Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Neurol., 07 April 2022
Sec. Sleep Disorders
This article is part of the Research Topic Interaction Between Neuropsychiatry and Sleep Disorders: from Mechanism to Clinical Practice View all 19 articles

Sleep Misperception and Associated Factors in Patients With Anxiety-Related Disorders and Complaint of Insomnia: A Retrospective Study

\nYingjie Liang,&#x;Yingjie Liang1,2Xu Zhao,&#x;Xu Zhao1,2Changyong Zhang
Changyong Zhang3*Guangya Liu
Guangya Liu4*Baili LuBaili Lu1Li HanLi Han1Fang Tong,&#x;Fang Tong1,2Xinyu Luo,&#x;Xinyu Luo1,2Chuang Hu,&#x;Chuang Hu1,2Hui Liu,&#x;Hui Liu1,2
  • 1Department of Psychiatry, Wuhan Mental Health Center, Wuhan, China
  • 2Department of Sleep Disorder, Wuhan Hospital for Psychotherapy, Wuhan, China
  • 3Department of Psychiatry, Wuhan Wudong Hospital (Wuhan Second Mental Hospital), Wuhan, China
  • 4Outpatient Office, Wuhan Jinyintan Hospital, Wuhan, China

Purpose: Data on sleep parameters by polysomnography (PSG) in patients with anxiety-related disorders are limited. Although the disturbance and risk factors of sleep misperception have been implicated in psychopathology, its role in anxiety-related disorders remains unclear. This retrospective study aimed to explore the characteristics and sleep parameters in patients with anxiety-related disorders and different sleep perception types, and to explore the associated factors for sleep misperception.

Methods: Patients with anxiety-related disorders who had complaint of insomnia for more than 3 months were collected at Wuhan Mental Health Center between December 2019 and July 2021. Patients underwent a two-night PSG monitoring and completed a self-reported sleep questionnaire. Behaviors were assessed using 30-item Nurses' Observation Scale for Inpatient Evaluation (NOSIE-30). Patients were divided into normal sleep perception (NSP), positive sleep perception abnormality [PSPA; overestimation of total sleep time (TST) >60 min], and negative sleep perception abnormality (NSPA; underestimation of TST >60 min) groups. PSG indicators and NOSIE-30 scores were compared among groups using the one-way analysis of variance and the Kruskal-Wallis test. Multiple linear regression analysis was performed to determine the associated factors for misperception index.

Results: The subjective and objective TST were 5.5 ± 1.9 h and 6.4 ± 1.7 h in 305 patients, respectively. Sixty-nine (22.6%) had PSPA, 80 (26.2%) had NSP, and 156 (51.1%) had NSPA. Subjective TST and objective sleep parameters were significantly different among groups. No statistical differences in NOSIE-30 subscale and total scores were observed among groups. Sex, rapid eye movement (REM)/TST (%), sleep efficiency, number of awakenings, Non-rapid eye movement of stage 2 sleep (NREM)/TST (%), REM spontaneous arousal times, sleep latency, diagnosis, social competence, and manifest psychosis were associated with sleep misperception.

Conclusion: Sleep misperception is common in patients with anxiety-related disorders. Various sleep perception types have different PSG profiles, but similar personal and social behaviors. These data may be helpful to conduct personalized treatment.

Introduction

Sleep is an important psychophysiological process for mental health (1). Sleep disturbance in mental disorders can negatively affect cognitive, emotional, and social functions (2). Anxiety-related disorders, as a branch of mental disorders, are characterized by abnormal maladaptive forms of emotional response to potential threat. The common types of anxiety-related disorders according to the fifth edition of Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-V) include generalized anxiety disorder, obsessive-compulsive disorder, social anxiety disorder, separation anxiety disorder, panic disorder, agoraphobia, and specific phobia (3). Increasing evidence demonstrates sleep disturbance in patients with anxiety-related disorders (4, 5), with a potentially bidirectional relationship. However, current findings in sleep profiles are mostly focused on individual anxiety-related disorders, and sleep assessment modality was inconsistent among studies (5). A comprehensive evaluation of sleep disturbance in this whole branch of mental disorders is needed.

Objective measures have been increasingly used for the analysis of sleep problems. However, an interesting phenomenon, discrepancy between subjective and objective sleep parameters, often appears. This sleep misperception is directly reflected in the divergence of subjective and objective total sleep time (TST), including underestimation and overestimation of TST. Patients will frequently go to the hospital due to dissatisfaction with sleep quality, seeking for solution. Sleep misperception has been implicated in many kinds of insomnia. The prevalence of sleep misperception in the sleep disorder population varied widely, ranging from 9.2 to 55.5% (6 7 9), due to different definitions of sleep misperception used. Polysomnography (PSG) is arguably the objective standard to assess sleep parameters. Although the diagnosis of insomnia is usually not based on objective parameters, it is necessary to use both objective and subjective methods for capturing the sleep experience and its characteristics. However, most of the current studies on sleep misperception excluded patients with mental disorders. Currently, only a small number of studies analyzed sleep misperception, with scarce data on subjective and objective sleep parameters in psychiatric patients. A study by Wenigmann et al. assessed psychiatric patients with sleep disorders, and sleep misperception was observed in 52% of the 249 examined patients (6). Bian et al. reported a sleep misperception rate of 43.2% in 148 patients with schizophrenia (7). Interestingly, 38.5% of patients with schizophrenia showed overestimation of their TST (7). However, sleep misperception with quantitative analysis of sleep parameters is limited in patients with anxiety-related disorders. Although a previous study preliminarily provided some evidence on sleep disturbance in anxiety-related disorders by collectively using meta-analysis and combining subjective and objective measures together, differences between subjective and objective data among patients with different sleep perception were not characterized. In addition, further studies are needed to identify the associated factors for sleep misperception.

This study aimed to explore the characteristics and sleep parameters in patients with anxiety-related disorders and different sleep perception types, and to explore the associated factors for sleep misperception. We hope to provide some reliable evidence for sleep assessment, clinical classification, and appropriate treatment of patients with anxiety-related disorders.

Methods

Patients

This was a retrospective study conducted in the Wuhan Mental Health Center between December 2019 and July 2021. The inclusion criteria were as follows: (1) age ≥18 years; (2) diagnosis of anxiety-related disorders according to DSM-V criteria (3); (3) stable disorder; (4) complaint of insomnia for more than 3 months; and (5) ability to understand the study objective and willingness to complete the assessments. The exclusion criteria were as follows: (1) severe anxiety, as indicated by Self-rating Anxiety Scale score ≥70 (8); (2) other DSM-V axis I disorders; (3) prior electroconvulsive therapy within 1 month; (4) cognitive impairment, as indicated by Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) score ≤27 (9); (5) abuse of drugs or other substances; (6) other significant sleep disorders according to the third edition of International Classification of Sleep Disorder (ICSD-3) (10), including obstructive sleep apnea hypopnea syndrome (OSAHS), rapid eye movement (REM) or non-REM (NREM) parasomnia, circadian rhythm sleep-wake disorders, and movement disorders (such as restless leg syndrome and periodic leg movement disorder); (7) have used any medicine or health care products to regulate sleep in the past 5 days; and/or (8) incomplete data. The study was approved by the Ethics Committee of Wuhan Mental Health Center (No.KY201908.28). Written informed consent was obtained from each patient before examinations.

Data Collection

Demographics, diagnosis of anxiety-related disorders, and medication history (in the last 6 months) were collected during the interview with a psychiatrist, two board-certified neurologists, and an expert in sleep medicine. After interview, patients underwent PSG monitoring and completed a self-reported sleep questionnaire. Blood pressure was measured before and after PSG monitoring.

Polysomnography Monitoring

Patients received two-night standard PSG monitoring using a Compumedics-Greal PSG monitor (Australia Compumedics Limited). To exclude other sleep disorders and first night effect, data during the second night were collected for analysis. All patients entered the monitoring room at 21:30 and were asked to schedule their sleep and wake-up time according to daily routine. The monitoring room was quiet and comfortable, with proper room temperature, humidity, and light. Psychoactive substances, such as alcohol, tea, and coffee, were not allowed. The monitoring started from 22:00 to 7:00 the next morning. Complete records of electroencephalogram, electrooculogram, electromyogram, electrocardiogram, thermal airflow sensor, nasal pressure transducer, chest and abdomen respiratory movement, limb movement, body position, blood oxygen saturation, and snore sensor indicators for 6 h or longer were deemed as a successful monitoring. Patients with unsuccessful monitoring needed to be monitored again at next night. Items were recorded and analyzed according to American Academy of Sleep Medicine (AASM) criteria, version 2.6 (11), including TST, sleep latency, sleep efficiency (TST/time in bed×100%), the percentage of NREM sleep (N1, N2, and N3 period) and REM sleep to TST, wake after sleep onset (WASO), number of awakenings, arousal index, and spontaneous arousal times. Arousal from sleep was deemed present when there were limb movements or respiratory events with oxygen desaturation for ≥10 s. Spontaneous arousal was defined as that which was not caused by respiratory events, oxygen desaturation, or limb movement. All Participants were asked to stop taking any sleep-affecting medication at least 5 days prior to testing as judged by our sleep specialist.

Self-Reported Sleep Questionnaire

After successful PSG monitoring, patients were instructed to fill a questionnaire with four questions: (1) What time did you go to bed last night? (2) How long did it take you to fall asleep last night? (3) What time did you wake up this morning? (4) How long did you sleep last night?

Sleep Perception

There are no recognized diagnostic criteria for sleep perception. According to previous studies (1214), sleep perception abnormality is defined as a more than 60 min difference between self-reported subjective TST and PSG-measured objective TST. Positive sleep perception abnormality (PSPA) is defined as subjective overestimation of TST (subjective TST—objective TST >60 min). Negative sleep perception abnormality (NSPA) is defined as subjective underestimation of TST (objective TST—subjective TST >60 min). Normal sleep perception (NSP) is defined as ≤60 min discrepancy between subjective TST and objective TST. Sleep perception is calculated as subjective TST/objective TST (15). Lastly, sleep perception is calculated as (objective TST—subjective TST) (17).

Nurses' Observation Scale for Inpatient Evaluation

The 30-item Nurses' Observation Scale for Inpatient Evaluation (NOSIE-30) is a highly sensitive scale to assess the behavioral changes in patients with psychiatric disorders (1619). A Chinese version of NOSIE-30 was used in this study (20), which comprises 7 subscales: social competence, social interest, personal neatness, irritability, manifest psychosis, retardation, and depression. The first three subscales indicate positive behaviors, while the last four reflect negative behaviors. Each item is scored using a 5-Likert scale ranging from 0 (never) to 4 (always). Higher score indicates a higher frequency of each particular behavior.

Statistical Analysis

Statistical analysis was performed using SPSS 22.0 (IBM Corp., Armonk, NY, USA). Continuous variables were tested for normal distribution using Shapiro-Wilk test. Continuous variables, in accordance with normal distribution, were expressed as mean ± standard deviation and were compared among groups using one-way analysis of variance with post-hoc Bonferroni correction. Continuous variables with skewed distribution were expressed as median [interquartile range IQR)], and were compared among groups using the Kruskal-Wallis test with post-hoc Bonferroni correction. Categorical variables were expressed as frequency (percentage) and were compared among groups using chi-square test or Fisher exact test, where appropriate. Multiple linear regression analysis was performed to determine the associated factors for misperception index, and stepwise method was used for the selection of independent variables. p < 0.05 was considered statistically significant.

Results

General Information and Sleep Parameters in the Total Patients With Anxiety-Related Disorders

Between December 2019 and July 2021, a total of 305 patients who met the eligibility criteria with complete data were included in the analysis (Figure 1). The mean age was 44.0 ± 16.6 years, and 182 (59.7%) were females. Of 305 patients, 150 (49.2%) had generalized anxiety disorder, 81 (26.6%) had obsessive-compulsive disorder, 20 (6.6%) had mixed anxiety disorder, 15 (4.9%) had somatic symptom-related anxiety disorder, 14 (4.6%) had social anxiety disorder, 12 (3.9%) had separation anxiety disorder, five (1.6%) had hypochondriac anxiety disorder, four (1.3%) had panic disorder, and four (1.3%) had specific phobia. Sixty-nine (22.6%) had PSPA, 80 (26.2%) had NSP, and 156 (51.1%) had NSPA (Table 1). Regarding the sleep parameters in all the 305 patients, the subjective and objective TST were 5.5 ± 1.9 h and 6.4 ± 1.7 h, respectively. The sleep latency was 44.4 ± 38.5 min, and the sleep efficiency was 73.2 ± 18.6%. The misperception index was 0.1 ± 0.5 (Table 2).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Study flowchart.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Patient characteristics.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Polysomnography indicators, sleep perception, and misperception index.

Comparisons of General Information, Sleep Parameters, and NOSIE-30 Scores Among Patients With Anxiety-Related Disorders and Different Sleep Perception Types

More females presented with PSPA or NSPA (p < 0.01) (Table 1). The lowest subjective TST and the highest objective TST were observed in the NSPA group [4 h (IQR, 4–6) and 8 h (IQR, 7–8)], followed by the NSP group [6 h (IQR, 5–6.3) and 6 h (IQR, 5–6)] and PSPA group [8 h (IQR, 7–8) and 4 h (IQR, 4–6)], with significant differences between each two groups after post-hoc Bonferroni correction (p < 0.01). Number of awakenings, total spontaneous arousal times, NREM spontaneous arousal times, sleep latency, sleep perception, and misperception index were also significantly different between each two groups (p < 0.01). Arousal index showed no statistical difference between the NSPA and NSP groups, but both were significantly higher than the PSPA group (p < 0.01). REM spontaneous arousal times and the percentage of REM sleep to TST showed no statistical differences between the NSPA and NSP groups, but all were significantly lower than the PSPA group (p < 0.01). The percentage of N1 period and N2 period to TST only showed significant differences between the NSP and PSPA groups (p < 0.01). The percentage of N3 period to TST only showed significant difference between the NSPA and PSPA groups (p < 0.01). Sleep efficiency and WASO showed no statistical differences between the NSP and PSPA groups, but all showed significant difference when compared with the NSPA group (p < 0.01) (Table 2). As shown in Table 3, there were no significant differences in subscale scores or total scores of NOSIE-30 among the three groups (p > 0.05).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Results of the nurses' observation scale for in-patient evaluation (NOSIE-30).

Multiple Linear Regression Analysis of Associated Factors for Sleep Perception

The explanatory variable was defined as the “sleep perception = objective TST–subjective TST.” Before performing multiple regressions, the first task was to conduct a variable screening. Using a forward stepwise regression method to select variables (age, sex, BMI, all variables shown in Tables 2, 3), the selection criteria was that significance-level p-value was <0.05. If the p-value is >0.05, the variable will be eliminated. The final retained variables were sex, REM/TST, sleep efficiency, number of awakenings, Non-rapid eye movement of stage 2 sleep (NREM)/TST (%), REM spontaneous arousal times, sleep latency, diagnosis, social competence, and manifest psychosis. If multiple collinearity tests (VIF, Variance Inflating Factor) are >5, it indicates a collinearity problem among variables. Otherwise, there is no collinearity problem among variables. Table 4 shows the regression results. Moreover, from the analysis of Table 4, we found that the VIF statistic of each explanatory variable was <5, indicating that there was no multiple collinearity problem among the variables, which was in line with the assumption of multiple linear regression. Then, Durbin-Watson (DW)test statistics was used to test whether the regression residue is self-phase when the DW statistic approached 2, indicating that the residue is not autocorrelated. The regression results showed that the DW was 1.920, close to 2, indicating that the residue has no autocorrelation.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Multiple linear regression analysis of associated factors for sleep perception.

The adjusted goodness-of-fit R2 was 0.663, indicating that each explanatory variable can explain 66.3% of the information volume of the explained variable, and more than half of the information can be explained, indicating a good fitting effect. The model gameplay test F statistic was 60.855, and the significance-level p-value was <0.05, suggesting that the established multiple linear regression was valid.

Finally, multiple linear regression analysis showed that the sleep perception was associated with sex, REM/TST, sleep efficiency, number of awakenings, N2/TST, REM spontaneous arousal times, sleep latency, diagnosis, social competence, and manifest psychosis (Table 4).

Discussion

This study compared clinical characteristics, sleep parameters, misperception index, and behaviors among patients with different sleep perception types (PSPA, NSP, and NSPA). Subjective TST and all the objective sleep parameters were significantly different among the three groups. Interestingly, although there were differences in the diagnoses of anxiety-related disorders among groups, similar personal and social behaviors were observed by NOSIE-30. Regarding results of the linear regression, younger age, more REM spontaneous arousal times, higher sleep latency, lower percentage of N3 period to TST, higher sleep efficiency, and higher WASO were associated with the increase in misperception index.

In our study, all the 305 patients were diagnosed with anxiety-related disorders, with the overall subjective TST of 5.5 ± 1.9 h and objective TST of 6.4 ± 1.7 h. This is consistent with previous studies that claimed that there is a universal difference between subjective and objective sleep time for all insomnia patients (2123). Subjective sleep disturbance, decreased TST, and sleep continuity are robust in patients with anxiety-related disorders, with minor evidence for diminished sleep depth (5). However, studies that directly examined the distribution of different sleep perception types among patients with anxiety-related disorders are scarce. Underestimation of TST is prevalent among general insomniacs with normal objective TST (14, 21). In addition, some insomniacs can accurately estimate sleep time, while some may overestimate their TST (14). This phenomenon is also observed in our study of patients with anxiety-related disorders, of whom 51.1% underestimated their sleep time, 22.6% accurately estimate sleep time, and 26.2% overestimated their sleep time. The proportion of patients with normal sleep perception in our study was largely lower than general insomniacs and other psychiatric patients with sleep disorders (6, 7, 21). This indicates that patients with anxiety-related disorders who complain of insomnia may have worse sleep misperception. Pre-sleep cognitive activity is common in patients with anxiety-related disorders which are associated with underestimation of sleep time (5). These patients might also have paranoid ideation. Despite that 67.3% of patients in the NSPA group had used sedative hypnotic drugs, they still underestimated the sleep time. However, the high use rate of sedative hypnotic drugs seems not entirely useless. There were a certain number of patients with NSP or overestimation of TST in our study, which might be attributed to the effect of sedative hypnotic drugs. It was also possible that patients who overestimated their sleep time had potential cognitive impairment. A similar situation was observed in a study by Bian et al. (7), which showed that 43.2% of patients with stable schizophrenia presented sleep misperception, including 38.5% with overestimation of TST and 4.7% with underestimation of TST (7). Patients with schizophrenia tend to exaggerate their sleep quality, even for those with stable disease after effective antipsychotics treatment (24). Brain structure abnormality and alterations in neurotransmitters related to time perception may exist in patients with schizophrenia. In addition, COMT and ANKK1 genes are considered to be the possible reasons that lead to time misperception in these patients (25). Further research is needed to confirm whether neurophysiological mechanisms also exist in patients with anxiety-related disorders which contribute to sleep misperception.

In this study, we found that the sleep structure was completely different among groups. Patients with underestimation of TST showed lower sleep latency and WASO, but more NREM spontaneous arousal times and higher sleep efficiency than those with overestimation of TST or NSP. A study by Liu et al. grouped patients with OSAHA using the same method and showed similar results with our study in sleep structure among groups (12). Although patients in the NSPA group entered the sleep cycle faster, they had more spontaneous arousal times during the NREM period. They had difficulty in entering the REM period and would wake rapidly, with lower percentage of REM period to TST and WASO. Thus, they might believe that they get insufficient sleep, owing to decreased sleep depth and poor sleep continuity. Frequent periodic electroencephalogram changes during NREM period might be the reasons for sleep misperception (26, 27), but this needs further research. Despite the different sleep structure among groups, no differences were observed in personal and social behaviors. Some previous studies also analyzed the impact of sleep misperception on psychiatric symptoms, but the results are controversial (7, 14, 22, 28). The association of sleep misperception with psychiatric symptoms needs further investigation.

Identifying associated factors can help us carry out personalized treatment in risk population, which is also one of the objectives of our study. Sex, REM/TST (%), sleep efficiency, number of awakenings, N2/TST (%), REM spontaneous arousal times, sleep latency, diagnosis, social competence, and manifest psychosis were associated with sleep misperception. This was expected and consistent with the interplay of sleep structural parameters and was supported by previous studies (5, 12).

Although patients with insomnia often show a discrepancy between self-reported and objective sleep parameters, none of the standard treatment has been identified for insomnia have specifically targeted misperception of sleep. In our study, 73.8% of patients used sedative hypnotic drugs with high frequency prescription for insomnia in the last 6 months. Unnecessary use of sedative hypnotic drugs may bring side effects or lead to drug addiction syndrome and disrupt the integrity of sleep. Hence, treatment for self-reported sleep disorders should be reconsidered. Sleep misperception can be improved after psychotherapy (such as cognitive-behavioral therapy) for patients with insomnia (29). This might also be feasible in patients with anxiety-related disorders without any side effects, which deserves further investigations.

To our knowledge, this is the first study that explored the discrepancy between objective measures and subjective experience among patients with anxiety-related disorders in a large-scale study in China. However, there are still some limitations. First, potential bias was inevitable due to the retrospective nature and single-center design. Second, no data of imaging and pathological examinations were available in this study. Finally, only a two-night PSG was performed. Sleep time and misperception data obtained during multiple nights (such as seven nights) or by averaging might provide more valuable information with little variability.

In conclusion, this study indicates that sleep misperception is prevalent in patients with anxiety-related disorders. Objective sleep parameters differed in patients with various sleep perception types, with similar personal and social behaviors. Our results provide quantitative evidence for further study of sleep misperception in patients with anxiety-related disorders.

Data Availability Statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author/s.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Ethics Committee of Wuhan Mental Health Center. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

CZ and GL were involved in study conception and design. YL, XZ, BL, LH, FT, XL, CH, and HL were involved in the acquisition of data. YL and XZ were involved in the analysis and interpretation of data and were involved in drafting the manuscript. All authors were involved in revising the manuscript. All authors have also read and approved the final manuscript.

Funding

This work was supported by the General Program of Wuhan Medical Research (Grant Number: WG20C03). The funding source had no role in the study design, collection, analysis, and interpretation of data, writing of the manuscript, or decision to submit the article for publication.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Acknowledgments

The authors thank all the patients and their families for the participation.

References

1. Harvey AG, Murray G, Chandler RA, Soehner A. Sleep disturbance as transdiagnostic: consideration of neurobiological mechanisms. Clin Psychol Rev. (2011) 31:225–35. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2010.04.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Baglioni C, Nanovska S, Regen W, Spiegelhalder K, Feige B, Nissen C, et al. Sleep and mental disorders: A meta-analysis of polysomnographic research. Psychol Bull. (2016) 142:969–90.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

3. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders.5th ed. Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Association (2013).

Google Scholar

4. Richards A, Kanady JC, Neylan TC. Sleep disturbance in PTSD and other anxiety-related disorders: an updated review of clinical features, physiological characteristics, and psychological and neurobiological mechanisms. Neuropsychopharmacology. (2020) 45:55–73. doi: 10.1038/s41386–0.19–0.486-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Cox RC, Olatunji BO. Sleep in the anxiety-related disorders: A meta-analysis of subjective and objective research. Sleep Med Rev. (2020) 51:101282. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2020.101282

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Wenigmann M, Gorzka RJ, Garling M, Spiegelhalder K, Höllmer H, Schulz H. Sleep state misperception in psychiatric patients. Somnologie. (2019) 23:43–8. doi: 10.1007/s11818–0.18–0.181-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Bian Y, Wang ZX, Han XL, Chen L, Zhu Y, Wu CJ. Sleep state misperception in schizophrenia: Are negative symptoms at work? Compr Psychiatry. (2016) 67:33–8. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2016.02.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Zung WW A. rating instrument for anxiety disorders. Psychosomatics. (1971) 12:371–9.

Google Scholar

9. Jansen WJ, Ossenkoppele R, Tijms BM, Fagan AM, Hansson O, Klunk WE, et al. Association of Cerebral Amyloid-β Aggregation With Cognitive Functioning in Persons Without Dementia. JAMA Psychiatry. (2018) 75:84–95. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2017.3391

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Thorpy M. Classification of sleep disorders. In: Guglietta A, editor. Drug Treatment of Sleep Disorders. Cham: Springer International Publishing (2015). p. 71-83.

Google Scholar

11. The AASM Manual for the Scoring of Sleep and Associated Events: Rules, Terminology and Technical Specifications, Version 2.3. Available at: https://aasm.org/clinical-resources/scoring-manual/ (Accessed Apr 1, 2016).

12. Liu Y, Tan H, Yu Y, Zeng Y, Xiao L. Analysis of Clinical Characteristics and Polysomnography Indicators of Obstructive Sleep Apnea-Hypopnea Syndrome Patients Based on Sleep Perception Types. Front Neurol. (2020) 11:988. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2020.00988

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Bastien CH, Ceklic T, St-Hilaire P, Desmarais F, Pérusse AD, Lefrançois J, et al. Insomnia and sleep misperception. Pathol Biol (Paris). (2014) 62:241–51. doi: 10.1016/j.patbio.2014.07.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Fernandez-Mendoza J, Calhoun SL, Bixler EO, Karataraki M, Liao D, Vela-Bueno A, et al. Sleep misperception and chronic insomnia in the general population: role of objective sleep duration and psychological profiles. Psychosom Med. (2011) 73:88–97. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e3181fe365a

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Choi SJ, Suh S, Ong J, Joo EY. Sleep misperception in chronic insomnia patients with obstructive sleep apnea syndrome: implications for clinical assessment. J Clin Sleep Med. (2016) 12:1517–25. doi: 10.5664/jcsm.6280

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Tan S, Zou Y, Wykes T, Reeder C, Zhu X, Yang F, et al. Group cognitive remediation therapy for chronic schizophrenia: a randomized controlled trial. Neurosci Lett. (2016) 626:106–11. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2015.08.036

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Zhang Q, Xie J-J. Association between schizophrenia and syphilis: a retrospective study in Xiamen, China. BMC Psychiatry. (2018) 18:273. doi: 10.1186/s12888–0.18-1869-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Hu Q, Wang C, Liu F, He J, Wang F, Wang W, et al. High serum levels of FGF21 are decreased in bipolar mania patients during psychotropic medication treatment and are associated with increased metabolism disturbance. Psychiatry Res. (2019) 272:643–8. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2018.12.159

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Tan S, Zhu X, Fan H, Tan Y, Yang F, Wang Z, et al. Who will benefit from computerized cognitive remediation therapy? Evidence from a multisite randomized controlled study in schizophrenia. Psychol Med. (2020) 50:1633–43. doi: 10.1017/S0033291719001594

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Li Y. Application of NOSIE in the study of neuroleptic treatment. Zhonghua Shen Jing Jing Shen Ke Za Zhi. (1987) 20:325–7.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

21. Moon HJ, Song ML, Cho YW. Clinical Characteristics of Primary Insomniacs with Sleep-State Misperception. J Clin Neurol. (2015) 11:358–63. doi: 10.3988/jcn.2015.11.4.358

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Dittoni S, Mazza M, Losurdo A, Testani E, Di Giacopo R, Marano G, et al. Psychological functioning measures in patients with primary insomnia and sleep state misperception. Acta Neurol Scand. (2013) 128:54–60. doi: 10.1111/ane.12078

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Herbert V, Pratt D, Emsley R, Kyle SD. Predictors of nightly subjective-objective sleep discrepancy in poor sleepers over a seven-day period. Brain Sci. (2017) 7:29. doi: 10.3390/brainsci7030029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Monti JM, BaHammam AS, Pandi-Perumal SR, Bromundt V, Spence DW, Cardinali DP, et al. Sleep and circadian rhythm dysregulation in schizophrenia. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. (2013) 43:209–16. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2012.12.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Gómez J, Jesús Marín-Méndez J, Molero P, Atakan Z, Ortuño F. Time perception networks and cognition in schizophrenia: a review and a proposal. Psychiatry Res. (2014) 220:737–44. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2014.07.048

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Parrino L, Milioli G, De Paolis F, Grassi A, Terzano MG. Paradoxical insomnia: the role of CAP and arousals in sleep misperception. Sleep Med. (2009) 10:1139–45. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2008.12.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Hsiao F-C, Tsai P-J, Wu CW, Yang C-M, Lane TJ, Lee H-C, et al. The neurophysiological basis of the discrepancy between objective and subjective sleep during the sleep onset period: an EEG-fMRI study. Sleep. (2018) 41. doi: 10.1093/sleep/zsy056

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Castelnovo A, Ferri R, Galbiati A, Rossi A, Zucconi M, Castronovo V, et al. Extreme sleep state misperception: From psychopathology to objective-subjective sleep measures. Int J Psychophysiol. (2021) 167:77–85. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2021.06.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Janku K, Smotek M, Farkova E, Koprivova J. Subjective-objective sleep discrepancy in patients with insomnia during and after cognitive behavioural therapy: An actigraphy study. J Sleep Res. (2020) 29:e13064. doi: 10.1111/jsr.13064

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: misperception index, sleep misperception, anxiety-related disorders, polysomnography, total sleep time

Citation: Liang Y, Zhao X, Zhang C, Liu G, Lu B, Han L, Tong F, Luo X, Hu C and Liu H (2022) Sleep Misperception and Associated Factors in Patients With Anxiety-Related Disorders and Complaint of Insomnia: A Retrospective Study. Front. Neurol. 13:836949. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2022.836949

Received: 16 December 2021; Accepted: 14 February 2022;
Published: 07 April 2022.

Edited by:

Bin Zhang, Southern Medical University, China

Reviewed by:

Axel Steiger, Ludwig Maximilian University of Munich, Germany
Lieke Hermans, Philips Research, Netherlands

Copyright © 2022 Liang, Zhao, Zhang, Liu, Lu, Han, Tong, Luo, Hu and Liu. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Changyong Zhang, 1242644693@qq.com; Guangya Liu, 1559052053@qq.com

These authors have contributed equally to this work

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.