Skip to main content

GENERAL COMMENTARY article

Front. Neurosci., 18 July 2017
Sec. Decision Neuroscience

Commentary: Winning a competition predicts dishonest behavior

  • 1Research Center for Brain Function and Psychological Science, Shenzhen University, Shenzhen, China
  • 2Center for Brain and Cognitive Sciences and School of Psychological and Cognitive Sciences, Peking University, Beijing, China
  • 3Department of Psychology, Centre for Gambling Research at UBC, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada

A commentary on
Winning a competition predicts dishonest behavior

by Schurr, A., and Ritov, I. (2016). Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 113, 1754–1759. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1515102113

Honesty is a crucial component in human cooperation, yet a large proportion of individuals choose to be dishonest on a daily basis (DePaulo et al., 1996). In fact, dishonesty is a pervasive problem that plagues society on both the global level (i.e., tax evasion leads to $3.1 trillion loss for governments annually; Tax Justice Network, 2011) and the local level (i.e., 32% of people are willing to lie on simple gumball tasks to increase their profit; Djawadi and Fahr, 2015).

Over the past several decades, researchers from economics (Gneezy, 2005; Charness and Dufwenberg, 2006), psychology (Mazar et al., 2008), and social neuroscience (Baumgartner et al., 2009; Zhu et al., 2014; Garrett et al., 2016) have been fascinated with understanding why people behave dishonestly. Classic economic theory suggests that people behave dishonestly when the benefits of lying outweigh the costs of getting caught (Becker, 1968), whereas an abundance of research suggests that certain psychological factors, such as perceived social class, can increase one's tendency to be dishonest (Piff et al., 2012). One proposed mechanism for this increase in dishonesty is a tendency for high-class individuals to be more oriented toward greed.

Recent findings by Schurr and Ritov (2016) demonstrated that winners were more likely to behave dishonestly following a social competition, either in a rigged laboratory task or even after recalling a past competitive victory. The effect was only reliable when the victory involved out-performing a competitor, and not when the win was determined by chance (on a lottery) or in reference to a personal goal (meeting a challenge). Schurr and Ritov's experiments ruled out the possibility that competence might explain their effects. The proposed mechanism, according to Schurr and Ritov, is that winning a competition increases feelings of entitlement, leading to subsequent dishonest behavior. Schurr and Ritov's experiments are well-designed and well-controlled, and we were impressed by their ingenious assay for dishonesty, based on the group deviation from chance on a dice-rolling game that allowed only the participant to cheat. However, their final experiment did not report the key test of mediation needed to support their hypothesis about entitlement. Indeed, their data provide no direct support for the role of entitlement in cheating after competitive wins.

The authors acknowledge that a variety of mechanisms could be at work, given the complex psychological nature of social competitions. We believe one strong candidate explanation, not acknowledged by Schurr and Ritov, is an increase in testosterone following competitive wins. Testosterone is one of the major sex steroids that plays a large role in human social interaction (see Eisenegger et al., 2011 for a review). The Biosocial Model of Status (BMS) posits that testosterone levels fluctuate as a function of competitive outcomes, such that winners show increased testosterone levels compared to losers (Booth et al., 1989). These endocrine changes have adaptive consequences for subsequent contests: wins enhance social status, and the testosterone rise facilitates competitive, aggressive behaviors needed to defend one's new position. For losers, the outcome signals a drop in social status, and a testosterone decrease may promote submissive behaviors that prevent physical harm or further loss of status. These effects were first observed in athletic competition and field data, and were further corroborated by using rigged laboratory tasks (Carré and Olmstead, 2015; Hamilton et al., 2015). Recent research suggests that testosterone promotes behaviors intended to maintain and seek social status rather than simply inducing aggressive behavior (Eisenegger et al., 2011). For example, testosterone potentiates aggressive responses to provocation, while it increases generosity in the absence of provocation in a modified Ultimatum Game (Dreher et al., 2016).

In Study 1, Schurr and Ritov adopted a conceptually similar rationale to the BMS, and the results were fully consistent with the predictions of the BMS. In Study 2, the authors replicated the effect by asking participants to recall an experience of winning a competition. Prior research has shown that the recall of a competitive victory elicited by watching a video clip of the win is sufficient to increase testosterone (Carré and Putnam, 2010). In Study 3, the critical role of social comparison was highlighted by showing that chance wins or achieving a personal goal did not induce dishonesty. Likewise, the outcome of a random lottery draw has no discernible influence on testosterone levels (Mazur and Lamb, 1980), and the tendency to attribute the success to external factors (i.e., luck and chance) buffered individuals' testosterone increases (Gonzalez-Bono et al., 1999, 2000). Given the role of testosterone in human social behavior, i.e., cheating (Lee et al., 2015), future experiments could fruitfully test the mediatory role of testosterone on dishonest behavior, which could help us to better understand the psychobiological precipitants of financial crises (Coates and Herbert, 2008) and have implications for future possibility of treating and/or preventing antisocial behavior.

Author Contributions

All authors listed have made a substantial, direct and intellectual contribution to the work, and approved it for publication.

Funding

YW is supported by National Natural Science Foundation of China (31600923).

Conflict of Interest Statement

LC is the Director of the Centre for Gambling Research at UBC, which is supported by funding from the British Columbia Lottery Corporation and the Province of British Columbia.

The other authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Baumgartner, T., Fischbacher, U., Feierabend, A., Lutz, K., and Fehr, E. (2009). The neural circuitry of a broken promise. Neuron 64, 756–770. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.11.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Becker, G. S. (1968). Crime and punishment: an economic approach. J. Polit. Econ. 76, 169–217. doi: 10.1086/259394

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Booth, A., Shelley, G., Mazur, A., Tharp, G., and Kittok, R. (1989). Testosterone, and winning and losing in human competition. Horm. Behav. 23, 556–571. doi: 10.1016/0018-506X(89)90042-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carré, J. M., and Olmstead, N. (2015). Social neuroendocrinology of human aggression: examining the role of competition-induced testosterone dynamics. Neuroscience 286, 171–186. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2014.11.029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carré, J. M., and Putnam, S. K. (2010). Watching a previous victory produces an increase in testosterone among elite hockey players. Psychoneuroendocrinology 35, 475–479. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2009.09.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Charness, G., and Dufwenberg, M. (2006). Promises and partnership. Econometrica 74, 1579–1601. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-0262.2006.00719.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coates, J. M., and Herbert, J. (2008). Endogenous steroids and financial risk taking on a London trading floor. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 105, 6167–6172. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0704025105

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

DePaulo, B. M., Kashy, D. A., Kirkendol, S. E., Wyer, M. M., and Epstein, J. A. (1996). Lying in everyday life. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 70:979. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.70.5.979

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Djawadi, B. M., and Fahr, R. (2015). “…and they are really lying”: clean evidence on the pervasiveness of cheating in professional contexts from a field experiment. J. Econ. Psychol. 48, 48–59. doi: 10.1016/j.joep.2015.03.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dreher, J., Dunne, S., Pazderska, A., Frodl, T., Nolan, J. J., and O'Doherty, J. P. (2016). Testosterone causes both prosocial and antisocial status-enhancing behaviors in human males. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 113, 11633–11638. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1608085113

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eisenegger, C., Haushofer, J., and Fehr, E. (2011). The role of testosterone in social interaction. Trends Cogn. Sci. 15, 263–271. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.04.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Garrett, N., Lazzaro, S. C., Ariely, D., and Sharot, T. (2016). The brain adapts to dishonesty. Nat. Neurosci. 19, 1727–1732. doi: 10.1038/nn.4426

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gneezy, U. (2005). Deception: The role of consequences. Am. Econ. Rev. 95, 384–394. doi: 10.1257/0002828053828662

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gonzalez-Bono, E., Salvador, A., Ricarte, J., Serrano, M. A., and Arnedo, M. (2000). Testosterone and attribution of successful competition. Aggress. Behav. 26, 235–240. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1098-2337(2000)26:3<235::AID-AB3>3.0.CO;2-L

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gonzalez-Bono, E., Salvador, A., Serrano, M. A., and Ricarte, J. (1999). Testosterone, cortisol, and mood in a sports team competition. Horm. Behav. 35, 55–62. doi: 10.1006/hbeh.1998.1496

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hamilton, L. D., Carré, J. M., Mehta, P. H., Olmstead, N., and Whitaker, J. D. (2015). Social neuroendocrinology of status: a review and future directions. Adapt. Hum. Behav. Physiol. 1, 202–230. doi: 10.1007/s40750-015-0025-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, J. J., Gino, F., Jin, E. S., Rice, L. K., and Josephs, R. A. (2015). Hormones and ethics: understanding the biological basis of unethical conduct. J. Exp. Psychol. 144, 891–897. doi: 10.1037/xge0000099

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mazar, N., Amir, O., and Ariely, D. (2008). The dishonesty of honest people: a theory of self-concept maintenance. J. Market. Res. 45, 633–644. doi: 10.1509/jmkr.45.6.633

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mazur, A., and Lamb, T. A. (1980). Testosterone, status, and mood in human males. Horm. Behav. 14, 236–246. doi: 10.1016/0018-506X(80)90032-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Piff, P. K., Stancato, D. M., Côté, S., Mendoza-Denton, R., and Keltner, D. (2012). Higher social class predicts increased unethical behavior. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 109, 4086–4091. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1118373109

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schurr, A., and Ritov, I. (2016). Winning a competition predicts dishonest behavior. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 113, 1754–1759. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1515102113

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tax Justice Network (2011). The Cost of Tax Abuse: A Briefing Paper on the Cost of Tax Evasion Worldwide. Available online at: http://www.tackletaxhavens.com/Cost_of_Tax_Abuse_TJN%20Research_23rd_Nov_2011.pdf (Accessed January 2, 2017).

Zhu, L., Jenkins, A. C., Set, E., Scabini, D., Knight, R. T., Chiu, P. H., et al. (2014). Damage to dorsolateral prefrontal cortex affects tradeoffs between honesty and self-interest. Nat. Neurosci. 17, 1319–1321. doi: 10.1038/nn.3798

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: competition, testosterone, dishonesty, decision making, behavioral economics

Citation: Wu Y, Blue PR and Clark L (2017) Commentary: Winning a competition predicts dishonest behavior. Front. Neurosci. 11:417. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2017.00417

Received: 20 April 2017; Accepted: 04 July 2017;
Published: 18 July 2017.

Edited by:

Bernd Weber, University of Bonn, Germany

Reviewed by:

Carlos Cueva, University of Alicante, Spain

Copyright © 2017 Wu, Blue and Clark. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Yin Wu, yinwu0407@gmail.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.