- 1Latin American Institute for Brain Health (BrainLat), Universidad Adolfo Ibanez, Adolfo Ibanez University, Santiago, Chile
- 2Cognitive Neuroscience Center (CNC), Universidad de San Andrés, and National Scientific and Technical Research Council (CONICET), Buenos Aires, Argentina
- 3Global Brain Health Institute, University of California, San Francisco, San Francisco, CA, United States
- 4Trinity College Dublin, Dublin, Ireland
- 5Pontificia Universidad Javeriana, Instituto de Envejecimiento, Bogotá, Colombia
- 6Memory and Cognition Center, Intellectus, Hospital Universitario San Ignacio, Bogotá, Colombia
- 7Memory and Aging Center, University of California, San Francisco, San Francisco, CA, United States
Prosocial values play a critical role in promoting care and concern for the well-being of others and prioritizing the common good of society. Evidence from population-based reports, cognitive neuroscience, and clinical studies suggests that these values depend on social cognition processes, such as empathy, deontological moral cognition, moral emotions, and social cooperation. Additionally, indirect evidence suggests that various forms of prosocial behaviors are associated with positive health outcomes at the behavioral, cardiovascular, immune, stress-related, and inflammatory pathways. However, it is unclear whether prosociality can positively influence brain health outcomes. In this perspective, we propose that prosocial values are not only influenced by brain conditions but could also potentially play a role in protecting brain health. We review studies from various fields that support this claim, including recent reports of prosociality-based interventions impacting brain health. We then explore potential multilevel mechanisms, based on the reduction of allostatic overload at behavioral, cardiovascular, immune, stress-related, and inflammatory levels. Finally, we propose potential prosociality-based interventions for improving brain health in at-risk populations, such as psychiatric and neurological patients, and individuals exposed to poverty or violence. Our perspective suggests that prosocial values may play a role in promoting and maintaining healthy brains.
Prosocial values and social cognition
Prosocial values are defined as the beliefs, attitudes, and behaviors that promote the well-being and welfare of others, with an emphasis on cooperation, helping, sharing, and altruism (1). Prosocial values promote care and concern for the welfare of others and are critical for prioritizing the common good of society at large. These include cognitive, moral, and socioemotional processes that prioritize the well-being of others and society over one’s own interests. These values are important for building strong and healthy communities, promoting positive social interactions, and creating a sense of social connectedness.
Studies in economics and psychology suggest that prosocial behavior improves common goods, such as increased cooperation and trust among individuals, better outcomes for collective action problems, and improved resource management. This effect has been particularly observed in challenging contexts like the pandemic (2–6). In social decision-making contexts, individuals tend to contribute more to a common pool of resources when they believe that others are also making contributions (7). Similarly, from childhood to adulthood, prosocial behavior can increase levels of cooperation and trust, leading to improved relationships and increased cooperation in both social and economic contexts (8–10). Overall, prosocial behavior can have a positive impact on common goods and improve the overall well-being of society.
Population-based reports, cognitive neuroscience, and clinical studies have linked prosocial behaviors with different social cognition processes, such as empathy, deontological moral cognition, moral emotions, and social cooperation. Empathy, or the ability to understand and share the feelings of others (11), has been closely linked to prosocial behavior (12). People who score higher on empathy measures are more likely to engage in behaviors that benefit others. Similarly, compassion involves an emotional response of understanding, caring, and alleviating the suffering of others. While empathy focuses on sharing and mirroring emotions, compassion goes further by involving active help and support. Deontological moral cognition refers to the beliefs and values supporting the groups’ benefits, even at the expense of potential negative individual consequences. It also emphasizes adherence to rules, duties, or principles, regardless of the consequences or outcomes. These moral processes have been found to play a significant role in shaping prosocial behavior (13). Moral emotions, such as guilt, shame, or counter-empathic emotions (envy, Schadenfreude), are complex affective processes linked to the ethical aspects of one’s actions or thoughts and have also been linked to prosocial behavior (14). Although typically seen as negative, counter-empathic emotions can promote prosocial behavior (15). Envy can inspire self-improvement or goal-setting, leading to positive outcomes for individuals and society. Meanwhile, Schadenfreude can foster social cohesion by highlighting unfairness or promoting group norms as a form of social regulation. Finally, social cooperation, or working together with others toward a common goal (i.e., sharing resources, coordinating efforts, establishing trust within a group) has been shown to be a key factor in prosocial behavior (16). Nevertheless, despite the evidence from real-life settings and neurocognitive correlates, a remaining question is whether prosociality can positively impact brain health outcomes, which refers to measures of cognitive, emotional, motor and neurological well-being, that can be influenced by various factors, including genetics, environment, lifestyle, or access to healthcare.
Could prosocial values play an unrecognized role in brain health?
According to the World Health Organization, brain health refers to the state of brain functioning across cognitive, sensory, social–emotional, behavioral and motor domains, allowing a person to realize their full potential over the life course, irrespective of the presence or absence of disorders (17). Various determinants affect brain development, adaptation, and response to stress and adversity, including physical and mental health, safe environments, security, lifelong learning, social connections, and access to quality services.
Multiple brain diseases (psychiatric and neurological conditions) compromise the core cognitive components of prosociality (18, 19), including empathy (11, 18, 20, 21), moral cognition (18, 22–24), moral emotions (15, 25), and social cooperation (18, 26–29). Conversely, it is not well understood if prosocial habits can induce brain changes. Can prosocial values not only be influenced by brain conditions but also play a crucial, yet overlooked, role in maintaining brain health?
Prosocial behaviors and social cognition have been associated with health in a variety of ways. Engaging in prosocial behaviors has been shown to have a positive impact on mental health (30). Prosocial activities are more likely to experience feelings of happiness, well-being, and social connectedness, which can help to reduce stress and anxiety (31). Even engaging in prosocial behaviors improve mood and reduce symptoms of depression and anxiety. For instance, prosocial behavior mitigates the adverse effects of stressors on emotional well-being, suggesting that it could be an effective stress-coping strategy (32). A statewide population-based study demonstrated that people who engage in volunteer work experience positive impacts on physical health, life satisfaction, social well-being, and reduced depression (33).
At physiological levels, prosociality has been linked with multimodal health. At cardiovascular level and physiological stress responses, prosocial behavior has been linked to improved health (34). For example, prosocial activities are associated with lower levels of inflammation (proinflammatory cytokine activity) (35), which is a risk factor for heart disease and other chronic diseases; and to lower blood pressure (36), which is another important factor for cardiovascular health. The immune system can also be negatively impacted by lack of prosocial behavior (37). Studies have found that individuals who engage in prosocial activities have stronger immune system, which can help to protect against a variety of health conditions (38). Neurohormonal circuitry in caregivers, particularly oxytocin and progesterone, may contribute to the health and longevity benefits associated with helping others due to their stress-buffering and restorative properties (38). Engaging in prosocial activities has been shown to reduce levels of cortisol (39) and inflammation (40), which are key factors in many chronic health conditions. Inflammation protects the overall health as part of the immune response to infection, injury, or harmful substances. While acute inflammation is crucial for protecting the body and maintaining health, chronic inflammation can have detrimental effects on health. Inflammation can cause damage to the body’s tissues and contribute to the development of conditions such as heart disease, arthritis, and cancer. By reducing inflammation, prosocial behaviors help to reduce the risk of multiple conditions and improve overall health.
Prosociality-based interventions are designed to enhance empathy, compassion, cooperation, and other prosocial traits that contribute to the well-being of others and foster positive social interactions. Such intervention can take various forms, such as educational programs, group activities, mindfulness practices, or cognitive-behavioral therapies. Some of these prosociality-based interventions have begun to show improvements in brain health (41–43). A longitudinal study (41) found that training in socio-affective and socio-cognitive skills resulted in specific changes in brain morphology among healthy adults, correlating with improvements in cognition and prosociality, and structural plasticity in social brain networks. Socio-affective training reduces experienced negative affect when processing images depicting human suffering and increases activation in the right supramarginal gyrus when confronted with negative stimuli (42). Another study discovered that induced prosocial skills, such as compassion for others and the ability to take another’s perspective, are associated with short-term changes in leukocyte telomere length (LTL) and accompanying changes in plasticity of social brain areas (43). Eudaimonic and hedonic lifestyles represent distinct approaches to health and well-being, with eudaimonic well-being focusing on pursuing meaning, personal growth, and self-realization, while hedonic well-being emphasizes seeking pleasure and avoiding pain. Eudaimonic well-being can improve mental health, enhance immune function, and increase longevity, while hedonic well-being can reduce stress, improve cardiovascular health, and enhance social connections (44). In brief, these studies suggest that behavioral changes related to prosociality induce short-term changes associated with improved brain health.
In summary, the available although still emerging indirect evidence suggests that prosocial behavior and social cognition can positively impact health at multiple levels, including behavioral, cardiovascular, immune, stress-related, inflammatory pathways, and brain health (Figure 1). These different effects could be understood as a mechanism for reducing the environmental demands and stress. Allostatic load refers to the cumulative wear and tear on the body’s stress systems that occurs over time as a result of chronic exposure to stress and involves a continuous process of energy balance instantiated by the brain to anticipate, regulate, and respond to environmental demands (45, 46). Allostatic overload can lead to alterations in the brain’s neural circuitry, neurotransmitter systems, and inflammatory responses, which in turn can impair cognitive functions, emotional regulation, and overall mental well-being. Prosocial behavior has been linked to reduced allostatic overload (39), which can help protect the brain’s health against stress and aging and improve overall health.
Figure 1. The relationship between proxies of prosocial values and brain health through biological pathways. This figure illustrates the proposed hypothesis that proxies of prosocial values, which include social cognitive and affective processes (empathy, deontological moral cognition, moral emotions, and social cooperation), can impact various behavioral and biological pathways. These encompass cardiometabolic, immune, stress-related, and inflammatory processes, which can subsequently enhance brain health by reducing allostatic overload. Integrating prosocial values into educational models (e.g., the A FORCE model), interventions, and public health policies may contribute to improved brain health, particularly in vulnerable populations.
Can prosociality help to improve brain health in populations at risk?
Diverse groups, including individuals affected by negative social or environmental exposures (47), as well as psychiatric (e.g., schizophrenia, anxiety, depression, PTSD) (48), and neurological conditions (e.g., Alzheimer’s disease, frontotemporal dementia) (45, 49), constitute populations at risk impacted by allostatic overload. Psychiatric and neurological patients, and individuals exposed to stressful conditions (i.e., poverty or violence) are exposed to allostatic overload (45, 46, 50–53). Prosociality has been linked to allostatic mechanisms (39, 54–56) and prosociality-based interventions have the potential to significantly improve brain health in different populations (41–43). Promoting prosocial values and behaviors can help to reduce allostatic load, improve mood and reduce symptoms of anxiety and depression, improve cardiovascular health, and strengthen the immune system (39, 46) and even directly influence brain health.
Given these potent brain health benefits, in addition to the well know benefic promoting care and concern for the well-being of others, prosociality could be promoted in science, politics, and governmental initiatives. The Global Brain Health Institute (GBHI) has developed “A FORCE” model, a prosociality value-based learning program, which is centered around the six core values of Authenticity, Fairness, Openness, Respect, Courage, and Empathy. A FORCE involves education and training on these values and how they can be applied in daily life and in the context of brain health. Through workshops, discussions, role-playing exercises, and other interactive activities the program help students to internalize these values and understand their importance in promoting brain health. This approach could be the first step for incorporate the value of prosociality into public health policies and programs. Value-based education across the lifespan could be particularly useful in children and adolescents, as well as to understanding how prosociality can improve the long-term cumulative burden across the lifespan.
Conclusion
In this perspective, we linked prosociality with a set of social cognitive processes and proposed that not only is it impacted by various disorders, but it also has the potential to play a role in promoting brain health. Emerging evidence supports this claim, in terms of reducing allostatic overload at behavioral, cardiovascular, immune, stress-related, and inflammatory levels. By incorporating prosocial values into education and public health policies, and developing targeted interventions to support prosocial behavior, we can help to improve brain health in populations that are particularly vulnerable to stress and adverse health outcomes.
Data availability statement
The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.
Author contributions
AI conceived this work and prepared the initial draft. DM and BM carefully revised the draft. All authors contributed to the contents of this article and approved the final version.
Funding
AI is partially supported by grants ANID/FONDECYT Regular (1210195, 1210176, and 1220995); ANID/FONDAP/15150012; ANID/PIA/ANILLOS ACT210096; ANID/FONDEF ID20I10152 and ID22I10029; ANID/FONDAP 15150012; Takeda CW2680521 and the MULTI-PARTNER CONSORTIUM TO EXPAND DEMENTIA RESEARCH IN LATIN AMERICA [ReDLat, supported by Fogarty International Center (FIC) and National Institutes of Health, National Institutes of Aging (R01 AG057234), Alzheimer’s Association (SG-20-725707), Rainwater Charitable foundation – Tau Consortium, and Global Brain Health Institute].
Conflict of interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Publisher’s note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
Author disclaimer
The contents of this publication are solely the responsibility of the authors and do not represent the official views of these Institutions.
References
1. Penner, LA, Dovidio, JF, Piliavin, JA, and Schroeder, DA. Prosocial behavior: multilevel perspectives. Annu Rev Psychol. (2005) 56:365–92. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.56.091103.070141
2. Van Bavel, JJ, Cichocka, A, Capraro, V, Sjåstad, H, Nezlek, JB, Pavlović, T, et al. National identity predicts public health support during a global pandemic. Nat Commun. (2022) 13:517. doi: 10.1038/s41467-021-27668-9
3. Pavlović, T, Azevedo, F, De, K, Riaño-Moreno, JC, Maglić, M, Gkinopoulos, T, et al. Predicting attitudinal and behavioral responses to COVID-19 pandemic using machine learning. PNAS Nexus. (2022) 1:pgac093. doi: 10.1093/pnasnexus/pgac093
4. Ibanez, A, Santamaria-Garcia, H, Barragan, AG, Kornhuber, A, Ton, AMM, Slachevsky, A, et al. The impact of SARS-CoV-2 in dementia across Latin America: a call for an urgent regional plan and coordinated response. Alzheimers Dement (N Y). (2020) 6:e12092. doi: 10.1002/trc2.12092
5. Santamaría-García, H, Burgaleta, M, Legaz, A, Flichtentrei, D, Córdoba-Delgado, M, Molina-Paredes, J, et al. The price of prosociality in pandemic times. Human Social Sci Commun. (2022) 9:15. doi: 10.1057/s41599-021-01022-2
6. Ibanez, A, and Kosik, KS. COVID-19 in older people with cognitive impairment in Latin America. Lancet Neurol. (2020) 19:719–21. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(20)30270-2
7. Rahal, RM, and Fiedler, S. Cognitive and affective processes of prosociality. Curr Opin Psychol. (2022) 44:309–14. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2021.10.007
8. Gonzalez-Gadea, ML, Sigman, M, Rattazzi, A, Lavin, C, Rivera-Rei, A, Marino, J, et al. Neural markers of social and monetary rewards in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and autism Spectrum disorder. Sci Rep. (2016) 6:30588. doi: 10.1038/srep30588
9. Huppert, E, Cowell, JM, Cheng, Y, Contreras-Ibáñez, C, Gomez-Sicard, N, Gonzalez-Gadea, ML, et al. The development of children's preferences for equality and equity across 13 individualistic and collectivist cultures. Dev Sci. (2019) 22:e12729. doi: 10.1111/desc.12729
10. Stallen, M, and Sanfey, AG. The cooperative brain. Neuroscientist. (2013) 19:292–303. doi: 10.1177/1073858412469728
11. Melloni, M, Lopez, V, and Ibanez, A. Empathy and contextual social cognition. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. (2014) 14:407–25. doi: 10.3758/s13415-013-0205-3
12. Wu, YE, and Hong, W. Neural basis of prosocial behavior. Trends Neurosci. (2022) 45:749–62. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2022.06.008
13. Holyoak, KJ, and Powell, D. Deontological coherence: a framework for commonsense moral reasoning. Psychol Bull. (2016) 142:1179–203. doi: 10.1037/bul0000075
14. Keltner, D, Kogan, A, Piff, PK, and Saturn, SR. The sociocultural appraisals, values, and emotions (SAVE) framework of prosociality: core processes from gene to meme. Annu Rev Psychol. (2014) 65:425–60. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-010213-115054
15. Santamaria-Garcia, H, Baez, S, Reyes, P, Santamaria-Garcia, JA, Santacruz-Escudero, JM, Matallana, D, et al. A lesion model of envy and Schadenfreude: legal, deservingness and moral dimensions as revealed by neurodegeneration. Brain J Neurol. (2017) 140:3357–77. doi: 10.1093/brain/awx269
16. Ibáñez, A, García, AM, Esteves, S, Yoris, A, Muñoz, E, Reynaldo, L, et al. Social neuroscience: undoing the schism between neurology and psychiatry. Soc Neurosci. (2018) 13:1–39. doi: 10.1080/17470919.2016.1245214
17. World Health Organization . Optimizing brain health across the life course: WHO position paper. Geneva: World Health Organization (2022).
18. Kennedy, DP, and Adolphs, R. The social brain in psychiatric and neurological disorders. Trends Cogn Sci. (2012) 16:559–72. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2012.09.006
19. Ibanez, A . The mind's golden cage and cognition in the wild. Trends Cogn Sci. (2022) 26:1031–4. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2022.07.008
20. Baez, S, Fittipaldi, S, de la Fuente, LA, Carballo, M, Ferrando, R, García-Cordero, I, et al. Empathy deficits and their behavioral, neuroanatomical, and functional connectivity correlates in smoked cocaine users. Prog Neuro Psychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. (2021) 110:110328. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2021.110328
21. Baez, S, Manes, F, Huepe, D, Torralva, T, Fiorentino, N, Richter, F, et al. Primary empathy deficits in frontotemporal dementia. Front Aging Neurosci. (2014) 6:262. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2014.00262
22. Baez, S, Couto, B, Torralva, T, Sposato, LA, Huepe, D, Montañes, P, et al. Comparing moral judgments of patients with frontotemporal dementia and frontal stroke. JAMA Neurol. (2014) 71:1172–6. doi: 10.1001/jamaneurol.2014.347
23. Escobar, MJ, Huepe, D, Decety, J, Sedeño, L, Messow, MK, Baez, S, et al. Brain signatures of moral sensitivity in adolescents with early social deprivation. Sci Rep. (2014) 4:5354. doi: 10.1038/srep05354
24. Hesse, E, Mikulan, E, Decety, J, Sigman, M, Garcia Mdel, C, Silva, W, et al. Early detection of intentional harm in the human amygdala. Brain J Neurol. (2016) 139:54–61. doi: 10.1093/brain/awv336
25. Baez, S, Trujillo-Llano, C, de Souza, LC, Lillo, P, Forno, G, Santamaría-García, H, et al. Moral emotions and their brain structural correlates across neurodegenerative disorders. J Alzheimer's Dis. (2023) 92:153–69. doi: 10.3233/jad-221131
26. Ibáñez, A, Billeke, P, de la Fuente, L, Salamone, P, García, AM, and Melloni, M. Reply: towards a neurocomputational account of social dysfunction in neurodegenerative disease. Brain J Neurol. (2017) 140:e15. doi: 10.1093/brain/aww316
27. Melloni, M, Billeke, P, Baez, S, Hesse, E, de la Fuente, L, Forno, G, et al. Your perspective and my benefit: multiple lesion models of self-other integration strategies during social bargaining. Brain J Neurol. (2016) 139:3022–40. doi: 10.1093/brain/aww231
28. Fittipaldi, S, Ibanez, A, Baez, S, Manes, F, Sedeno, L, and Garcia, AM. More than words: social cognition across variants of primary progressive aphasia. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2019) 100:263–84. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.02.020
29. Ibanez, A, and Manes, F. Contextual social cognition and the behavioral variant of frontotemporal dementia. Neurology. (2012) 78:1354–62. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3182518375
30. Miles, A, Andiappan, M, Upenieks, L, and Orfanidis, C. Using prosocial behavior to safeguard mental health and foster emotional well-being during the COVID-19 pandemic: a registered report protocol for a randomized trial. PLoS One. (2021) 16:e0245865. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0245865
31. Bavel, JJV, Baicker, K, Boggio, PS, Capraro, V, Cichocka, A, Cikara, M, et al. Using social and behavioural science to support COVID-19 pandemic response. Nature human. Behaviour. (2020) 4:460–71. doi: 10.1038/s41562-020-0884-z
32. Raposa, EB, Laws, HB, and Ansell, EB. Prosocial behavior mitigates the negative effects of stress in everyday life. Clin Psychol Sci. (2016) 4:691–8. doi: 10.1177/2167702615611073
33. Yeung, JWK, Zhang, Z, and Kim, TY. Volunteering and health benefits in general adults: cumulative effects and forms. BMC Public Health. (2017) 18:8. doi: 10.1186/s12889-017-4561-8
34. Lazar, L, and Eisenberger, NI. The benefits of giving: effects of prosocial behavior on recovery from stress. Psychophysiology. (2022) 59:e13954. doi: 10.1111/psyp.13954
35. Chiang, JJ, Eisenberger, NI, Seeman, TE, and Taylor, SE. Negative and competitive social interactions are related to heightened proinflammatory cytokine activity. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. (2012) 109:1878–82. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1120972109
36. Whillans, AV, Dunn, EW, Sandstrom, GM, Dickerson, SS, and Madden, KM. Is spending money on others good for your heart? Health Psychol. (2016) 35:574–83. doi: 10.1037/hea0000332
37. Gryksa, K, and Neumann, ID. Consequences of pandemic-associated social restrictions: role of social support and the oxytocin system. Psychoneuroendocrinology. (2022) 135:105601. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2021.105601
38. Brown, SL, and Brown, RM. Connecting prosocial behavior to improved physical health: contributions from the neurobiology of parenting. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2015) 55:1–17. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.04.004
39. Miller, JG . Physiological mechanisms of prosociality. Curr Opin Psychol. (2018) 20:50–4. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2017.08.018
40. Muscatell, KA, and Inagaki, TK. Beyond social withdrawal: new perspectives on the effects of inflammation on social behavior. Brain Behav Immun Health. (2021) 16:100302. doi: 10.1016/j.bbih.2021.100302
41. Valk, SL, Bernhardt, BC, Trautwein, FM, Böckler, A, Kanske, P, Guizard, N, et al. Structural plasticity of the social brain: differential change after socio-affective and cognitive mental training. Sci Adv. (2017) 3:e1700489. doi: 10.1126/sciadv.1700489
42. Favre, P, Kanske, P, Engen, H, and Singer, T. Decreased emotional reactivity after 3-month socio-affective but not attention- or meta-cognitive-based mental training: a randomized, controlled, longitudinal fMRI study. Neuro Image. (2021) 237:118132. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2021.118132
43. Puhlmann, LMC, Valk, SL, Engert, V, Bernhardt, BC, Lin, J, Epel, ES, et al. Association of Short-term Change in leukocyte telomere length with cortical thickness and outcomes of mental training among healthy adults: a randomized clinical trial. JAMA Netw Open. (2019) 2:e199687. doi: 10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2019.9687
44. Ryff, CD, and Singer, BH. Know thyself and become what you are: a Eudaimonic approach to psychological well-being. J Happiness Stud. (2008) 9:13–39. doi: 10.1007/s10902-006-9019-0
45. Migeot, JA, Duran-Aniotz, CA, Signorelli, CM, Piguet, O, and Ibáñez, A. A predictive coding framework of allostatic-interoceptive overload in frontotemporal dementia. Trends Neurosci. (2022) 45:838–53. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2022.08.005
46. McEwen, BS . Stress, adaptation, and disease: Allostasis and Allostatic load. Ann N Y Acad Sci. (1998) 840:33–44. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1998.tb09546.x
47. Vermeulen, R, Schymanski, EL, Barabási, AL, and Miller, GW. The exposome and health: where chemistry meets biology. Science. (2020) 367:392–6. doi: 10.1126/science.aay3164
48. Bottaccioli, AG, Bottaccioli, F, and Minelli, A. Stress and the psyche-brain-immune network in psychiatric diseases based on psychoneuroendocrine immunology: a concise review. Ann N Y Acad Sci. (2019) 1437:31–42. doi: 10.1111/nyas.13728
49. De Felice, FG, Gonçalves, RA, and Ferreira, ST. Impaired insulin signalling and allostatic load in Alzheimer disease. Nat Rev Neurosci. (2022) 23:215–30. doi: 10.1038/s41583-022-00558-9
50. McEwen, BS, Bowles, NP, Gray, JD, Hill, MN, Hunter, RG, Karatsoreos, IN, et al. Mechanisms of stress in the brain. Nat Neurosci. (2015) 18:1353–63. doi: 10.1038/nn.4086
51. McEwen, BS . Brain on stress: how the social environment gets under the skin. Proc Natl Acad Sci. (2012) 109:17180–5. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1121254109
52. Karatsoreos, IN, and McEwen, BS. Psychobiological allostasis: resistance, resilience and vulnerability. Trends Cogn Sci. (2011) 15:576–84. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.10.005
53. McEwen, BS, and Akil, H. Revisiting the stress concept: implications for affective disorders. J Neurosci Off J Soc Neurosci. (2020) 40:12–21. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0733-19.2019
54. Kim, H . Stability or plasticity? – a hierarchical Allostatic regulation model of medial prefrontal cortex function for social valuation. Front Neurosci. (2020) 14:281. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2020.00281
55. Schulkin, J . Social Allostasis: anticipatory regulation of the internal milieu. Front Evol Neurosci. (2011) 2:111. doi: 10.3389/fnevo.2010.00111
Keywords: prosocial values, prosociality, social cognition, brain health, Allostasis, empathy, cooperation, moral cognition
Citation: Ibanez A, Matallana D and Miller B (2023) Can prosocial values improve brain health? Front. Neurol. 14:1202173. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2023.1202173
Edited by:
Görsev Yener, İzmir University of Economics, TürkiyeReviewed by:
Sunhae Sul, Pusan National University, Republic of KoreaJudith Aharon Peretz, Rambam Health Care Campus, Israel
Peter S. Pressman, University of Colorado Denver, United States
Copyright © 2023 Ibanez, Matallana and Miller. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Agustin Ibanez, agustin.ibanez@gbhi.org