Skip to main content

PERSPECTIVE article

Front. Neurosci., 14 August 2018
Sec. Brain Imaging Methods
This article is part of the Research Topic Advances in Multi-Scale Analysis of Brain Complexity View all 14 articles

Developmental Trajectory of Infant Brain Signal Variability: A Longitudinal Pilot Study

  • 1Research Center for Child Mental Development, Kanazawa University, Kanazawa, Japan
  • 2Health Administration Center, University of Fukui, Fukui, Japan
  • 3Faculty of Education, Kanazawa University, Kanazawa, Japan
  • 4Department of Computer Science, Chiba Institute of Technology, Narashino, Japan

The infant brain shows rapid neural network development that considerably influences cognitive and behavioral abilities in later life. Reportedly, this neural development process can be indexed by estimating neural signal complexity. However, the precise developmental trajectory of brain signal complexity during infancy remains elusive. This study was conducted to ascertain the trajectory of magnetoencephalography (MEG) signal complexity from 2 months to 3 years of age in five infants using multiscale entropy (MSE), which captures signal complexity at multiple temporal scales. Analyses revealed scale-dependent developmental trajectories. Specifically, signal complexity predominantly increased from 5 to 15 months of age at higher temporal scales, whereas the complexity at lower temporal scales was constant across age, except in one infant who showed decreased complexity. Despite a small sample size limiting this study’s power, this is the first report of a longitudinal investigation of changes in brain signal complexity during early infancy and is unique in its application of MSE analysis of longitudinal MEG data during infancy. The results of this pilot study may serve to further our understanding of the longitudinal changes in the neural dynamics of the developing infant brain.

Introduction

Infancy is a period of remarkable neural development in the brain that is reflected by increasing cognitive and behavioral capacities for external circumstances or internal changes in later life (Cao et al., 2017). Recent advances in neuroimaging devices and analysis techniques have been used to visualize the development of brain functions. The human brain is a complex system that is characterized by its astonishing signal variability, which operates over a wide range of temporal and spatial scales. This brain signal variability facilitates learning and optimal environmental adaptation to the changing demands of a dynamic environment (Faisal et al., 2008). This complexity also conveys important information about neural system dynamics and their alterations (reviewed in Stam, 2005; Garrett et al., 2013; Takahashi, 2013).

An entropy-based approach, multiscale entropy (MSE) analysis, has been proposed to estimate the physiological signal complexity on multiple temporal scales using coarse-graining procedures (Costa et al., 2005). This extension to multiple time scales enables the capture of long-range temporal correlations in a time series. MSE has been successfully applied in the investigation of developmental changes in brain signal complexity from infancy through adolescence and into adulthood (McIntosh et al., 2008; Lippe et al., 2009; Polizzotto et al., 2015; Takahashi et al., 2016). However, no study has explored the longitudinal changes in brain signal complexity during the early stages of development despite the significant importance of examining within-subject developmental trajectories (Giedd et al., 1999; Sowell et al., 2004; Shaw et al., 2008). This is due to the large variance in the developmental pattern during infancy (Landa et al., 2012), a period in which developmental disorders frequently emerge (Bolton et al., 2012; Lemcke et al., 2013).

We characterized the trajectory of brain signal complexity of typically developing infants, aged 5 to 36 months, using MSE applied to MEG. MEG is suited for measuring the infant brain because it offers a non-invasive and quiet environment during measurement. Additionally, MEG allows the mother to accompany the infant to provide encouragement and comfort, as well as enabling her to decide whether the experiment should be paused or continued. Furthermore, in the assessment of signal complexity, MEG can directly measure brain magnetic fields in the cortex with high temporal resolution (Kikuchi et al., 2011; Yoshimura et al., 2012; Takahashi et al., 2016).

Methods

Data for the present study were obtained from an ongoing longitudinal study of infants. In this study, we analyzed five infants (one female and four males) who were 36 months of age at the time of analysis. They were recruited from Kanazawa University at 1 month old, and follow-up examinations and MEG experiments were conducted once a month (ideally every month). Participants had no history of developmental problems at the time of the latest measurement.

All mothers agreed to their infant’s participation in the study and had full knowledge of the experimental nature of the research. Written informed consent was obtained prior to participation. The study was approved by the Ethics Committee of the Kanazawa University Hospital, and all procedures were performed in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki.

Experimental Procedure

Magnetoencephalography data were recorded using a 151-channel Superconducting Quantum Interference Device (SQUID) whole-head coaxial gradiometer MEG system for children (PQ 1151 R; Yokogawa/KIT, Kanazawa, Japan) installed at the MEG Center of Ricoh Company, Ltd. (Kanazawa, Japan). During recording, the participant lay supine on a bed in a magnetically shielded room (Daido Steel, Nagoya, Japan) with his or her head inside the MEG system helmet. The infant’s mother and one research member remained in the shielded room to keep the infant comfortable and encourage the infant to maintain a steady body position when necessary. The infants were carefully monitored using a video monitoring system to assess their compliance with the instructions and to record any notable artifacts, such as head motion, inappropriate head position. Before recording, infants or their mother selected a video program according to their preference from a number of video programs (e.g., popular Japanese animations and TV programs). All infants viewed silent video programs projected onto a screen throughout the recording session to promote a consistent state and attention. MEG recordings were conducted every month when possible.

Data Analysis

Magnetic fields were sampled at 2000 Hz per channel (bandpass filter 0.16–200 Hz). Offline analysis was performed using a BrainVision Analyzer 2 (Brain Products GmbH, Gilching, Germany) and MATLAB (the MathWorks Inc., Natick, MA, United States). The raw MEG data were resampled at 500 Hz with 1.5–60-Hz bandpass and 60-Hz notch filters. MEG data were segmented for 5 s (2500 data points: 5 s × 500 Hz). Artifacts such as eye movements, blinks, cardiac activities, and muscle activities were visually identified and excluded from analyses. The children’s head movements were video monitored throughout the session. At the epoch selection stage, clear head motion artifacts were eliminated by confirmation of head motion in the videos at the time of the MEG artifacts by an MEG expert who was blinded to the identity of the subjects. Contaminated data were also eliminated by an MEG expert who was blinded to the identity of the subjects. A minimum of 50 segments were recorded for each subject. Finally, we randomly selected 50 segments (i.e., a 250 s recording period) from all artifact-free segments of each recording. For each subject, MSE values were calculated separately for each of the selected segments and were then averaged into a single value as the mean MSE.

MSE Analysis

Multiscale entropy analysis quantifies the complexity of a time series using different time scales (Costa et al., 2002). For the extension to multiple time scales, the original MEG time series {x1, x2, …, xN} is coarse-grained to {y1(τ), y2(τ), …, yN/τ(τ)} by the temporal scale τ with non-overlapping windows as follows.

yj(τ)=(1/τ)i=(j1)τ+1jτxi,1jN/τ.

The complexity of each scale can be measured through the calculation of sample entropy (SampEn), which assesses the predictability of a time series. The SampEn was calculated for each series {y1(τ), y2(τ), …, yN/τ(τ)}. The SampEn is the negative of the logarithmic conditional probability that two sequences of m consecutive data points that are mutually similar (within a given tolerance r) will remain similar at the next point (m + 1) in the dataset (N), where m is the space of the dimension and r is the effective filter for measuring the consistency of a time series (Richman and Moorman, 2000). Considering the MEG time series {x1, x2, …, xN} as observations of a stochastic variable x, the dynamic SampEn is defined as

hsample (r, m, N)=loge [Cm+1 (r)/Cm (r)],

where Cm (r) = {number of pairs (i, j) with |zimzjm | < r, ij}/{number of all probable pairs, i.e., (Nm + 1) (Nm)}. Therein, z = y (τ); zm is a vector of an m sample time series of (Nm) length, and |zimzjm| denotes the distance between points zim and zjm. In this study, we used m = 2 and r = 0.2. SampEn values were computed for 1–20 scales that correspond to 2–40 ms (Temporal scales in ms = tau 1000 ms/sampling frequency).

Power Spectral Analysis

Along with MSE calculations, spectral power analysis was performed for each epoch that was used for the MSE calculation as a comparative MSE analysis. We calculated the spectral density (amplitude) using a fast Fourier transform. A Hamming window was applied to each epoch for spectral power analysis.

Surrogate Analysis

We derived surrogate data using a Fourier transformation to the MEG data to detect non-linearity in the MEG data (Vakorin and McIntosh, 2012; Grandy et al., 2016). Specifically, the time-series of each epoch was Fourier transformed, and then its phase was randomized and applied to an inverse Fourier transform. Using 10 types of seeds for randomization, we derived 10 surrogate data per epoch and then calculated an average value among their SampEn values of surrogate data. We compared the SampEn values for the original time series to the SampEn values for the surrogate data.

Results

Figure 1 shows the averaged (across all sensors into a single value) developmental trajectory of the spectral power (Figure 1, upper panels) and MSE (Figure 1, lower panels) across five infants aged from 5 to 36 months old. All infants demonstrated an increase in the MSE value with age. The Jonckheere-Terpstra test was used to test for an age-related trend in MSE values, and statistically significant age-related trends were identified for coarse time scales (31–40 ms, scales: 16–20) (TJT = 459.0, standard error = 34.8, z = 5.6, p < 0.001). Figure 2 shows the averaged developmental trajectory in each time scale bin (Figure 2A) and the topography of MSE values across different ages (Figure 2B). A more detailed examination of our results revealed that the remarkable increase in MSE identified for longer time scales (31–40 ms, scale: 16–20) was predominantly observed at ages up to 15 months and was found across brain regions (Figure 2). After 15 months of age, this increase tended to slow. However, the power spectral analysis also showed an increase in power in the theta and alpha bands. This increase was more prominent after 15 months of age, while the MSE change was more prominent during the earlier infancy periods. Regarding the shorter time scales (2–10 ms, scale: 1–5), the developmental trajectory of MSE varied across subjects. For instance, some infants showed constant MSE values across development, whereas one infant showed a gradual decrease (Figure 1, bottom panels).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 1. Array plot showing the developmental trajectory of spectral power (top panels) and MSE (bottom panels) for each infant and their average (X-axis, age, months; Y-axis, frequency and time scales).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 2. (A) Each line shows the trajectory of the averaged MSE value across different time scales in 8 age bins (1–5, 6–10, 11–15, 16–20, 21–25, 26–30, 31–35, and 36–40 months of age). (B) Topography of the MSE value at a coarse time scale of 40 ms (scale = 20) across 7 age bins (5, 10, 15, 20, 25, 30, and 35 months of age).

In the surrogate analysis, we found region- and scale-specific entropy alterations in the surrogate data, which may suggest an inherent non-linearity in the MEG data (data not shown). Specifically, in the surrogate data, the SampEn increased near the frontal and temporo-occipital regions. Interestingly, this region-specific SampEn alteration was more prominent for smaller temporal scales (less than 20 ms) and was frequently identified during early infancy (5–10 months of age).

Discussion

The neurodevelopmental trajectory of infancy has received much attention because infancy is a critical period of brain development in which cognitive and behavioral abilities are enhanced (Cao et al., 2017) and neurodevelopmental disorders, such as autism spectrum disorder (ASD), are predicted to develop. This is the first longitudinal investigation of how brain signal complexity, which represents neural system dynamics, changes during infancy. The analysis revealed scale-dependent developmental trajectories of MEG signal complexity. Specifically, we found an increase in signal complexity for longer time scales, whereas the changes in complexity varied across infants for shorter time scales.

Many studies have investigated age-related signal complexity changes from late childhood into adulthood. Polizzotto et al. (2015) examined the MSE of resting-state EEG results in healthy subjects aged 8–22 years old. They reported an age-related increase in entropy in lower scales and a decrease in entropy for higher scales. McIntosh et al. (2008) calculated MSE changes in EEG during a face recognition visual memory task in children (8–15 years old) and young adults (20–33 years old). They found an age-related increase in EEG complexity that was significantly correlated with the accuracy of task performance. This observation was replicated by the same group using MEG (Misic et al., 2010), confirming the characteristic shape of the MSE curve and its prominent task-dependent increase during development. We have also demonstrated an age-related increase in MEG signal complexity. However, enhanced complexity was identified in children with ASD, particularly in earlier childhood (Takahashi et al., 2016). Compared to the changes that occur during the period from childhood to adolescence, brain signal complexity during infancy has been addressed by few studies. Lippe et al. (2009) investigated EEG signal complexity in response to visual and auditory stimulation in children ranging from 1 month to 5 years of age. They found a task-dependent increase in EEG complexity with aging. However, these studies were based on a cross-sectional study design. Despite the small number of subjects, a unique aspect of this study is that we longitudinally investigated the development of MEG complexity during infancy.

In these contexts, our study provides the longitudinal underpinnings for the concept of significant shifts in brain signal complexity with aging (Garrett et al., 2013). Notably, for higher scales, we captured a robust developmental MSE profile across infants and across MEG sessions despite conditional inconsistencies (i.e., selected videos, emotions, or physical conditions), which may indicate the potential usefulness of MSE as a reliable and clinically useful trait biomarker of the infant brain. For instance, we have demonstrated a linear age-related increase in complexity at higher scales across 40–110 month-old children (Takahashi et al., 2016). Additionally, enhanced brain signal variability was observed in children with ASD, which was conformed for younger children. On the other hand, Bosl et al. (2011) examined resting-state EEG complexity by MSE in typically developing infants and infants with a high risk of ASD across the ages of 6–24 months, and they found consistently lower EEG complexity at higher scales in the high-risk group, particularly at 9–12 months of age. These inconsistent results may be attributed to the different age ranges of these two previous studies on children with ASD.

Considering biological background, the observed rapid increase in MEG complexity in the high time scale (i.e., lower frequency range) at approximately 5–15 months old might demonstrate the development of long-range network-related cognitive processing. Given that long-range communication between multiple brain areas is driven by slow waves (i.e., theta and beta waves) (Wang, 2010), MEG complexity in a high time scale (i.e., lower frequency range) may be useful and a non-invasive biomarker of brain maturation in infants. Pujol et al. (2006) assessed myelination from birth to 3 years of age in children’s brains using three-dimensional MRI imaging. Intriguingly, this volumetric study demonstrated that a period of rapid myelination started after the 5th month and reached the mature appearance by the 18th month, and the study revealed the relationship with vocabulary acquisition in children. This period of rapid myelination is almost the same as the period in which we observed a rapid change in the present study.

However, contrary to the developmental trajectory for higher scales, the developmental trajectory of MSE for lower scales is diverse across infants, and the reason for this difference remains unclear. Lippe et al. (2009) reported a rapid increase in complexity at lower scales, especially during the early stage of infancy (1–2 months old vs. 2–8 months old) that is followed by a gradual increase. This may suggest the possibility that complexity at lower scales (corresponding to ≤16 ms) saturates by 8 months of age. This may partially explain our finding of a constant complexity value across age after 5 months of age at lower scales. Theoretically, SampEn at finer (i.e., lower) time scales is based on wider frequencies, whereas coarser (i.e., higher) time scales are based on narrower frequencies (i.e., high frequency is filtered out). Signal variabilities in different frequencies must be reflected by differences in time scale. Therefore, a frequency-specific role in the differentiation of cognitive processing (Fries, 2015) and differences in maturational speed (Uhlhaas et al., 2009) may underlie these contradictory findings between the results from high and low time scales.

Surrogate analysis showed a region- and scale-specific increase in surrogate data compared to that in MSE from original data, which may suggest an inherent non-linearity in the MEG data. Furthermore, the developmental trajectory of the spectral power and MSE differed. Specifically, an increase in the power spectral seemed to be prominent after 12 months of age, whereas an increase in MSE emerged from early infancy until 15 months of age. Therefore, we assume that the enhancement in MSE with development may be associated with non-linear processes and may be independent of spectral power. In addition, as the outputs of neuronal networks are produced by interactions due to both local dense interconnectivity and sparse long-range excitatory projections (Friston et al., 1995), the resulting dynamics could be expected to operate at multiple scales.

Some potential limitations of the present study must be considered. First, despite frequent MEG recording, we were only able to follow five infants, which precluded statistical evaluation. Second, we did not correct for cognitive behavioral or psychological assessments, which might strengthen our claims. Third, the confounding influence of head motion cannot be excluded from potentially influencing the MSE results. Finally, as a technical consideration, the recent advent of cortical source localization techniques was not applied due to difficulties in performing MRI on infants. Although several limitations must be considered, our findings for the examination of MEG signal variability using MSE may add another dimension to the previously identified neural dynamics of development and may provide useful biomarkers for typically and abnormally developing brains.

Author Contributions

YY, YM, and MK designed the study. CH, YY, and HK recruited the participants. CH, YY, TI, and DS performed the experiments. CH, SN, and TT analyzed the results and wrote the manuscript. All authors participated in revising the manuscript and approved the final draft of the manuscript.

Funding

This work was supported by JSPS KAKENHI Grant Number JP15K19717 to CH and JP16K10206 to TT and was partially supported by the Center of Innovation Program from the Japan Science and Technology Agency, JST and JST CREST Grant Number JPMJCR17A4, Japan.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

The reviewer C-KP and the handling Editor declared their shared affiliation.

Acknowledgments

We wish to thank the study participants and their families. We also thank Sachiko Kitagawa for technical assistance and Wasin Hawaree from Fukui University of Technology for data analysis.

Abbreviations

MEG, magnetoencephalography.

References

Bolton, P. F., Golding, J., Emond, A., and Steer, C. D. (2012). Autism spectrum disorder and autistic traits in the avon longitudinal study of parents and children: precursors and early signs. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 51, 249 e25–260 e25. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2011.12.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bosl, W., Tierney, A., Tager-Flusberg, H., and Nelson, C. (2011). EEG complexity as a biomarker for autism spectrum disorder risk. BMC Med. 9:18. doi: 10.1186/1741-7015-9-18

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cao, M., Huang, H., and He, Y. (2017). Developmental connectomics from infancy through early childhood. Trends Neurosci. 40, 494–506. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2017.06.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Costa, M., Goldberger, A. L., and Peng, C. K. (2002). Multiscale entropy analysis of complex physiologic time series. Phys. Rev. Lett. 89:068102. doi: 10.1103/PhysRevLett.89.068102

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Costa, M., Goldberger, A. L., and Peng, C. K. (2005). Multiscale entropy analysis of biological signals. Phys. Rev. E Stat. Nonlin. Soft Matter Phys. 71(2 Pt 1):021906. doi: 10.1103/PhysRevE.71.021906

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Faisal, A. A., Selen, L. P., and Wolpert, D. M. (2008). Noise in the nervous system. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 9, 292–303. doi: 10.1038/nrn2258

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fries, P. (2015). Rhythms for cognition: communication through coherence. Neuron 88, 220–235. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.09.034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Friston, K. J., Tononi, G., Sporns, O., and Edelman, G. M. (1995). Characterising the complexity of neuronal interactions. Hum. Brain Mapp. 3, 302–314. doi: 10.1002/hbm.460030405

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Garrett, D. D., Samanez-Larkin, G. R., MacDonald, S. W., Lindenberger, U., McIntosh, A. R., and Grady, C. L. (2013). Moment-to-moment brain signal variability: a next frontier in human brain mapping? Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 37, 610–624. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.02.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Giedd, J. N., Blumenthal, J., Jeffries, N. O., Castellanos, F. X., Liu, H., Zijdenbos, A., et al. (1999). Brain development during childhood and adolescence: a longitudinal MRI study. Nat. Neurosci. 2, 861–863. doi: 10.1038/13158

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grandy, T. H., Garrett, D. D., Schmiedek, F., and Werkle-Bergner, M. (2016). On the estimation of brain signal entropy from sparse neuroimaging data. Sci. Rep. 6:23073. doi: 10.1038/srep23073

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kikuchi, M., Shitamichi, K., Yoshimura, Y., Ueno, S., Remijn, G. B., Hirosawa, T., et al. (2011). Lateralized theta wave connectivity and language performance in 2- to 5-year-old children. J. Neurosci. 31, 14984–14988. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2785-11.2011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Landa, R. J., Gross, A. L., Stuart, E. A., and Bauman, M. (2012). Latent class analysis of early developmental trajectory in baby siblings of children with autism. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 53, 986–996. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2012.02558.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lemcke, S., Juul, S., Parner, E. T., Lauritsen, M. B., and Thorsen, P. (2013). Early signs of autism in toddlers: a follow-up study in the danish national birth cohort. J. Autism. Dev. Disord. 43, 2366–2375. doi: 10.1007/s10803-013-1785-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lippe, S., Kovacevic, N., and McIntosh, A. R. (2009). Differential maturation of brain signal complexity in the human auditory and visual system. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 3:48. doi: 10.3389/neuro.09.048.2009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McIntosh, A. R., Kovacevic, N., and Itier, R. J. (2008). Increased brain signal variability accompanies lower behavioral variability in development. PLoS Comput. Biol. 4:e1000106. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000106

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Misic, B., Mills, T., Taylor, M. J., and McIntosh, A. R. (2010). Brain noise is task dependent and region specific. J. Neurophysiol. 104, 2667–2676. doi: 10.1152/jn.00648.2010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Polizzotto, N., Takahashi, T., Walker, C., and Cho, R. (2015). Wide range multiscale entropy changes through development. Entropy 18:12. doi: 10.3390/e18010012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pujol, J., Soriano-Mas, C., Ortiz, H., Sebastián-Gallés, N., Losilla, J. M., and Deus, J. (2006). Myelination of language-related areas in the developing brain. Neurology 66, 339–343. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000201049.66073.8d

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Richman, J. S., and Moorman, J. R. (2000). Physiological time-series analysis using approximate entropy and sample entropy. Am. J. Physiol. Heart Circ. Physiol. 278, H2039–H2049. doi: 10.1152/ajpheart.2000.278.6.H2039

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shaw, P., Kabani, N. J., Lerch, J. P., Eckstrand, K., Lenroot, R., Gogtay, N., et al. (2008). Neurodevelopmental trajectories of the human cerebral cortex. J. Neurosci. 28, 3586–3594. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5309-07.2008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sowell, E. R., Thompson, P. M., Leonard, C. M., Welcome, S. E., Kan, E., and Toga, A. W. (2004). Longitudinal mapping of cortical thickness and brain growth in normal children. J. Neurosci. 24, 8223–8231. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.1798-04.2004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stam, C. J. (2005). Nonlinear dynamical analysis of EEG and MEG: review of an emerging field. Clin. Neurophysiol. 116, 2266–2301. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2005.06.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Takahashi, T. (2013). Complexity of spontaneous brain activity in mental disorders. Prog. Neuropsychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 45, 258–266. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2012.05.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Takahashi, T., Yoshimura, Y., Hiraishi, H., Hasegawa, C., Munesue, T., Higashida, H., et al. (2016). Enhanced brain signal variability in children with autism spectrum disorder during early childhood. Hum. Brain Mapp. 37, 1038–1050. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23089

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Uhlhaas, P. J., Roux, F., Singer, W., Haenschel, C., Sireteanu, R., and Rodriguez, E. (2009). The development of neural synchrony reflects late maturation and restructuring of functional networks in humans. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 106, 9866–9871. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0900390106

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vakorin, V. A., and McIntosh, A. R. (2012). “Mapping the multi-scale information content of complex brain signals,” in Principles of Brain Dynamics: Global State Interactions, eds M. Rabinovich, K. Friston, and P. Varona (Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press), 183–208.

Google Scholar

Wang, X. J. (2010). Neurophysiological and computational principles of cortical rhythms in cognition. Physiol. Rev. 90, 1195–1268. doi: 10.1152/physrev.00035.2008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yoshimura, Y., Kikuchi, M., Shitamichi, K., Ueno, S., Remijn, G. B., Haruta, Y., et al. (2012). Language performance and auditory evoked fields in 2- to 5-year-old children. Eur. J. Neurosci. 35, 644–650. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2012.07998.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: infant development, magnetoencephalography (MEG), multiscale entropy, complexity, longitudinal change

Citation: Hasegawa C, Takahashi T, Yoshimura Y, Nobukawa S, Ikeda T, Saito DN, Kumazaki H, Minabe Y and Kikuchi M (2018) Developmental Trajectory of Infant Brain Signal Variability: A Longitudinal Pilot Study. Front. Neurosci. 12:566. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2018.00566

Received: 21 November 2017; Accepted: 27 July 2018;
Published: 14 August 2018.

Edited by:

Albert Yang, Harvard Medical School, United States

Reviewed by:

Vasily Vakorin, Simon Fraser University, Canada
Chung-Kang Peng, Harvard Medical School, United States
Shih-Jen Tsai, Taipei Veterans General Hospital, Taiwan

Copyright © 2018 Hasegawa, Takahashi, Yoshimura, Nobukawa, Ikeda, Saito, Kumazaki, Minabe and Kikuchi. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Tetsuya Takahashi, takahash@u-fukui.ac.jp

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.