- 1Psychology Service, Hospital of Bressanone (SABES-ASDAA) - Teaching Hospital of the Paracelsus Medical Private University (PMU), Bressanone-Brixen, Italy
- 2Department of Neurorehabilitation, Hospital of Vipiteno (SABES-ASDAA), Vipiteno-Sterzing, Italy - Teaching Hospital of the Paracelsus Medical Private University (PMU), Salzburg, Austria
- 3Istituti Clinici Scientifici Maugeri, IRCCS, Department of Biomedical Engineering of Montescano Institute, Pavia, Italy
- 4Department of Neurology, Hospital of Bolzano (SABES-ASDAA) - Teaching Hospital of the Paracelsus Medical Private University (PMU), Bolzano, Italy
- 5Brain Institute, Tel Aviv Soursky Medical Center, Tel Aviv, Israel
- 6Faculty of Medicine and Sagol School of Neurosciences, Tel Aviv University, Tel Aviv, Israel
- 7FENNSI Group, Hospital Nacional de Parapléjicos, Servicio de Salud de Castilla La Mancha, Toledo, Spain
Introduction
Parkinson's disease (PD) is a chronic and progressive neurodegenerative disorder characterized by asymmetrical limb bradykinesia, rigidity and tremor. A plethora of cognitive, emotional and vegetative non-motor symptoms are also frequent and can greatly impact on daily living (1). Among them, fatigue is estimated to occur in about 50% of patients with PD (2–4). Even more important, one-third of these subjects report this symptom as one of the most disabling in terms of quality of life (5). Friedman et al. (3) proposed to define PD fatigue as a sense of exhaustion unexplained by drug effects, other medical, or psychiatric disorders, present for a defined period, and associated with other fatigue-related symptoms, such as reduced motivation and non-restorative rest, or constraints (6). There are neither established empirical approaches to the treatment of fatigue in PD nor accepted pathophysiological mechanisms underlying this debilitating symptom (7–9). Based on several published studies (10–13), we hypothesize that fatigue in PD could be an indirect expression of attention deficit, a common cognitive disturbance in PD that can frequently be interpreted by the patient as fatigue. A recent report has linked attention deficits and fatigue in several neurological disorders, including PD (14). Studying intra-individual variability (IIV) in reaction times (RTs) and the use of transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) could have the power to shed light on the interaction between fatigue and attention. Together with a brief review of the proposed models for explaining fatigue in PD, we suggest why IIV and TMS could help in understanding this symptom and propose a framework for future studies.
Fatigue in PD, the link with attention and study modalities
Fatigue in PD is not related to decreased force-generating capacity during voluntary muscular contractions but to an increased sense of effort in both motor and cognitive tasks (15, 16). Disruption of the reciprocal loop between the striatum, frontal and limbic structures, following dopamine depletion (17, 18), was shown to be associated with fatigue in PD (19, 20). Nevertheless, the effect of dopaminergic therapies on fatigue remains unclear (21–23) and it is actually known that other non-dopaminergic networks are involved in fatigue generation (24). Coherently, it has been reported that the presence of fatigue is associated with serotoninergic denervation in the basal ganglia (BG) and limbic circuits (12). These changes could disrupt the integrity of different motor-cognitive processes (12), leading to a dissociation between motivation to act and motor execution, which could finally result in reluctance to move and feeling of fatigue (25). Because of performing movements is a decision-making process and the choice to move is taken considering the effort necessary to reach the goal (26), pathological fatigue could emerge from deficient evaluation of internal (somatic) input associated with abnormal feedback of perceived exertion (27). Accordingly, clinical and experimental evidence suggest that fatigued PD patients present in decision-making processes (28). The orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) is implicated in decision-making process, as well as in emotion regulation and reward processing. A strong contribution to the process of decision-making is provided by the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC), that is mainly involved in executive functions and is associated with attention to the selection of action (29). Despite different relations have been observed between fatigue and cognitive, motivational and emotional problems (in terms of depression, episodic anxiety, cognitive apathy, sleepiness, and subjective memory impairment) (13, 30), this symptom has been found to affect also non-depressed and non-demented PD patients to the same extent (31). In a prospective, 8-year longitudinal study of 233 PD patients (32), fatigue was found to be persistent in more than half of the patients and the authors concluded that non-motor features, such as depression and excessive daytime sleepiness, cannot explain fatigue (32). Using resting-state functional MRI in drug-naïve patients with early PD, Tessitore et al. (10) found that fatigue is associated with decreased connectivity within the supplementary motor area and increased connectivity within the prefrontal and posterior cingulate hubs of the default mode network. In line with these data, it has been found that PD patients with fatigue manifest significantly lower executive network efficiency, lower accuracy and less efficient attentional-alerting network (11). Other relevant findings support a link between PD-related fatigue and attention-demanding motor tasks (33). Martino et al. (34) designed a protocol based on sequential finger opposition movements paced to a 2 Hz metronome signal and repeated continuously for 5 min (34). This motor task requires high attentional demand and both spatial and temporal accuracy (33, 34). The authors administered this finger sequential task to PD patients with and without fatigue and found that the accuracy of fatigued PD patients deteriorated more than in non-fatigued PD patients, and that change over time correlated significantly with the burden of subjective fatigue complaints. Interestingly, subjective fatigue complaints were not associated with performance deterioration on an internally paced (un-cued) version of the same task (33).
All these observations lead toward the hypothesis that fatigued PD patients could fail in initiating and maintaining attentional tasks that require self-motivation and/or manifest an inability to inhibit/control the occurrence of excessive or distracting internal and external stimuli (10, 25, 27). Despite the plausible link between attention and fatigue in patients with PD, the lack of extensive neuropsychological evaluations, together with the absence of any neurophysiological measures, does not allow to disentangle the complex interplay between fatigue and attention in PD and the pathophysiology of this symptom remains still largely unknown.
Classically, researchers referred on reaction times (RTs) tasks for exploring the link between “central” fatigue and attention. These studies highlighted that patients with fatigue manifest defective attention (35, 36) and present increased IIV in RTs tasks exploring executive attention (37). IIV consists of within-person fluctuations in cognitive performance and their (in-)stability across time. It is believed to reflect the brain activity at different neural level (37) as provides information about attentional/executive control demand (38, 39), thus representing a useful method for understanding the neurological dysfunctions (40–42). Low IIV (i.e., high consistency across scores) is hypothesized to reflect neurological integrity, whereas high IIV (i.e., low consistency across scores) could be indicative of neurological compromise (43). IIV has been found to be greater in PD patients relative to controls, both in global cognition and in attentive functions (41, 44–46). These findings are in line with the notion that deficits in attention present early in the disease course and are among the most frequent non-motor symptoms in PD (47).
Because of the progressive degeneration of the dopaminergic transmission in PD alters the direct pathway leading to the need for a massive exploitation of executive-attentive resources to express motor behaviors (48–55), the measure of IIV and inconsistency could be extremely relevant also as a motor-cognitive marker.
By referring to IIV analysis of RTs, different studies aimed to define the attentive impairment and its relation with fatigue in other neurological conditions. A recent study (56) assessed 74 post-COVID patients complaining of high levels of fatigue with computerized Sustained Attention and Stroop tasks. For studying IIV, RTs distributions of performances in computerized tasks were fitted with ex-Gaussian distribution. In sustained attention task, mean, μ, σ and τ values were significantly higher in patients with respect to controls (56). These findings strengthen the role of these measures for detecting links between perceived fatigue and attentive deficits in neurological patients.
The pathophysiology of neural pathways involved in fatigue and attention deficits could be further explored through TMS (57–65). In a typical TMS study, the researchers first determine the threshold required to activate a muscle. The threshold is typically defined as a stimulation intensity required to evoke a Motor Evoked Potential (MEP) of > 50 micronV recorded from the target muscle in five of ten trials. In normal subjects, intermittent submaximal exercise is accompanied by increase in TMS-evoked MEP amplitude during exercise before fatigue develops, whereas, after fatigue has developed, a decrease in MEP amplitude relative to baseline can be found. Both, post-exercise facilitation and post-exercise depression are most likely mediated by cortical mechanisms (66). Lou et al. (67) found that PD patients in “off” state have more pronounced post-exercise facilitation and absent post-exercise depression compared with normal controls. A small dose of levodopa/carbidopa (100/25 mg) reduced the MEP amplitudes. Therefore, the investigators concluded that dopamine may play a role in exacerbated physical fatigability in PD because levodopa/carbidopa normalized abnormal cortico-neural excitability in these patients (65, 67). The increased MEP amplitude and more pronounced post-exercise facilitation might represent compensatory mechanisms for reduced excitatory inputs from the premotor and the supplementary motor areas in PD (65, 67). These findings related to MEP measures do not completely explain the cognitive, attention-related aspects concerning fatigue and the contribution of other non-dopaminergic pathways in fatigue generation in PD. Nevertheless, the potential relevance of TMS in addressing the pathophysiology of fatigue in PD could be clearly understood when looking at the sequence effect (SE). SE is characterized by progressive slowness in speed or a decrease in amplitude of sequential movements and it represents a main feature of bradykinesia (68). It may be associated with altered cortical excitability: as the BG are important for planning movement amplitude, the aberrant output from the BG to the motor cortex may produce this abnormality (69). Different studies have followed the hypothesis that SE observed during the execution of complex movements may be related to fatigue (70, 71). Nevertheless, it remains still unclear whether fatigue correlates with these motor-behavioral abnormalities in PD or not. In this concern, Bologna and colleagues (72) investigated whether objective measures of bradykinesia (amplitude, speed and decrement of repetitive finger tapping) have any relationship with neurophysiological measures in primary motor cortex as assessed by means of TMS measures. PD patients tapped more slowly and with smaller amplitude than normal, and displayed decrement as tapping progressed. They also had steeper input/output curves, reduced short-interval intracortical inhibition and a reduced response to the paired associative stimulation protocol. Further, bradykinesia features correlated with the slope of the input/output curve and the after-effects of the paired associative stimulation protocol (72). These results suggest that a tight relation linking neurophysiological changes in primary motor cortex and bradykinesia exist. Therefore, because of SE (as a main feature of bradykinesia) and less efficient attentional-alerting network could be both related to some extent with perceived fatiguability, we could assume that different TMS measures, given their potential to study complex neural networks, may be useful to explore deeper the pathophysiology of fatigue in PD. As a matter of fact, TMS has been adopted to unveil fatigue-related mechanisms in different neurological conditions (57–65). In patients with multiple sclerosis, the Cortical Silent Period (CSP), an intracortical, mainly GABAB-mediated inhibitory phenomenon, was found to be shorter in patients than in controls (57). As fatigue developed, CSP changes involved both the “fatigued” and the “unfatigued” muscles, suggesting a cortical spread of central fatigue mechanisms. Interestingly, chronic therapy with amantadine annulled differences in CSP duration between controls and patients, possibly through restoration of more physiological levels of intracortical inhibition in the motor cortex (57). In a recent cross-sectional observational study (61) in 59 non-depressed stroke survivors suffering from non-exercise induced fatigue (PSF), the authors examined the relationship between inter-hemispheric inhibitory balance (IIB) of homolog neural populations and subjectively reported PSF severity (measured with Fatigue Severity Scale). The authors found an association between individuals' levels of IIB in M1 and the reported levels of persistent PSF (61). Interestingly, IIB has been previously linked properly with attentional and affective disorders (61). In patients suffering from long-lasting fatigue and/or cognitive difficulties after mild SARS-CoV-2 infection longer CSP, together with impairments in long-interval intracortical inhibition and short-latency afferent inhibition, was found, thus indicating altered GABAB-ergic and cholinergic neurotransmission (64).
Study proposal and framework
We believe that literature data go through the idea that future studies putting together IIV in attentive RTs analysis and a wide TMS-based assessment could contribute to the knowledge of the intricate link between fatigue, motor behavior alterations and attention deficits in PD.
Starting from these assumptions, in the following section we will outline which clinical, IIV and TMS paradigms we intend to use in future studies for better understanding the pathophysiological mechanisms underlying fatigue in PD.
Evaluation of fatigue
Presence of fatigue can be defined based on the 16-item Parkinson Fatigue Scale (PFS-16) (73), which was developed for use in routine clinical practice and has been recommended for screening and rating the severity of fatigue in PD taking into account possible overlapping with other motor and non-motor symptoms (74). PFS-16 provides a measure of fatigue, which is independent of affective, sleep and cognitive disturbances. Brown et al. (73), using the full Likert scale, found that an average score greater than 2.9 distinguishes those who experienced fatigue from those who did not with a sensitivity of 81.0% and specificity of 85.7%. Therefore, according with this finding and previous reports (11), a PFS-16 threshold of 2.9 can be adopted to define the presence of fatigue and to differentiate “fatigued PD patients” and “non-fatigued PD patients”.
IIV in RTs
We intend to apply the study of IIV to the following computerized RTs tasks in fatigued and non-fatigued PD patients in order to unveil whether such aspects of mental-cognitive fatigue could be related to dysfunctions in attentional networks:
Sustained attention task
SAT evaluates the speed with which subjects respond to a specific environmental stimulus that are presented at randomized intervals. For example, patients have to press a response button as quickly as possible after the appearance, on the computer screen, of a target that disappear immediately after striking the response key (56, 75).
Stroop task
ST assesses to inhibit cognitive interference, which occurs when automated processing of a stimulus feature affects the simultaneous processing of another attribute of the same stimulus (76). The task can be divided into two conditions: word Color Naming (WCN), and Color Naming (CN). In this paradigm (56, 75), patients have to press corresponding keys related to differently colored circles (CN) as fast as possible. In WCN (“interference condition”) names of colors are printed in inconsistent colors, and subjects have to press a key corresponding to the color of the ink instead of the word's meaning. Thereafter, participants will have to perform a less automated task (naming ink color) while inhibiting the interference arising from a more automated task (reading the word). The difference between WCN and CN is considered an expression of the Interference component (77).
Multiple-choice task
MCT assess selective attention. The target can be a number (1, 2, 3) whose presentation is randomized. Each number can be associated to a different response button. In each trial patients have to press as quickly as possible the response button associated with the number that appeared on the screen. The accuracy of responses is evaluated by counting the errors (56, 75).
For each task, the mean value can be computed and RTs distributions can be fit with ex-Gaussian distribution using maximum likelihood estimation ad a bounded Simplex algorithm (78). From the resulting ex-Gaussian function three parameters, μ, σ, and τ can be obtained: the first two parameters (μ and σ) correspond to the mean and standard deviation of the estimated Gaussian component (sensory-motor and automatic processes), the third parameter (τ) is the mean of the estimated exponential component (central, attentive and decision-related processes of executive attention).
Application of TMS
We intend to adopt TMS for understanding motor cortex excitability and the functioning of intracortical circuits in fatigued and non-fatigued PD patients, to carry on a neurophysiological evaluation of motor fatigue and to evaluate possible relations with mental-cognitive fatigue. Therefore, the following measures will be collected:
Resting motor threshold
RMT is defined as the lowest TMS intensity (expressed in percentage of the maximum stimulator output) that evoked MEPs of at least 50 μV peak-to-peak amplitude in five of ten successive trials (79).
Cortical silent period
CSP reflects an intracortical, mainly GABAB-mediated inhibitory phenomenon, and is defined as the time elapsing from the end of the MEP until the recurrence of voluntary tonic electromyographic activity (79).
Short and long interval intracortical inhibition
SICI is thought to represent GABAA-receptor-mediated fast inhibitory post-synaptic potentials (IPSPs) in corticospinal neurons, while LICI is considered a phenomenon dependent on slow IPSPs mediated through GABAB-receptors (80).
Short-latency afferent inhibition
SAI is a marker of inhibitory sensorimotor integration that depends mainly on the excitatory effect of cholinergic thalamocortical projections onto the inhibitory GABAergic cortical network (81).
TMS evaluation of neuromuscular fatigue
Neuromuscular fatigue is typically assessed via sustained isometric maximal voluntary contraction (82). MEP amplitude and CSP duration can be evaluated to assess neuromuscular fatigue 10 min before (PRE) and 2 min after (POST) a 1-min fatiguing motor task. After a fatiguing isometric exercise, MEPs evoked in the resting target muscle are depressed for about half an hour. The CSP, on the opposite, increases after a fatiguing isometric muscle effort likely with the physiological purpose to reduce corticomotor output and prevent excessive peripheral exhaustion (83).
Conclusions
To the best of our knowledge, no studies have investigated IIV in parkinsonian subjects with respect to their level of fatigue and no data examining the relations between IIV in RTs and TMS measures actually exist. Combining these measures and correlating the results of neuropsychological investigations with the neurophysiological ones could help in the attempt to understand whether, and to what extent, alterations of the attentional system contribute to the perception of physical and mental fatigue in Parkinsonian patients. Therefore, results from further studies adopting these neuropsychological and neurophysiological measures and based on this framework could help in understanding physical and cognitive fatigability in PD.
Author contributions
PO, DF, AR, JS, and SD substantial contributions to the paper conception. PO, DF, and NG drafting the work. VV, LSa, RM, SB, AO, and LSe revising the paper critically for important intellectual content. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.
Conflict of interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Publisher's note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
References
1. Martinez-Martin P, Rodriguez-Blazquez C, Kurtis MM, Chaudhuri KR. The impact of non-motor symptoms on health-related quality of life of patients with Parkinson's disease. Mov Disord. (2011) 26:399–406. doi: 10.1002/mds.23462
2. Herlofson K, Larsen JP. Measuring fatigue in patients with Parkinson's disease - the Fatigue Severity Scale. Eur J Neurol. (2002) 9:595–600. doi: 10.1046/j.1468-1331.2002.00444.x
3. Friedman JH, Beck JC, Chou JL, Clark G, Fagundes CP, Goetz CZ. Fatigue in Parkinson's disease: report from a mutidisciplinary symposium. NPJ Parkinsons Dis. (2016) 2:15025. doi: 10.1038/npjparkd.2015.25
4. Barone P, Antonini A, Colosimo C, Marconi R, Morgante L, Avarello TP. The PRIAMO study: a multicenter assessment of nonmotor symptoms and their impact on quality of life in Parkinson's disease. Mov Disord. (2009) 24:1641–9. doi: 10.1002/mds.22643
5. Herlofson K, Larsen JP. The influence of fatigue on health-related quality of life in patients with Parkinson's disease. Acta Neurol Scand. (2003) 107:1–6. doi: 10.1034/j.1600-0404.2003.02033.x
6. Barsevick AM, Irwin MR, Hinds P, Miller A, Berger A, Jacobsen P. Recommendations for high-priority research on cancer-related fatigue in children and adults. J Natl Cancer Inst. (2013) 105:1432–40. doi: 10.1093/jnci/djt242
7. Kostić VS, Tomić A, Ječmenica-Lukić M. The pathophysiology of fatigue in parkinson's disease and its pragmatic management. Mov Disord Clin Pract. (2016) 3:323–30. doi: 10.1002/mdc3.12343
8. Elbers RG, Berendse HW, Kwakkel G. Treatment of fatigue in Parkinson disease. JAMA. (2016) 315:2340–1. doi: 10.1001/jama.2016.5260
9. Elbers RG, Verhoef J, van Wegen EE, Berendse HW, Kwakkel G. Interventions for fatigue in Parkinson's disease. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. (2015) 2015:Cd010925. doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD010925.pub2
10. Tessitore A, Giordano A, Micco RDe, Caiazzo G, Russo A, Cirillo M. Functional connectivity underpinnings of fatigue in “Drug-Naïve” patients with Parkinson's disease. Mov Disord. (2016) 31:1497–505. doi: 10.1002/mds.26650
11. Pauletti C, Mannarelli D, Locuratolo N, Pollini L, Currà A, Marinelli L. Attention in Parkinson's disease with fatigue: evidence from the attention network test. J Neural Transm. (2017) 124:335–45. doi: 10.1007/s00702-016-1637-z
12. Pavese N, Metta V, Bose SK, Chaudhuri KR, Brooks DJ. Fatigue in Parkinson's disease is linked to striatal and limbic serotonergic dysfunction. Brain. (2010) 133:3434–43. doi: 10.1093/brain/awq268
13. Siciliano M, Trojano L, Micco RDe, Mase ADe, Garramone F, Russo A, et al. behavioural, and cognitive correlates of fatigue in early, de novo Parkinson disease patients. Parkinsonism Relat Disord. (2017) 45:63–8. doi: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2017.10.004
14. Kuppuswamy H. Role of selective attention in fatigue in neurological disorders. Eur J Neurol. (2023) 2:15739. doi: 10.1111/ene.15739
15. Pageaux B, Marcora SM, Rozand V, Lepers R. Mental fatigue induced by prolonged self-regulation does not exacerbate central fatigue during subsequent whole-body endurance exercise. Front Hum Neurosci. (2015) 9:67. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00067
16. Lou JS, Kearns G, Oken B, Sexton G, Nutt J. Exacerbated physical fatigue and mental fatigue in Parkinson's disease. Mov Disord. (2001) 16:190–6. doi: 10.1002/mds.1042
17. Mimura M, Oeda R, Kawamura M. Impaired decision-making in Parkinson's disease. Parkinsonism Relat Disord. (2006) 12:169–75. doi: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2005.12.003
18. Pagonabarraga J, García-Sánchez C, Llebaria G, Pascual-Sedano B, Gironell A, Kulisevsky J. Controlled study of decision-making and cognitive impairment in Parkinson's disease. Mov Disord. (2007) 22:1430–5. doi: 10.1002/mds.21457
19. Abe K, Takanashi M, Yanagihara T. Fatigue in patients with Parkinson's disease. Behav Neurol. (2000) 12:103–6. doi: 10.1155/2000/580683
20. Abe K, Takanashi M, Yanagihara T, Sakoda S. Pergolide mesilate may improve fatigue in patients with Parkinson's disease. Behav Neurol. (2001) 13:117–21. doi: 10.1155/2002/473140
21. Franssen M, Winward C, Collett J, Wade D, Dawes H. Interventions for fatigue in Parkinson's disease: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Mov Disord. (2014) 29:1675–8. doi: 10.1002/mds.26030
22. Herlofson K, Kluger BM. Fatigue in Parkinson's disease. J Neurol Sci. (2017) 374:38–41. doi: 10.1016/j.jns.2016.12.061
23. Siciliano M, Trojano L, Santangelo G, Micco RDe, Tedeschi G, Tessitore A. Fatigue in Parkinson's disease: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Mov Disord. (2018) 33:1712–23. doi: 10.1002/mds.27461
24. Schifitto G, Friedman JH, Oakes D, Shulman L, Comella CL, Marek K. Fatigue in levodopa-naive subjects with Parkinson disease. Neurology. (2008) 71:481–5. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000324862.29733.69
25. Chaudhuri A, Behan PO. Fatigue and basal ganglia. J Neurol Sci. (2000) 179:34–42. doi: 10.1016/S0022-510X(00)00411-1
26. Walton ME, Kennerley SW, Bannerman DM, Phillips PE, Rushworth MF. Weighing up the benefits of work: behavioral and neural analyses of effort-related decision making. Neural Netw. (2006) 19:1302–14. doi: 10.1016/j.neunet.2006.03.005
27. Chaudhuri A, Behan PO. Fatigue in neurological disorders. Lancet. (2004) 363:978–88. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(04)15794-2
28. Sáez-Francàs N, Hernández-Vara J, Corominas-Roso M, Alegre J, Jacas C, Casas M. Relationship between poor decision-making process and fatigue perception in Parkinson's disease patients. J Neurol Sci. (2014) 337:167–72. doi: 10.1016/j.jns.2013.12.003
29. Lau HC, Rogers RD, Ramnani N, Passingham RE. Willed action and attention to the selection of action. Neuroimage. (2004) 21:1407–15. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.10.034
30. Siciliano M, Trojano L, Micco RDe, Russo A, Tedeschi G, Tessitore A. Subjective memory decline in Parkinson's disease patients with and without fatigue. Parkinsonism Relat Disord. (2020) 70:15–9. doi: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2019.11.017
31. Friedman JH, Brown RG, Comella C, Garber CE, Krupp LB, Lou JS. Fatigue in Parkinson's disease: a review. Mov Disord. (2007) 22:297–308. doi: 10.1002/mds.21240
32. Alves G, Wentzel-Larsen T, Larsen JP. Is fatigue an independent and persistent symptom in patients with Parkinson disease? Neurology. (2004) 63:1908–11. doi: 10.1212/01.WNL.0000144277.06917.CC
33. Martino D, Tamburini T, Zis P, Rosoklija G, Abbruzzese G, Ray-Chaudhuri K. An objective measure combining physical and cognitive fatigability: correlation with subjective fatigue in Parkinson's disease. Parkinsonism Relat Disord. (2016) 32:80–6. doi: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2016.08.021
34. Avanzino L, Tacchino A, Abbruzzese G, Quartarone A, Ghilardi MF, Bonzano L. Recovery of motor performance deterioration induced by a demanding finger motor task does not follow cortical excitability dynamics. Neuroscience. (2011) 174:84–90. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2010.11.008
35. Scheffers MK, Johnson R, Grafman J, Dale JK, Straus SE. Attention and short-term memory in chronic fatigue syndrome patients: an event-related potential analysis. Neurology. (1992) 42:1667–75. doi: 10.1212/WNL.42.9.1667
36. Boksem MA, Meijman TF, Lorist MM. Effects of mental fatigue on attention: an ERP study. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res. (2005) 25:107–16. doi: 10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2005.04.011
37. MacDonald SW, Nyberg L, Bäckman L. Intra-individual variability in behavior: links to brain structure, neurotransmission and neuronal activity. Trends Neurosci. (2006) 29:474–80. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2006.06.011
38. Bellgrove MA, Hester R, Garavan H. The functional neuroanatomical correlates of response variability: evidence from a response inhibition task. Neuropsychologia. (2004) 42:1910–6. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.05.007
39. Stuss DT, Murphy KJ, Binns MA, Alexander MP. Staying on the job: the frontal lobes control individual performance variability. Brain. (2003) 126:2363–80. doi: 10.1093/brain/awg237
40. Kälin AM, Pflüger M, Gietl AF, Riese F, Jäncke L, Nitsch RM. Intraindividual variability across cognitive tasks as a potential marker for prodromal Alzheimer's disease. Front Aging Neurosci. (2014) 6:147. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2014.00147
41. Burton CL, Strauss E, Hultsch DF, Moll A, Hunter MA. Intraindividual variability as a marker of neurological dysfunction: a comparison of Alzheimer's disease and Parkinson's disease. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. (2006) 28:67–83. doi: 10.1080/13803390490918318
42. Gorus E, Raedt RDe, Lambert M, Lemper JC, Mets T. Reaction times and performance variability in normal aging, mild cognitive impairment, and Alzheimer's disease. J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol. (2008) 21:204–18. doi: 10.1177/0891988708320973
43. Slifkin AB, Newell KM. Is variability in human performance a reflection of system noise? Curr Dir Psychol Sci. (1998) 7:170–7. doi: 10.1111/1467-8721.ep10836906
44. Frias CMde, Dixon RA, Fisher N, Camicioli R. Intraindividual variability in neurocognitive speed: a comparison of Parkinson's disease and normal older adults. Neuropsychologia. (2007) 45:2499–507. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.03.022
45. Camicioli RM, Wieler M, Frias CMde, Martin WR. Early, untreated Parkinson's disease patients show reaction time variability. Neurosci Lett. (2008) 441:77–80. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2008.06.004
46. Jones JD, Burroughs M, Apodaca M, Bunch J. Greater intraindividual variability in neuropsychological performance predicts cognitive impairment in de novo Parkinson's disease. Neuropsychology. (2020) 34:24–30. doi: 10.1037/neu0000577
47. Dujardin K, Tard C, Duhamel A, Delval A, Moreau C, Devos D. The pattern of attentional deficits in Parkinson's disease. Parkinsonism Relat Disord. (2013) 19:300–5. doi: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2012.11.001
48. Morris ME, Iansek R, Galna B. Gait festination and freezing in Parkinson's disease: pathogenesis and rehabilitation. Mov Disord. (2008) 23 Suppl 2:S451–60. doi: 10.1002/mds.21974
49. Morris ME, Martin CL, Schenkman ML. Striding out with Parkinson disease: evidence-based physical therapy for gait disorders. Phys Ther. (2010) 90:280–8. doi: 10.2522/ptj.20090091
50. Morris M, Iansek R, McGinley J, Matyas T, Huxham F. Three-dimensional gait biomechanics in Parkinson's disease: evidence for a centrally mediated amplitude regulation disorder. Mov Disord. (2005) 20:40–50. doi: 10.1002/mds.20278
51. Ferrazzoli D, Ortelli P, Iansek R, Volpe D. Rehabilitation in movement disorders: From basic mechanisms to clinical strategies. Handb Clin Neurol. (2022) 184:341–55. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-12-819410-2.00019-9
52. Ferrazzoli D, Ortelli P, Madeo G, Giladi N, Petzinger GM, Frazzitta G. Basal ganglia and beyond: The interplay between motor and cognitive aspects in Parkinson's disease rehabilitation. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2018) 90:294–308. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.05.007
53. Yogev G, Giladi N, Peretz C, Springer S, Simon ES, Hausdorff JM. Dual tasking, gait rhythmicity, and Parkinson's disease: which aspects of gait are attention demanding? Eur J Neurosci. (2005) 22:1248–56. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2005.04298.x
54. Yogev-Seligmann G, Hausdorff JM, Giladi N. The role of executive function and attention in gait. Mov Disord. (2008) 23:329–42. doi: 10.1002/mds.21720
55. Redgrave P, Rodriguez M, Smith Y, Rodriguez-Oroz MC, Lehericy S, Bergman H. Goal-directed and habitual control in the basal ganglia: implications for Parkinson's disease. Nat Rev Neurosci. (2010) 11:760–72. doi: 10.1038/nrn2915
56. Ortelli P, Benso F, Ferrazzoli D, Scarano I, Saltuari L, Sebastianelli L. Global slowness and increased intra-individual variability are key features of attentional deficits and cognitive fluctuations in post COVID-19 patients. Sci Rep. (2022) 12:13123. doi: 10.1038/s41598-022-17463-x
57. Santarnecchi E, Rossi S, Bartalini S, Cincotta M, Giovannelli F, Tatti E. Neurophysiological correlates of central fatigue in healthy subjects and multiple sclerosis patients before and after treatment with amantadine. Neural Plast. (2015) 2015:616242. doi: 10.1155/2015/616242
58. Versace V, Sebastianelli L, Ferrazzoli D, Romanello R, Ortelli P, Saltuari L. Intracortical GABAergic dysfunction in patients with fatigue and dysexecutive syndrome after COVID-19. Clin Neurophysiol. (2021) 132:1138–43. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2021.03.001
59. Ortelli P, Ferrazzoli D, Sebastianelli L, Engl M, Romanello R, Nardone R. Neuropsychological and neurophysiological correlates of fatigue in post-acute patients with neurological manifestations of COVID-19: Insights into a challenging symptom. J Neurol Sci. (2021) 420:117271. doi: 10.1016/j.jns.2020.117271
60. Liepert J, Mingers D, Heesen C, Bäumer T, Weiller C. Motor cortex excitability and fatigue in multiple sclerosis: a transcranial magnetic stimulation study. Mult Scler. (2005) 11:316–21. doi: 10.1191/1352458505ms1163oa
61. Ondobaka S, Doncker WDe, Ward N, Kuppuswamy A. Neural effective connectivity explains subjective fatigue in stroke. Brain. (2022) 145:285–94. doi: 10.1093/brain/awab287
62. Maeda F, Pascual-Leone A. Transcranial magnetic stimulation: studying motor neurophysiology of psychiatric disorders. Psychopharmacology. (2003) 168:359–76. doi: 10.1007/s00213-002-1216-x
63. Samii A, Wassermann EM, Ikoma K, Mercuri B, George MS, O'Fallon A. Decreased postexercise facilitation of motor evoked potentials in patients with chronic fatigue syndrome or depression. Neurology. (1996) 47:1410–4. doi: 10.1212/WNL.47.6.1410
64. Ortelli P, Ferrazzoli D, Sebastianelli L, Maestri R, Dezi S, Spampinato D. Altered motor cortex physiology and dysexecutive syndrome in patients with fatigue and cognitive difficulties after mild COVID-19. Eur J Neurol. (2022) 29:1652–62. doi: 10.1111/ene.15278
65. J.S. Lou. Physical and mental fatigue in Parkinson's disease: epidemiology, pathophysiology and treatment. Drugs Aging. (2009) 26:195–208. doi: 10.2165/00002512-200926030-00002
66. Brasil-Neto JP, Cohen LG, Hallett M. Central fatigue as revealed by postexercise decrement of motor evoked potentials. Muscle Nerve. (1994) 17:713–9. doi: 10.1002/mus.880170702
67. Lou JS, Benice T, Kearns G, Sexton G, Nutt J. Levodopa normalizes exercise related cortico-motoneuron excitability abnormalities in Parkinson's disease. Clin Neurophysiol. (2003) 114:930–7. doi: 10.1016/S1388-2457(03)00040-3
68. Benecke R, Rothwell JC, Dick JP, Day BL, Marsden CD. Disturbance of sequential movements in patients with Parkinson's disease. Brain. (1987) 110:361–79. doi: 10.1093/brain/110.2.361
69. Berardelli S, Rona M, Inghilleri H, Manfredi M. Cortical inhibition in Parkinson's disease. A study with paired magnetic stimulation. Brain. (1996) 119:71–7. doi: 10.1093/brain/119.1.71
70. Kang SY, Wasaka T, Shamim EA, Auh S, Ueki Y, Lopez GJ. Characteristics of the sequence effect in Parkinson's disease. Mov Disord. (2010) 25:2148–55. doi: 10.1002/mds.23251
71. Berardelli A, Rothwell JC, Thompson PD, Hallett M. Pathophysiology of bradykinesia in Parkinson's disease. Brain. (2001) 124:2131–46. doi: 10.1093/brain/124.11.2131
72. Bologna M, Guerra A, Paparella G, Giordo L, Alunni Fegatelli D, Vestri AR. Neurophysiological correlates of bradykinesia in Parkinson's disease. Brain. (2018) 141:2432–44. doi: 10.1093/brain/awy155
73. Brown RG, Dittner A, Findley L, Wessely SC. The Parkinson fatigue scale. Parkinsonism Relat Disord. (2005) 11:49–55. doi: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2004.07.007
74. Friedman JH, Alves G, Hagell P, Marinus J, Marsh L, Martinez-Martin P. Fatigue rating scales critique and recommendations by the Movement Disorders Society task force on rating scales for Parkinson's disease. Mov Disord. (2010) 25:805–22. doi: 10.1002/mds.22989
75. Ferrazzoli D, Ortelli P, Maestri R, Bera R, Gargantini R, Palamara G. Focused and sustained attention is modified by a goal-based rehabilitation in parkinsonian patients. Front Behav Neurosci. (2017) 11:56. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2017.00056
76. J.R. Stroop. Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. J Exp Psychol. (1935) 18:643–62. doi: 10.1037/h0054651
77. Strauss ESM. A Compendium of Neuropsychological Tests : Administration, Norms, and Commentary. Oxford: Oxford University Press (2006).
78. Farrell S, Lewandowsky S. An Introduction to Cognitive Modeling, An Introduction to Model-Based Cognitive Neuroscience. New York, NY: Springer Science + Business Media (2015), 3–24.
79. Rossini PM, Burke D, Chen R, Cohen LG, Daskalakis Z, Iorio RDi. Non-invasive electrical and magnetic stimulation of the brain, spinal cord, roots and peripheral nerves: Basic principles and procedures for routine clinical and research application. An updated report from an IFCN Committee. Clin Neurophysiol. (2015) 126:1071–107. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2015.02.001
80. Ziemann U, Reis J, Schwenkreis P, Rosanova M, Strafella A, Badawy R. TMS and drugs revisited 2014. Clin Neurophysiol. (2015) 126:1847–68. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2014.08.028
81. Alle H, Heidegger T, Kriváneková L, Ziemann U. Interactions between short-interval intracortical inhibition and short-latency afferent inhibition in human motor cortex. J Physiol. (2009) 587:5163–76. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2009.179820
82. Ljubisavljević M, Milanović S, Radovanović S, Vukcević I, Kostić V, Anastasijević R. Central changes in muscle fatigue during sustained submaximal isometric voluntary contraction as revealed by transcranial magnetic stimulation. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. (1996) 101:281–8. doi: 10.1016/0924-980X(96)95627-1
Keywords: fatigue, attention, Parkinson's disease, intra-individual variability, TMS, frontal lobe
Citation: Ortelli P, Versace V, Saltuari L, Randi A, Stolz J, Dezi S, Maestri R, Buechner S, Giladi N, Oliviero A, Sebastianelli L and Ferrazzoli D (2023) Looking deeper: does a connection exist between fatigue and attentional deficits in Parkinson's disease? A conceptual framework. Front. Neurol. 14:1212876. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2023.1212876
Received: 28 April 2023; Accepted: 13 July 2023;
Published: 11 August 2023.
Edited by:
Giovanni Abbruzzese, University of Genoa, ItalyReviewed by:
Laura Avanzino, University of Genoa, ItalyCopyright © 2023 Ortelli, Versace, Saltuari, Randi, Stolz, Dezi, Maestri, Buechner, Giladi, Oliviero, Sebastianelli and Ferrazzoli. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Paola Ortelli, cGFvbGEub3J0ZWxsaSYjeDAwMDQwO3NhYmVzLml0
†These authors share first authorship
‡These authors share last authorship