Skip to main content

SYSTEMATIC REVIEW article

Front. Neurol., 03 November 2023
Sec. Neuro-Otology
This article is part of the Research Topic Reviews in Neuro-Otology View all 8 articles

Listening effort and fatigue among cochlear implant users: a scoping review

  • 1Experimental Laboratory of Translational Neurosciences and Dento-Otolaryngology, Faculty of Medicine and Health Sciences, University of Antwerp, Antwerp, Belgium
  • 2Department of Otorhinolaryngology/Head and Neck Surgery, Antwerp University Hospital, Antwerp, Belgium
  • 3Department of Education, Health and Social Work, University College Ghent, Ghent, Belgium

Introduction: In challenging listening situations, speech perception with a cochlear implant (CI) remains demanding and requires high levels of listening effort, which can lead to increased levels of listening-related fatigue. The body of literature on these topics increases as the number of CI users rises. This scoping review aims to provide an overview of the existing literature on listening effort, fatigue, and listening-related fatigue among CI users and the measurement techniques to evaluate them.

Methods: The Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) Statements were used to conduct the scoping review. The search was performed on PubMed, Scopus, and Web of Science to identify all relevant studies.

Results: In total, 24 studies were included and suggests that CI users experience higher levels of listening effort when compared to normal hearing controls using scales, questionnaires and electroencephalogram measurements. However, executing dual-task paradigms did not reveal any difference in listening effort between both groups. Uncertainty exists regarding the difference in listening effort between unilateral, bilateral, and bimodal CI users with bilateral hearing loss due to ambiguous results. Only five studies were eligible for the research on fatigue and listening-related fatigue. Additionally, studies using objective measurement methods were lacking.

Discussion: This scoping review highlights the necessity for additional research on these topics. Moreover, there is a need for guidelines on how listening effort, fatigue, and listening-related fatigue should be measured to allow for study results that are comparable and support optimal rehabilitation strategies.

1. Introduction

Hearing loss is a sensory deficit that has already been investigated extensively due to the well-known communication difficulties experienced by the hearing-impaired population in their daily life. When hearing thresholds deteriorate, communication difficulties and the impact on daily life may increase with, presumably, the highest impact for patients with severe-to-profound hearing loss. Cochlear implantation (CI) has become the standard treatment for patients with this degree of hearing loss (1).

The outcome of CI is frequently investigated using Patient-Reported Outcome Measures (PROMs) and speech comprehension tests (2). Research on health-related quality of life (HRQoL) using PROMs such as the Nijmegen Cochlear Implant Questionnaire and the Abbreviated Profile of Hearing Aid Benefits have observed significant improvement in levels of HRQoL after CI (3, 4). The speech recognition abilities can be evaluated using speech comprehension tests with words or sentences either in quiet or in noise. To date, improvements in speech perception after CI are already demonstrated (5).

Although satisfactory speech comprehension in quiet can be reached with a CI, regardless of the large inter-individual differences between CI users, the electric way of listening remains different from acoustic listening. An adequate frequency resolution is necessary in various listening situations but can only be provided to a limited extent by the CI, leading to difficulties in understanding speech in noise (6). Such adverse listening situations may lead to increased levels of listening effort and subsequent listening-related fatigue among CI users.

Listening effort is defined by Pichora-Fuller et al. (7) as “the deliberate allocation of mental resources to overcome obstacles in goal pursuit when carrying out a task, with listening effort applying more specifically when tasks involve listening”. Both subjective and objective (i.e., physiological and behavioral) measurements can be used to assess listening effort. Previous research used visual analog scales (VAS), Likert scales, or questionnaires such as the Speech, Spatial, and Qualities of Hearing scale (SSQ) to investigate the subjectively perceived listening effort, as these subjective measurements are quick and easy to complete and administer (8, 9). Both behavioral measurements, e.g., reaction time and dual-task paradigms, and physiological methods, e.g., pupillometry, electroencephalography (EEG), event-related potentials (ERP), and functional magnetic resonance imaging, objectively evaluate listening effort (1014).

General fatigue (further referred to as fatigue) is defined by Hornsby et al. (15) as “a general feeling of being tired, worn out, or sluggish” although a number of definitions are used in the literature. Listening-related fatigue on the other hand is the result of prolonged and high levels of listening effort (16). Both terms are used interchangeably in the literature and have been found to be important contributors to reduced QoL, cognitive abilities, and workplace productivity (15, 17). Fatigue and listening-related fatigue can be investigated using subjective methods such as focus group discussions and questionnaires, e.g., Profile of Mood States (POMS), the Fatigue Assessment Scale (FAS), and the Vanderbilt Fatigue Scale for Adults (VFS-A), and objective methods including evaluation of task performance, biological markers, and (electro)physiological measurements (1821).

The large inter-individual differences in speech comprehension and the finding that only 22% of the variance can be explained by pre-, per-, and postoperative factors (e.g., pure tone average, duration of hearing loss, surgical approach and brand) lead to an increasing interest into the influencing factors of the CI outcome to optimize rehabilitation (22). No research has yet examined the role of listening effort on inter-individual differences.

To provide proper rehabilitation, it is important to further investigate listening effort, fatigue, and listening-related fatigue among CI users and the different measurement methods available to examine this. To date, two reviews have been published (23, 24). Ohlenforst et al. (24) conducted a systematic review of listening effort and included two studies that investigated this among CI users. The review by Holman et al. (23) could only include three studies that examined fatigue among CI users. The two studies concluded that the small number of included studies and their heterogeneity resulted in inconclusive findings. Moreover, the reviews did not primarily focus on cochlear implants. Given the increasing body of literature and growing number of studies on these topics, it is crucial to provide an update. Therefore, the aim of this scoping review is to provide an overview of the current knowledge regarding listening effort, fatigue, and listening-related fatigue among adult CI users, as well as the various measurement methods to investigate this.

2. Materials and methods

The scoping review was preregistered on 7 June 2022 at the Open Science Framework (OSF) Registries (10.17605/OSF.IO/QP68U). The Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) Statements and the framework of Arksey and O’Malley were used to identify all relevant research studies (25, 26). An extensive search was carried out on 11 May 2022 and 10 January 2023 using the following databases: PubMed, Scopus, and Web of Science. Studies that investigated listening effort, fatigue, and/or listening-related fatigue among adults with CI were included. No distinction was made to exclude studies based on whether the CI users had unilateral or bilateral, prelingual or acquired hearing loss, or were unilaterally or bilaterally implanted. An overview of the search strategy for the PubMed database is represented in Table 1. After removing all duplicates, 206 abstracts were screened. Studies were excluded if the studies included a different study population (e.g., people with normal hearing, children, animals, participants without CI), did not investigate listening effort, fatigue, or listening-related fatigue (e.g., quality of life and speech intelligibility), or were protocol or congress papers. The influencing factors on listening effort, fatigue, and listening-related fatigue such as patient-related variables and device-related factors related to CI hardware and software are beyond the scope of this review. As a result, studies that investigated solely the influencing factors were excluded as well. Studies in languages other than English or Dutch were excluded. After the first screening stage, full texts were read resulting in 22 included studies. Hand-searching revealed two additional studies, bringing the number of included studies to 24. The search strategy is presented in detail in Figure 1.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Overview of the search strategy PubMed database.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. PRISMA flow diagram of the search strategy.

3. Results

Out of the 24 included studies, 3 studies had a qualitative study design and 21 studies had a quantitative study design. A total of 19 studies evaluated listening effort and 3 studies investigated fatigue and/or listening-related fatigue. Two studies included both listening effort and fatigue or listening-related fatigue in their study. All studies are presented in Appendix 1 where author, year of publication, number of participants, measurement methods, and outcome are described. These studies will be discussed in detail in this section, starting with the studies that examined listening effort followed by the studies that investigated fatigue and listening-related fatigue.

3.1. Listening effort

Twenty-one studies documented listening effort, the increased effort that is needed when listening, among CI users. Both subjective and objective measurements were used in the included studies to investigate this. Subjective measurements including focus group discussions, scales, and questionnaires were used in 18 studies. In nine studies, listening effort was objectively evaluated with dual-task paradigms, EEG, and pupillometry. Table 2 provides an overview of the different measurement techniques used in the included studies.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Information on the subjective and objective listening effort measurement methods used in the included articles.

Hughes et al. (38) organized a focus group discussion for the development of a theoretical framework of listening effort in adult CI users (n = 9), severe-to-profound hearing aids users (n = 4) waiting for their cochlear implantation, and spouses with normal hearing (n = 2). Hughes et al. (38) concluded that listening effort is a complex, multidimensional construct and defined it as “the mental work undertaken when: (1) attending to the auditory signal, (2) processing auditory information, and (3) adapting to and compensating for the hearing loss.”

3.1.1. Listening effort in CI users compared with controls with normal hearing

Eight studies compared listening effort between CI users and controls with normal hearing.

3.1.1.1. Scales and questionnaires

Based on subjective measurements, listening effort among CI users seems to be higher compared to controls with normal hearing (11, 18, 3942). Abdel-Latif and Meister (10) could confirm a higher listening effort for CI users compared to participants with normal hearing when measuring listening effort at the same signal-to-noise ratio (SNR). When a speech intelligibility of 50% and 80% was targeted in CI users and controls with normal hearing, listening effort did not differ between both groups (10). When measuring at the same performance level, the SNR differed between both groups with higher SNRs for the CI group compared to controls with normal hearing, which may explain the lack of difference between both groups (10). Although different measurement methods were used in these studies, similar results were established. The measurement method of each study is presented in Appendix 1. The similar outcome in those studies may indicate that subjective measurements can be useful to investigate listening effort in this population. It might, therefore, also be useful to conduct these subjective measurements in addition to the traditional hearing assessment, provided that further research is conducted to determine the most appropriate scale or questionnaire to use.

3.1.1.2. Electroencephalogram

Finke et al. (11) executed EEG to investigate the relationship between listening effort and endogenous ERP during the execution of an auditory oddball paradigm. According to the results based on the subjective measurement, Finke et al. (11) found a higher listening effort in CI users compared to controls with normal hearing as prolonged higher-order processing measured with the P3 was found in CI users.

3.1.1.3. Pupillometry

Wagner et al. (14) concluded that pupil dilation differed in response magnitude, time course, morphology, latencies, and number of peak dilations among the included unilateral CI users. Compared to participants with normal hearing, the function of pupil dilation differed in time course, shape, and location and the phasic event-related pupil dilation was smaller and decreased at a slower rate in CI users, which might suggest a difference in exerted effort for speech processing between both groups (14).

3.1.1.4. Dual-task paradigm

Abdel-Latif and Meister (10) and Perreau et al. (42) could not confirm differences in listening effort between CI users and controls with normal hearing executing dual-task paradigms. The primary task in the study of Abdel-Latif and Meister (10) was a speech recognition task at a performance level of 80% while for the secondary task, the participant had to push a button when a white cross appeared on the computer screen. Perreau et al. (42) executed the Hearing-in-Noise Test (HINT) consisting of 20 sentences at a performance level of 50% at 6 SNR conditions as the primary task and the Stroop test, a test where the participant had to respond in which color the written color is presented, as a secondary task (43). This contradictory finding with the other measurement methods might suggest that the executed dual-task paradigms, especially the secondary tasks, might not be sensitive enough to measure the subtle mechanisms of listening effort between both groups (10).

3.1.2. Listening effort in CI users compared with hearing impaired controls and among different CI groups

3.1.2.1. Scales and questionnaires

Dwyer et al. (40) and Alhanbali et al. (18) investigated the difference in listening effort between CI users and hearing aid users. They found no difference in subjective listening effort between CI users, hearing aid users, and single-sided deafness (SSD) based on the SSQ and the Effort Assessment scale (18, 40). These findings may suggest that self-reported listening effort cannot be predicted by the hearing level, that hearing level is not related to the self-reported listening effort, or that the questionnaires used are not sensitive enough to infer a difference between hearing-impaired groups (18).

Several studies investigated subjective listening effort among different CI groups with contradictory findings possibly caused by differences in study population and subjective measurement methods. No difference in listening effort was found between bilateral and bimodal CI users, between unilateral and bimodal CI users, and between unilateral, bilateral, and short-electrode CI users based on the Effort Assessment scale and the SSQ (8, 42, 44). Noble et al. (9), who also executed the SSQ, concluded that bilateral CI users might experience less listening effort compared to unilateral and bimodal CI users. This finding was confirmed by McRackan et al. (32) measuring listening effort with the Cochlear Implant-related Quality of Life (CIQOL) item bank. Furthermore, Gifford et al. (45) concluded that patients with bilateral electric-acoustic stimulation experience lower levels of listening effort compared to bimodal CI users based on their VAS ratings of listening difficulty. Devocht et al. (46) reported that the decline of listening effort with increasing SNR is bigger for bimodal CI users than for unilateral CI users. The results of Noble et al. (9), McRackan et al. (32), and Devocht et al. (46) are in consistency with the finding that better hearing preservation in the implanted ear will result in better speech understanding (45).

Studies investigating the impact after cochlear implantation all executed the full version of the SSQ (3, 9, 40, 47). After cochlear implantation, subjective listening effort seems to decrease in CI users with SSD and with asymmetric hearing loss (3, 47). Dwyer et al. (40) reported a significant decrease in listening effort in bilateral and bimodal CI users compared to SSD patients, while Noble et al. (9) stated that listening effort decreases after unilateral or bilateral implantation but would not decrease significantly in bimodal CI users. The lack of a significant decrease in bimodal CI users after implantation is somehow unexpected and therefore Noble et al. (9) stated that further research is necessary in the specific patient group of bimodal CI users.

3.1.2.2. Dual-task paradigm

In accordance with their subjective measurement method, Sladen et al. (44) could not find a difference in listening effort between bilateral and bimodal CI users using a dual-task paradigm, and no significant difference in listening effort between unilateral and bimodal CI users was found by Yüksel et al. (48). However, Perreau et al. (42), who established a difference in listening effort between unilateral and bilateral CI users based on their subjective measurement, could not confirm this by executing their dual-task paradigm. The sensitivity of the secondary task might have influenced these results (10).

3.1.2.3. Pupillometry

Burg et al. (49) compared listening effort between the bilateral and unilateral conditions in 12 CI users. An increased pupil dilation, indicating higher listening effort, was found for the bilateral condition compared to the unilateral condition (49). With increasing task performance, pupil dilation increases. The increased task engagement for the bilateral condition might explain the higher listening effort for this condition (49).

3.1.2.4. Electroencephalogram

Dual-task paradigms and pupillometry are indirect measures of brain activity, whereas EEG measurements may lead to a better knowledge of the brain regions responsible for increased listening effort (36). Dimitrijevic et al. (36) investigated the relationship between listening effort, alpha power, and neural entrainment measuring EEG when executing the digits-in-noise (DIN) test, in which the participant had to repeat three spoken digits in the presence of background noise (50). The alpha power may be a neural correlate of listening effort and neural entrainment relates the acoustic feature fluctuations with the brain activity fluctuations (12, 36). Dimitrijevic et al. (36) concluded that higher left frontal inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) alpha power results in an increment of subjective listening effort measured with the NASA Task Load scale. Furthermore, the coherence of the speech envelope between the auditory cortex and the envelope of typical human syllables situated at the 2–5 Hertz (Hz) range was investigated. Dimitrijevic et al. (36) found that listening effort increases with a lower coherence between the speech envelope and the auditory cortex in the 2–5 Hz range. Paul et al. (12) investigated the relationship between subjective listening effort measured with a 10-point scale and the within-subject variability of cortical alpha power. In contrast to the study of Dimitrijevic et al. (36) and Paul et al. (12) found that the left frontal IFG alpha power declines when subjective listening effort increases although this result was not significant. This contradictory finding may be due to differences in study design and signal processing (12). Furthermore, Paul et al. (12) found that the alpha power in the parietal brain regions seems to be higher for medium subjective listening effort while a lower alpha power was found for lower and higher subjective listening effort (12).

Speech-sound processing in CI users can be measured using auditory ERP, more specifically with the N1–N2 complex. Finke et al. (11) executed an auditory ERP measurement to investigate the relationship between the neural processing of words in different background conditions, the verbal abilities of CI users, and speech intelligibility. They found a relationship between the ERP measurements and the subjective listening effort, with higher listening effort ratings being associated with prolonged N2/N4 latencies (11).

3.1.3. Correlation between subjective and objective measurements

Two of the studies that used both subjective and objective measurements to investigate listening effort discussed the correlation between both measurements. Abdel-Latif and Meister (10) and Perreau et al. (42) found no correlation between their Adaptive Categorical Listening Effort Scaling (ECALES) and 10-point scale and their dual-task paradigm. The low sensitivity of the secondary task of the dual-task paradigm might be a reason why no correlation between the subjective and objective measurement could be found (10). On the other hand, this lack of correlation might suggest that different measurement methods tap into different underlying dimensions of listening effort (10, 51).

3.2. Fatigue and listening-related fatigue

Fatigue is “a general feeling of being tired, worn out, or sluggish” (15), while prolonged listening in situations that require a lot of listening effort may lead to listening-related fatigue (16). Five studies documented fatigue and/or listening-related fatigue among CI users. The different measurement techniques used in these studies are presented in Table 3.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Measurement methods of fatigue and listening-related fatigue used in the included articles.

3.2.1. The effect of CI on fatigue and listening-related fatigue

Two out of the five included studies examined fatigue and listening-related fatigue using a qualitative study design through focus group discussions (19, 53). McRackan et al. (53) developed a theoretical framework to understand fatigue in CI users who reported increased levels of general fatigue. The participants were stratified into three focus groups based on their communication abilities with their implants. The same levels of fatigue were mentioned regardless of the performance, although the increased levels of fatigue were mentioned by the middle- and high-performing groups only for complex listening situations, while the low-performance group mentioned fatigue in all communication situations (53). Davis et al. (19) used a focus group to investigate listening-related fatigue in hearing aid users and CI users to develop a theoretical framework. Listening-related fatigue might be driven by situational determinants, characteristics of the listening situation and motivation, and coping strategies used in these situations (19). Only 3 out of the 43 participants in this study were CI users, which might have limited their input. Although qualitative research is useful for understanding the concept of fatigue and listening-related fatigue, the results might not be generalizable to the CI population as only a small study population was included.

3.2.2. Fatigue and listening-related fatigue in CI users compared with controls with normal hearing, hearing impaired controls and among different CI groups

3.2.2.1. Scales and questionnaires

Dwyer et al. (41) investigated fatigue in a group of students consisting of CI users, hearing aid users, and controls with normal hearing with the fatigue subscale of the POMS. Additionally, Dwyer et al. (41) developed a three-item questionnaire to investigate fatigue and listening-related fatigue. They found no difference in levels of fatigue between CI users and controls with normal hearing based on the POMS but observed higher listening-related fatigue when executing the three-item Likert scale (41). The association could be confirmed by Alhanbali et al. (18) who found increased levels of fatigue on their FAS among CI users, hearing aid users, and SSD patients compared to the participants with normal hearing without difference in fatigue between the hearing impaired groups. Furthermore, FAS scores above percentile 95 of the control group were defined as extreme levels of fatigue by Alhanbali et al. (51), who found extreme levels of fatigue in 10% of the CI users. However, Dwyer et al. (41) stated that none of the CI users reported severe levels of fatigue when defined as a score that exceeds the normative means by ±1.5 SDs (41). Both studies differ in sample size, age of participants, and definition of extreme/severe fatigue. The participants in the study of Dwyer et al. (41) were all students, while Alhanbali et al. (18) included older participants. As the study population of Dwyer et al. (41) is rather specific, the results may not be generalizable to the entire CI population. The aim of the POMS is to measure different moods, with fatigue being only one of them. As a result, the POMS may not be sensitive enough to measure listening-related fatigue as a significant difference between the CI users and the participants with normal hearing could be found in the same study population when using the self-developed three-item questionnaire to examine listening-related fatigue.

The impact on fatigue after the sequential implantation of a second CI was investigated by Härkönen et al. (54). With the second CI, fatigue after the working day was reduced (54). Caution is necessary to interpret these data as only one question was used to evaluate fatigue before and after implantation.

3.2.2.2. Objective measurements

All results in this section were gained using subjective measurements. There are, to the best of our knowledge, currently no studies that investigated fatigue and listening-related fatigue among CI users using objective measurements. However, it may be possible to investigate this in this population when monitoring task performance to observe the fatigue-related decrements in cognitive processing or with the use of biological markers and (electro)physiological techniques (15).

3.3. Association between listening effort and fatigue

Three studies investigated the association between listening effort, fatigue, and listening-related fatigue among CI users. High levels of listening effort might be associated with increased levels of fatigue and listening-related fatigue (19, 41, 51). As it was already suggested that the POMS was not sensitive enough to measure listening-related fatigue, this may explain the lack of association found between listening effort and fatigue using this questionnaire (41).

4. Discussion

This scoping review aimed to provide an overview of the current knowledge of listening effort, fatigue, and listening-related fatigue among CI users and the different measurement methods. Due to the heterogeneity of the methodologies used to measure this, inconclusive results were found.

The results of this scoping review show that cochlear implantation may reduce listening effort in hearing-impaired individuals. However, when listening effort in CI users was compared with controls with normal hearing, hearing impaired controls without CI, and between unilateral, bilateral, and bimodal CI users, the results of the included studies were inconclusive. A possible explanation for these inconclusive results is the variance in the study population. There are great variances in the number and age range of participants and the composition of the control group. Additionally, the CI users in the studies differ in type and brand of CI and in the distribution of unilateral, bilateral, and bimodal CI.

A second possible explanation for the inconclusive results is the use of different measurement methods. Although questionnaires, dual-task paradigms, pupillometry, and EEG measurements have good reliability, the lack of correlation between these measurements may support the idea that listening effort is a multidimensional construct, which makes it difficult to measure with only one measurement method (51). This hypothesis is supported by the Framework for Understanding Effortful Listening (FUEL) model by Pichora-Fuller et al. (7). This model stated that a mismatch of the cognitive demands and cognitive resources, allocated by multiple dimensions (e.g., arousal level, evaluation of task demands, and capacity), will lead to effort (7).

Because listening effort is a complex multidimensional construct, it is not yet clear how it should be measured. This might explain the variance in measurement methods used in the included studies and stresses the need for clarification on which method should be used to measure listening effort among CI users to support further research into these topics. This knowledge is crucial to determine the impact of hearing loss, CI, and CI parameters on listening effort. The subjective, behavioral, and physiological measurements have their advantages and disadvantages. First of all, subjective measurement techniques are quick, easy, and fast to administer in clinical practice but participants may give socially desirable answers or the answers may be influenced by their interpretation (10, 51). In the included studies, a great variance of scales and (parts of) questionnaires were used. However, comparable results were achieved when comparing CI users with controls with normal hearing when utilizing different scales and questionnaires. When investigating the difference between CI users and hearing aid users and between different CI groups, the self-reported questionnaires yielded conflicting results. These inconsistent results were also reported in the systematic review conducted by Ohlenforst et al. (24). This review aimed to investigate if hearing loss and hearing aids have an impact on listening effort (24). The authors noted a great variety in scales and questionnaires and recommended avoiding such variety to facilitate further research (24). To our knowledge, only one validated instrument has been developed specifically to measure listening effort. The Effort Assessment scale (EAS) developed by Alhanbali et al. (18) could be a potential questionnaire that is appropriate for assessing subjective listening effort among the hearing-impaired population. The EAS may be useful in clinical practice as it comprises only six questions, three from the listening effort subscale of the SSQ and three additional questions. Another strength of the EAS is that it is not specifically designed to measure listening effort in CI users, making it suitable for assessing hearing aid users and controls with normal hearing. This ensures that all research on this topic can be conducted using the same questionnaire, making it easier to compare results. Based on our review, it appears that the subscale of the SSQ as well as the EAS can measure differences in listening effort between CI users and controls with normal hearing. However, these measures do not appear to be sensitive enough to compare listening effort between CI users and hearing-impaired controls without CI or among CI groups. Another questionnaire deserving attention is the Listening Effort Questionnaire – Cochlear Implant (LEQ-CI). This questionnaire is currently in development and has not yet been validated (55). The LEQ-CI comprises 29 questions divided into four scales: effort of attending, effort of processing, effort associated with adapting and compensating for a hearing loss, and motivation. Due to the comprehensive survey and questioning of various aspects of listening effort, this questionnaire also holds promise for future research in the CI population. One disadvantage of the LEQ-CI is that it is specifically designed to measure listening effort in the CI population and therefore may be less suitable for investigating the hearing aid population and controls with normal hearing. The disadvantages of the self-reported subjective measurement methods can be avoided by using objective measurements. During a dual-task paradigm, two tasks have to be executed at the same time, resulting in this method mimicking a real-life situation where multitasking is necessary (51). This behavioral measurement is also easy to administer and interpret but may be too time-consuming to perform in a clinical setting (51). In our review, the dual-task paradigms did not reveal any differences between CI users and controls with normal hearing, hearing aid users, or between CI groups, and were not associated with subjective measures. The systematic review conducted by Ohlenforst et al. (24) revealed varying outcomes among the studies that utilized dual-task paradigms in patients with hearing loss and hearing aids. Both our study and the review by Ohlenforst et al. (24) suggest that differences in tasks may account for the differences in listening effort found. Furthermore, it remains unclear whether the absence of differences in listening effort measured with dual-task paradigms suggests that this measurement is not sensitive enough to measure small changes or differences in listening effort, or that the tasks of the paradigms assess different cognitive processing stages (10, 24). Pupillometry and EEG measurements are useful to obtain a temporal indication of the mental processing without conscious control of the participant (14, 51). All the studies included in our review reported a difference in listening effort between CI users and controls with normal hearing. These findings are consistent with the systematic review conducted by Ohlenforst et al. (24) as most of their included studies also found elevated levels of listening effort caused by hearing loss compared to controls with normal hearing and a positive effect of hearing aid amplification. As a result, it can be postulated that pupillometry and EEG measurements are useful for evaluating listening effort. However, objective measurements are more complex to execute in the clinical practice and are susceptible to external factors such as motivation, mental state, and task engagement of the CI user (14, 51). This is an important disadvantage of these measurement methods. Additionally, there are currently no standards for the objective measurements, limiting their use to within-person comparisons. Nevertheless, further exploration of the EEG measurements may prove beneficial for research purposes in order to broaden our understanding of the brain regions that are responsible for an increasement in listening effort.

It was not in our scope to discuss possible influencing factors of listening effort though inconclusive results were reported in the literature. It is currently not clear what the impact is of patient-related factors such as age, duration of hearing impairment prior to implantation, cognition, and employability on listening effort (10, 32, 42), and whether speech recognition is associated with listening effort (912, 32, 39). CI users with a lower working memory capacity seem to exert higher levels of listening effort (39) although Abdel-Latif and Meister (10) and Perreau et al. (42) found no association between listening effort and working memory capacity or processing speed in CI users. Previous studies could not establish whether specific CI algorithms and microphone technologies reduce listening effort or if listening effort is associated with spectral resolution (44, 56, 57). On the other hand, it has already been suggested that a slower speaking rate and the availability of a relevant semantic context may reduce listening effort in CI users (58, 59). Furthermore, listening effort seems to decrease with increasing SNR levels (12, 42, 60) although, even in favorable listening conditions, listening is still an effort for CI users (60). Considering that listening remains an effort for CI users and the higher listening effort reported in CI compared to controls with normal hearing, it is interesting to further explore the influencing factors as there is still much ambiguity about it.

Only five studies investigated the effect of fatigue or listening-related fatigue in CI users, which highlights the need for further research as no conclusions can be made based on the available literature. In addition, studies also differ in study population, methodology, and measurement methods. Furthermore, there is a need for standardized terminology regarding fatigue and listening-related fatigue as well as a standardized methodology to measure it. Fatigue is a complex phenomenon that encompasses both short-term and long-term fatigue and can be influenced not only by hearing loss but also by factors such as personal motivation, emotions, lifestyle, and coping strategies (6164). Similarly to the measurement methods for evaluating listening effort, subjective and objective measurements can be used to determine fatigue. In the studies included in our review, both the FAS and the subscale of the POMS were employed to measure fatigue, however, contradictory outcomes were observed (18, 41). The study conducted by Hornsby et al. (15) in hearing-impaired patients supported the lack of difference found by Dwyer et al. (41) between hearing-impaired individuals and controls with normal hearing, as no difference was found when administering the POMS and the multi-dimensional fatigue symptoms inventory-short form, which is a fatigue questionnaire developed to investigate cancer-related fatigue (65). Recently, a promising validated listening-related fatigue questionnaire, the VFS-A, was developed by Hornsby et al. (20). The VFS-A can be useful to execute along with objective measurements such as monitoring the task performance, biological markers, and (elektro)physiological measurements. Studies that conducted objective measurements to investigate fatigue and listening-related fatigue among the hearing-impaired population are scarce (51, 66). To the best of our knowledge, only one study implemented a dual-task paradigm to monitor task performance and proposed a decrease in fatigue when utilizing hearing aids (67). This measurement method has some limitations as boredom or a lack of engagement can also lead to a decrease in task performance, or the learning effect can lead to an absence of a decrease in task performance while fatigue still develops (15, 51, 61). In the review conducted by Holman et al. (66), which investigated the effect of hearing loss on listening-related fatigue, all studies except one used subjective measures. Most of the studies included in the review reported elevated levels of fatigue or listening-related fatigue in the hearing-impaired population (66). Nevertheless, it has been suggested that decreased cortisol levels are seen when fatigue occurs (6870). While Dwyer et al. (41) measured the cortisol levels, they did not detect any difference between CI users and controls with normal hearing. This finding is supported by Hick and Tharpe (71) and Kramer et al. (72), suggesting that measuring cortisol levels might not be sensitive enough to detect differences in fatigue. In contrast, Bess et al. (73) discovered elevated cortisol levels among children with hearing impairments compared to peers with normal hearing. Other objective measurements used to investigate fatigue are ERP measurements, in which Key et al. (74) identified a difference in the P300 before and after performing a speech task. Similar to these studies, our review did not reveal a consensus regarding the measurement methods of fatigue. The absence of correlation between the measurement methods may indicate a low reliability and sensitivity of these measurements or that fatigue, similar to listening effort, is a multidimensional construct, possibly consisting of different types of fatigue (e.g., physical fatigue, mental fatigue, emotional fatigue, and vigor) (65). One important limitation requiring further research attention is the absence of objective measurements to assess listening-related fatigue, as they are currently unavailable, to the best of our knowledge. An important limitation of a scoping review is the lack of exclusion of studies based on their methodology, which may limit this review by the quality of the included studies. Moreover, a language bias could occur as possible interesting studies in languages other than Dutch or English were excluded.

5. Conclusion

Recently, there has been an increased interest in listening effort, fatigue, and listening-related fatigue to provide proper rehabilitation. Research in these areas is in its early stages. Current research employs a wide range of measurement methods, which makes it difficult to compare studies. The utilization of a uniform definition and measurement of listening effort, fatigue, and listening-related fatigue will enable further research to be compared. However, a standardized measurement method has not yet been developed for CI users or hearing-impaired groups. With the rising number of CI users, there is a need to review the current literature that focuses on this population. This scoping review suggests that CI users experience higher levels of listening effort, fatigue, and listening-related fatigue. When it is clear how listening effort and fatigue should be measured, further research can determine the impact of a CI on listening effort, fatigue, and listening-related fatigue as well as the factors that contribute to a decrease or increase in these effects. Furthermore, there is a need for guidelines to measure listening effort, fatigue, and listening-related fatigue in clinical practice as this might eventually optimize the current rehabilitation of the CI population.

Author contributions

CP: Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. LJ: Conceptualization, Supervision, Writing – review & editing. ML: Conceptualization, Supervision, Writing – review & editing. GM: Writing – review & editing. AG: Writing – review & editing. OV: Writing – review & editing. VR: Conceptualization, Supervision, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. This work was supported by an unrestrictive research grant (Cochlear Benelux grant: Investigator-Initiated Research Grant IIR-2337) paid to the hospital.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

1. Völter, C, Götze, L, Haubitz, I, Dazert, S, and Thomas, JP. Benefits of Cochlear implantation in middle-aged and older adults. Clin Interv Aging. (2020) 15:1555–68. doi: 10.2147/CIA.S255363

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. McRackan, TR, Bauschard, M, Hatch, JL, Franko-Tobin, E, Droghini, HR, Velozo, CA, et al. Meta-analysis of Cochlear implantation outcomes evaluated with general health-related patient-reported outcome measures. Otol Neurotol. (2018) 39:29–36. doi: 10.1097/MAO.0000000000001620

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Dillon, MT, Buss, E, Rooth, MA, King, ER, Deres, EJ, Buchman, CA, et al. Effect of Cochlear implantation on quality of life in adults with unilateral hearing loss. Audiol Neurotol. (2018) 22:259–71. doi: 10.1159/000484079

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Hirschfelder, A, Gräbel, S, and Olze, H. The impact of cochlear implantation on quality of life: the role of audiologic performance and variables. Otolaryngol Head Neck Surg. (2008) 138:357–62. doi: 10.1016/j.otohns.2007.10.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Budenz, CL, Cosetti, MK, Coelho, DH, Birenbaum, B, Babb, J, Waltzman, SB, et al. The effects of cochlear implantation on speech perception in older adults. J Am Geriatr Soc. (2011) 59:446–53. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2010.03310.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Fetterman, BL, and Domico, EH. Speech recognition in background noise of cochlear implant patients. Otolaryngol Head Neck Surg. (2002) 126:257–63. doi: 10.1067/mhn.2002.123044

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Pichora-Fuller, MK, Kramer, SE, Eckert, MA, Edwards, B, Hornsby, BWY, Humes, LE, et al. Hearing impairment and cognitive energy: the framework for understanding effortful listening (FUEL). Ear Hear. (2016) 37:5S–27S. doi: 10.1097/AUD.0000000000000312

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Farinetti, A, Roman, S, Mancini, J, Baumstarck-Barrau, K, Meller, R, Lavieille, JP, et al. Quality of life in bimodal hearing users (unilateral cochlear implants and contralateral hearing aids). Eur Arch Otorhinolaryngol. (2015) 272:3209–15. doi: 10.1007/s00405-014-3377-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Noble, W, Tyler, R, Dunn, C, and Bhullar, N. Unilateral and bilateral cochlear implants and the implant-plus-hearing-aid profile: comparing self-assessed and measured abilities. Int J Audiol. (2008) 47:505–14. doi: 10.1080/14992020802070770

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Abdel-Latif, M. Speech recognition and listening effort in Cochlear implant recipients and Normal-hearing listeners. Front Neurosci. (2022) 15:725412. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2021.725412

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Finke, M, Büchner, A, Ruigendijk, E, Meyer, M, and Sandmann, P. On the relationship between auditory cognition and speech intelligibility in cochlear implant users: an ERP study. Neuropsychologia. (2016) 87:169–81. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2016.05.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Paul, BT, Chen, J, Le, T, Lin, V, and Dimitrijevic, A. Cortical alpha oscillations in cochlear implant users reflect subjective listening effort during speech-in-noise perception. PLoS One. (2021) 16:e0254162. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0254162

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Wild, CJ, Yusuf, A, Wilson, DE, Peelle, JE, Davis, MH, and Johnsrude, IS. Effortful listening: the processing of degraded speech depends critically on attention. J Neurosci. (2012) 32:14010–21. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1528-12.2012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Wagner, AE, Nagels, L, Toffanin, P, Opie, JM, and Baskent, D. Individual variations in effort: assessing Pupillometry for the hearing impaired. Trends Hear. (2019) 23:233121651984559. doi: 10.1177/2331216519845596

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Hornsby, BW, Naylor, G, and Bess, FH. A taxonomy of fatigue concepts and their relation to hearing loss. Ear Hear. (2016) 37:136s–44s. doi: 10.1097/AUD.0000000000000289

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. McGarrigle, D, Stewart, K, and Munro, A. Measuring listening-related effort and fatigue in school-aged children using pupillometry. J Exp Child Psychol. (2017) 161:95–112. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2017.04.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Hua, H, Emilsson, M, Ellis, R, Widén, S, Möller, C, and Lyxell, B. Cognitive skills and the effect of noise on perceived effort in employees with aided hearing impairment and normal hearing. Noise Health. (2014) 16:79–88. doi: 10.4103/1463-1741.132085

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Alhanbali, D, and Lloyd, M. Self-reported listening-related effort and fatigue in hearing-impaired adults. Ear Hear. (2017) 38:e39–48. doi: 10.1097/AUD.0000000000000361

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Davis, H, Schlundt, D, Bonnet, K, Camarata, S, Bess, FH, and Hornsby, B. Understanding listening-related fatigue: perspectives of adults with hearing loss. Int J Audiol. (2021) 60:458–68. doi: 10.1080/14992027.2020.1834631

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Hornsby, C, Cho, D, and McGarrigle, B. Development and validation of the Vanderbilt fatigue scale for adults (VFS-A). Psychol Assess. (2021) 33:777–88. doi: 10.1037/pas0001021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Michielsen, HJ, Vries, J, GLV, H, FVD, V, and Sijtsma, K. Examination of the dimensionality of fatigue. Eur J Psychol Assess. (2004) 20:39–48. doi: 10.1027/1015-5759.20.1.39

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Lazard, DS, Vincent, C, Venail, F, van de Heyning, P, Truy, E, Sterkers, O, et al. Pre-, per- and Postoperative factors affecting performance of Postlinguistically deaf adults using Cochlear implants: a new conceptual model over time. PLoS One. (2012) 7:e48739. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0048739

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Holman, JA, Hornsby, BWY, Bess, FH, and Naylor, G. Can listening-related fatigue influence well-being? Examining associations between hearing loss, fatigue, activity levels and well-being. Int J Audiol. (2021) 60:47–59. doi: 10.1080/14992027.2020.1853261

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Ohlenforst, B, Zekveld, AA, Jansma, EP, Wang, Y, Naylor, G, Lorens, A, et al. Effects of hearing impairment and hearing aid amplification on listening effort: a systematic review. Ear Hear. (2017) 38:267–81. doi: 10.1097/AUD.0000000000000396

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Moher, D, Liberati, A, Tetzlaff, J, and Altman, DG. Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: the PRISMA statement. PLoS Med. (2009) 6:e1000097. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.1000097

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Arksey, H, and O'Malley, L. Scoping studies: towards a methodological framework. Int J Soc Res Methodol. (2005) 8:19–32. doi: 10.1080/1364557032000119616

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Hennink, MM. Focus group discussions. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press (2013).

Google Scholar

28. Wewers, ME, and Lowe, NK. A critical review of visual analogue scales in the measurement of clinical phenomena. Res Nurs Health. (1990) 13:227–36. doi: 10.1002/nur.4770130405

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Gatehouse, S, and Noble, W. The speech, spatial and qualities of hearing scale (SSQ). Int J Audiol. (2004) 43:85–99. doi: 10.1080/14992020400050014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Likert, R. A technique for the measurement of attitudes. Arch Psychol. (1932) 22:142–55.

Google Scholar

31. Krueger, M, Schulte, M, Brand, T, and Holube, I. Development of an adaptive scaling method for subjective listening effort. J Acoust Soc Am. (2017) 141:4680–93. doi: 10.1121/1.4986938

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. McRackan, TR, Hand, BN, Velozo, CA, and Dubno, JR. Association of Demographic and Hearing-Related Factors with Cochlear implant-related quality of life. JAMA Otolaryngol Head Neck Surg. (2019) 145:422–30. doi: 10.1001/jamaoto.2019.0055

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Hart, SG, and Staveland, LE. Development of NASA-TLX (task load index): results of empirical and theoretical research In: PA Hancock and N Meshkati, editors. Advances in psychology. 52: North-Holland (1988). 139–83.

Google Scholar

34. Gagné, J-P, Besser, J, and Lemke, U. Behavioral assessment of listening effort using a dual-task paradigm:a review. Trends Hear. (2017) 21:2331216516687287. doi: 10.1177/2331216516687287

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Amichetti, NM, Neukam, J, Kinney, AJ, Capach, N, March, SU, Svirsky, MA, et al. Adults with cochlear implants can use prosody to determine the clausal structure of spoken sentences. J Acoust Soc Am. (2021) 150:4315–28. doi: 10.1121/10.0008899

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Dimitrijevic, A, Smith, ML, Kadis, DS, and Moore, DR. Neural indices of listening effort in noisy environments. Sci Rep. (2019) 9:11278. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-47643-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Michael-Titus, A, Revest, P, and Shortland, P. 3—CLINICAL EXAMINATION In: A Michael-Titus, P Revest, and P Shortland, editors. The nervous system. 2nd ed. Edinburgh: Churchill Livingstone (2010). 47–58.

Google Scholar

38. Hughes, SE, Hutchings, HA, Rapport, FL, McMahon, CM, and Boisvert, I. Social connectedness and perceived listening effort in adult cochlear implant users: a grounded theory to establish content validity for a new patient-reported outcome measure. Ear Hear. (2018) 39:922–34. doi: 10.1097/AUD.0000000000000553

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Dingemanse, G, and Goedegebure, A. The relation of hearing-specific patient-reported outcome measures with speech perception measures and acceptable noise levels in cochlear implant users. Int J Audiol. (2020) 59:416–26. doi: 10.1080/14992027.2020.1727033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Dwyer, F, and Reeder, E. Effects of unilateral input and mode of hearing in the better ear: self-reported performance using the speech, spatial and qualities of hearing scale. Ear Hear. (2014) 35:126–36. doi: 10.1097/AUD.0b013e3182a3648b

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Dwyer, G, Bess, D, and Spahr, H. Diurnal cortisol levels and subjective ratings of effort and fatigue in adult cochlear implant users: a pilot study. Am J Audiol. (2019) 28:686–96. doi: 10.1044/2019_AJA-19-0009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Perreau, AE, Wu, YH, Tatge, B, Irwin, D, and Corts, D. Listening effort measured in adults with normal hearing and cochlear implants. J Am Acad Audiol. (2017) 28:685–97. doi: 10.3766/jaaa.16014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Stroop, JR. Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. J Exp Psychol Gen. (1992) 121:15–23. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.121.1.15

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Sladen, DP, Nie, Y, and Berg, K. Investigating speech recognition and listening effort with different device configurations in adult cochlear implant users. Cochlear Implants Int. (2018) 19:119–30. doi: 10.1080/14670100.2018.1424513

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Gifford, RH, Davis, TJ, Sunderhaus, LW, Menapace, C, Buck, B, Crosson, J, et al. Combined electric and acoustic stimulation with hearing preservation: effect of Cochlear implant low-frequency cutoff on speech understanding and perceived listening difficulty. Ear Hear. (2017) 38:539–53. doi: 10.1097/AUD.0000000000000418

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Devocht, J, Chalupper, S, and George, V. The benefits of bimodal aiding on extended dimensions of speech perception: intelligibility, listening effort, and sound quality. Trends Hear. (2017) 21:233121651772790. doi: 10.1177/2331216517727900

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Lopez, EM, Dillon, MT, Park, LR, Rooth, MA, Richter, ME, Thompson, NJ, et al. Influence of Cochlear implant use on perceived listening effort in adult and pediatric cases of unilateral and asymmetric hearing loss. Otol Neurotol. (2021) 42:e1234–41. doi: 10.1097/MAO.0000000000003261

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Yüksel, M, Taşdemir, İ, and Çiprut, A. Listening effort in Prelingual Cochlear implant recipients: effects of spectral and temporal auditory processing and contralateral acoustic hearing. Otol Neurotol. (2022) 43:e1077–84. doi: 10.1097/MAO.0000000000003690

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Burg, EA, Thakkar, TD, and Litovsky, RY. Interaural speech asymmetry predicts bilateral speech intelligibility but not listening effort in adults with bilateral cochlear implants. Front Neurosci. (2022) 16:1038856. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2022.1038856

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Smits, C, Kapteyn, TS, and Houtgast, T. Development and validation of an automatic speech-in-noise screening test by telephone. Int J Audiol. (2004) 43:15–28. doi: 10.1080/14992020400050004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Alhanbali, D, and Millman, M. Measures of listening effort are multidimensional. Ear Hear. (2019) 40:1084–97. doi: 10.1097/AUD.0000000000000697

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. McNair, D, Lorr, M, and Droppleman, L. Manual for the profile of mood states. San Diego, CA: Educational and Industrial Testing Services (1971).

Google Scholar

53. McRackan, TR, Velozo, CA, Holcomb, MA, Camposeo, EL, Hatch, JL, Meyer, TA, et al. Use of adult patient focus groups to develop the initial item bank for a cochlear implant quality-of-life instrument. JAMA Otolaryngol Head Neck Surg. (2017) 143:975–82. doi: 10.1001/jamaoto.2017.1182

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Härkönen, K, Kivekäs, I, Rautiainen, M, Kotti, V, Sivonen, V, and Vasama, JP. Sequential bilateral cochlear implantation improves working performance, quality of life, and quality of hearing. Acta Otolaryngol. (2015) 135:440–6. doi: 10.3109/00016489.2014.990056

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Hughes, SE, Frances, R, Alan, W, Isabelle, B, Catherine, MM, and Hayley, AH. Study protocol for the validation of a new patient-reported outcome measure (PROM) of listening effort in cochlear implantation: the listening effort questionnaire-cochlear implant (LEQ-CI). BMJ Open. (2019) 9:e028881. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2018-028881

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Pals, C, Sarampalis, A, Beynon, A, Stainsby, T, and Başkent, D. Effect of spectral channels on speech recognition, comprehension, and listening effort in Cochlear-implant users. Trends Hear. (2020) 24:233121652090461. doi: 10.1177/2331216520904617

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Büchner, A, Schwebs, M, and Lenarz, T. Speech understanding and listening effort in cochlear implant users–microphone beamformers lead to significant improvements in noisy environments. Cochlear Implants Int. (2020) 21:1–8. doi: 10.1080/14670100.2019.1661567

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Winn, M. Rapid release from listening effort resulting from semantic context, and effects of spectral degradation and Cochlear implants. Trends Hear. (2016) 20:233121651666972. doi: 10.1177/2331216516669723

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Winn, MB, and Teece, KH. Listening effort is not the same as speech intelligibility score. Trends Hear. (2021) 25:233121652110276. doi: 10.1177/23312165211027688

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Dingemanse, G, and Goedegebure, A. Listening effort in Cochlear implant users: the effect of speech intelligibility, noise reduction processing, and working memory capacity on the pupil dilation response. J Speech Lang Hear Res. (2022) 65:392–404. doi: 10.1044/2021_JSLHR-21-00230

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Hockey, R. The psychology of fatigue: Work, effort and control, vol. xv. New York, NY, US: Cambridge University Press (2013). 272 p.

Google Scholar

62. Holman, JA, Drummond, A, Hughes, SE, and Naylor, G. Hearing impairment and daily-life fatigue: a qualitative study. Int J Audiol. (2019) 58:408–16. doi: 10.1080/14992027.2019.1597284

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Barnes, CM, and Van Dyne, L. I'm tired': differential effects of physical and emotional fatigue on workload management strategies. Hum Relat. (2009) 62:59–92. doi: 10.1177/0018726708099518

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Herlambang, MB, Taatgen, NA, and Cnossen, F. The role of motivation as a factor in mental fatigue. Hum Factors. (2019) 61:1171–85. doi: 10.1177/0018720819828569

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Stein, KD, Jacobsen, PB, Blanchard, CM, and Thors, C. Further validation of the multidimensional fatigue symptom inventory-short form. J Pain Symptom Manag. (2004) 27:14–23. doi: 10.1016/j.jpainsymman.2003.06.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Holman, JA, Drummond, A, and Naylor, G. The effect of hearing loss and hearing device fitting on fatigue in adults: a systematic review. Ear Hear. (2021) 42:1–11. doi: 10.1097/AUD.0000000000000909

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Hornsby, BW. The effects of hearing aid use on listening effort and mental fatigue associated with sustained speech processing demands. Ear Hear. (2013) 34:523–34. doi: 10.1097/AUD.0b013e31828003d8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Nemeroff, CB. The neurobiology of depression. Sci Am. (1998) 278:42–9. doi: 10.1038/scientificamerican0698-42

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Cleare, AJ. The HPA axis and the genesis of chronic fatigue syndrome. Trends Endocrinol Metab. (2004) 15:55–9. doi: 10.1016/j.tem.2003.12.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Kumari, M, Badrick, E, Chandola, T, Adam, EK, Stafford, M, Marmot, MG, et al. Cortisol secretion and fatigue: associations in a community based cohort. Psychoneuroendocrinology. (2009) 34:1476–85. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2009.05.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Hick, CB, and Tharpe, AM. Listening effort and fatigue in school-age children with and without hearing loss. J Speech Lang Hear Res. (2002) 45:573–84. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2002/046)

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

72. Kramer, SE, Teunissen, CE, and Zekveld, AA. Cortisol, chromogranin a, and pupillary responses evoked by speech recognition tasks in normally hearing and hard-of-hearing listeners: a pilot study. Ear Hear. (2016) 37:126s–35s. doi: 10.1097/AUD.0000000000000311

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

73. Bess, FH, Gustafson, SJ, Corbett, BA, Lambert, EW, Camarata, SM, and Hornsby, BW. Salivary cortisol profiles of children with hearing loss. Ear Hear. (2016) 37:334–44. doi: 10.1097/AUD.0000000000000256

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

74. Key, AP, Gustafson, SJ, Rentmeester, L, Hornsby, BWY, and Bess, FH. Speech-processing fatigue in children: auditory event-related potential and behavioral measures. J Speech Lang Hear Res. (2017) 60:2090–104. doi: 10.1044/2016_JSLHR-H-16-0052

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Appendix 1

Summary of the study characteristics classified in alphabetic order.

CI, cochlear implant; NH, normal hearing; LE, listening effort; HA, hearing aid; SSD, single sided deafness; EAS, Electric-Acoustic Stimulation system; IFG, inferior frontal gyrus; ERPD, event-related pupil dilation.

Keywords: cochlear implantation, listening effort, fatigue, listening-related fatigue, scoping review, bilateral hearing loss

Citation: Philips C, Jacquemin L, Lammers MJW, Mertens G, Gilles A, Vanderveken OM and Van Rompaey V (2023) Listening effort and fatigue among cochlear implant users: a scoping review. Front. Neurol. 14:1278508. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2023.1278508

Received: 16 August 2023; Accepted: 18 September 2023;
Published: 03 November 2023.

Edited by:

Yoav Gimmon, University of Haifa, Israel

Reviewed by:

James W. Dias, Medical University of South Carolina, United States
Davide Brotto, University of Padua, Italy

Copyright © 2023 Philips, Jacquemin, Lammers, Mertens, Gilles, Vanderveken and Van Rompaey. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Cato Philips, cato.philips@uza.be

These authors have contributed equally to this work

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.