. Thaut1,2
  • 1Music and Health Research Collaboratory, Faculty of Music, University of Toronto, Toronto, ON, Canada
  • 2Research Imaging Centre, Campbell Family Mental Health Research Institute, Centre for Addiction and Mental Health, University of Toronto, Toronto, ON, Canada
  • 3Division of Brain, Imaging and Behaviour – Systems Neuroscience, Krembil Research Institute, University Health Network, University of Toronto, Toronto, ON, Canada
  • 4Morton and Gloria Shulman Movement Disorders Clinic and The Edmond J. Safra Program in Parkinson’s Disease, Neurology Division, Department of Medicine, Toronto Western Hospital, University Health Network, University of Toronto, Toronto, ON, Canada
  • 5Baycrest Health Sciences, Rotman Research Institute, Toronto, ON, Canada

Auditory-motor entrainment using rhythmic auditory stimulation (RAS) has been shown to improve motor control in healthy persons and persons with neurologic motor disorders such as Parkinson’s disease and stroke. Neuroimaging studies have shown the modulation of corticostriatal activity in response to RAS. However, the underlying neurochemical mechanisms for auditory-motor entrainment are unknown. The current study aimed to investigate RAS-induced dopamine (DA) responses in basal ganglia (BG) during finger tapping tasks combined with [11C]-(+)-PHNO-PET in eight right-handed young healthy participants. Each participant underwent two PET scans with and without RAS. Binding potential relative to the non-displaceable compartment (BPND) values were derived using the simplified reference tissue method. The task performance was measured using absolute tapping period error and its standard deviation. We found that the presence of RAS significantly improved the task performance compared to the absence of RAS, demonstrated by reductions in the absolute tapping period error (p = 0.007) and its variability (p = 0.006). We also found that (1) the presence of RAS reduced the BG BPND variability (p = 0.013) and (2) the absence of RAS resulted in a greater DA response in the left ventral striatum (VS) compared to the presence of RAS (p = 0.003), These suggest that the absence of external cueing may require more DA response in the left VS associated with more motivational and sustained attentional efforts to perform the task. Additionally, we demonstrated significant age effects on D2/3 R availability in BG: increasing age was associated with reduced D2/3 R availability in the left putamen without RAS (p = 0.026) as well as in the right VS with RAS (p = 0.02). This is the first study to demonstrate the relationships among RAS, DA response/D2/3 R availability, motor responses and age, providing the groundwork for future studies to explore mechanisms for auditory-motor entrainment in healthy elderly and patients with dopamine-based movement disorders.

Introduction

Rhythmic auditory stimulation (RAS) – presented either as single auditory beats or metronome clicks embedded in instrumental music – has shown to improve motor control in healthy persons and persons with neurologic motor disorders such as Parkinson’s disease (PD) and stroke (Miller et al., 1996; Thaut et al., 1996, 2002; McIntosh et al., 1997; Massie et al., 2009). Reduction in variability of motor timing, electromyography recruitment, and movement kinematics as well as increases in speed are among the positive effects demonstrated.

These benefits result from rhythmic auditory entrainment. Entrainment refers to the frequency locking of two oscillating bodies that can move in stable periodic cycles (Thaut, 2015). The rhythmic frequency provides the brain (already equipped with internal time keeper mechanism) with an additional externally triggered time keeper, which generates a precise temporal interval as a continuous time reference (Thaut, 2015). Importantly, the auditory system is more precise and faster to detect temporal patterns than other sensory systems such as visual and tactile systems (Shelton and Kumar, 2010).

Auditory rhythm can prime and time muscle activation by providing precise anticipatory time cues for motor planning and execution (Paltsev and E-lner, 1967; Rossignol and Jones, 1976), which increases the readiness to move and improves subsequent response quality (Thaut, 2015). Once auditory-motor entrainment occurs, movements stays locked to the auditory rhythm presented even when subtle tempo changes occur in the auditory stimuli that are not consciously perceived (Thaut et al., 1998a,b; Large et al., 2002).

The auditory and the motor systems are connected through widely distributed and hierarchically organized neural networks from cortical to subcortical, brain stem, and cerebellar regions (Thaut, 2003; Schmahmann and Pandya, 2006; Felix et al., 2011; Konoike et al., 2012). Functional MRI studies have shown that listening to regular auditory rhythm modulated activities in premotor (Chen et al., 2006, 2008), cortico-basal ganglia (BG) including putamen, caudate, and pallidum (Grahn and Brett, 2007; Grahn and Rowe, 2009, 2013), and cortico-cerebellar (Thaut et al., 2009; Konoike et al., 2012) networks. It also led to the rapid and precise brain wave entrainment, mainly in beta oscillation bands in the motor areas such as supplementary motor area (SMA) and cerebellum (Fujioka et al., 2012; Crasta et al., 2018). In addition to auditory rhythm, music generally modulates activity in widely distributed brain areas, particularly in the limbic regions including the nucleus accumbens/ventral striatum (VS) (Blood and Zatorre, 2001; Menon and Levitin, 2005; Salimpoor et al., 2011, 2013; Koelsch, 2014; Mueller et al., 2015). Furthermore, anatomical (Hackett, 2015) and resting functional MRI (Helmich et al., 2010) studies demonstrated connectivity between superior temporal gyrus and striatum (i.e., putamen and caudate). These suggest a close link between auditory areas and BG.

However, it is not well understood how dopamine (DA) in BG is involved in auditory-motor entrainment due to a paucity of research. Neuroimaging studies can investigate the neural mechanisms by employing a synchronization paradigm (Thaut et al., 1998a,b), in which finger tapping is synchronized to external auditory cueing that is thought to occur through entrainment (Braunlich et al., 2018). This can be contrasted to a continuation task (i.e., finger tapping without external auditory cueing) to elucidate the role of external auditory cueing in motor timing (Koshimori and Thaut, 2018; Teghil et al., 2018). To our knowledge, all of the studies using the synchronization/continuation task design employed fMRI and showed that cortical motor areas and cerebellum were activated during auditory-motor entrainment (Rao et al., 1997; Jäncke et al., 2000; Toyomura et al., 2012), but whose activation was similar to that during the continuation task. However, activation of the putamen and SMA was absent during the synchronization task (Rao et al., 1997; Lewis et al., 2004; Toyomura et al., 2012). These suggest that external auditory cueing may not have an extra role in brain activation in motor areas or that external auditory cueing may not require cortico-basal ganglia activity in young healthy adults. On the other hand, one PET study in PD suggested the association between dopaminergic function measured by baseline striatal dopaminergic denervation and auditory-motor synchronization performance (Miller et al., 2013). In addition, pharmacological studies in PD (McIntosh et al., 1997; Rochester et al., 2010) suggested that DA may play a partial role in auditory-motor entrainment or that intact dopaminergic system may be required for auditory-motor entrainment (Koshimori and Thaut, 2018).

To date there have been no studies investigating the role of BG DA in auditory-motor entrainment using the synchronization/continuation paradigm. Studying dopaminergic responses may be of particular importance to understand how PD benefits from RAS, since BG is an important subcortical structure for timing perception, which is crucial for movements and is negatively affected by dopamine depletion, yet persons with PD have significantly improved motor control with RAS, especially in gait performance (Ghai et al., 2018). The current study is therefore intended to investigate neurochemical mechanisms underlying the effect of RAS through dopamine responses in young healthy adults, serving as a baseline response to which later studies with healthy elderly and patients with dopamine-deficient movement disorders can be compared.

For this purpose, we employed [11C]-(+)-PHNO-PET to measure dopaminergic function during auditory-motor entrainment. Compared to the commonly used D2/3 R radioligand, [11C]-racropride, it shows higher sensitivity in detecting D3 receptors (Narendran et al., 2006), which allows for better quantification of the regions with greater expression of D3 receptors such as the ventral striatum (VS) and globus pallidus (GP) (Narendran et al., 2006; Willeit et al., 2006). In addition, being an agonist radioligand, it is more sensitive to competition with endogenous DA in vivo compared to an antagonist radioligand, [11C]-racropride (Caravaggio et al., 2014, 2016), which is more advantageous in a functional study. It has been employed in the studies that investigated task-induced functional changes (Mizrahi et al., 2014) and task-induced functional changes associated with reward and motivation (Caravaggio et al., 2018).

We hypothesized that (1) finger tapping performance would be better with RAS compared to No-RAS, (2) the PET outcome measure, BPND values would be significantly different between the task conditions in BG and its regions, and (3) finger tapping performance would be associated with BPND values.

Materials and Methods

Participants

Twelve right-handed healthy participants (seven women and five men) aged from 18 to 35 years enrolled in this study. The participants were recruited through self-referral in response to advertisements. Inclusion criteria were: no history of neurologic/psychiatric disorders or major medical conditions, no hearing issues, no contradictions for PET and MRI scans (e.g., metal implants, claustrophobia, pacemaker, or pregnancy), no alcohol, medication or drug dependency or abuse, right-handedness confirmed by the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory (Oldfield, 1971), no current depression assessed by Beck Depression Inventory II (BDI II). The study was carried out in accordance with the recommendations of the Ethical Committee of the Centre for Addiction and Mental Health. All participants gave written informed consent in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. The protocol was approved by the Ethical Committee of the Centre for Addiction and Mental Health.

Study Design

Each participant underwent two [11C]-(+)-PHNO PET and one MRI scans. During one scan, a participant was performing a finger-tapping task with RAS (RAS condition) and during the other scan, a finger-tapping task without RAS (No-RAS condition). A finger tapping task started upon the radioligand injection to be consistent across the participants (Figure 1). During each scan, there were three task blocks with 10-min rests in between. One block consists of six tapping sessions with each lasting for 2 min followed by a 1-min rest. The total time of one task block lasted for 17 min. After completion of three task blocks, a participant rested until the PET scan was completed. The order of the two PET scans was counterbalanced across participants.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Study design for finger tapping task during PET scan. (A) PET scan consisted of three task blocks with two breaks in between followed by resting. (B) Each task block consisted of six tapping sessions with breaks in between, starting with 1 Hz metronome clicks. RAS was presented in the entire task block in the RAS condition whereas it was faded after six beats/clicks in the No-RAS condition.

Finger Tapping Task

Rhythmic auditory stimulation was programmed using Psychophysics Toolbox Version 3 (Brainard, 1997; Pelli, 1997; Kleiner et al., 2007) and run on Matlab (R2016a). It was presented through speakers above the participant’s head located outside of the scanner. The loudness of RAS was tested after the participant laid down on the scanner bed. It was set at a comfortable listening level determined by each participant. In addition, a keyboard on which the participant tapped was stabilized at a comfortable location. Participants performed the task briefly before the radioligand injection. Beep sounds indicated the start and the end of each tapping session. In the RAS condition, a participant was asked to tap his/her right index finger to six different rhythmic cues including (1) 1 Hz beats embedded in music (Capricho Catalán by Isaac Albéniz), (2) 4 Hz metronome clicks, (3) 2 Hz beats with random tempo changes below the threshold of conscious perception (4% of interbeat interval) embedded in music (Allegro Robusto by Bela Bartok), (4) 4 Hz beats embedded in music (Rondo a Capriccio “Rage Over My Lost Penny” by Ludwig von Beethoven), (5) 1 Hz metronome clicks, and (6) 2 Hz with tempo changes continuously modulated on a cosine function below the threshold of conscious perception (4% of interbeat interval) embedded in music (Allegro Robusto by Bela Bartok). Our choice of the auditory stimuli regarding the frequency and presentation mode was based on extensive research in synchronization paradigms using auditory rhythm and finger tapping in the past 50 years in healthy persons and persons with movement disorders: the most pronounced entrainment effects have been demonstrated ranging between 1 and 4 Hz (e.g., Michon and van der Valk, 1967; Rao et al., 1997; Thaut et al., 1998a,b; Stephan et al., 2002; Thaut and Kenyon, 2003; Molinari et al., 2005; Repp, 2005; Thaut et al., 2008; Braunlich et al., 2018). The current study aimed to investigate DA responses across the typical frequency range of entrainment. In order to enhance beat perception, a musical context via melody and harmony components was added to half of the rhythm presentation (Thaut et al., 1997; Nozaradan et al., 2011). The choice of the order and mode of presentation of RAS was also intended to reduce fatigue of the participants and boredom of the task. The RAS was presented in the same order across the participants. In the No-RAS condition, a participant was asked to tap his/her right index finger to the six cue-in beats/clicks following the same rhythm presentation and order as in the RAS condition. After six beats/clicks, the cue was faded, and participants continued to tap without the external cuing. Participants practiced each finger tapping task outside of the scanner on the scan day. The finger tapping performance was recorded on Matlab (R2016a) and measured using the absolute tapping period error and its standard deviation. The absolute tapping period error was the absolute differences between interstimulus intervals and interresponse intervals. The absolute tapping period errors that were 50% longer or shorter than the target intervals were excluded as outliers from the analysis. In addition, the first six taps were excluded from the analysis.

MRI Acquisition

Each participant underwent one structural MRI scan acquired with a General Electric Discovery MR 750 3T scanner with 8-channel head coil (General Electric, Milwaukee, WI, United States). Proton density-weighted MRIs (oblique plane, 84 slices; matrix of 256 × 192; 22 cm field of view; 2 cm slice thickness; echo time = Min Full; repetition time = 6000 ms; flip angle = 8°) were used for co-registration to PET images for region of interest (ROI) delineation.

PET Acquisition

Each participant underwent two [11C]-(+)-PHNO scans acquired with a high resolution PET/CT Siemens-Biograph HiRez XVI (Siemens Molecular Imaging, Knoxville, TN, United States), operating in 3D mode with an in-plane resolution of approximately 4.6 mm full width at half-maximum. The radiosynthesis of [11C]-(+)-PHNO ([11C]-(+)-4-propyl-9-hydroxynaphthoxazine) has been described in detail elsewhere (Wilson et al., 2005). Briefly, [11C]-propionyl chloride was reacted with 9-hydroxynaphthoxazine to generate a [11C]-amide, which is subsequently reduced by lithium aluminum hydride. Purification by HPLC and formulation give radiochemically pure [11C]-(+)-PHNO as a sterile, pyrogen-free solution suitable for human studies.

Prior to the PET scan, a low dose (0.2 mSv) CT scan was performed and used for attenuation correction. In order to prevent head movement during the PET scan, a thermoplastic facemask was custom-fitted to each participant and attached to a head-fixation system (Tru-Scan Imaging, Annapolis). For each scan, ∼10 mCi [11C]-(+)-PHNO was injected as a bolus into an intravenous line placed in an antecubital vein. The emission data were acquired over 90 min. For each 3D sinogram, data were normalized with attenuation and scatter corrected before applying fourier rebinning to convert the 3D sinograms into 2D sinograms. The 2D sinograms were then reconstructed into image space using a 2D filtered back projection algorithm, with a ramp filter at Nyquist cutoff frequency. After reconstruction, a Gaussian filter with a 5 mm FWHM was applied and the images calibrated to nCi/cc. The spatial resolution of the reconstructed images was 2 mm × 2 mm × 2 mm.

PET Image Processing

[11C]-(+)-PHNO images were processed using in-house software ROMI platform (Rusjan et al., 2006). The preprocessing steps included (1) motion correction if necessary, (2) transformation of a standard brain template with a set of predefined ROIs to match individual high-resolution MR images, (3) refinement of the ROIs from the transformed template based on the gray matter probability of voxels in the individual MR images (segmentation step), (4) coregistration of the individual MR images to the PET images to transform the individual refined ROIs to the PET image space, and (5) extraction of time activity curves of the ROIs. We chose ROI analysis over voxel-based analysis due to poor spatial resolution of PET imaging. Our ROIs included the entire BG and its regions (i.e., putamen, caudate, VS, and GP). Binding potential relative to the non-displaceable compartment (BPND) were extracted bilaterally in the ROIs using the simplified reference tissue model with the cerebellum (excluding the vermis) as a reference region using PMOD (version 3.6).

Statistical Analysis

The normality was tested on the behavioral and [11C]-(+)-PHNO PET outcome measures using a Shapiro–Wilk test. Correlation analyses were performed between age and music experience, and BPND values. Depending on the results of these tests, appropriate statistical analyses were used to test differences in BPND values between conditions as well as correlations (1) between BPND values and task performance for each condition and (2) between percentage changes in BPND value and percentage changes in task performance. The statistical analyses were conducted using SPSS (version 20). The significance level for the statistical analyses was set at p < 0.05 (Bonferroni corrected).

Results

Participants

Among 12 participants who enrolled in this study, one participant withdrew from the study because of a schedule conflict. Two participants were excluded because their behavioral data were not recorded due to a technical failure. In addition, one participant did not perform the finger tapping task in the No-RAS condition as practiced. Therefore, a total of eight participants (four women and four men) were included in the analyses. Among them, five participants had their PET scans with RAS-condition first. The demographic characteristics and their PET scan parameters were presented in Table 1. Music experiences varied from none (N = 2), to 1 year (N = 1), 5 years (N = 5), 9 years (N = 1), and 10 years (N = 1). The PET parameters including the amount injected, specific activity, and mass inject were not significantly different between conditions.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Demographics and PET scan parameters of eight right-handed young healthy participants.

Normality of Outcome Measures

The Shapiro–Wilk test did not show any significant results in normality of BPND values. Therefore, the statistical analyses were conducted using parametric tests.

Finger Tapping Performance

Rhythmic auditory stimulation significantly improved the finger tapping task performance in young healthy individuals (Figure 2). Two-sided paired t-tests revealed that the absolute tapping period error was significantly reduced with RAS compared to without RAS (0.027 ± 0.009 vs. 0.036 ± 0.014, t(7) = 3.8, p = 0.007). Similarly, the variability of the absolute tapping period error was significantly reduced with RAS compared to without RAS (0.038 ± 0.015 vs. 0.052 ± 0.018, t(7) = 3.9, p = 0.006). The better finger tapping performance in the RAS-condition was consistently found across the participants regardless of the scan order. The number of outliers did not significantly differ between conditions, indicating that the participants engaged equally in both tasks and that the number of taps did not affect the BPND differences (Wessel et al., 1997). The outliers accounted for approximately 2% of the finger tapping performance on average in each condition.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Mean differences in finger tapping performance between No-RAS and RAS conditions. In the RAS condition, both absolute tapping period error and its variability were significantly reduced compared to in the No-RAS condition. Error bars represent standard deviation.

BPND Values

A two-sided paired t-test did not reveal a significant difference in the BG BPND value between the task conditions. However, the BPND value was significantly higher in the RAS condition compared to in the No-RAS condition in the left VS (2.942 ± 0.407 vs. 2.625 ± 0.541; t(7) = 4.515, p = 0.003), indicating less DA responses in the RAS condition compared to in the No-RAS condition in this region (Figure 3). The higher BPND values in the left VS in the RAS-condition was also consistently found across the participants regardless of the scan order. There were no significant differences in any other ROIs. Because RAS significantly reduced behavioral variability, we also explored whether the variability in BPND value in the entire BG (measured by the standard deviation across the participants and regions) also differed between conditions. Similar to the behavioral result, the RAS condition resulted in significantly less variability in BPND value in the entire BG compared to the No-RAS condition (0.3 vs. 0.42, t(7) = 3.289, p = 0.013) (Figure 4).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Mean BPND images in the left ventral striatum (L VS) in MNI space for each condition generated using SPM for visualization. The ROI mask was shown in the image in the upper left corner. In the RAS condition, BPND was significantly higher compared to in the No-RAS condition, suggesting that RAS was associated with less DA responses. Error bars represent standard error of the mean.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Mean differences in BPND variability changes between No-RAS and RAS conditions across the participants and regions of interest. In the RAS condition, the variability was significantly reduced compared to in the No-RAS condition. Error bars represent standard error of the mean. STDEV: standard deviation.

Correlations Between BPND Values in the Left Ventral Striatum and Finger Tapping Performance

To further investigate the DA responses in the left VS, we first performed a Pearson correlation analysis to investigate the confounding effects of age and music experience on finger tapping performance and BPND values in the BG regions. Age showed significant negative correlations with the BPND values in the left putamen in the No-RAS condition (r = -0.77, p = 0.026) and in the right VS in the RAS-condition (r = -0.789, p = 0.02). Because there were no confounding effects on the BPND values in the left VS, we used a bivariate correlation to test (1) between BPND values in the left VS and finger tapping performance for each condition and (2) between the percentage change of the BPND values in the left VS and of finger tapping performance between conditions. No significant correlations were found.

Discussion

This is the first study to investigate DA responses with [11C]-(+)-PHNO PET during auditory-motor entrainment using RAS in young healthy adults. Our major findings include that the presentation of RAS significantly improved finger tapping task performance and that the presentation of RAS led to significantly reduced DA responses in the left VS. In addition, increasing age was associated with reduced D2/3 R availability in the right VS in the RAS-condition and in the left putamen in the No-RAS condition.

As we hypothesized, the presentation of RAS improved finger tapping task performance in young healthy adults as indicated by reductions of absolute tapping period error and its variability measured using the standard deviations. This is consistent with previous literature (Rao et al., 1997; Jantzen et al., 2005) and corroborated the positive effects of RAS on motor behaviors (Ghai et al., 2018). Consistent with our hypothesis, BPND values would significantly different between conditions in BG. More specifically, the absence of RAS resulted in a significantly greater DA response in the left VS. The VS is part of the mesolimbic dopamine pathway and is implicated to play an important role in reward and motivational processing (Richard et al., 2013; Berridge and Kringelbach, 2015; Saga et al., 2017). The significant finding in the left laterization in the VS may be associated with its functional connectivity with the default mode network (DMN) (Zhang et al., 2017). DMN becomes activated in the thought process in which attention is internally directed such as episodic memory retrieval and planning (Spreng, 2012). Therefore, the absence of RAS may have reflected more motivational/attentional efforts directed toward the internal control of motor timing without auditory rhythmic cueing. Contrary to our hypothesis, finger tapping performance was not associated with dopaminergic function. This may be because the motor responses are more associated with cortical motor areas or cortico-subcortical connectivity.

In addition to these major findings, less D2/3 R availability in the left putamen was associated with increasing age in the No-RAS condition. The striatal D2/3 R availability is highly age-sensitive, as demonstrated in younger healthy adults (Yang et al., 2003). Furthermore, young healthy adults who display the D2R polymorphism associated with reduced D2 R availability (Jönsson et al., 1999) showed increased striatal activation during a perceptual timing task (Wiener et al., 2014). Therefore, less D2/3 R availability in the No-RAS condition observed in our data may be an age associated compensatory response for a more challenging/effortful task condition. These suggest that individual variance in D2/3 R availability due to age and the polymorphism needs to account for in the future studies.

Increasing age was also associated with less D2/3 R availability in the right VS in the RAS condition. Music modulates brain activity in the NAc/VS (Menon and Levitin, 2005; Salimpoor et al., 2011). The right laterization may be associated with rightward processing of music (Menon and Levitin, 2005; Zatorre et al., 2007), musical pleasure (Menon and Levitin, 2005; Salimpoor et al., 2011), reward (Martin-Soelch et al., 2011), and/or emotion (Molochnikov and Cohen, 2014). It is unknown why increased age was associated with less D2/3 R availability in the right VS in the RAS condition as there is no literature to support this finding. DA changes in the mesocorticolimbic system associated with reward and motivation may partially contribute to some of the age-related motor performance (Seidler et al., 2010). Further investigations are needed including a larger number of participants as well as including self-reports on emotion and valence concerned with RAS.

The current study has shed light on the roles of DA in auditory-motor entrainment using RAS in a small sample size of young healthy adults. The initial findings warrant future studies to confirm and further elucidate the current findings with a larger sample size of younger and older healthy adults as well as persons with dopamine-based movement disorders such as PD. If RAS can modulate DA responses in PD, it is of particular interest because the dopaminergic role in the enhancement of motor control in PD with RAS is unknown. The knowledge of baseline D2/3 R availability will allow to determine whether or not the tasks induce significant DA release and will also help to interpret the significant reduction in the variability of DA responses in BG with RAS. It was interesting to find that both neural and behavioral variability measures decreased in the RAS condition although a direct correlational analysis could not be done due to the different metric calculations. A future study can address whether the presence of RAS would modulate individual DA functions to behave similarly. In addition, the knowledge of genetic variations in the D2/3 R subtypes will be of use. As a significant difference in DA response was observed in the VS that is associated with musical pleasure, reward, and motivation, the administration of associated self-reports regarding RAS may be able to further clarify the interpretations of the current findings.

Author Contributions

YK, AS, S-sC, and MT conceived and designed the study. YK, MV, and VS acquired the data. YK analyzed the data. YK and MT interpreted the data. YK drafted the manuscript. AS, MV, VS, S-sC, SH, and MT critically reviewed the manuscript.

Funding

This study was supported by the Faculty of Music, University of Toronto. MT is supported by the Canada Research Chair Program.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Berridge, K. C., and Kringelbach, M. L. (2015). Pleasure systems in the brain. Neuron 86, 646–664. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.02.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blood, A. J., and Zatorre, R. J. (2001). Intensely pleasurable responses to music correlate with activity in brain regions implicated in reward and emotion. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 98, 11818–11823. doi: 10.1073/pnas.191355898

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brainard, D. H. (1997). The psychophysics toolbox. Spa.Vis. 10, 433–436. doi: 10.1163/156856897X00357

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Braunlich, K., Seger, C. A., Jentink, K. G., Bauard, I., Kuger, B. M., and Thaut, M. H. (2018). Rhythmic auditory cues shape neural network recruitment in parkinson’s disease during repetitive motor behavior. Eur. J. Neurosci. [Epub ahead of print]. doi: 10.1111/ejn.14227

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Caravaggio, F., Fervaha, G., Browne, C. J., Gerretsen, P., Remington, G., and Graff-Guerrero, A. (2018). Reward motivation in humans and its relationship to dopamine D2/3receptor availability: A pilot study with dual [11C]-raclopride and [11C]-(+)-PHNO imaging. J. Psychopharmacol. 32, 357–366. doi: 10.1177/0269881118756059

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Caravaggio, F., Kegeles, L. S., Wilson, A. A., Remington, G., Borlido, C., Mamo, D. C., et al. (2016). Estimating the effect of endogenous dopamine on baseline [11C]-(+)-PHNO binding in the human brain. Synapse 70, 453–460. doi: 10.1002/syn.21920

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Caravaggio, F., Nakajima, S., Borlido, C., Remington, G., Gerretsen, P., Wilson, A., et al. (2014). Estimating endogenous dopamine levels at D 2 and D 3 receptors in humans using the agonist radiotracer 11 C-(+)-PHNO. Neuropsychopharmacology 39, 2769–2776. doi: 10.1038/npp.2014.125

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, J., Nakamura, M., Kawamura, T., Takahashi, T., and Nakahara, D. (2006). Roles of pedunculopontine tegmental cholinergic receptors in brain stimulation reward in the rat. Psychopharmacology 184, 514–522. doi: 10.1007/s00213-005-0252-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, J. L., Penhune, V. B., and Zatorre, R. J. (2008). Listening to musical rhythms recruits motor regions of the brain. Cereb. Cortex 18, 2844–2854. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhn042

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crasta, J. E., Thaut, M. H., Anderson, C. W., Davies, P. L., and Gavin, W. J. (2018). Auditory priming improves neural synchronization in auditory-motor entrainment. Neuropsychologia 117, 102–112. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2018.05.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Felix, R. A., Fridberger, A., Leijon, S., Berrebi, A. S., and Magnusson, A. K. (2011). Sound rhythms are encoded by postinhibitory rebound spiking in the superior paraolivary nucleus. J. Neurosci. 31, 12566–12578. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2450-11.2011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fujioka, T., Trainor, L. J., Large, E. W., and Ross, B. (2012). Internalized timing of isochronous sounds is represented in neuromagnetic beta oscillations. J. Neurosci. 32, 1791–1802. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4107-11.2012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ghai, S., Ghai, I., Schmitz, G., and Effenberg, A. O. (2018). Effect of rhythmic auditory cueing on parkinsonian gait: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Sci. Rep. 8, 1–19. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-16232-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grahn, J. A., and Brett, M. (2007). Rhythm and beat perception in motor areas of the brain. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 19, 893–906. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2007.19.5.893

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grahn, J. A., and Rowe, J. B. (2009). Feeling the beat: Premotor and striatal interactions in musicians and nonmusicians during beat perception. J. Neurosci. 29, 7540–7548. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2018-08.2009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grahn, J. A., and Rowe, J. B. (2013). Finding and feeling the musical beat: striatal dissociations between detection and prediction of regularity. Cereb. Cortex 23, 913–921. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs083

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hackett, T. A. (2015). “Anatomic organization of the auditory cortex,” in Handbook of Clinical Neurology, eds G. G. Celesia and G. Hickok (Amsterdam: Elsevier).

Google Scholar

Helmich, R. C., Derikx, L. C., Bakker, M., Scheeringa, R., Bloem, B. R., and Toni, L. (2010). Spatial remapping of cortico-striatal connectivity in parkinson’s disease. Cereb. Cortex. 20, 1175–1186. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhp178

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jäncke, L., Loose, R., Lutz, K., Specht, K., and Shah, N. J. (2000). Cortical activations during paced finger-tapping applying visual and auditory pacing stimuli. Cogn. Brain Res. 10, 51–66. doi: 10.1016/S0926-6410(00)00022-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jantzen, K. J., Steinberg, F. L., and Kelso, J. A. S. (2005). Functional MRI reveals the existence of modality and coordination-dependent timing networks. Neuroimage 25, 1031–1042. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.12.029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jönsson, E. G., Nöthen, M. M., Grünhage, F., Farde, L., Nakashima, Y., Propping, P., et al. (1999). Polymorphisms in the dopamine D2 receptor gene and their relationships to striatal dopamine receptor density of healthy volunteers. Mol. Psychiatr. 4, 290–296. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4000532

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kleiner, M., Brainard, D., Pelli, D., Ingling, A., Murray, R., and Broussard, C. (2007). What’s new in psychtoolbox-3. Perception 36, 1–16.

Google Scholar

Koelsch, S. (2014). Brain correlates of music-evoked emotions. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 15, 170–180. doi: 10.1038/nrn3666

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Konoike, N., Kotozaki, Y., Miyachi, S., Miyauchi, C. M., Yomogida, Y., Akimoto, Y., et al. (2012). Rhythm information represented in the fronto-parieto-cerebellar motor system. Neuroimage 63, 328–338. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.07.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koshimori, Y., and Thaut, M. (2018). Future perspectives on neural mechanisms underlying rhythm and music based neurorehabilitation in Parkinson’s disease. Ageing Res. Rev. 47, 133–139. doi: 10.1016/j.arr.2018.07.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Large, E. W., Fink, P., and Kelso, J. A. S. (2002). Tracking simple and complex sequences. Psychol. Res. 66, 3–17. doi: 10.1007/s004260100069

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lewis, P. A., Wing, A. M., Pope, P. A., Praamstra, P., and Mirall, R. C. (2004). Brain activity correlates differently with increasing temporal complexity of thythms during initialisation, synchronisation, and continuation phases of paced finger tapping. Neuropsychologia 42, 1301–1312. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.03.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martin-Soelch, C., Szczepanik, J., Nugent, A., Barhaghi, K., Rallis, D., Herscovitch, P., et al. (2011). Lateralization and gender differences in the dopaminergic response to unpredictable reward in the human ventral striatum. Eur. J. Neurosci. 33, 1706–1715. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2011.07642.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Massie, C., Malcolm, M. P., Greene, D., and Thaut, M. (2009). The effects of constraint-induced therapy on kinematic outcomes and compensatory movement patterns: an exploratory study. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 90, 571–579. doi: 10.1016/j.apmr.2008.09.574

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McIntosh, G. C., Brown, S. H., Rice, R. R., and Thaut, M. H. (1997). Rhythmic auditory-motor facilitation of gait patterns in patients with parkinson’s disease. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatr. 62, 22–26. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.62.1.22

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Menon, V., and Levitin, D. J. (2005). The rewards of music listening: response and physiological connectivity of the mesolimbic system. Neuroimage 28, 175–184. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.05.053

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Michon, J. A., and van der Valk, N. J. (1967). A dynamic model of timing behavior. Acta Psychol. 27, 204–212. doi: 10.1016/0001-6918(67)90061-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miller, N., Kwak, Y., Bohnen, N., Müller, M. L., Dayalu, P., and Seidler, R. D. (2013). The pattern of striatal dopaminergic denervation explains sensorimotor synchronization accuracy in parkinson’s disease. Behav. Brain Res. 257, 100–110. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2013.09.032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miller, R. A., Thaut, M. H., McIntosh, G. C., and Rice, R. R. (1996). Components of EMG symmetry and variability in parkinsonian and healthy elderly gait. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. Electromyogr. Mot. Control 101, 1–7. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(95)00209-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mizrahi, R., Kenk, M., Suridjan, I., Boileau, I., George, T. P., McKenzie, K., et al. (2014). Stress-induced dopamine response in subjects at clinical high risk for schizophrenia with and without concurrent cannabis use. Neuropsychopharmacology 39, 1479–1489. doi: 10.1038/npp.2013.347

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Molinari, M., Leggio, M. G., Filippini, V., Gioia, M. C., Cerasa, A., and Thaut, M. H. (2005). Sensorimotor transduction of time information is preserved in subjects with cerebellar damage. Brain Res. Bull. 67, 448–458. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2005.07.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Molochnikov, I., and Cohen, D. (2014). Hemispheric differences in the mesostriatal dopaminergic system. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 8:110. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2014.00110

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mueller, K., Fritz, T., Mildner, T., Richter, M., Schulze, K., Lepsien, J., et al. (2015). Investigating the dynamics of the brain response to music: a central role of the ventral striatum/nucleus accumbens. Neuroimage 116, 68–79. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.05.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Narendran, R., Slifstein, M., Guillin, O., Hwang, Y., Hwang, D. R., Scher, E., et al. (2006). Dopamine (D2/3) receptor agonist positron emission tomography radiotracer [11C]-(+)-PHNO is a D3 receptor preferring agonist in vivo. Synapse 60, 485–495. doi: 10.1002/syn.20325

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nozaradan, S., Peretz, I., Missal, M., and Mouraux, A. (2011). Tagging the neuronal entrainment to beat and meter. J. Neurosci. 31, 10234–10240. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0411-11.2011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oldfield, R. C. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness: the edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia 9, 97–113. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(71)90067-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paltsev, E. I., and E-lner, A. M. (1967). Change in the functional state of the segmental apparatus of the spinal cord under the influence of acoustic stimuli and its role in bringing about an arbitray movement. Biofizika 12, 1064–1070.

Pelli, D. G. (1997). The videotoolbox software for visual psychophysics: tranforming numbers into movies. Spa. Vis. 10, 437–442. doi: 10.1163/156856897X00366

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rao, S. M., Harrington, D. L., Haaland, K. Y., Bobholz, J. A., Cox, R. W., and Binder, J. R. (1997). Distributed neural systems underlying the timing of movements. J. Neurosci. 17, 5528–5535. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.17-14-05528.1997

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Repp, B. H. (2005). Sensorimotor synchronization: a review of the tapping literature. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 12, 969–992. doi: 10.3758/BF03206433

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Richard, J. M., Castro, D. C., DiFeliceantonio, A. G., Robinson, M. J. F., and Berridge, K. C. (2013). Mapping brain circuits of reward and motivation: In the footsteps of ann kelley. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 37, 1919–1931. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.12.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rochester, L., Rafferty, D., Dotchin, C., Msuya, O., Minde, V., and Walker, R. W. (2010). The effect of cueing therapy on single and dual-task gait in a drug naïve population of people with parkinson’s disease in northern tanzania. Mov. Disord. 25, 906–911. doi: 10.1002/mds.22978

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rossignol, S., and Jones, G. M. (1976). Audio-spinal influence in man studied by the H-reflex and its possible role on rhythmic movements synchronized to sound. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 41, 83–92. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(76)90217-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rusjan, P., Mamo, D., Ginovart, N., Hussey, D., Vitcu, I., Yasuno, F., et al. (2006). An automated method for the extraction of regional data from PET images. Psychiatr. Res. Neuroimag. 147, 79–89. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2006.01.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Saga, Y., Hoshi, E., and Tremblay, L. (2017). Roles of multiple globus pallidus territories of monkeys and humans in motivation, cognition and action: an anatomical, physiological and pathophysiological review. Front. Neuroanat. 11:30. doi: 10.3389/fnana.2017.00030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Salimpoor, V. N., Benovoy, M., Larcher, K., Dagher, A., and Zatorre, R. J. (2011). Anatomically distinct dopamine release during anticipation and experience of peak emotion to music. Nat. Neurosci. 14, 257–262. doi: 10.1038/nn.2726

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Salimpoor, V. N., van den Bosch, I., Kovacevic, N., McIntosh, A. R., and Dagher, A. Z. R. (2013). Interactions between the nucleus accumbens and auditory cortices predict music reward value. Science 340, 216–219. doi: 10.1126/science.1231059

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmahmann, J., and Pandya, D. (2006). Fiber Pathways of the Brain. Oxford: Oxford University Press. doi: 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195104233.001.0001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seidler, R. D., Bernard, J. A., Burutolu, T. B., Fling, B. W., Gordon, M. T., Gwin, J. T., et al. (2010). Motor control and aging: links to age-related brain structural, functional, and biochemical effects. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 34, 721–733. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiorev.2009.10.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shelton, J., and Kumar, G. P. (2010). Comparison between auditory and visual simple reaction times. Neurosci. Med. 01, 30–32. doi: 10.4236/nm.2010.11004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spreng, N. R. (2012). The fallacy of a “task-negative” network. Front. Psychol. 3:145. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00145

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stephan, K. M., Thaut, M. H., Wunderlich, G., Schicks, W., Tian, B., Tellmann, L., et al. (2002). Conscious and subconscious sensorimotor synchronization–prefrontal cortex and the influence of awareness. Neuroimage 15, 345–352. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0929

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Teghil, A., Boccia, M., D’Antonio, F., Di Vita, A., de Lena, C., and Guariglia, C. (2018). Neural substrates of internally-based and externally-cued timging: an activation likelihood estimation (ALE) meta-analysis of fMRI studies. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 96, 197–209. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.10.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thaut, M. (2003). Neural basis of rhythmic timing networks in the human brain. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 999, 364–373. doi: 10.1196/annals.1284.044

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thaut, M. H. (2015). The discovery of human auditory-motor entrainment and its role in the development of neurologic music therapy. Prog. Brain Res. 217, 253–266. doi: 10.1016/bs.pbr.2014.11.030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thaut, M. H., Demartin, M., and Sanes, J. N. (2008). Brain networks for integrative rhythm formation. PloS One 3:e2312. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0002312

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thaut, M. H., and Kenyon, G. P. (2003). Rapid motor adaptations to subliminal frequency shifts during syncopated rhythmic sensorimotor synchronization. Hum. Mov. Sci. 22, 321–338. doi: 10.1016/S0167-9457(03)00048-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thaut, M. H., Kenyon, G. P., Hurt, C. P., McIntosh, G. C., and Hoemberg, V. (2002). Kinematic optimization of spatiotemporal patterns in paretic arm training with stroke patients. Neuropsychologia 40, 1073–1081. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(01)00141-145

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thaut, M. H., McIntosh, G. C., and Rice, R. R. (1997). Rhthmic facilitation of gait training in hemiparetic stroke rehabilitation. J. Neurol. Sci. 151, 207–212. doi: 10.1016/S0022-510X(97)00146-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thaut, M. H., McIntosh, G. C., Rice, R. R., Miller, R. A., Rathbun, J., and Brault, J. M. (1996). Rhythmic auditory stimulation in gait training for parkinson’s disease patients. Mov. Disord. 11, 193–200. doi: 10.1002/mds.870110213

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thaut, M. H., Miller, R. A., and Schauer, L. M. (1998a). Multiple synchronization strategies in rhythmic sensorimotor tasks: phase vs period correction. Biol. Cybern. 79, 241–250. doi: 10.1007/s004220050474

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thaut, M. H., Tian, B., and Azimi-Sadjadi, M. R. (1998b). Rhythmic finger tapping to cosine-wave modulated metronome sequences: evidence of subliminal entrainment. Hum. Mov. Sci. 17, 839–863. doi: 10.1016/S0167-9457(98)00031-31

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thaut, M. H., Stephan, K. M., Wunderlich, G., Schicks, W., Tellmann, L., Herzog, H., et al. (2009). Distinct cortico-cerebellar activations in rhythmic auditory motor synchronization. Cortex 45, 44–53. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2007.09.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Toyomura, A., Shibata, M., and Kuriki, S. (2012). Self-paced and externally triggered rhythmical lower limb movements: a functional MRI study. Neurosci. Lett. 516, 39–44. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2012.03.049

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wessel, K., Zeffiro, T., Toro, C., and Hallett, M. (1997). Self-paced versus metronome-paced finger movements. J. Neuroimaging 7, 145–151. doi: 10.1111/jon199773145

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wiener, M., Lee, Y. S., Lohoff, F. W., and Coslett, H. B. (2014). Individual differences in the morphometry and activation of time perception networks are influenced by dopamine genotype. Neuroimage 89, 10–22. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.11.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Willeit, M., Ginovart, N., Kapur, S., Houle, S., Hussey, D., Seeman, P., et al. (2006). High-affinity states of human brain dopamine D2/3 receptors imaged by the agonist [11C]-(+)-PHNO. Biol. Psychiatr. 59, 389–394. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.09.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wilson, A. A., McCormick, P., Kapur, S., Willeit, M., Garcia, A., Hussey, D., et al. (2005). Radiosynthesis and evaluation of [11C]-(+)-4-propyl-3,4,4a,5,6, 10b-hexahydro-2H-naphtho[1,2-b][1,4]oxazin-9-ol as a potential radiotracer for in vivo imaging of the dopamine D2 high-affinity state with positron emission tomography. J. Med. Chem. 48, 4153–4160. doi: 10.1021/jm050155n

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yang, Y. K., Chiu, N. T., Chen, C. C., Chen, M., Yeh, T., and Lee, I. H. (2003). Correlation between fine motor activity and striatal dopamine D2 receptor density in patients with schizophrenia and healthy controls. Psychiatr. Res. Neuroimag. 123, 191–197. doi: 10.1016/S0925-4927(03)00066-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zatorre, R. J., Chen, J. L., and Penhune, V. B. (2007). When the brain plays music: auditory–motor interactions in music perception and production. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 8, 547–558. doi: 10.1038/nrn2152

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, S., Hu, S., Chao, H. H., and Li, C. R. (2017). Hemispheric lateralization of resting-state functional connectivity of the ventral striatum: an exploratory study. Brain Struct. Funct. 222, 2573–2583. doi: 10.1007/s00429-016-1358-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: finger tapping, rhythmic auditory stimulation, D2/3 receptors, dopamine, PET, [11C]-(+)-PHNO, auditory-motor entrainment, basal ganglia

Citation: Koshimori Y, Strafella AP, Valli M, Sharma V, Cho S-s, Houle S and Thaut MH (2019) Motor Synchronization to Rhythmic Auditory Stimulation (RAS) Attenuates Dopaminergic Responses in Ventral Striatum in Young Healthy Adults: [11C]-(+)-PHNO PET Study. Front. Neurosci. 13:106. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2019.00106

Received: 27 September 2018; Accepted: 29 January 2019;
Published: 14 February 2019.

Edited by:

Simone Dalla Bella, Université de Montréal, Canada

Reviewed by:

Dan Zhang, Tsinghua University, China
Patrick K. A. Neff, University of Regensburg, Germany

Copyright © 2019 Koshimori, Strafella, Valli, Sharma, Cho, Houle and Thaut. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Yuko Koshimori, eXVrby5rb3NoaW1vcmlAbWFpbC51dG9yb250by5jYQ==

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.