Skip to main content

GENERAL COMMENTARY article

Front. Neurosci., 24 August 2015
Sec. Neurogenesis

Rewiring, forgetting and learning. Commentary: A critical period for experience-dependent remodeling of adult-born neuron connectivity

  • Cell Biology Department, Faculty of Sciences, National Autonomous University of Mexico, Mexico city, Mexico

A commentary on
A critical period for experience-dependent remodeling of adult-born neuron connectivity

by Bergami, M., Masserdotti, G., Temprana, S. G., Motori, E., Eriksson, T.M., Gobel, J., et al. (2015). Neuron 85, 710-717. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.01.001

Thirty years after the discovery that neurons are generated in the adult brain (Altman and Das, 1965), studies of the link between hippocampal neurogenesis and behavior revealed that exercise and environmental enrichment are highly effective at maximizing the proliferation and survival of adult-born neurons (Van Praag et al., 1999b; Bruel-Jungerman et al., 2005). Depending on the neurogenesis markers used, enriched animals exhibit a 50–200% increase in the number of new neurons compared with animals housed in standard home cages. Based on the premise that these new cells are important for cognition, several experiments show that exercise or environmental enrichment results in noticeable improvements in hippocampal-dependent task performance (Van Praag et al., 1999a; Van der Borght et al., 2007; Creer et al., 2010). Furthermore, the depletion of adult-born neurons with antimitotic drug treatment or focal radiation blocks the effect of environmental enrichment on memory (Bruel-Jungerman et al., 2005; Meshi et al., 2006). However, in nearly all of these studies, enrichment occurred before learning but not after learning. In fact, this experimental design is routinely used in the functional study of neurogenesis, not only with enrichment but also with other manipulations.

However, two in vivo (Feng et al., 2001; Akers et al., 2014) and one in silico (Weisz and Argibay, 2012) experiment show that when environmental enrichment occurs after learning, previously-acquired memories are forgotten. These three studies show that as a consequence of more cells added to the hippocampal circuit, memory retention suffers. This seemingly counterintuitive result can be explained by a detailed analysis of the morphological changes driven by running and enrichment and their effect on neuronal connectivity. Specifically, newly generated axon terminals can juxtapose with or displace previously established synapses (Akers et al., 2014), thus degrading pre-existing memory circuits and resulting in forgetting.

By taking advantage of the rabies virus to retrogradely infect monosynaptic neurons (Wickersham et al., 2007), a new study by Bergami et al. (2015) shows that this neurogenesis-induced rewiring occurs more extensively than previously thought. The authors created a retrovirus that tags both newly generated neurons and all neurons that have synaptic contacts with these new neurons with a TVA receptor, allowing tagged neurons to be labeled with permanently expressed GFP. They then counted the number of retroinfected cells within the dentate gyrus, the entire hippocampus, and other areas of the brain. They found that the number of cells that made synaptic contact with newborn hippocampal neurons was higher after environmental enrichment. Newly generated neurons in enriched mice received more innervation from cells in the dentate gyrus, as well as increased innervation from populations of inhibitory neurons in the hippocampus. Furthermore, newborn neurons in enriched mice received more innervation from the entorhinal cortex, mammillary bodies, and septal nuclei.

Previous studies show that newly formed synapses between adult-generated dentate gyrus neurons and CA3 pyramidal neurons can compete with or even displace previously-established synapses (See references in Deng et al., 2010). The mechanism by which new synapses juxtapose with or displace new synapses is unknown. However, assuming that the displaced synapses critically support a pre-existing memory circuit, such competition and displacement could explain why neurogenesis destabilizes previously acquired memories and leads to forgetting (Frankland et al., 2013; Akers et al., 2014). If we consider that forgetting can be adaptive in an ever-changing environment, we can conclude that neurogenesis is a process that actively supports the disruption of memory in response to environmental stimulation.

Bergami et al. (2015) show that new neurons generated by environmental enrichment are eager to form new synapses, not only within the canonical DG-CA3 circuit but also with more distant regions of the brain involved in cognition. Therefore, it is likely that this neurogenesis-induced restructuring of brain circuitry would not only induce forgetting of pre-existing memories but also enhance the formation of new memories. In a natural environment, therefore, the hippocampus may constantly be generating new memories while at the same time constantly erasing memories that are not exported and consolidated in cortical modules, as suggested by Kitamura and Inokuchi (2014). This constant rewiring is costly and complex and thus may only be active when there is a strong cognitive demand. Bergami et al. (2015) show that when such cognitive demand exists due to environmental enrichment, a hippocampal neurogenesis-induced rewiring program predominates and even modifies the way in which hippocampal neurons communicate with cells in other structures. Although there is a need for further studies to elucidate how hippocampal neurogenesis impacts distant brain areas, adult-generated neurons appear to coordinate a dynamic rewiring of memory circuits that include several forebrain areas.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The author declares that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

The author acknowledges the kind comments and revision on this manuscript by Dr. Katherine Akers.

References

Akers, K. G., Martinez-Canabal, A., Restivo, L., Yiu, A. P., De Cristofaro, A., Hsiang, H. L., et al. (2014). Hippocampal neurogenesis regulates forgetting during adulthood and infancy. Science 344, 598–602. doi: 10.1126/science.1248903

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Altman, J., and Das, G. D. (1965). Autoradiographic and histological evidence of postnatal hippocampal neurogenesis in rats. J. Comp. Neurol. 124, 319–335. doi: 10.1002/cne.901240303

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bergami, M., Masserdotti, G., Temprana, S. G., Motori, E., Eriksson, T. M., Göbel, J., et al. (2015). A critical period for experience-dependent remodeling of adult-born neuron connectivity. Neuron 85, 710–717. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.01.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bruel-Jungerman, E., Laroche, S., and Rampon, C. (2005). New neurons in the dentate gyrus are involved in the expression of enhanced long-term memory following environmental enrichment. Eur. J. Neurosci. 21, 513–521. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2005.03875.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Creer, D. J., Romberg, C., Saksida, L. M., Van Praag, H., and Bussey, T. J. (2010). Running enhances spatial pattern separation in mice. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 107, 2367–2372. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0911725107

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Deng, W., Aimone, J. B., and Gage, F. H. (2010). New neurons and new memories: how does adult hippocampal neurogenesis affect learning and memory? Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 11, 339–350. doi: 10.1038/nrn2822

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Feng, R., Rampon, C., Tang, Y. P., Shrom, D., Jin, J., Kyin, M., et al. (2001). Deficient neurogenesis in forebrain-specific presenilin-1 knockout mice is associated with reduced clearance of hippocampal memory traces. Neuron 32, 911–926. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(01)00523-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frankland, P. W., Köhler, S., and Josselyn, S. A. (2013). Hippocampal neurogenesis and forgetting. Trends Neurosci. 36, 497–503. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2013.05.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kitamura, T., and Inokuchi, K. (2014). Role of adult neurogenesis in hippocampal-cortical memory consolidation. Mol. Brain 7:13. doi: 10.1186/1756-6606-7-13

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meshi, D., Drew, M. R., Saxe, M., Ansorge, M. S., David, D., Santarelli, L., et al. (2006). Hippocampal neurogenesis is not required for behavioral effects of environmental enrichment. Nat. Neurosci. 9, 729–731. doi: 10.1038/nn1696

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van der Borght, K., Havekes, R., Bos, T., Eggen, B. J., and Van der Zee, E. A. (2007). Exercise improves memory acquisition and retrieval in the Y-maze task: relationship with hippocampal neurogenesis. Behav. Neurosci. 121, 324–334. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.121.2.324

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Praag, H., Christie, B. R., Sejnowski, T. J., and Gage, F. H. (1999a). Running enhances neurogenesis, learning, and long-term potentiation in mice. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 96, 13427–13431. doi: 10.1073/pnas.96.23.13427

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Praag, H., Kempermann, G., and Gage, F. H. (1999b). Running increases cell proliferation and neurogenesis in the adult mouse dentate gyrus. Nat. Neurosci. 2, 266–270. doi: 10.1038/6368

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weisz, V. I., and Argibay, P. F. (2012). Neurogenesis interferes with the retrieval of remote memories: forgetting in neurocomputational terms. Cognition 125, 13–25. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2012.07.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wickersham, I. R., Lyon, D. C., Barnard, R. J., Mori, T., Finke, S., Conzelmann, K. K., et al. (2007). Monosynaptic restriction of transsynaptic tracing from single, genetically targeted neurons. Neuron 53, 639–647. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.01.033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: neurogenesis, hippocampus and memory, rabies virus, forgetting, enrichment

Citation: Martínez-Canabal A (2015) Rewiring, forgetting and learning. Commentary: A critical period for experience-dependent remodeling of adult-born neuron connectivity. Front. Neurosci. 9:298. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2015.00298

Received: 23 June 2015; Accepted: 06 August 2015;
Published: 24 August 2015.

Edited by:

Grigori Enikolopov, Stony Brook University, USA

Reviewed by:

Alexey Terskikh, Sanford-Burnham Medical Research Institute, USA

Copyright © 2015 Martínez-Canabal. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Alonso Martínez-Canabal, acanabal@ifc.unam.mx

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.