- 1Department of Neurology, University Hospital Schleswig-Holstein, Lübeck, Germany
- 2Center of Brain, Behavior and Metabolism (CBBM), University of Lübeck, Lübeck, Germany
- 3Institute of Psychology II, University Lübeck, Lübeck, Germany
We read with great interest the article by Oh and coworkers about “Visuospatial cognition in acute unilateral peripheral vestibulopathy” in the September edition of this journal Frontiers in Neurology (Section Neuro-Otology). The authors tested visuospatial perception by the Visual Object and Space Perception (VOSP) battery established by Warrington and James (1), validated by Rapport et al. (2), and visuospatial memory by the Block design test (3) and the Corsi block-tapping test (4) within the first 2 days (acute phase) and 4 weeks (recovery phase) after symptom onset of acute unilateral vestibulopathy (UVF).
There are several animal studies showing that unilateral experimental vestibular lesions elicit signs of spatial memory and visuospatial deficits which have been related to vestibulo-cortical projections, in particular to the hippocampal formation (5–7). However, these findings have not convincingly been confirmed in patient studies yet (8–12) but there is some-though not consistent (13–15)-evidence that patients with bilateral vestibular failure reveal visuospatial deficits or even path navigation impairments in addition to volumetric changes of the hippocampus (8, 16, 17).
Based on this background, the study by Oh et al. investigated visuospatial perception and memory in a large cohort (n = 72) of patients with UVF. As a main result, the patients showed normal visuospatial functions in the acute and recovery phase of the disease according to the normal reference values established by the standardization sample of the test founders. It is important to realize that the statistical differences between healthy subjects and patients were found within normal limits. This needs to be kept in mind when the authors speculate on the potential origin of visuospatial perception and spatial memory impairments.
We felt encouraged to point out the readers's risk of misunderstanding these results since at least two related studies also interpreted values within the normal reference range to be pathological:
First, Ayar et al. (12) used the Judgement of Line Orientation test (JLO) (18) to test visuospatial function in 21 patients with acute UVF (mean disease duration: 15.6 days). Patients and healthy control subjects revealed normal values according to the normal reference range. Patients had significantly lower but normal values (21 to 26, normal: >19) and yet their data were considered as a pathological sign of visuospatial function, even despite the fact that the significant difference disappeared when depression and anxiety scores were taken into account (multiple regression analysis).
Second, very recently, Obermann et al. (19) compared cognitive functions in 65 patients with UVF and healthy subjects, including the JLO test (18). Patients had lower (23.7 vs. 26.4) but normal values (>19) in the JLO test 5 months after symptom onset, i.e., at a time when the visuospatial group differences in the study by Oh et al. had already resolved.
UVF usually persists over a few months which unfortunately was not tested in the follow-up examination by Oh et al. (20). This could have helped to elucidate if the group differences are related to the vestibular system, in particular since JLO values decreased with greater semicircular canal paresis in the study by Ayar et al. (12).
In all three studies, the significant group differences were interpreted as signs of visuospatial impairment. Since they refer to the normal reference range of the original test studies (1, 18) they lack convincing evidence that unilateral vestibulopathy elicit visuospatial perception and memory impairments. Based on the large cohort sizes of these UVF studies it appears rather unlikely that these particular cognitive functions are altered in UVF patients.
These analytic approaches raise the question whether statistical differences within the numeric range defined as being “normal” or “healthy” in the studies establishing these neuropsychological tests, should be used for a pathological interpretation, in particular when the differences cannot be confirmed by a follow-up examination of the same tests (20).
Investigations on subthreshold or premanifesting disorders have been found to be a valuable approach to distinguish trait and state signs in several neurogenetic and neurodegenerative disorders (21, 22) but group differences within normal limits are usually classified as state signs only when they turn pathological during follow-up investigations. In contrast, the group differences in the study by Oh et al. resolved after 4 weeks. We recommend follow-up studies on vestibular and visuospatial function to investigate whether the group differences within the normal reference range have any pathological, e.g. neurodegenerative meaning. We consider this an important clarification to reduce the readers' risk of misunderstanding and potential inappropriate citations of these important data.
Author contributions
CH: Writing – original draft. AF: Formal analysis, Methodology, Validation, Writing – review & editing. AS: Methodology, Validation, Writing – review & editing.
Funding
The author(s) declare that no financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.
Conflict of interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
The author(s) declared that they were an editorial board member of Frontiers, at the time of submission. This had no impact on the peer review process and the final decision.
Publisher's note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
References
1. Warrington EK, James M. VOSP: The Visual Object and Space Perception Battery. Bury St Edmunds: Thames Valley Test Company (1991).
2. Rapport LJ, Millis SR, Bonello PJ. Validation of the Warrington theory of visual processing and the visual object and space perception battery. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. (1998) 20:211–20. doi: 10.1076/jcen.20.2.211.1169
3. Schorr D, Bower GH, Kiernan RJ. Stimulus variables in the block design task. J Consult Clin Psychol. (1982) 50:479. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.50.4.479
4. Kessels RP, van Zandvoort MJ, Postma A, Kappelle LJ, de Haan EH. The corsi block-tapping task: standardization and normative data. Appl Neuropsychol. (2000) 7:252–8. doi: 10.1207/S15324826AN0704_8
5. Hitier M, Besnard S, Smith PF. Vestibular pathways involved in cognition. Front Integr Neurosci. (2014) 8:59. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2014.00059
6. Smith PF. The vestibular system and cognition. Curr Opin Neurol. (2017) 30:84–9. doi: 10.1097/WCO.0000000000000403
7. Nguyen TT, Nam GS, Kang JJ, Han GC, Kim JS, Dieterich M, et al. The differential effects of acute right- vs. left-sided vestibular deafferentation on spatial cognition in unilateral labyrinthectomized mice. Front Neurol. (2021) 12:789487. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2021.789487
8. Popp P, Wulff M, Finke K, Ruhl M, Brandt T, Dieterich M. Cognitive deficits in patients with a chronic vestibular failure. J Neurol. (2017) 264:554–63. doi: 10.1007/s00415-016-8386-7
9. Dordevic M, Sulzer S, Barche D, Dieterich M, Arens C, Muller NG. Chronic, mild vestibulopathy leads to deficits in spatial tasks that rely on vestibular input while leaving other cognitive functions and brain volumes intact. Life. (2021) 11:1369. doi: 10.3390/life11121369
10. Grabherr L, Cuffel C, Guyot JP, Mast FW. Mental transformation abilities in patients with unilateral and bilateral vestibular loss. Exp Brain Res. (2011) 209:205–14. doi: 10.1007/s00221-011-2535-0
11. Borel L, Redon-Zouiteni C, Cauvin P, Dumitrescu M, Deveze A, Magnan J, et al. Unilateral vestibular loss impairs external space representation. PLoS ONE. (2014) 9:e88576. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0088576
12. Ayar DA, Kumral E, Celebisoy N. Cognitive functions in acute unilateral vestibular loss. J Neurol. (2020) 267:153–9. doi: 10.1007/s00415-020-09829-w
13. Gottlich M, Jandl NM, Sprenger A, Wojak JF, Munte TF, Kramer UM, et al. Hippocampal gray matter volume in bilateral vestibular failure. Hum Brain Mapp. (2016) 37:1998–2006. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23152
14. Jandl NM, Sprenger A, Wojak JF, Gottlich M, Munte TF, Kramer UM, et al. Dissociable cerebellar activity during spatial navigation and visual memory in bilateral vestibular failure. Neuroscience. (2015) 305:257–67. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2015.07.089
15. Bosmans J, Gommeren H, zu Eulenburg P, Gilles A, Mertens G, Van Ombergen A, et al. Is vestibular function related to human hippocampal volume? J Vestib Res. (2023) 1−11. doi: 10.3233/VES-230076
16. Brandt T, Schautzer F, Hamilton DA, Bruning R, Markowitsch HJ, Kalla R, et al. Vestibular loss causes hippocampal atrophy and impaired spatial memory in humans. Brain. (2005) 128:2732–41. doi: 10.1093/brain/awh617
17. Schöberl F, Pradhan C, Grosch M, Brendel M, Jostes F, Obermaier K, et al. Bilateral vestibulopathy causes selective deficits in recombining novel routes in real space. Sci Rep. (2021) 11:2695. doi: 10.1038/s41598-021-82427-6
18. Benton AL, Varney NR, Hamsher KD. Visuospatial judgment. a clinical test. Arch Neurol. (1978) 35:364–7. doi: 10.1001/archneur.1978.00500300038006
19. Obermann M, Gebauer A, Arweiler-Harbeck D, Lang S, Seilheimer B, Kleinschnitz C, et al. Cognitive deficits in patients with peripheral vestibular dysfunction. Eur J Neurol. (2023). doi: 10.1111/ene.15907
20. Oh SY, Nguyen TT, Kang JJ, Kirsch V, Boegle R, Kim JS, et al. Visuospatial cognition in acute unilateral peripheral vestibulopathy. Front Neurol. (2023) 14:1230495. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2023.1230495
21. Machner B, Sprenger A, Behrens MI, Ramirez A, Bruggemann N, Klein C, et al. Eye movement disorders in Atp13a2 mutation carriers (Park9). Mov Disord. (2010) 25:2687–9. doi: 10.1002/mds.23352
Keywords: visuospatial, vestibulopathy, normal reference range values, cognition, memory
Citation: Helmchen C, Fellbrich A and Sprenger A (2023) Normal visuospatial function in unilateral vestibulopathy: on the challenge of group differences within normal reference data. Front. Neurol. 14:1334277. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2023.1334277
Received: 06 November 2023; Accepted: 27 November 2023;
Published: 13 December 2023.
Edited by:
Sun-Young Oh, Jeonbuk National University, Republic of KoreaReviewed by:
Gi-sung Nam, Chosun University, Republic of KoreaCopyright © 2023 Helmchen, Fellbrich and Sprenger. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Christoph Helmchen, christoph.helmchen@uni-luebeck.de