Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Neurol., 10 April 2024
Sec. Neurotrauma
This article is part of the Research Topic Assessment and Treatment Interventions for Traumatic Brain Injury View all 13 articles

Color discrimination in fixed saturation level of patients with acute traumatic injury

Leonardo R. Nicolau da Costa,Leonardo R. Nicolau da Costa1,2Joyce B. SousaJoyce B. Sousa3Felipe Andr C. BritoFelipe André C. Brito1Yuzo IgarashiYuzo Igarashi4Janildes Maria Silva GomesJanildes Maria Silva Gomes4Carlos Augusto LoboCarlos Augusto Lobão1Marcelo Fernandes CostaMarcelo Fernandes Costa5Leticia MiquiliniLeticia Miquilini6Givago Silva Souza,
Givago Silva Souza1,4*
  • 1Instituto de Ciências Biológicas, Universidade Federal do Pará, Belém, Brazil
  • 2Hospital Metropolitano de Urgência e Emergência, Belém, Brazil
  • 3Centro de Ciências Biológicas e da Saúde, Universidade do Estado do Pará, Belém, Brazil
  • 4Núcleo de Medicina Tropical, Universidade Federal do Pará, Belém, Brazil
  • 5Instituto de Psicologia, Universidade de São Paulo, São Paulo, Brazil
  • 6Núcleo de Teoria e Pesquisa do Comportamento, Universidade Federal do Pará, Belém, Brazil

Introduction: Traumatic brain injury (TBI) is an important public health concern and that may lead to severe neural sequels, such as color vision deficits.

Methods: We evaluated the color vision of 10 TBI patients with normal cognitive function using a color discrimination test in a fixed saturation level. We also analyzed computerized tomography scans to identify the local of the brain damages.

Results: Four TBI patients that had lesions in brain areas of the ventral visual streams, five TBI patients had lesions inferred in brain areas of the dorsal visual stream, and one TBI patient had lesion in the occipital area. All the patients had cognitive and color vision screened and they had characterized the chromatic discrimination at high and low saturation. All participants had no significant cognitive impairment in the moment of the color vision test. Additionally, they had perfect performance for discrimination of chromatic stimulus at high saturation and similar to controls (n = 37 age-matched participants). Three of four TBI patients with lesions in the ventral brain and one patient with lesion in the occipital area had impairment of the chromatic discrimination at low saturation. All TBI patients with lesions in the dorsal brain had performance similar or slightly worse than the controls.

Conclusion: Chromatic discrimination at low saturation was associated to visual damage in the ventral region of the brain and is a potential tool for functional evaluation of brain damage in TBI patients.

Introduction

Traumatic brain injury (TBI) is worldwide public health concern and is the main cause of mortality among different head traumas (1). Survivors of this injury develop permanent neurological sequelae that dramatically affect the quality of life (2). Although cognitive, motor, and sensory deficits are among the main consequences of TBI (3), visual impairments are also a relatively common occurrence (4). TBI can affect visual function through a direct lesion in cortical areas or in pathways that connect the eyes to the brain (5). Visual functions usually affected by TBI include oculomotor dysfunction, visual acuity impairment, and visual field defects (6). Color vision deficits have been reported to be a sequela of TBI (79).

There is a hypothesis regarding the functioning of cortical processing of visual information in which the primary visual cortex receives visual input from the retina and the lateral geniculate nucleus, segregating it into two cortical processing pathways (1012). One destination for the output information from the primary visual cortex would be the lower regions of the temporal lobe, which exhibit biased processing in object properties enabling conscious perception and recognition (13, 14). This pathway is known as the ventral pathway of the visual system. Another destination from the primary visual cortex would be regions located in the posterior parietal lobe, whose processing is biased towards information related to the spatial location of the object and mediates visually guided motor actions (15, 16). This pathway is known as the dorsal pathway of the visual system. Figure 1 shows schematic illustration, depicting location of presumed ventral and dorsal visual streams.

Figure 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. A schematic illustration, depicting location of presumed ventral and dorsal streams of the visual pathway.

Several studies have documented that color information is mainly processed by the ventral stream of the visual system (1719), with important contributions from the ventral occipitotemporal cortex and posterior fusiform gyrus (9, 20, 21). In the cortex, color signals are processed along the ventral pathway from V1–V2 to V4, and Inferior Temporal (TE) region (22, 23). Within each of these areas, studies have shown clustering of color-selective regions (24). However, some investigations have shown that color information plays an important role in a cortical region of the dorsal stream of the visual system (19) and that both visual streams interact and influence one another (10).

Studies have reported that damage to localized regions of the cerebral cortex can lead to cerebral achromatopsia, without significant impairment to other visual aspects (25, 26). Total or partial achromatopsia have been reported following ventral visual system disturbances (27). Patients with TBI experience direct damage to several brain areas and are potentially at risk for developing some degree of cerebral achromatopsia.

Many visual tests have been used to identify cerebral dyschromatopsia (27) such as color naming test, Ishihara plates, and Farnsworth-Munsell 100-hue test (26, 2830). Shin et al. (31) and Igarashi et al. (32) reported how the use of chromatic discrimination in fixed saturation stimuli can be an alternative for rapid screening of chromatic perceptual losses. In this approach, mosaic arrangements are shown with a target and a background that can only be discriminated based on the chromatic saturation difference between them. Our hypothesis is that patients with TBI located in areas associated with the ventral visual pathway would make a greater number of target identification errors compared to patients with injuries located in cortical areas associated with the dorsal visual pathway. The present investigation reports a case series of chromatic discrimination at fixed saturation levels in 10 patients in the acute stage of TBI.

Methods

Subjects

The sample consisted of 10 TBI patients who attended the Urgency and Emergency Metropolitan Hospital of Belém, Pará, Brazil, a reference hospital for TBI cases. All patients in the present study had no visual complaints before the trauma they sustained or during the period of visual evaluation, and all had visual acuity of at least 20/40. Patient demographic information and the corresponding causes of TBI are summarized in Table 1. Thirty-seven control age-matched participants were recruited (27.8 years old ±7.4), and all had normal fundus and best-corrected visual acuity to 20/20.

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Demographic features of the TBI patients and information of the TBI history of each patient.

All participants provided informed written consent to participate in the present investigation, and all the procedures were approved by the Ethics Committee of the Tropical Medicine Center of the Federal University of Pará (report #2436948).

All TBI patients underwent two screening evaluations using the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) and Ishihara test, and a functional description of their chromatic discrimination using a chromatic discrimination task in fixed-saturation condition. All subjects were binocularly tested. All patients were examined at the time of hospital discharge. The authors also had access to computed tomography (CT) scans of the brain to localize brain area(s) that were potentially damaged.

MMSE

The cognitive function of the patients was evaluated using the MMSE (33). The examination comprised 10 tasks to evaluate spatiotemporal orientation, registration, attention, calculation, recall, language, reading, repetition, writing, and visuomotor skills. The test score ranged between 0 (worst performance) and 30 (best performance). We considered four cut-off levels to classify the severity of cognitive impairment: score > 24 for no cognitive impairment; score between 19–23 for mild cognitive impairment; score between 10–18 for moderate cognitive impairment; and score ≤ 9 for severe cognitive impairment.

Ishihara test

The 2016 book version of the Ishihara test with 14 plates was used to screen for problems suggestive of congenital red-green color vision deficiency. Each plate was shown for 3 s under natural daylight. The participants’ task was to indicate the number displayed in the pseudoisochromatic plate. Eight or more errors were considered to represent an altered result (suggestive of dichromacy).

Chromatic discrimination in fixed saturation level

A chromatic discrimination test followed previous published study (32). The test was programmed in MATLAB environment language (R2017a, Mathworks, Natick, MA, United States). A 15′′ liquid crystal display was used (color resolution, 8 bits per gun; spatial resolution, 1,366 × 768 pixels; temporal resolution, 60 Hz). The display was gamma calibrated using a colorimeter (CS-100A, Konica Minolta, Osaka, Japan).

The test consisted of a pseudoisochromatic stimulus sequence composed of a mosaic of circles randomly distributed across the display (9.7° × 5.4° of visual angle). Luminance noise was applied in the mosaic, in which 6 values of luminance were linearly distributed between 5 and 25 cd/m2. A set of circles with chromaticity different from the mosaic field (reference chromaticity: CIE 1976, u’ = 0.219; v’ = 0.48) composing a squared target (1.5° of visual angle).

The participants’ task was to identify where the target was in four alternative positions (up, bottom, left, or right). The test consisted of two stages: pre-test (10 trials); and test (80 trials). In the first 10 presentations (pre-test stage), the chromatic vector of the target chromaticity was 0.07 u’v’ units in the CIE1976 color diagram (high saturated chromaticities) and the 10 chromatic axes between 0 and 342 degrees were chosen randomly to paint the target at each trial. It was avoided that axes on the color confusion lines were chosen. These presentations (2 s duration) were used to evaluate whether the patient understood the commands to be tested. The criterion to follow to the next stage of the test was perfect performance in the high saturated colors stage.

In the second stage of the test, the target chromaticity was shown in 20 chromatic axes (0,18, 36, 54, 72, 90, 108, 126, 144, 162, 180, 198, 216, 234, 252, 270, 288, 306, 324, 342, as shown in Figure 1) in the CIE1976 color diagram, chromatic vector of 0.03 u’v’ units in the CIE1976 color diagram, and four trials of each of the chromatic axes were performed the. Each presentation of the stimulus had 2 s duration and it was interleaved by a dark screen for 1.5 s. The performance of the task was quantified by the number of errors in the identification of the correct target position. In perfect performance, the error value is 0 and, in the worse performance, the error value is 80. Test duration was about 6–7 min.

Representative examples of the stimulus in the first and second stages of the test are shown in Figure 2.

Figure 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Visual stimulation used in the first stage (A) and second stage (B) of the chromatic discrimination test. The chromaticity of the target was displayed along 20 chromatic axes spaced at 18-degree intervals in the CIE-1976 color space (C). Yellow line represents the titran confusion line, Red line represents the protan confusion line, and Green line represents the deutan confusion line.

CT scan evaluation

Conventional CT scans of the head were evaluated by an experienced neurosurgeon to identify the location of brain damage.

Data analysis

To find the cut-off value of the chromatic discrimination performance, we fitted Poisson distribution to the distribution of the number of errors of the control using least mean square method. We considered the 99% percentile in the best fitted binomial function to the data distribution as the cut-off for normal color vision (Normal color vision ≤3 errors, Altered color vision >3 errors). The performance of each TBI patient was compared to this cut-off to be qualified as normal or altered.

Results

Cognitive evaluation

Patient scores on the MMSE ranged between 27 and 30, while all controls demonstrated the maximum score (score = 30) in the examination. TBI patients exhibited more difficulty in executing the visuomotor skills task. The MMSE score of each patient and the task in which they exhibited impaired execution is shown in Table 2 leftmost columns.

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Results of the MMSE for each TBI patient and summary of the results obtained from the different evaluations.

Ishihara test results

All patients and controls demonstrated perfect performance in the Ishihara test (Table 2).

Brain imaging results

Five patients exhibited lesions in the brain regions belonging to the dorsal visual system (left occipitoparietal region, left parietal region, right frontal region, and frontoparietal region), while four patients exhibited damage in brain areas of the ventral visual system (right frontotemporal region, right temporal region, left frontotemporal region, left temporal region, and right frontotemporal region). One patient had a lesion in the left occipital region, which probably affected both visual streams. The location of brain damage in each patient is shown in Table 2 rightmost columns. CT images of 4 patients are shown in Figure 3.

Figure 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Computerized tomography images from 4 TBI patients. (A) Patient P6 had left temporal lesion. (B) Patient P7 had frontotemporal lesion. (C) Patient P1 had left occipitoparietal lesion. (D) Patient P3 had right parietal lesion. White lines represent the visible border of the brain damaged area.

Chromatic discrimination in fixed-saturation

All patients and controls showed perfect performance on the first stage of the high-saturation color discrimination test, indicating that they all understood how to perform the task. In the second stage of the test, in which the stimuli with low saturation were presented, the performance of healthy participants ranged from 0 to 4 errors, while the performance of traumatic brain injury patients ranged from 1 to 52, as seen in Table 2.

Patients with lesions in cortical areas related to the dorsal pathway exhibited performances of 1, 2 (2 participants), 4, and 5 errors, while patients with lesions in cortical areas associated with the ventral pathway showed performances of 1, 7, 45, and 52 errors. The sole participant with occipital lobe damage made 11 errors in the test.

Figure 4 shows the distribution of the control group (green bars) and the indication of the performance from each patient with inferred dorsal stream lesion (blue arrows), inferred ventral stream lesion (red arrows), and in with occipital lesion (gray arrow).

Figure 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Distribution of the errors in the chromatic discrimination test at low saturated stimulus (green bars). The performance of the patients with inferred dorsal brain lesions are indicated by blue arrows, while the performance of the patients with inferred ventral brain lesions are indicated by red arrows. The performance of the patient with occipital lesion is indicated by gray arrow.

All patients in whom the brain lesion was inferred from the dorsal visual system had errors in the normal range (P2, P3, and P10) or had altered vision with errors just above the cut-off for normal color vision (P1 and P9). Three out of four patients with lesions in the ventral visual stream had errors above the cut-off performance of the controls (P4, P6, and P7) and 1 of them had performance in the range of controls (P5). P6 and P7 had the worst performance among all TBI patients. P6 had a color vision loss especially around the deutan color confusion line and P7 had a diffuse color vision loss. The patient with an occipital lesion (P8) demonstrated more errors than the controls.

Discussion

Our main finding was that TBI patients with inferred lesions in the ventral visual system stream exhibited poor performance for chromatic discrimination in fixed-saturation than TBI patients with inferred brain lesion in the dorsal visual stream. Because color is mainly processed by the ventral visual stream, its functional impairment could be a non-invasive indicator of a specific lesion of this neural pathway.

We used a computer-controlled portable test that enabled us to screen several chromatic axes relatively quickly (6–7 min). We used a test that displayed pseudoisochromatic stimulus with fixed saturation at the target. In the first stage of our test, we used highly saturated colors to investigate severe impairment of color vision and the patients’ understanding of the psychophysical test what is in agreement with the normal results in Ishihara test. Although this stage of the test is comparable to the Ishihara test, we tested more than red-green vision. This is a first indication of partial color vision loss, since they can discriminate the target in high chromatic saturation. In the second stage of the test, we used a less saturated chromaticity (0.03 u’v’ units in the CIE 1976), which was approximately two times higher than the color discrimination thresholds for adults (34, 35). In this stage, we found that the patients demonstrated errors in different chromatic axes, similar to those observed in other investigations (27).

We consider that our results cannot be explained by a lack of patient comprehension of the test commands. The MMSE scores indicated that all patients had normal cognition (36) in the moment of the test execution, and all of them demonstrated perfect performance on the stage of the test that presented target with high saturated chromaticities (0.07 u’v’ units in the CIE1976 color diagram). We interpreted that errors in the stage of low saturated chromaticities (0.03 u’v’ units in the CIE1976 color diagram) represented an acquired loss of chromatic discrimination.

There is an important debate regarding the existence of a brain area that exclusively processes color information (3739). The ventral occipitotemporal cortex and posterior fusiform gyrus are brain areas where lesions commonly lead to disturbances of color vision in humans (21, 40). Patients P6 and P7 had the highest error rates in the color discrimination tasks, and their lesions involved the ventral region of the temporal cortex. Patient P8 had the third highest number of errors and had a lesion in the occipital cortex, with probably both streams affected. Patient P4 had the fourth highest number of errors and had a lesion in the right temporal cortex. One case (patient P5) with lesion in the temporal cortex demonstrated good performance in the chromatic discrimination task, and it could suggest that the lesion was possibly anterior to the ventral visual stream. Patients with lesions in the dorsal region of the brain (P1, P2, P3, P9, and P10) performed similarly or slightly worse than the control group.

The number of patients we studied was small and need to be increased to find stronger associations between the location of brain damage and color vision performance. Most similar studies encounter the same limitation; moreover, the study of cerebral achromatopsia is mainly based on case reports or meta-analyses of several different studies (20, 27). Another limitation was that because of the feasibility of application, we implemented the MMSE score as the sole basis to exclude or define cognitive impairment. MMSE is used in clinical setting, however, remains a screening tool rather than a systematic method to diagnose and precisely describe the cognitive impairment, and a much broader battery of tests (including analysis of verbal, executive and memory function, etc.) should be implemented to declare their patients as not cognitively impaired (41). Then, the designation of our patients as having normal cognition should be interpreted with caution.

Our findings suggest that color vision evaluation can be used to assist the diagnosis of functional damage in the ventral visual stream of patients with TBI. The chromatic discrimination test at fixed saturation can be administered quickly and may enable better comprehension of the individual condition of TBI patient’s brain.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by Ethical Committee of the Federal University of Pará. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study. Written informed consent was obtained from the individual(s) for the publication of any potentially identifiable images or data included in this article.

Author contributions

LN: Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Supervision, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. JS: Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Visualization, Writing – review & editing. FB: Formal analysis, Software, Writing – review & editing. YI: Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – review & editing. JG: Formal analysis, Investigation, Writing – review & editing. CL: Conceptualization, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – review & editing. MC: Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Supervision, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. LM: Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Supervision, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. GS: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Software, Supervision, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. This work was supported by grants from CNPq (#431748/2016-0). GSS and MFC are CNPq fellows.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

The author(s) declared that they were an editorial board member of Frontiers, at the time of submission. This had no impact on the peer review process and the final decision.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

1. Lasry, O, Liu, EY, Powell, GA, Ruel-Laliberté, J, Marcoux, J, and Buckeridge, DL. Epidemiology of recurrent traumatic brain injury in the general population: a systematic review. Neurology. (2017) 89:2198–209. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000004671

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Hawthorne, G, Kaye, AH, Gruen, R, Houseman, D, and Bauer, I. Traumatic brain injury and quality of life: initial Australian validation of the QOLIBRI. J Clin Neurosci. (2011) 18:197–202. doi: 10.1016/j.jocn.2010.06.015

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Arciniegas, DB, Held, K, and Wagner, P. Cognitive impairment following traumatic brain injury. Curr Treat Options Neurol. (2002) 4:43–57. doi: 10.1007/s11940-002-0004-6

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Sen, N. An insight into the vision impairment following traumatic brain injury. Neurochem Int. (2017) 111:103–7. doi: 10.1016/j.neuint.2017.01.019

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Ventura, RE, Balcer, LJ, and Galetta, SL. The neuro-ophthalmology of head trauma. Lancet Neurol. (2014) 13:1006–16. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(14)70111-5

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Brahm, KD, Wilgenburg, HM, Kirby, J, Ingalla, S, Chang, CY, and Goodrich, GL. Visual impairment and dysfunction in combat-injured servicemembers with traumatic brain injury. Optom Vis Sci. (2009) 86:817–25. doi: 10.1097/OPX.0b013e3181adff2d

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Young, RS, Fishman, GA, and Chen, F. Traumatically acquired color vision defect. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. (1980) 19:545–9.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

8. Short, RA, and Graff-Radford, NR. Localization of hemiachromatopsia. Neurocase. (2001) 7:331–7. doi: 10.1093/neucas/7.4.331

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Meadows, JC. Disturbed perception of colours associated with localized cerebral lesions. Brain. (1974) 97:615–32. doi: 10.1093/brain/97.1.615

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Milner, AD. How do the two visual streams interact with each other? Exp Brain Res. (2017) 235:1297–308. doi: 10.1007/s00221-017-4917-4

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Goodale, MA, and Milner, AD. Separate visual pathways for perception and action. Trends Neurosci. (1992) 15:20–5. doi: 10.1016/0166-2236(92)90344-8

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Milner, AD, and Goodale, MA. Two visual systems re-viewed. Neuropsychologia. (2008) 46:774–85. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.10.005

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Grill-Spector, K, Weiner, KS, Kay, K, and Gomez, J. The functional neuroanatomy of human face perception. Ann Rev Vision Sci. (2017) 3:167–96. doi: 10.1146/annurev-vision-102016-061214

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Zhou, Q, Du, J, Gao, R, Hu, S, Yu, T, Wang, Y, et al. Discriminative neural pathways for perception-cognition activity of color and face in the human brain. Cereb Cortex. (2023) 33:1972–84. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhac186

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Galletti, C, and Fattori, P. The dorsal visual stream revisited: stable circuits or dynamic pathways? Cortex. (2018) 98:203–17. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2017.01.009

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Cooper, SA, and O'Sullivan, M. Here, there and everywhere: higher visual function and the dorsal visual stream. Pract Neurol. (2016) 16:176–83. doi: 10.1136/practneurol-2015-001168

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Conway, BR. Color signals through dorsal and ventral visual pathways. Vis Neurosci. (2014) 31:197–209. doi: 10.1017/S0952523813000382

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Lafer-Sousa, R, Conway, BR, and Kanwisher, NG. Color-biased regions of the ventral visual pathway lie between face-and place-selective regions in humans, as in macaques. J Neurosci. (2016) 36:1682–97. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3164-15.2016

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Claeys, KG, Dupont, P, Cornette, L, Sunaert, S, Van Hecke, P, De Schutter, E, et al. Color discrimination involves ventral and dorsal stream visual areas. Cerebral cortex. (2004) 14:803–822. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhh040

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Beauchamp, MS, Haxby, JV, Jennings, JE, and DeYoe, EA. An fMRI version of the Farnsworth-Munsell 100-hue test reveals multiple color-selective areas in human ventral occipitotemporal cortex. Cereb Cortex. (1999) 9:257–63. doi: 10.1093/cercor/9.3.257

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Beauchamp, MS, Haxby, JV, Rosen, AC, and DeYoe, EA. A functional MRI case study of acquired cerebral dyschromatopsia. Neuropsychologia. (2000) 38:1170–9. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(00)00017-8

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Boynton, GM. Color vision: how the cortex represents color. Curr Biol. (2002) 12:R838–40. doi: 10.1016/S0960-9822(02)01347-7

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Chang, M, Xian, S, Rubin, J, and Moore, T. Latency of chromatic information in area V4. J Physiol Paris. (2014) 108:11–7. doi: 10.1016/j.jphysparis.2013.05.006

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Rentzeperis, I, Nikolaev, AR, Kiper, DC, and van Leeuwen, C. Distributed processing of color and form in the visual cortex. Front Psychol. (2014) 5:932. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00932

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Crognale, MA, Duncan, CS, Shoenhard, H, Peterson, DJ, and Berryhill, ME. The locus of color sensation: cortical color loss and the chromatic visual evoked potential. J Vis. (2013) 13:15. doi: 10.1167/13.10.15

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Heywood, CA, Wilson, B, and Cowey, A. A case study of cortical colour “blindness” with relatively intact achromatic discrimination. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. (1987) 50:22–9. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.50.1.22

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Bouvier, SE, and Engel, SA. Behavioral deficits and cortical damage loci in cerebral achromatopsia. Cereb Cortex. (2006) 16:183–91. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhi096

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Adachi-Usami, E, Tsukamoto, M, and Shimada, Y. Color vision and color pattern visual evoked cortical potentials in a patient with acquired cerebral dyschromatopsia. Doc Ophthalmol. (1995) 90:259–69. doi: 10.1007/BF01203861

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Green, GJ, and Lessell, S. Acquired cerebral dyschromatopsia. Arch Ophthalmol. (1977) 95:121–8. doi: 10.1001/archopht.1977.04450010121012

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Pearlman, AL, Birch, J, and Meadows, JC. Cerebral color blindness: an acquired defect in hue discrimination. Ann Neurol. (1979) 5:253–61. doi: 10.1002/ana.410050307

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Shin, YJ, Park, KH, Hwang, JM, Wee, WR, Lee, JH, Lee, IB, et al. A novel color vision test for detection of diabetic macular edema. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. (2014) 55:25–32. doi: 10.1167/iovs.13-11698

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Igarashi, Y, Meireles, LKG, Costa Brito, FA, da Costa, LRN, Dos Santos, FJ, Miquilini, L, et al. Chromatic discrimination in fixed saturation levels from trichromats and subjects with congenital color vision deficiency. Sci Rep. (2022) 12:5603. doi: 10.1038/s41598-022-09531-z

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Folstein, MF, Folstein, SE, and McHugh, PR. "Mini-mental state". A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res. (1975) 12:189–98. doi: 10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Paramei, GV, and Oakley, B. Variation of color discrimination across the life span. J Opt Soc Am A Opt Image Sci Vis. (2014) 31:A375–84. doi: 10.1364/JOSAA.31.00A375

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Paramei, GV. Color discrimination across four life decades assessed by the Cambridge colour test. J Opt Soc Am A Opt Image Sci Vis. (2012) 29:A290–7. doi: 10.1364/JOSAA.29.00A290

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Crum, RM, Anthony, JC, Bassett, SS, and Folstein, MF. Population-based norms for the Mini-mental status examination by age and educational level. JAMA. (1993) 269:2386–91. doi: 10.1001/jama.1993.03500180078038

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Chao, L, and Martin, A. Cortical regions associated with perceiving, naming, and knowing about colors. J Cogn Neurosci. (1999) 11:25–35. doi: 10.1162/089892999563229

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Zeki, S, Aglioti, S, Mckeefry, D, and Berlucchi, G. The neurological basis of conscious color perception in a blind patient. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. (1999) 96:14124–9. doi: 10.1073/pnas.96.24.14124

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Zeki, S, and Marini, L. Three cortical stages of colour processing in the human brain. Brain. (1998) 121:1669–85. doi: 10.1093/brain/121.9.1669

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Murphey, D, Yoshor, D, and Beauchamp, M. Perception matches selectivity in the human anterior color center. Curr Biol. (2008) 18:216–20. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2008.01.013

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Mitchell, AJ. A meta-analysis of the accuracy of the mini-mental state examination in the detection of dementia and mild cognitive impairment. J Psychiatr Res. (2009) 43:411–31. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2008.04.014

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: traumatic brain injuries, visual system, ventral stream, visual processing, color vision

Citation: Nicolau da Costa LR, Sousa JB, Brito FAC, Igarashi Y, Gomes JMS, Lobão CA, Costa MF, Miquilini L and Souza GS (2024) Color discrimination in fixed saturation level of patients with acute traumatic injury. Front. Neurol. 15:1363167. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2024.1363167

Received: 29 December 2023; Accepted: 02 April 2024;
Published: 10 April 2024.

Edited by:

Sarah C. Hellewell, Curtin University, Australia

Reviewed by:

Misha Vorobyev, The University of Auckland, New Zealand
Jacek Szczygielski, University of Rzeszow, Poland

Copyright © 2024 Nicolau da Costa, Sousa, Brito, Igarashi, Gomes, Lobão, Costa, Miquilini and Souza. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Givago Silva Souza, givagosouza@ufpa.br

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.