Skip to main content

SYSTEMATIC REVIEW article

Front. Neurol., 05 April 2024
Sec. Pediatric Neurology
This article is part of the Research Topic Prenatal diagnosis and follow up of children with CNS abnormalities diagnosed in uterus View all 3 articles

Could early life DHA supplementation benefit neurodevelopment? A systematic review and meta-analysis

  • Key Laboratory of Birth Defects and Related Diseases of Women and Children of MOE, Department of Pediatrics, West China Second University Hospital, Sichuan University, Chengdu, Sichuan, China

Background: Docosahexaenoic acid (DHA) plays a crucial role in the growth and functional development of the infant brain. However, the impact of additional DHA supplementation on neurodevelopment in infants remains controversial in randomized controlled trials. In this systematic review and meta-analysis, we aimed to investigate the effects of prenatal and postnatal DHA supplementation on neurodevelopment.

Methods: We systematically searched the MEDLINE, EMBASE, and Cochrane Library electronic databases using a predefined strategy until 8 February 2024. We extracted relevant study characteristics and outcomes related to the nervous system. Two independent reviewers critically evaluated the included studies to assess their validity and risk of bias.

Results: A total of 21 studies met our inclusion criteria, one study was removed after quality assessment, and the meta-analysis included 9 randomized controlled trials. The meta-analysis results indicated that there was no statistically significant difference between the DHA supplementation group and the placebo group, as assessed by the Mental Development Index [MDI; mean difference (MD), 0.41; 95% confidence interval (CI), −0.91 to 1.73; p = 0.55]. However, the DHA group had a significantly higher Psychomotor Development Index (PDI) than the placebo group (MD, 1.47; 95% CI, 0.23 to 2.72; p = 0.02). Subgroup analyses based on populations showed that DHA supplementation was superior to placebo for infants in both MDI (language score conversion; MD, 2.05; 95% CI, −0.16 to 4.26; p = 0.07) and PDI (MD, 1.94; 95% CI, 0.23 to 3.65; p = 0.03). Other subgroup analyses indicated no statistical differences between the two groups. The remaining assessments that could not be summarized quantitatively underwent a narrative evaluation.

Conclusion: Based on the BSID assessments, DHA supplementation in infants may have potential neurodevelopmental benefits. Because the meta-analysis included few high-quality articles and had some limitations, more relevant articles are needed to address the need for separate DHA supplementation in infants, pregnant women, and lactating mothers.

Systematic review registration: https://www.crd.york.ac.uk/prospero/display_record.php?ID=CRD42022348100, identifier: CRD42022348100.

Introduction

Sufficient nutrition supplementation during the prenatal and early postnatal phases plays a crucial role in facilitating programmed and optimal developmental processes. Conversely, malnutrition hinders the normal progression of development and increases the risk of metabolic or cardiovascular diseases later in life. These observations underscore the significance of nutrition supplementation during the peri-pregnancy period (1). It is recommended that infants consume 100 mg of DHA per day during the first year of life (2, 3), while for pregnant women, many authorities and expert scientific organizations recommend an additional 200 mg of DHA per day (4, 5). Several organizations, including the March of Dimes, the American Academy of Pediatrics, and the Food and Agriculture Organization of the United Nations, agree that pregnant and lactating women should have a minimal intake of 200–300 mg of DHA per day (6). The best way to supplement DHA is usually through dietary intake of foods containing DHA, such as deep-sea fish (such as salmon, cod, tuna, etc.), cod liver oil, seaweed, etc. In addition, nutritional supplements containing DHA are also a good choice, such as fish oil or algae oil. While the provision of essential nutrients is widely recognized, the potential benefits of additional nutrition supplementation for pregnant women and postnatal infants remain a subject of ongoing debate (7), particularly concerning elements implicated in neurological development and even academic performance. Inadequate or restricted nutrition intake essential for neuronal growth is associated with cognitive and motor development delays and neurodevelopmental impairments. Nonetheless, further investigation is required to elucidate whether supplementary nutrition interventions can potentially enhance the development of the neurological system.

Among the various essential nutrients, docosahexaenoic acid (DHA, 22:6 omega-3) stands out as a long-chain polyunsaturated fatty acid (LC-PUFA) belonging to the ω-3 (n–3) family. DHA possesses a 22-carbon chain and 6 cis double bonds (22:6n – 3) and is highly concentrated in neural tissues. During cerebral development, it has been observed to accumulate in the fetal brain (810). Some studies have shown that reduced levels of DHA in the brain have been associated with impaired neurogenesis and neurite growth and alter the metabolism of several neurotransmitters, including dopamine, serotonin, and acetylcholine (1113), highlighting its crucial role in the functional maturation of the infant’s brain (14). Typically, postnatal DHA is predominantly obtained through maternal breast milk, which serves as the natural and optimal source of infant nutrition and development, offering numerous benefits for maternal health (15). Maternal breast milk naturally contains significant quantities of DHA, ensuring adequate prenatal and postnatal growth. A comprehensive study analyzing breast milk from over 2,400 women reported an average DHA concentration of 0.32/100 g in fatty acids (FAs) (14). Nevertheless, the potential benefits of additional DHA supplementation for developing the neurological system remain uncertain. While specific studies have demonstrated that DHA supplementation enhances cognitive and visual development and function in preterm infants (8), several studies have presented contrasting findings.

Consequently, there is a need to ascertain the potential effects of dietary supplementation with DHA on neurological development in infants. Moreover, it is vital to acknowledge the crucial role of DHA in maintaining neural function (16, 17). However, the existing clinical trials investigating DHA supplementation in infants or pregnant women have been hindered by limitations such as small sample sizes and disparate evaluation systems, preventing the comprehensive synthesis of all relevant findings. Therefore, the present systematic review aims to provide a comprehensive assessment and meta-analysis of the available evidence regarding the efficacy of DHA supplementation during the prenatal and postnatal period, explicitly examining its impact on mental and psychomotor development. The secondary outcomes are the effects of DHA supplementation on visual acuity and cognition in children, which were briefly analyzed.

Methods

Protocol

This review complied with the Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews and Interventions recommendations and was recorded according to the PRISMA systematic review guidelines (18). The systematic review protocol was registered in the International Prospective Register of Systematic Reviews (PROSPERO) under registration number CRD42022348100. After researching, the number of cohort studies related to DHA supplementation was small. In contrast, RCT demonstrate significant advantages in establishing causal relationships, controlling confounding variables, and enhancing both internal and external validity of research. Therefore, we ultimately opted to include only RCTs, which differed slightly from from our protocol.

Search strategy

The systematic search was conducted in three electronic databases: PubMed, EMBASE (Ovid), and Cochrane Library. The search included human-involved studies and was not restricted by publication date. Keywords in the search strategy included [docosahexaenoic acid], [supplements], and [nervous systems] and their synonyms. The entire databases search strategies can be found in Supplementary Appendix 1. The search was updated to the end of February 2024 with language restricted to English. Relevant studies and potential research were searched and reviewed manually.

Study selection

Citations were collected in a reference manager software program (EndNote X9), and duplicates were eliminated. Citations initially selected by the systematic search were first retrieved as titles and/or abstracts and preliminarily screened. Then, relevant reports were retrieved as complete manuscripts and assessed for compliance with eligibility criteria.

We followed the population-intervention-comparison-outcomes-study design (PICOS) model for defining our eligibility criteria:

1. Population: pregnant women or postpartum nursing mothers without high-risk pregnancy (e.g., psychiatric disorders or pregnancy-induced complications including hypertension, preeclampsia) or infants without any genetic or chromosomal abnormalities.

2. Intervention: DHA supplementation (other polyunsaturated fatty acids were much smaller than that of DHA, e.g., DHA: EPA ≥ 5:1).

3. Comparison: Placebo or no fatty acids supplementation.

4. Outcomes: The outcome evaluation should include any parameters in neurological development: The primary outcome was neurodevelopment, which was explicitly examined using the mental (MDI) and psychomotor development (PDI) indices on Bayley Scale of Infant and Toddler Development (BSID). The secondary outcomes included visual acuity, behavioral development, sleep quality, language skills, cognitive development, and attention variables.

5. Study: randomized, controlled trials; we excluded animal studies conference abstracts or protocols and studies where we could not calculate or obtain missing outcome data.

Data extraction

Two investigators (Ruolan Hu and Yifei Li) independently assessed the eligibility of reports at the title and/or abstract level, with a supervisor (Jinrong Li) determining the divergences together. Studies that met the inclusion criteria were selected for further analysis. We manually searched the reference lists of included studies, existing systematic reviews and all articles citing the included studies on Google Scholar. Then, relevant reports were retrieved as complete manuscripts and assessed for inclusion and exclusion criteria compliance.

The following data were extracted by two investigators (Ruolan Hu and Yifei Li) using a standardized study form: authors’ names, publication year, study design, follow-up period, location, sample size, gestational age, birth weight, intervention protocol, actual DHA intake, supplementary time, placebo, measurement type, and results. The third investigator (Jinrong Li) cross-checked the information of the included studies.

Risk of bias assessment

The risk of bias (ROB) of randomized controlled trials was assessed by two independent reviewers using the ROB tool in the Cochrane Handbook 5.1.0 (19). The indicators of ROB include random sequence generation, allocation concealment, blinding of patients and caregivers, blinding of outcome assessment, data completeness, selective outcome reporting, and other biases. Each indicator will be judged as high risk, low risk, or unclear risk for the evaluation result. Two investigators independently conducted all risk of bias assessments, and we resolved any differences in their assessments through team discussion.

Publication bias

Publication bias was assessed using a funnel plot, a commonly employed method to evaluate potential biases. RevMan 5.4 software was utilized to estimate publication bias for each pooled result in this meta-analysis. The software generated a visual representation, presenting a distribution of individual study results as independent symbols. These representative plots were instrumental in illustrating the potential presence of publication bias. In cases where the figure displayed a symmetric pattern, it indicated the absence of publication bias in the pooled data. Conversely, an asymmetric figure suggested the existence of publication bias.

Sensitivity analysis

One set of study data was systematically removed to determine whether any single study was incurring undue weight in the analysis. The pooled results for the remaining studies were rechecked to determine whether the results had a significant change. Sensitivity analysis was conducted for every study.

Data synthesis and analysis

We performed the meta-analysis using RevMan 5.4. We used the mean difference (MD) with 95% CI for continuous data to present the pooled results. Furthermore, the inverse-variance (IV) method was used for continuous outcomes. All statistical methods used were confirmed by a statistician trained in meta-analyses and systematic reviews. We used I2 statistics as a guide to assess heterogeneity, along with a visual inspection of forest plots. I2 > 50% indicated significant heterogeneity across studies (20). To explore the source of heterogeneity, we performed subgroup analyses by the studied population (DHA supplementation during pregnancy, lactation and infant direct supplementation). p < 0.05 was considered statistically significant in this trial, except where otherwise specified. If the standard deviations (SDs) were not reported directly, the variances could be imputed from the standard error (SE). For the assessments that could not be pooled, a narrative evaluation was performed to present the results of a systematic review.

Results

Study selection

A total of 304 articles were initially identified through a systematic search, with an additional article obtained through a manual search. After removing duplicates, 267 articles remained and underwent further review. Screening the titles and abstracts resulted in selecting 35 articles considered potential candidates for full-text evaluation. Articles were excluded based on several criteria: (1) not being RCTs, (2) involving subjects other than pregnant women, children, or breastfeeding mothers, (3) utilizing complex supplements with high polyunsaturated fatty acids (except for DHA) content, and (4) lacking neurodevelopmental assessments as part of their outcome evaluations. Ultimately, 21 studies met this systematic review’s rigorous inclusion and exclusion criteria (2141) (Figure 1).

Figure 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Flow diagram of literature search according to the PRISMA statement.

Risk of bias

Figure 2 summarizes the quality assessment results for the included studies. The majority of the trials were categorized as having an unclear risk of bias. The most common deficiencies identified among the enrolled studies are as follows: nine studies did not provide a research protocol, five studies lacked sufficient information regarding allocation concealment, five studies did not provide blinding information, nine studies had a significant number of participants dropping out, one of them did not mention the reasons of dropping out, five studies did not provide detailed information about the generation of the allocation sequence, and one study reported original data as standard error while the other papers reported standard deviation, which hindered the calculation of meta-analysis.

Figure 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Risk of bias summary: judgments about each risk of bias item for each included study.

Overall, according to the criteria of overall quality, 5 of the 21 studies were identified as high quality (26, 27, 3638, 41) with no items indicating unclear or high risk of bias; 14 studies were evaluated as moderate quality (23, 2932, 39) with no items at high risk of bias. However, two studies were still considered low quality (22, 24, 25, 28, 42). Although nearly half of the participants dropped out in Van Goor’s study (22), the number of participants who dropped out for each reason in the experimental and control groups was similar. Thus, Van Goor’s study (22) was eligible for the systematic review, while Scott’s study (24) was excluded because its low quality.

General study characteristics

Finally, the 20 RCTs were included in this systematic review. However, only 5 RCTs utilized the same outcome measurement tool, namely the BSID-II, and 4 RCTs used the BSID-III, which can convert scores to MDI and PDI, thus could be combined for meta-analysis (4346). Nonetheless, the remaining 12 papers will be included and analyzed in this systematic review. Table 1 provides a summary of the key characteristics of the included studies. All of the studies incorporated in this review were RCTs. The majority of the studies (55%) were conducted in the United States (21, 2832, 34, 3841), while others were carried out in Australia (26, 35, 37), Mexico (25), Canada (21, 36), the Netherlands (24), Turkey (23), India (27), New Zealand and Singapore (35, 37). The publication dates of the included studies ranged from 2000 to 2022, indicating the continued interest in DHA administration. Among the 20 studies, 7 employed a postnatal DHA supplementation strategy for infants, 9 described a protocol involving DHA supplementation during pregnancy, 2 provided maternal DHA supplementation after birth, and 2 were maternal DHA supplementation from pregnancy to postpartum. The sample sizes of the included studies varied, ranging from 27 to 863 participants (25, 31). The primary outcomes in the neurodevelopment evaluation were the MDI and PDI based on the BSID. Additionally, secondary outcomes encompassed parameters such as visual acuity, behavioral development, sleep quality, language skills, cognitive development, and attention variables.

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Characteristics of included studies in the systematic-review.

MDI and PDI

Nine studies were included in the assessment of MDI and PDI scores, based on the BSID, to evaluate the impact of DHA supplementation on neurodevelopment. Four of these studies applied BSID-III language score conversion to include meta-analyses of mental development (43). The overall pooled results for MDI assessment (Figure 3) indicated no statistically significant difference (MD = 0.41, 95% CI −0.91 to 1.73, I2 = 29%, p = 0.55). Subgroup analysis based on the administration of DHA postnatally to infants (MD = 2.05, 95% CI −0.16 to 4.26, I2 = 0%, p = 0.07), during maternal pregnancy (MD = −1.08, 95% CI −2.25 to 0.09, I2 = 0%, p = 0.07), and to breastfeeding mother (MD = 2.14, 95% CI −0.54 to 4.83, I2 = 0%, p = 0.12) also failed to reveal a significant difference. The results of converting cognitive scores to MDI by applying Lowe et al.’s (45) conversion formula [Bayley-III score = (0.59 X Bayley II) + 52] were presented in Supplementary Appendix 2. Nevertheless, the DHA supplementation to infants differed significantly from the control group (MD = 3.80, 95% CI 0.58 to 7.03, I2 = 0%, p = 0.02). Other subgroups [during pregnancy (MD = −0.72, 95% CI –1.99 to 0.55, I2 = 0%, p = 0.26)] and mother (MD = 0.89, 95% CI −2.48 to 4.26, I2 = 0%, p = 0.60) showed no significant difference between the experimental group and control group. Likewise, there was no statistical difference overall (MD = 0.16, 95% CI −1.08 to 1.40, I2 = 5%, p = 0.80).

Figure 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Forest plot summarizing the effect of DHA supplement on infants’ neurodevelopment through MDI (language scores conversion).

For PDI assessment (Figure 4), the overall pooled results demonstrated a statistically significant difference (MD = 1.47, 95% CI 0.23 to 2.72, I2 = 39%, p = 0.02). DHA administration to postnatal infants (MD = 1.92, 95% CI 0.23 to 3.65, I2 = 0%, p = 0.03) positively affected on PDI evaluation. However, other two subgroups analysis indicated that DHA supplementation administered during maternal pregnancy (MD = 0.61, 95% CI −0.99 to 2.21, I2 = 46%, p = 0.45) and to breastfeeding mother (MD = 4.34, 95% CI −2.66 to 11.34, I2 = 78%, p = 0.22) did not result in a significant difference.

Figure 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Forest plot summarizing the effect of DHA supplement on infants’ neurodevelopment through PDI.

We divided the studies on infant DHA supplementation into two subgroups: term infants and preterm infants, for meta-analysis (Supplementary Appendix 3). The results indicated DHA group had significantly higher scores (MDI through cognitive score conversion) than the placebo group among full-term infants (MD = 4.10, 95% CI 0.68 to 7.52, I2 = 0%, p = 0.02). However, there were no significant differences between DHA and placebo groups in other outcomes whether premature or term infant. Separate forest plots corresponding to each subgroup analysis were shown in Supplementary Appendix 3.

Visual development

Four studies were conducted to investigate the effectiveness of DHA supplementation on visual acuity during early life, utilizing different methodologies such as Sweep VEP and Teller Acuity Cards. Due to the variations in data formats, the reported results could not be combined for meta-analysis. Jensen et al. (30) reported no significant differences in VEP between the DHA intervention and control groups at 4 months. However, Judge et al. (28) found that DHA supplementation during pregnancy improved offspring visual acuity at 4 months (p = 0.018), although this improvement was not sustained at 6 months. Similar findings were observed in assessments using Teller Acuity Cards. Mulder et al. reported that infants receiving DHA supplementation had a reduced risk of delayed visual acuity development, achieving 3.3 cycles/degree based on Teller Acuity Cards assessment at 2 months (OR 2.50, 95% CI 1.02–6.14, p = 0.03). However, DHA supplementation did not significantly improve visual acuity at 12 months (OR 1.23, 95% CI 0.61–2.49, p = 0.35) (21).

Colombo et al. (38) showed that DHA supplementation did not affect look duration or habituation parameters (visual attention) across the first year.

Birch et al. (40) reported that infants fed control formula had significantly poorer visual evoked potential visual acuity at 12 month of age than infants who received the DHA-supplemented formulas (p < 0.001). There were no significant differences in visual evoked potential visual acuity between the three amounts of DHA supplementation (0.32% DHA, 0.64% DHA, 0.96% DHA) for either site at any age tested which indicated higher amounts of DHA supplementation were not associated with additional improvement of visual acuity.

The articles mentioned above on visual demonstrate the beneficial effects of DHA supplementation for visual acuity in pregnant women (21, 28), while Colombo’s study (38) showed DHA supplementation did not affect visual attention. Similar results were observed in full-term infants receiving DHA supplementation (40). However, no apparent benefits were observed when supplementing mothers with DHA (30).

Auditory function

In all groups, Unay et al. (15) reported significant decreases in absolute wave and interpeak latencies (measured using brainstem auditory evoked potential) at 16 weeks of age. However, these reductions were more pronounced in healthy, full-term newborns who received DHA supplementation than those who did not (p < 0.05).

Language skills

Seven studies reported the results of language skill measurement. Among them, four studies used the BSID scale (26, 33, 35, 36), while one study employed Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals Preschool (CELF-P2) (41). These studies demonstrated no statistical difference in language skill evaluation at 12, 18, and 48 months. However, Meldrum et al. (33) conducted a study utilizing the Macarthur–Bates Communicative Development Inventory and found that term infants who received high-dose DHA-enriched diets exhibited significantly better performance in language assessments at 12 and 18 months of age. Compared to the placebo group, these children demonstrated higher percentile ranks in both later-developing gestures (p = 0.007; p = 0.002, respectively) and the total number of gestures (p = 0.023; p = 0.006, respectively). Furthermore, another study reported that children who received DHA supplementation showed enhanced language development in terms of words understood (OR 3.22, 95% CI 1.49–6.94, p = 0.002; OR 2.77, 95% CI 1.23–6.28, p = 0.03) and sentences produced (OR 2.61, 95% CI 1.22–5.58, p = 0.01; OR 2.60, 95% CI 1.15–5.89, p = 0.02) at 14 and 18 months, respectively, as assessed by the McArthur Communicative Developmental Inventory (21).

There did not appear to be a consistent outcome regarding the benefits of DHA supplementation among infants, pregnant women, and mothers for language development.

Cognitive development

Four studies (26, 33, 35, 36) used the BSID-III scale to evaluate cognitive development. None of the studies’ cognitive composite scores differed between children in the DHA and placebo groups.

Similar to language development, there is no consensus on the benefits of DHA supplementation in infants, pregnant women, and mothers for cognitive performance.

Behavior

The study conducted by Gustafson et al. (31) revealed significantly higher scores in the motor [t(25) = 1.87, p = 0.038] and autonomic clusters [t(25) = 1.99, p = 0.029] based on the Neonatal Behavioral Assessment Scale (NBAS) among children whose mothers received DHA supplementation during pregnancy compared to those who received a placebo. Meldrum et al. reported that term infants who received high-dose DHA had higher scores, indicating a reduced risk of psychological disorders for anxious/depressed behaviors, as assessed through the Achenbach Child Behavior Checklist (33). What’s more, Gould et al. (37) observed that the percentages of preterm infants with a clinical diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder, attention deficit–hyperactivity disorder, or other behavioral or neurologic disorders were similar in the two groups. Furthermore, Makrides et al. (41) also revealed that autism and hyperactivity disorders did not differ between groups.

Supplementing full-term infants and pregnant women with DHA appeared to reduce the risk of psychological disorders, whereas supplementation for preterm infants does not.

Executive function

Judge et al. (21) demonstrated that infants whose mothers consumed 214 mg/day of DHA during pregnancy experienced significantly fewer arousals in quiet sleep [t(44) = 2.17, p < 0.05] and active sleep [t(44) = 2.21, p < 0.05] compared to infants who did not receive DHA on the first postnatal day. Furthermore, ANCOVA analysis indicated that neonates in the DHA group (supplementation for pregnancies) continued to exhibit significantly fewer arousals in quiet sleep on the second postnatal day. Makrides et al. (41) indicated that the DHA group had poorer scores on some parentally reported scales of executive functioning and behavior at 4 years of age. However, the differences were small and unlikely to be clinically significant because all measures were within the normal range.

Attention

Colombo et al. (38) reported that prenatal maternal DHA supplementation conferred advantages for the infants on attentional tasks (sustained attention and behavioral state) during the first year of life (at 4, 6, and 9 months of age). Nevertheless, Hewawasam et al. (35) showed no evidence of a difference between groups in preterm infant of the latency of distractibility (adjusted MD: 0·08 s, 95% CI −0·81, 0·97; p = 0·86) at 18 months of age.

Publication bias

Visual examination of the funnel plots (Figures 5AC) revealed slight to moderate asymmetry, indicating the possibility of some publication bias across all outcomes. These findings suggest that while no obvious publication bias was observed, it cannot be completely excluded as a potential influence on the present meta-analysis. However, given the limited amount of data included, it is essential to acknowledge that publication bias cannot be definitively ruled out as a factor impacting the results of this meta-analysis.

Figure 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. (A) Funnel plots detailing publication bias in the selected studies of DHA supplementation’s effect on infants’ MDI (language scores conversion). (B) Funnel plots detailing publication bias in the selected studies of DHA supplementation’s effect on infants’ PDI. (C) Funnel plots detailing publication bias in the selected studies of DHA supplementation’s effect on infants’ MDI (cognitive scores conversion).

Sensitivity analysis

During the sensitivity analysis, the impact of individual studies on the overall effect estimate was assessed by sequentially removing one study at a time and reanalyzing the remaining studies. It was observed that no single study had a substantial influence on the pooled effect estimate when comparing the results of the present systematic review with the meta-analysis. This indicates the robustness and reliability of the findings obtained in this study.

Discussion

DHA plays a crucial role in developmenting a healthy brain (47). The human brain exhibits significantly higher DHA levels than eicosapentaenoic acid (EPA) (48). Extensive research has shown that DHA supplementation during pregnancy and lactation and the fortification of infant food with DHA leads to increased DHA levels in infant tissues and improved neurological and visual development (49). In addition, the impact of DHA deficiencies on cognitive functions has been extensively studied based on animal models. DHA deficiency can lead to synaptic plasticity, region-specific brain developmental disorders, and an increased risk for neurocognitive impairment (50). Opinions on DHA supplementation have been changing. Until 1998, expert panels of the FDA in the United States and working groups of Canadian authorities did not recommend the inclusion of LC-PUFAs, including DHA, in infant formulas. However, in 2021, the European Commission revised its recommendation on DHA from an optional ingredient to a mandatory nutrient (51). Additionally, there has been a significant expansion in the global omega-3 market, particularly DHA, due to the rising demand for DHA as a vital component of infant formula and supplements (52). Despite the growing usage of DHA, no meta-analysis currently assesses the potential benefits of DHA supplementation alone in the development of the infant nervous system. Therefore, it is imperative to investigate whether infants can benefit from DHA supplementation address this important knowledge gap.

Regarding the primary outcomes, the meta-analysis revealed that DHA supplementation did not benefit mental development. This finding remained consistent across all subgroup analyses, except for DHA supplementation to infants. Moreover, the psychomotor development did significantly differ between the two groups. A discussion according to the subgroup follows.

Supplementation for infants

The meta-analysis results indicated the potential benefits of DHA supplementation for infants in mental development when given to postnatal infants (cognitive scores conversion). However, we need to note that the MDI of applying language conversion did not show a difference between DHA and placebo groups, indicating that different conversion formulas had heterogeneity. Similar to the total effect, the DHA group of infants had higher psychomotor development scores. The PDI in BSID-II primarily assessed gross and fine movements in infants and young children, whereas the Motor Scale in the Bayley-III assesses gross, fine movements, and hand-eye coordination, and thus may be somewhat heterogeneous. We hoped that more accurate conversion methods between BSID-II and BSID-III would be available in the future. Supplementation of DHA to infants after birth appeared to have mental and psychomotor benefits. For secondary outcomes, we noticed that most studies demonstrated some short-term positive effects of DHA supplementation on infant visual acuity and attention immediately after birth. In contrast, medium-term follow-up results showed no significant differences between the DHA supplementation and control groups. A recent study also demonstrated that higher dietary DHA intake during the first year was significantly associated with better cognitive function at 12 months and improved motor function at 12 months of corrected age, but not at 24 months in very low birth weight infants (53).

Supplementation for pregnant women

In the subgroup analysis of DHA supplementation for pregnant women, neither MDI nor PDI differed between the DHA and placebo groups. This finding was also shown in cognitive and language scales; Combined with the results of DHA supplementation in infants, the observed benefits of DHA supplementation appear to be time-dependent, with DHA promoting neurological development and maturation in the early stages but not improving neurological function once the neurons have matured. For example, Makrides’ four-year follow-up showed that the differences observed at 18 months (including cognitive delay and mean language scores) were no longer present at 4 years of age (41). This suggests that DHA may facilitate the differentiation and proliferation of neural stem cells but does not contribute significantly to the functional maintenance of mature neural cells. After the maturation stage of the neurological system, specific training becomes more critical for an infant’s neurodevelopment. Moreover, in the long-term follow-up, many other factors would implicate neurodevelopment, alleviating the positive effects of DHA supplementary.

Supplementation for lactating mother

Maternal DHA supplementation was mentioned in only a few articles. This meta-analysis showed no benefit of maternal DHA supplementation only after delivery on mental and psychomotor development in their offspring. However, the results of the two articles included in the meta-analysis suggested that PDI of the supplemented group was higher at 30 months of age and infants exposed to DHA performed better on the Bayley-III language score, compared with the placebo group.

In addition to the participants of DHA supplementation, there are other factors that affect infant neurodevelopment. Preterm infants are known to be more deficient in DHA than full-term newborns (6), and DHA supplementation in preterm infants seems to be essential. Of the included studies, three (3537) explicitly mentioned preterm infants, two (36, 37) of which measured different indicators at different points in time for the same group of participants. There were no differences in cognitive, language, motor scale and attention between the DHA and placebo group, while subgroup analysis in Guillot et al. (36) study suggested a potential benefit for language in preterm neonates born before 27 weeks gestational age. The DHA intake in Guillot’s (36) (1.2 g/day) study was much higher than that in the other two studies (35, 37) (60 mg/kg/day), indicating that preterm infants need large amounts of DHA supplementation. What’s more, the dose of DHA supplementation is an important factor. As the recommended supplementation dose of DHA mentioned in introduction, we defined the high dose of DHA directly supplementing infants ≥ 100 mg/day and to pregnant women and lactating women ≥ 300 mg/day. Most studies have applied high doses of DHA supplementation, and there is no consensus on whether DHA improves neurodevelopment. However, Some studies indicated that certain concentrations of DHA can be beneficial for neurodevelopment, and higher concentrations did not confer additional benefit (32, 35, 40). Only two studies (same participants) supplemented low doses of DHA for infants initially, one indicated that using an enteral DHA emulsion until 36 weeks of postmenstrual age was associated with modestly higher FSIQ scores at 5 years of age than control feeding. We conjectured it was because the participants were preterm infants. For pregnant women and lactating mothers, high doses of DHA did not significantly benefit over low doses for the BSID measurement of neurodevelopment in infants, but high dose of DHA appeared to be beneficial for visual acuity. While higher DHA doses (800 mg/day, which was the highest dose of DHA supplementation for pregnant women) might lead to poorer scores on some parentally reported scales of executive functioning and behavior in 4-year-olds (41), the DHA group did not show any difference at 18 months (26).

Based on our results, supplementation for infants with DHA is effective for their neurodevelopment. However, this only shows a significant advantage in PDI, and the results differ with different conversion methods. Therefore, more high-quality evidence is needed to confirm the benefit of DHA supplementation in infants and young children, and we cannot definitively decide whether DHA supplementation should be given in infants. There are few studies on DHA supplementation in preterm infants, and we cannot draw a firm conclusion. For pregnant women, our results were inconsistent, although DHA intake during pregnancy is important to ensure that the fetus obtains an adequate level. A study in this meta-analysis yielded negative results regarding the effectiveness of high-dose DHA supplementation for pregnancies, so further research is warranted to explore this topic in greater depth. The need of DHA supplementation for lactating mother is uncertain.

This review has several notable limitations that should be acknowledged. Firstly, the included studies exhibited significant heterogeneity in participant demographics (encompassing postnatal infants, pregnant mothers, and breastfeeding mothers) and differences in assessment tool editions (BSID-II and BSID-III). Secondly, there was considerable variability in the DHA dosages administered across the studies included in our meta-analysis, and the precise intake of DHA by term infants in the two RCTs remains unclear and two studies were based on infants’ weight. Therefore, it is challenging to definitively conclude whether DHA supplementation has a negligible impact on neurodevelopment. Thirdly, the number of clinical studies examining DHA supplementation in infants alone remains limited, necessitating further research to establish conclusive findings. Lastly, the meta-analysis encompassed numerous parameters for assessing neurological development, making it challenging to synthesize and reconcile their divergent findings.

Conclusion

The meta-analysis’s supplementation of infants with DHA at certain doses appears beneficial for neurodevelopment, both mental and psychomotor development. However, this is based on a small study and our article has some limitations. Moreover, existing research findings provide some evidence supporting the short-term neurological benefits of DHA supplementation during the peri-pregnancy phase. Conversely, no clear long-term benefits or harms were demonstrated for pregnancies receiving DHA-supplemented formula. The necessity of DHA supplementation in lactating women is uncertain and needs to be explored in more related articles. In the future, it would be beneficial to conduct further investigations to evaluate the effectiveness of DHA supplementation in various populations, especially preterm infants.

Data availability statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/Supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding authors.

Author contributions

RH: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Software, Validation, Writing – original draft. JX: Data curation, Investigation, Methodology, Software, Writing – original draft. YL: Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Project administration, Resources, Software, Supervision, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. JL: Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Project administration, Software, Supervision, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. YH: Investigation, Project administration, Supervision, Validation, Visualization, writing, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare that financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. This work was supported by the Application Foundation Program of Science and Technology Department of Sichuan Province (2021YJ0453).

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fneur.2024.1295788/full#supplementary-material

Abbreviations

DHA, Docosahexaenoic acid; LC-PUFA, Long-chain polyunsaturated fatty acids; MDI, Mental Development Index; PDI, Psychomotor Development Index; BSID, Bayley Scales of Infant and Toddler Development; NBAS, Neonatal Behavioral Assessment Scale; VEP, Visual evoked potential; ROB, Risk of bias; WPPSI, Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence; CELF-P2, Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals Preschool, second edition.

References

1. Thurow, R. The first 1,000 days: a crucial time for mothers and children-and the world. Breastfeed Med. (2016) 11:416–8. doi: 10.1089/bfm.2016.0114

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Guesnet, P, Pugo-Gunsam, P, Maurage, C, Pinault, M, Giraudeau, B, Alessandri, JM, et al. Blood lipid concentrations of docosahexaenoic and arachidonic acids at birth determine their relative postnatal changes in term infants fed breast milk or formula. Am J Clin Nutr. (1999) 70:292–8. doi: 10.1093/ajcn.70.2.292

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Jouanne, M, Oddoux, S, Noël, A, and Voisin-Chiret, AS. Nutrient requirements during pregnancy and lactation. Nutrients. (2021) 13:692. doi: 10.3390/nu13020692

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Koletzko, B, Cremer, M, Flothkötter, M, Graf, C, Hauner, H, Hellmers, C, et al. Diet and lifestyle before and during pregnancy—practical recommendations of the Germany-wide healthy start—young family network. Geburtshilfe Frauenheilkd. (2018) 78:1262–82. doi: 10.1055/a-0713-1058

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Koletzko, B, Cetin, I, and Brenna, JT. Dietary fat intakes for pregnant and lactating women. Br J Nutr. (2007) 98:873–7. doi: 10.1017/S0007114507764747

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Smith, SL, and Rouse, CA. Docosahexaenoic acid and the preterm infant. Matern Health Neonatol Perinatol. (2017) 3:22. doi: 10.1186/s40748-017-0061-1

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Harding, JE, Cormack, BE, Alexander, T, Alsweiler, JM, and Bloomfield, FH. Advances in nutrition of the newborn infant. Lancet. (2017) 389:1660–8. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(17)30552-4

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Weiser, MJ, Butt, CM, and Mohajeri, MH. Docosahexaenoic acid and cognition throughout the lifespan. Nutrients. (2016) 8:99. doi: 10.3390/nu8020099

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Clandinin, MT, Chappell, JE, Leong, S, Heim, T, Swyer, PR, and Chance, GW. Intrauterine fatty acid accretion rates in human brain: implications for fatty acid requirements. Early Hum Dev. (1980) 4:121–9. doi: 10.1016/0378-3782(80)90015-8

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Martinez, M. Tissue levels of polyunsaturated fatty acids during early human development. J Pediatr. (1992) 120:S129–38. doi: 10.1016/S0022-3476(05)81247-8

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Innis, SM. Human milk: maternal dietary lipids and infant development. Proc Nutr Soc. (2007) 66:397–404. doi: 10.1017/S0029665107005666

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Innis, SM. Dietary (n-3) fatty acids and brain development. J Nutr. (2007) 137:855–9. doi: 10.1093/jn/137.4.855

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Bazan, NG. Cell survival matters: docosahexaenoic acid signaling, neuroprotection and photoreceptors. Trends Neurosci. (2006) 29:263–71. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2006.03.005

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Horrocks, LA, and Yeo, YK. Health benefits of docosahexaenoic acid (DHA). Pharmacol Res. (1999) 40:211–25. doi: 10.1006/phrs.1999.0495

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Salminen, S, Stahl, B, Vinderola, G, and Szajewska, H. Infant formula supplemented with biotics: current knowledge and future perspectives. Nutrients. (2020) 12:952. doi: 10.3390/nu12071952

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Wang, Q, Cui, Q, and Yan, C. The effect of supplementation of long-chain polyunsaturated fatty acids during lactation on neurodevelopmental outcomes of preterm infant from infancy to school age: a systematic review and Meta-analysis. Pediatr Neurol. (2016) 59:54–61.e1. doi: 10.1016/j.pediatrneurol.2016.02.017

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Campoy, C, Escolano-Margarit, MV, Anjos, T, Szajewska, H, and Uauy, R. Omega 3 fatty acids on child growth, visual acuity and neurodevelopment. Br J Nutr. (2012) 107:S85–S106. doi: 10.1017/S0007114512001493

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Moher, D, Liberati, A, Tetzlaff, J, and Altman, DG. Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: the PRISMA statement. PLoS Med. (2009) 6:e1000097. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.1000097

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Sterne, JAC, Savović, J, Page, MJ, Elbers, RG, Blencowe, NS, Boutron, I, et al. RoB 2: a revised tool for assessing risk of bias in randomised trials. BMJ. (2019) 366:l4898. doi: 10.1136/bmj.l4898

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Higgins, JP, Thompson, SG, Deeks, JJ, and Altman, DG. Measuring inconsistency in meta-analyses. BMJ. (2003) 327:557–60. doi: 10.1136/bmj.327.7414.557

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Mulder, KA, King, DJ, and Innis, SM. Omega-3 fatty acid deficiency in infants before birth identified using a randomized trial of maternal DHA supplementation in pregnancy. PLoS One. (2014) 9:e83764. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0083764

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

22. van Goor, SA, Dijck-Brouwer, DA, Erwich, JJ, Schaafsma, A, and Hadders-Algra, M. The influence of supplemental docosahexaenoic and arachidonic acids during pregnancy and lactation on neurodevelopment at eighteen months. Prostaglandins Leukot Essent Fatty Acids. (2011) 84:139–46. doi: 10.1016/j.plefa.2011.01.002

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Unay, B, Sarici, SU, Ulaş, UH, Akin, R, Alpay, F, and Gökçay, E. Nutritional effects on auditory brainstem maturation in healthy term infants. Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed. (2004) 89:177F–1179F. doi: 10.1136/adc.2002.021014

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Scott, DT, Janowsky, JS, Carroll, RE, Taylor, JA, Auestad, N, and Montalto, MB. Formula supplementation with long-chain polyunsaturated fatty acids: are there developmental benefits? Pediatrics. (1998) 102:E59. doi: 10.1542/peds.102.5.e59

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Ramakrishnan, U, Stinger, A, DiGirolamo, AM, Martorell, R, Neufeld, LM, Rivera, JA, et al. Prenatal docosahexaenoic acid supplementation and offspring development at 18 months: randomized controlled trial. PLoS One. (2015) 10:e0120065. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0120065

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Makrides, M, Gibson, RA, McPhee, AJ, Yelland, L, Quinlivan, J, and Ryan, P. Effect of DHA supplementation during pregnancy on maternal depression and neurodevelopment of young children: a randomized controlled trial. JAMA. (2010) 304:1675–83. doi: 10.1001/jama.2010.1507

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Khandelwal, S, Kondal, D, Chaudhry, M, Patil, K, Swamy, MK, Metgud, D, et al. Effect of maternal docosahexaenoic acid (DHA) supplementation on offspring neurodevelopment at 12 months in India: a randomized controlled trial. Nutrients. (2020) 12:3041. doi: 10.3390/nu12103041

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Judge, MP, Harel, O, and Lammi-Keefe, CJ. A docosahexaenoic acid-functional food during pregnancy benefits infant visual acuity at four but not six months of age. Lipids. (2007) 42:117–22. doi: 10.1007/s11745-006-3007-3

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Judge, MP, Cong, X, Harel, O, Courville, AB, and Lammi-Keefe, CJ. Maternal consumption of a DHA-containing functional food benefits infant sleep patterning: an early neurodevelopmental measure. Early Hum Dev. (2012) 88:531–7. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2011.12.016

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Jensen, CL, Voigt, RG, Prager, TC, Zou, YL, Fraley, JK, Rozelle, JC, et al. Effects of maternal docosahexaenoic acid intake on visual function and neurodevelopment in breastfed term infants. Am J Clin Nutr. (2005) 82:125–32. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/82.1.125

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Gustafson, KM, Carlson, SE, Colombo, J, Yeh, HW, Shaddy, DJ, Li, S, et al. Effects of docosahexaenoic acid supplementation during pregnancy on fetal heart rate and variability: a randomized clinical trial. Prostaglandins Leukot Essent Fatty Acids. (2013) 88:331–8. doi: 10.1016/j.plefa.2013.01.009

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Drover, JR, Hoffman, DR, Castañeda, YS, Morale, SE, Garfield, S, Wheaton, DH, et al. Cognitive function in 18-month-old term infants of the DIAMOND study: a randomized, controlled clinical trial with multiple dietary levels of docosahexaenoic acid. Early Hum Dev. (2011) 87:223–30. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2010.12.047

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Meldrum, SJ, D'Vaz, N, Simmer, K, Dunstan, JA, Hird, K, and Prescott, SL. Effects of high-dose fish oil supplementation during early infancy on neurodevelopment and language: a randomised controlled trial. Br J Nutr. (2012) 108:1443–54. doi: 10.1017/S0007114511006878

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Keenan, K, Hipwell, A, McAloon, R, Hoffmann, A, Mohanty, A, and Magee, K. The effect of prenatal docosahexaenoic acid supplementation on infant outcomes in African American women living in low-income environments: a randomized, controlled trial. Psychoneuroendocrinology. (2016) 71:170–5. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2016.05.023

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Hewawasam, E, Collins, CT, Muhlhausler, BS, Yelland, LN, Smithers, LG, Colombo, J, et al. DHA supplementation in infants born preterm and the effect on attention at 18 months' corrected age: follow-up of a subset of the N3RO randomised controlled trial. Br J Nutr. (2021) 125:420–31. doi: 10.1017/S0007114520002500

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Guillot, M, Synnes, A, Pronovost, E, Qureshi, M, Daboval, T, Caouette, G, et al. Maternal high-dose DHA supplementation and neurodevelopment at 18-22 months of preterm children. Pediatrics. (2022) 150:5819. doi: 10.1542/peds.2021-055819

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Gould, JF, Makrides, M, Gibson, RA, Sullivan, TR, McPhee, AJ, Anderson, PJ, et al. Neonatal docosahexaenoic acid in preterm infants and intelligence at 5 years. N Engl J Med. (2022) 387:1579–88. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa2206868

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Colombo, J, Gustafson, KM, Gajewski, BJ, Shaddy, DJ, Kerling, EH, Thodosoff, JM, et al. Prenatal DHA supplementation and infant attention. Pediatr Res. (2016) 80:656–62. doi: 10.1038/pr.2016.134

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Birch, EE, Garfield, S, Hoffman, DR, Uauy, R, and Birch, DG. A randomized controlled trial of early dietary supply of long-chain polyunsaturated fatty acids and mental development in term infants. Dev Med Child Neurol. (2000) 42:174–81. doi: 10.1017/s0012162200000311

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Birch, EE, Carlson, SE, Hoffman, DR, Fitzgerald-Gustafson, KM, Fu, VL, Drover, JR, et al. The DIAMOND (DHA intake and measurement of neural development) study: a double-masked, randomized controlled clinical trial of the maturation of infant visual acuity as a function of the dietary level of docosahexaenoic acid. Am J Clin Nutr. (2010) 91:848–59. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.2009.28557

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Makrides, M, Gould, JF, Gawlik, NR, Yelland, LN, Smithers, LG, Anderson, PJ, et al. Four-year follow-up of children born to women in a randomized trial of prenatal DHA supplementation. JAMA. (2014) 311:1802–4. doi: 10.1001/jama.2014.2194

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Hoffman, DR, Theuer, RC, Castañeda, YS, Wheaton, DH, Bosworth, RG, O'Connor, AR, et al. Maturation of visual acuity is accelerated in breast-fed term infants fed baby food containing DHA-enriched egg yolk. J Nutr. (2004) 134:2307–13. doi: 10.1093/jn/134.9.2307

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Robertson, CM, Hendson, L, Biggs, WS, and Acton, BV. Application of the Flynn effect for the Bayley III scales. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. (2010) 164:1072–3. doi: 10.1001/archpediatrics.2010.199

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Moore, T, Johnson, S, Haider, S, Hennessy, E, and Marlow, N. Relationship between test scores using the second and third editions of the Bayley scales in extremely preterm children. J Pediatr. (2012) 160:553–8. doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2011.09.047

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Lowe, JR, Erickson, SJ, Schrader, R, and Duncan, AF. Comparison of the Bayley II mental developmental index and the Bayley III cognitive scale: are we measuring the same thing? Acta Paediatr. (2012) 101:e55–8. doi: 10.1111/j.1651-2227.2011.02517.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Feng, Y, Zhou, H, Zhang, Y, Perkins, A, Wang, Y, and Sun, J. Comparison in executive function in Chinese preterm and full-term infants at eight months. Front Med. (2018) 12:164–73. doi: 10.1007/s11684-017-0540-9

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Brenna, JT, and Carlson, SE. Docosahexaenoic acid and human brain development: evidence that a dietary supply is needed for optimal development. J Hum Evol. (2014) 77:99–106. doi: 10.1016/j.jhevol.2014.02.017

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Dyall, SC. Long-chain omega-3 fatty acids and the brain: a review of the independent and shared effects of EPA, DPA and DHA. Front Aging Neurosci. (2015) 7:52. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2015.00052

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Makrides, M, Neumann, MA, and Gibson, RA. Perinatal characteristics may influence the outcome of visual acuity. Lipids. (2001) 36:897–900. doi: 10.1007/s11745-001-0799-0

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Rioux, FM, Lindmark, G, and Hernell, O. Does inadequate maternal iron or DHA status have a negative impact on an infant's functional outcomes? Acta Paediatr. (2006) 95:137–44. doi: 10.1080/08035250500281814

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Tounian, P, Bellaïche, M, and Legrand, P. ARA or no ARA in infant formulae, that is the question. Arch Pediatr. (2021) 28:69–74. doi: 10.1016/j.arcped.2020.10.001

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Finco, AMO, Mamani, LDG, Carvalho, JC, de Melo Pereira, GV, Thomaz-Soccol, V, and Soccol, CR. Technological trends and market perspectives for production of microbial oils rich in omega-3. Crit Rev Biotechnol. (2017) 37:656–71. doi: 10.1080/07388551.2016.1213221

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Gsoellpointner, M, Thanhaeuser, M, Eibensteiner, F, Ristl, R, Jilma, B, Fuiko, R, et al. Polyunsaturated fatty acid intake during complementary feeding and neurodevelopmental outcome in very low birth weight infants. Nutrients. (2023) 15:3141. doi: 10.3390/nu15143141

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: DHA, supplementation, offspring neurodevelopment, systematic review, meta-analysis

Citation: Hu R, Xu J, Hua Y, Li Y and Li J (2024) Could early life DHA supplementation benefit neurodevelopment? A systematic review and meta-analysis. Front. Neurol. 15:1295788. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2024.1295788

Received: 17 September 2023; Accepted: 22 March 2024;
Published: 05 April 2024.

Edited by:

Chiara Doneda, Children Hospital V. Buzzi, Italy

Reviewed by:

Sandeep Kumar Singh, Indian Scientific Education and Technology Foundation, India
Chiara Nava, Ospedale dei Bambini Vittore Buzzi, Italy

Copyright © 2024 Hu, Xu, Hua, Li and Li. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Yifei Li, bGl5Zndjc2hAc2N1LmVkdS5jbg==; Jinrong Li, bGlqaW5yb25nMjI0QDE2My5jb20=

These authors have contributed equally to this work

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.