Skip to main content

REVIEW article

Front. Neurol., 25 February 2022
Sec. Neuroepidemiology
This article is part of the Research Topic The Impact of Chronic Kidney Disease on Cognitive Brain Health View all 10 articles

Impact of Chronic Kidney Disease on Brain Structure and Function

  • 1Department of Radiation Oncology, Carver College of Medicine, University of Iowa, Iowa City, IA, United States
  • 2Division of Nephrology, Dialysis, and Transplantation, University of Iowa Stead Family Children's Hospital, Iowa City, IA, United States

Chronic kidney disease (CKD) affects more than 37 million American adults. Adult-onset CKD is typically attributed to acquired comorbidities such as aging, type II diabetes, and cardiovascular disease. Conversely, congenital abnormalities of the kidney and urinary tract are the most common cause of CKD in children. Both adult and pediatric patients with CKD are at risk for neurocognitive dysfunction, particularly in the domain of executive function. The exact mechanism for neurocognitive dysfunction in CKD is not known; however, it is conceivable that the multisystemic effects of CKD—including hypertension, acidosis, anemia, proteinuria, and uremic milieu—exert a detrimental effect on the brain. Quantitative neuroimaging modalities, such as magnetic resonance imaging (MRI), provide a non-invasive way to understand the neurobiological underpinnings of cognitive dysfunction in CKD. Adult patients with CKD show differences in brain structure; however, much less is known about the impact of CKD on neurodevelopment in pediatric patients. Herein, this review will summarize current evidence of the impact of CKD on brain structure and function and will identify the critical areas for future research that are needed to better understand the modifiable risk factors for abnormal brain structure and function across both pediatric and adult CKD populations.

Introduction

Neurocognitive deficits have been well-described in both the adult and pediatric chronic kidney disease (CKD) and end-stage kidney disease (ESKD) populations (1, 2). These deficits are associated with longer duration of kidney disease (1, 3, 4), metabolic acidosis (5, 6), proteinuria/microalbuminuria (1, 7, 8), anemia (1, 9, 10), and hypertension (11, 12). Even subtle neurocognitive deficits have broad impacts on quality of life, as they contribute to poorer high school graduation rates and long-term underemployment in the adult CKD population (13). The cognitive complications of CKD may be linked to an aberrant “kidney-brain axis” whereby decreases in kidney function, CKD-associated sequelae (including cardiovascular disease), and concomitant inflammatory milieu all negatively impact the brain, leading to increased risk of cognitive impairment in parallel with CKD progression (1416).

Unfortunately, our understanding of the neurobiology of cognition in CKD is limited because of a lack of concurrent neuroimaging and neurocognitive assessment. Neurodevelopment is a dynamic process occurring throughout the human lifespan, with the most rapid neurodevelopmental changes occurring in childhood and adolescence (17)—specifically, reductions in cortical gray matter (e.g., dendritic pruning) and concomitant white matter (myelin) deposition (18). Our understanding of normal developmental processes in the context of chronic disease is limited. Use of magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) provides an opportunity to examine the brain structure, as it relates to CKD, in a noninvasive manner.

To reduce the burden of neurocognitive deficits within the CKD population, there is an urgent need for new approaches to patient care applied across CKD lifespan. Such approaches require understanding the effects of CKD progression and severity on both the adult and pediatric brain. In this review we will 1) address existing literature specific to the impact of CKD on brain structure and function and 2) discuss potential CKD-associated risk factors for abnormal brain structure and function in both pediatric and adult populations.

How Does CKD Impact Brain Structure and Function?

Neuroimaging in Adult CKD

More than 37 million adults in the United States are living with CKD and millions more are living with either undiagnosed CKD or with an increased lifetime risk of developing CKD. Diabetes and hypertension are the leading causes of CKD in adults, contributing to almost 66% of CKD cases in the United States (19). Cardiovascular disease remains the major cause of death for individuals with CKD.

Structural brain findings of adult patients with CKD–not receiving renal replacement therapy–have demonstrated the presence of cerebral atrophy, as well as decreased cerebral density in both white and gray matter. Poor kidney function has been associated with glomerular small vessel disease, and in the 2008 study by Ikram et al. (20) hemodynamic similarities were investigated between the kidneys and vascular beds of the brain by MRI. Here, impaired kidney function was associated with smaller brain volume, smaller deep white matter volume, and more white matter lesions (20). Other randomized trials, including the Systolic blood PRessure INterventional Trial (SPRINT), examined the effects of hypertension treatment on the structure of the brain in patients with and without CKD, with cognitive endpoints noted (21). The SPRINT study found that CKD patients who were on standard blood pressure treatments had increased risks of mortality and major cardiovascular events, demonstrated mild cognitive impairment, and showed small vessel ischemic disease (white matter lesions) by MRI.

White matter hyperintensities are noted more frequently within the adult CKD/ESKD population. These hyperintensities are often associated with increased cerebrovascular risk and may predict risk of stroke and dementia (22). Diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) is an MRI technique used to provide detailed images of the brain. MRI-DTI measures the rate at which water moves through the brain's white matter (23). While macrostructural MRI markers, such as white matter hyperintensities, correlate with reduced kidney function, loss of white matter microstructural integrity may be a more sensitive measure of white matter disease (24). White matter degeneration and white matter abnormalities may be attributed to several causes. First, the kidneys and brain share several perfusion pathways in which cardiovascular and hemodynamic deviances may damage both organs simultaneously. Other factors, including the hypertension seen in many CKD patients, may also play a role in the association between kidney function and white matter integrity (25). Finally, impaired kidney function can lead to increased circulating inflammatory factors. Proinflammatory factors decrease serum nitric oxide in the brain vasculature, and this could contribute to cerebral hypoperfusion, which, in turn, could lead to white matter damage as well (25).

Cerebral hypoperfusion has been implicated in neurodevelopmental disorders. Hemodynamic disturbances during CKD may play a role in the regulation of cerebral blood flow (26), potentially linking CDK to cognitive problems. An analysis based on the Rotterdam Study (27) found that lower estimated glomerular filtration rate (eGFR) is independently associated with lower cerebral blood flow (27). More recently, Lepping et al. (28) used arterial spin labeling to assess cerebral blood flow in a cohort of EKSD patients and age-matched controls. The authors' goal was identify kidney-associated brain changes following kidney transplantation. Here, after kidney transplant, cerebral blood decreased in ESKD patients to values comparable to controls. White matter integrity, as measured by fractional anisotropy and by mean diffusivity with MRI-DTI, also increased and decreased, respectively, post-kidney transplant (28). These measurements of white matter integrity taken after kidney transplant were comparable to controls. Taken together, these data suggest that proper kidney function is essential for regulation of blood flow to the brain and hemodynamic homeostasis.

Neuroimaging in Pediatric CKD

In contrast to adult CKD, congenital anomalies of the kidney and urinary tract are the leading causes of CKD among children ages birth to 4 years, whereas systemic diseases, infection, and glomerular disease (e.g., focal segmental glomerular sclerosis) become the leading causes of kidney failure in the older pediatric population (29). Up to half of all children with congenital CKD will experience a decline in kidney function so severe as to require dialysis and eventually, a kidney transplant (30). Thus, children with congenital anomalies of the kidney and urinary tract are faced with a lifetime of CKD and of potential detrimental impacts on the developing brain.

Neuroimaging studies in the pediatric population have focused mainly on children with moderate to severe CKD (including dialysis and transplant populations). The effect of kidney disease in these children introduces many confounding factors that affect brain function, such as uremia. Another limitation of imaging studies in older children is that these studies cannot provide direct information about the key stages of brain development that occur from birth to 4 years of age. These issues signal a critical gap in our understanding of the kidney-brain axis during periods of peak neurodevelopment.

Cystic kidney diseases are some of the leading causes of early-onset inherited kidney disorders. These diseases are often characterized by enlarged kidneys with multiple cysts and progressive kidney impairment. Autosomal dominant and autosomal recessive polycystic kidney disease, as well as Meckel's syndrome, are a few examples of renal ciliopathies seen in the pediatric population. Cysts develop due to uncontrolled epithelial cell proliferation, growth, and altered cell polarity—events that occur downstream of abnormal cilia-dependent signaling. Due to the early onset of renal ciliopathy-induced CKD and concomitant development of severe hypertension, it is thought that ciliopathies are a risk factor for neurocognitive dysfunction secondary to CKD (31). The spectrum of neurocognitive deficits ranges from relatively benign (akin to that seen in polycystic kidney diseases) to more progressive deficits in neurocognition (such as that seen in Joubert syndrome). One cross-sectional, control-matched analysis, which involved the Chronic Kidney Disease in Children (CKiD) cohort, compared a group of autosomal recessive polycystic kidney disease patients with mild-to-moderate CKD patients with respect to intellectual functioning, academic achievement, attention regulation, executive functioning, and behavior (31). No differences were observed between these two disease cohorts; however, further investigation into the potential effects of renal ciliopathies on neurocognition in the pediatric population is needed.

The majority of published pediatric CKD studies evaluating volumetric brain structure have used computerized tomography (CT) imaging data obtained in a clinical setting, rather than quantitative MRI data obtained as a part of a specific research focus (19). Brain atrophy is well-described in the early pediatric nephrology literature (prior to 1990): up to 60% of patients had atrophy that was not clearly related to etiology of disease or CKD-associated sequelae such as hypertension (32). Qualitative CT imaging also provides evidence for global cerebral atrophy, silent white matter infarcts, and ventriculomegaly in advanced pediatric CKD (19). In other studies, lower cerebral density (33, 34) and ventriculomegaly secondary to brain atrophy (35) were found to be associated with the need for pediatric hemodialysis (especially duration), but not with peritoneal dialysis (36).

To date, there are only four published studies that have utilized quantitative, research-based MRI sequences to evaluate the brain in CKD (26, 3740). Hartung and colleagues (41) used MRI to examine brain structure in 85 patients aged 8–25 with CKD, encompassing a mix of pre- and post-transplant patients (37). The authors reported subtle, cortical gray matter abnormalities; however, their findings were significant only in unadjusted models and did not persist in models adjusted for age and gender. In this study, observed volumetric brain differences were more prominent among the kidney transplant recipients compared to those with pre-transplant CKD. More recently, our group has observed significant reductions in overall cerebellar gray matter volume and unexpectedly, an increase in cortical (cerebral) gray matter volume among children ages 6–16 years old with early-stage CKD, relative to controls (39). The degree of cerebellar volume reduction was associated with estimated glomerular filtration rate (Figure 1). Volumetric reduction in the cerebellar gray matter was also associated with poorer performance in tests of executive function. The volumetric increase in the cerebral gray matter was associated with poorer mathematics performance (Figure 2).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Neuroanatomical differences between controls and patients with pediatric chronic kidney disease CKD. (A) Shows the standardized group estimates (x-axis) and 95% confidence limits of the estimates for each of the regions of interest (ROI) included in the analysis (y-axis). Estimates are adjusted for age, socioeconomic status (SES) and maternal education. The red (vertical) line marks 0, or no significant effect of group on ROI. Red circles mark significant group estimates. (B) Shows the relationship between estimated glomerular filtration rate, eGFR, (x-axis) and standardized cerebellum gray matter volume (y-axis) in the CKD group. Reproduced with permission from Solomon et al. (39).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Structure-function relationships in the pediatric chronic kidney disease (CKD) group. Significant associations are illustrated for cerebellum gray matter and category fluency (A) and cerebrum gray matter and arithmetic (B). Results were adjusted for age, parental SES and maternal education. Reproduced with permission from Solomon et al. (39).

CKD is a known risk factor for cerebrovascular disease, with white matter being particularly susceptible to the effects of altered vascular tone. Matsuda et al. (38) evaluated the impact of CKD on brain white matter within a cohort of 49 children, including 29 children with CKD of varying stages, ranging from pre- to post-transplantation. Diffusion tensor imaging was used to compare white matter microstructure in CKD patients compared to controls.

Patients with CKD were found to have abnormal white matter microstructure (specifically, within the anterior limb of the internal capsule), as reflected by decreased white matter fractional anisotropy and increased mean diffusivity and radial diffusivity. Within the sample, 21% of CKD participants had evidence of focal and multifocal white matter injuries compared to healthy controls. No neurocognitive data were obtained in this study; thus, the clinical significance of these white matter findings remains unclear.

Recently, our group identified global abnormalities in white matter microstructural integrity in CKD patients compared to controls. The global decrease in white matter fractional anisotropy was driven by regional reductions within the body of the corpus callosum, cerebral, cingulum (hippocampus), and posterior limb of the internal capsule (Figure 3). Despite these significant differences in white matter integrity, we found no significant association between the neurocognitive abilities of CKD patients and white matter fractional anisotropy. Likewise, there were no CKD-associated medical variables that emerged as predictors for decreased white matter integrity.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Group differences in global and regional white matter fractional anisotropy. Patient group (case or control) is shown on the x-axis and white matter fractional anisotropy on the y-axis. Individual observations are represented by small, gray circles. Means and 95% confidence limits of the means are shown for each group. (A) Shows the global fractional anisotropy, and (B–G) show white matter fractional anisotropy for the corpus callosum, cerebral peduncle, posterior limb of the internal capsule, retrolenticular internal capsule, external capsule, and cingulum hippocampus, respectively.

Cerebral blood flow, which is a reliable measure of cerebrovascular integrity, can be quantified using arterial spin labeling MRI. Based on data from the Neurocognitive Assessment and Magnetic Resonance Imaging Analysis of Children and Young Adults with Chronic Kidney Disease (NiCK) study cohort (41), Liu et al. (26) found, unexpectedly, that global cerebral blood flow is higher in children with CKD compared to healthy controls (41). These findings are in line with adult data showing increased global cerebral blood flow, again as determined using arterial spin labeling sequences (42). Since increases in cerebral blood flow are associated with reduced hematocrit level in children with CKD (26), it has been hypothesized that the higher cerebral blood flow reflects a physiological compensation for the chronic anemia typically associated with advanced CKD.

Disease or Development: Delineating Risk Factors for Brain Abnormalities in the Setting of CKD

Cognitive deficit in CKD has been linked to a variety of mechanisms associated with decreased kidney function, including concomitant uremia, proteinuria, anemia, metabolic acidosis, and cardiovascular disease (43). These CKD-associated medical sequelae become more prominent in CKD stage 3 and beyond (eGFR <60 ml/min/1.73 m2). Elevated blood urea nitrogen (uremia) is a CKD-associated comorbidity that often appears together with cognitive problems. Neurons, such as the noradrenergic and serotonergic neurons responsible for sleep/wake cycles and motor control, as well as the acetylcholinergic neurons responsible for memory, may be particularly sensitive to high levels of uremic milieu (44). Alterations in the monoaminergic neurons may contribute to the transient development of cognitive impairment that is seen in patients with advanced CKD requiring dialysis (44). Although uremia is commonly invoked as a primary etiology for cognitive deficit in CKD, a clear neuroimaging correlate to link changes in the brain with symptomatic uremia is lacking. One recent case series evaluated MRI scans from patients with clinically documented uremic encephalopathy and showed bilateral basal ganglia lesions in the majority of images (45). However, since no neurocognitive phenotype was described in this study, no inference can be drawn as to whether the lesions noted were associated with cognitive deficit.

Metabolic acidosis may play a direct role in cognitive impairment in adults with CKD. Animal data suggest that metabolic acidosis perpetuates neuronal dysfunction by upregulation of excitatory synapses on gaba-aminobutyric acid-ergic neurons (44, 46). The SPRINT-MIND [Systolic Blood Pressure Intervention Trial Memory and Cognition IN Decreased Hypertension (21)] cohort assesses adults with hypertension and includes adults with CKD. Data from this study showed that decreases in serum bicarbonate were independently associated with lower performance on tests of executive function (5). Harshman et al. (6) evaluated the impact of metabolic acidosis in relationship to blood pressure variability in pediatric CKD patients using the CKiD cohort. The researchers found that the effect of increased blood pressure variability on executive function was attenuated in the setting of higher serum bicarbonate levels.

Hypertension (HTN), a CKD-associated comorbidity, can affect the severity and course of cerebrovascular disease. Consequently, HTN is a potentially modifiable risk factor for cognitive defects in both pediatric and adult CKD populations. As noted previously, the SPRINT study is the largest intervention study to date looking at the effect of intensive blood pressure control on cardiovascular outcomes among persons at high risk for cardiovascular disease. The SPRINT trial demonstrated a role for intensive systolic blood pressure control (goal of <120 mm Hg) in the reduction of probable dementia within an adult CKD subgroup (21). Importantly, data from the SPRINT-MIND sub-analysis found no detrimental effect of intensive lowering of systolic blood pressure on brain perfusion or volumetric structure (47).

The neurocognitive deficits in the adult CKD population exacerbate comorbidities and contribute to a lower quality of life. The most frequently impaired cognitive domains in the adult CKD population include executive function, orientation, and attention (48). Multiple studies have reported increases in cognitive impairment with increasing age; however, even the younger kidney transplant candidate in the adult population has a relatively high burden of cognitive impairments compared to the average adult. The conclusions from Chu et al. (48) suggest that transplant centers consider screening kidney disease patients for global cognitive impairment throughout their clinical care regardless of age (48).

The association between hypertension, CKD, and cognition has received a significant amount of focus within the prospective CKiD cohort study. Lande et al. (12) evaluated data from children with mild to moderate CKD who have elevated blood pressure [i.e., systolic or diastolic blood pressure > 90th percentile for age (49)]. Children with elevated blood pressure were more likely to have lower nonverbal IQ than normotensive children. Within the analysis, it was also noted that the blood pressure index (i.e., the subject's blood pressure divided by the 95th percentile blood pressure for that subject's gender, age, and height) correlated inversely with nonverbal IQ, and this relationship was maintained even after controlling for demographic and disease related variables.

The neurocognitive deficits in the pediatric CKD population have also been assessed through batteries such as the Penn Computerized Neurocognitive Battery (50). This test revealed that children and young adults with CKD have lower accuracy in tests of complex cognition compared to their age-matched peers, as well as deficits in verbal reasoning, nonverbal reasoning, and spatial processing. Patients with CKD also had lower accuracy for attention but were found to have faster response times, possibly indicating greater impulsivity.

While unlikely to be the underlying cause of adult cognitive impairment, genetic factors may influence the pathogenesis of cognitive dysfunction in pediatric CKD patients. Both single-gene variants and copy number variants have been implicated as potential factors influencing cognitive deficit in pediatric CKD. Genomic differences associated with pediatric CKD were analyzed as part of the CKiD study (51); the aim was to determine whether genetic factors (in addition to, or perhaps rather than, renal impairment) were responsible for the subtle neurocognitive differences seen in pediatric CKD. Children with CKD-associated genomic disorders were found to score significantly poorer on all measures of intelligence and executive function compared to noncarriers (52).

Variation in the klotho gene has been associated with both an accelerated aging phenotype as well as development and progression of CKD (53, 54). Klotho is expressed in the brain, with high levels of mRNA expression in the choroid plexus, hippocampus, and cerebellar Purkinje cells (55, 56). The klotho gene is also highly expressed in the kidney, where Klotho acts as a coreceptor for fibroblast growth factor 23 (FGF23) in regulating calcium and phosphorus homeostasis (57). Homozygous klotho knockout mice and CKD subjects have similar phenotypes, suggesting that klotho dysfunction may contribute to CKD progression (54, 58). Additionally, Klotho-deficient mice demonstrate an accelerated aging phenotype characterized by neurodegeneration and cognitive deficits (59, 60). In both CKD patients and in mice lacking Klotho function, plasma FGF23 levels increase (61). Limited preclinical suggests that elevation of FGF23 is associated with abnormalities of hippocampal neural networks. While previous studies evaluating the effect of FGF23 on cognition have been equivocal in adults, data from the CKiD cohort suggests that a higher plasma FGF23 level is associated with higher cognitive impairment and lower performance in tests of executive function (55, 62).

Recent evidence suggests that there is crosstalk between the kidney and brain, and that this “kidney-brain axis” is sensitive to cellular oxidative stress and chronic inflammatory processes (14). The crosstalk, mediated by reactive oxygen species and inflammatory markers, may contribute to the high prevalence of cognitive impairment observed during the progression of CKD. Emerging data suggests that the metabolic interactions of the “kidney-brain axis” are likely mediated—at least in part–by the activities of hormetic processes and a phenomenon dependent on the severity of disease (63, 64). Oxidant-induced inflammatory pathways could be promising therapeutic targets for the protection of neurocognitive function in developing children who have CKD.

Neuroimaging in CKD: Where Do We Go From Here?

In contrast to adult CKD populations, there is a paucity of systematic, quantitative neuroimaging studies in young children with CKD. The reasons for this are multiple. Certainly, there are clear challenges to obtaining quality images from non-sedated, young children. Furthermore, published pediatric CKD neuroimaging studies have relied on patient samples with heterogenous disease stage and etiology, making it difficult to pinpoint mechanisms of neurocognitive deficits across the lifespan. Additionally, there has been a stark lack of attention to the impact of CKD on early brain development: to date, only one neuroimaging study has been published that evaluated the brain in very young CKD patients (children younger than 8 years of age) (39).

The value of incorporating neuroscience-oriented analyses into pediatric CKD research is that this combined approach provides the clinical-translational tools needed to identify potential neurobiological mechanisms that bring about neurocognitive deficits in CKD children. This means moving beyond the use of clinical scans to describe brain structure in pediatric CKD; by their nature, these scans can highlight only a limited range of the potential neurobiological mechanisms. Multisite collaborations are necessary to address limitations related to small sample sizes and heterogeneity of neuroimaging research in pediatric CKD. And for these multisite neuroimaging initiatives to be effective, collaboration between nephrologists and neuroscientists is essential as well.

Neuroimaging research in the field of CKD requires tandem assessment of cognition and kidney disease sequelae, in order to identify patients who are at risk for neurocognitive deficits and also to learn how early brain changes relate to CKD progression. In comparison to the pediatric literature, the adult CKD-brain literature is robust and incorporates a comprehensive approach to the study of the brain. This should serve as an example for the field of pediatric nephrology to embrace when designing future CKD-brain studies. Neurocognitive difficulties emerge in early childhood, during early CKD, and signal a need for a greater understanding of how the developing brain is affected by this life-long, chronic disease process. Thus, longitudinal neurological assessment into adulthood is essential, and must continue through the period when CKD progresses to the point of requiring dialysis and/or kidney transplant. Future neuroimaging research is necessary to elucidate the neurobiological underpinnings of cognitive deficits in CKD.

Author Contributions

The original manuscript, Impact of Chronic Kidney Disease on Brain Structure and Function, was ES and LH. Both authors had equal contribution to manuscript research, writing, and editing. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This work was supported by R01DK128835 - active National Institutes of Health/NIDDK “Brain anatomical imaging and neurocognition in pediatric kidney disease (BRAIN KID)” (to LH).

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

1. Hooper SR, Gerson AC, Butler RW, Gipson DS, Mendley SR, Lande MB, et al. Neurocognitive functioning of children and adolescents with mild-to-moderate chronic kidney disease. Clin J Am Soc Nephrol. (2011) 6:1824–30. doi: 10.2215/CJN.09751110

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Chen K, Didsbury M, van Zwieten A, Howell M, Kim S, Tong A, et al. Neurocognitive and educational outcomes in children and adolescents with CKD: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Clin J Am Soc Nephrol. (2018) 13:387–97. doi: 10.2215/CJN.09650917

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Hooper SR, Gerson AC, Johnson RJ, Mendley SR, Shinnar S, Lande MB, et al. Neurocognitive social-behavioral, and adaptive functioning in preschool children with mild to moderate kidney disease. J Dev Behav Pediatr. (2016) 37:231–8. doi: 10.1097/DBP.0000000000000267

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Hooper SR, Laney N, Radcliffe J, Moodalbail D, Hartung EA, Ruebner RL, et al. Executive functioning in children, adolescents, and young adults with chronic kidney disease. J Dev Behav Pediatr. (2015) 36:734–42. doi: 10.1097/DBP.0000000000000221

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Dobre M, Gaussoin SA, Bates JT, Chonchol MB, Cohen DL, Hostetter TH, et al. Serum bicarbonate concentration and cognitive function in hypertensive adults. Clin J Am Soc Nephrol. (2018) 13:596–603. doi: 10.2215/CJN.07050717

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Harshman LA, Kogon AJ, Matheson MB, Johnson RJ, Shinnar S, Gerson AC, et al. Bicarbonate blood pressure, and executive function in pediatric CKD-is there a link? Pediatr Nephrol. (2020) 35:1323–30. doi: 10.1007/s00467-020-04507-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Stern RA, Alosco ML. The Oxford Handbook of Adult Cognitive Disorders. New York, NY: Oxford University Press (2019).

Google Scholar

8. Kurella Tamura M, Muntner P, Wadley V, Cushman M, Zakai NA, Bradbury BD, et al. Albuminuria, kidney function, and the incidence of cognitive impairment among adults in the United States. Am J Kidney Dis. (2011) 58:756–63. doi: 10.1053/j.ajkd.2011.05.027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Kurella Tamura M, Xie D, Yaffe K, Cohen DL, Teal V, Kasner SE, et al. Vascular risk factors and cognitive impairment in chronic kidney disease: the Chronic Renal Insufficiency Cohort (CRIC) study. Clin J Am Soc Nephrol. (2011) 6:248–56. doi: 10.2215/CJN.02660310

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Kurella Tamura M, Vittinghoff E, Yang J, Go AS, Seliger SL, Kusek JW, et al. Anemia and risk for cognitive decline in chronic kidney disease. BMC Nephrol. (2016) 17:13. doi: 10.1186/s12882-016-0226-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Lande MB, Mendley SR, Matheson MB, Shinnar S, Gerson AC, Samuels JA, et al. Association of blood pressure variability and neurocognition in children with chronic kidney disease. Pediatr Nephrol. (2016) 31:2137–44. doi: 10.1007/s00467-016-3425-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Lande MB, Gerson AC, Hooper SR, Cox C, Matheson M, Mendley SR, et al. Casual blood pressure and neurocognitive function in children with chronic kidney disease: a report of the children with chronic kidney disease cohort study. Clin J Am Soc Nephrol. (2011) 6:1831–7. doi: 10.2215/CJN.00810111

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Rosenkranz J, Bonzel KE, Bulla M, Michalk D, Offner G, Reichwald-Klugger E, et al. Psychosocial adaptation of children and adolescents with chronic renal failure. Pediatr Nephrol. (1992) 6:459–63. doi: 10.1007/BF00874014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Miranda AS, Cordeiro TM, Dos Santos Lacerda Soares TM, Ferreira RN, Simoes ESAC. Kidney-brain axis inflammatory cross-talk: from bench to bedside. Clin Sci (Lond). (2017) 131:1093–105. doi: 10.1042/CS20160927

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Bugnicourt JM, Godefroy O, Chillon JM, Choukroun G, Massy ZA. Cognitive disorders and dementia in CKD: the neglected kidney-brain axis. J Am Soc Nephrol. (2013) 24:353–63. doi: 10.1681/ASN.2012050536

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Kurella Tamura M, Tam K, Vittinghoff E, Raj D, Sozio SM, Rosas SE, et al. Inflammatory markers and risk for cognitive decline in chronic kidney disease: the CRIC study. Kidney Int Rep. (2017) 2:192–200. doi: 10.1016/j.ekir.2016.10.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Pomponio R, Erus G, Habes M, Doshi J, Srinivasan D, Mamourian E, et al. Harmonization of large MRI datasets for the analysis of brain imaging patterns throughout the lifespan. Neuroimage. (2020) 208:116450. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116450

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Giedd JN, Blumenthal J, Jeffries NO, Castellanos FX, Liu H, Zijdenbos A, et al. Brain development during childhood and adolescence: a longitudinal MRI study. Nat Neurosci. (1999) 2:861–3. doi: 10.1038/13158

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Moodalbail DG, Reiser KA, Detre JA, Schultz RT, Herrington JD, Davatzikos C, et al. Systematic review of structural functional neuroimaging findings in children adults with CKD. Clin J Am Soc Nephrol. (2013) 8:1429–48. doi: 10.2215/CJN.11601112

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Ikram MA, Vernooij MW, Hofman A, Niessen WJ, van der Lugt A, Breteler MM. Kidney function is related to cerebral small vessel disease. Stroke. (2008) 39:55–61. doi: 10.1161/STROKEAHA.107.493494

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Group SMIftSR, Williamson JD, Pajewski NM, Auchus AP, Bryan RN, Chelune G, et al. Effect of intensive vs standard blood pressure control on probable dementia: a randomized clinical trial. JAMA. (2019) 321:553–61. doi: 10.1001/jama.2018.21442

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Khatri M, Wright CB, Nickolas TL, Yoshita M, Paik MC, Kranwinkel G, et al. Chronic kidney disease is associated with white matter hyperintensity volume: the Northern Manhattan Study (NOMAS). Stroke. (2007) 38:3121–6. doi: 10.1161/STROKEAHA.107.493593

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Debette S, Markus HS. The clinical importance of white matter hyperintensities on brain magnetic resonance imaging: systematic review and meta-analysis. Bmj. (2010) 341:c3666. doi: 10.1136/bmj.c3666

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Sedaghat S, Cremers LG, de Groot M, Hoorn EJ, Hofman A, van der Lugt A, et al. Kidney function and microstructural integrity of brain white matter. Neurology. (2015) 85:154–61. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000001741

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Makin SD, Cook FA, Dennis MS, Wardlaw JM. Cerebral small vessel disease and renal function: systematic review and meta-analysis. Cerebrovasc Dis. (2015) 39:39–52. doi: 10.1159/000369777

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Liu HS, Hartung EA, Jawad AF, Ware JB, Laney N, Port AM, et al. Regional cerebral blood flow in children and young adults with chronic kidney disease. Radiology. (2018) 288:849–58. doi: 10.1148/radiol.2018171339

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Sedaghat S, Vernooij MW, Loehrer E, Mattace-Raso FU, Hofman A, van der Lugt A, et al. Kidney function and cerebral blood flow: the rotterdam study. J Am Soc Nephrol. (2016) 27:715–21. doi: 10.1681/ASN.2014111118

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Lepping RJ, Montgomery RN, Sharma P, Mahnken JD, Vidoni ED, Choi IY, et al. Normalization of cerebral blood flow, neurochemicals, and white matter integrity after kidney transplantation. J Am Soc Nephrol. (2021) 32:177–87. doi: 10.1681/ASN.2020050584

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. McEnery PT, Stablein DM, Arbus G, Tejani A. Renal transplantation in children: A report of the North American Pediatric Renal Transplant Cooperative Study. New England J Med. (1992) 326:1727–32. doi: 10.1056/NEJM199206253262602

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Seikaly MG, Ho PL, Emmett L, Fine RN, Tejani A. Chronic renal insufficiency in children: the 2001 Annual Report of the NAPRTCS. Pediatr Nephrol. (2003) 18:796–804. doi: 10.1007/s00467-003-1158-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Hartung EA, Matheson M, Lande MB, Dell KM, Guay-Woodford LM, Gerson AC, et al. Neurocognition in children with autosomal recessive polycystic kidney disease in the CKiD cohort study. Pediatr Nephrol. (2014) 29:1957–65. doi: 10.1007/s00467-014-2816-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Steinberg A, Efrat R, Pomeranz A, Drukker A. Computerized tomography of the brain in children with chronic renal failure. Int J Pediatr Nephrol. (1985) 6:121–6.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

33. Schnaper HW, Cole BR, Hodges FJ, Robson AM. Cerebral cortical atrophy in pediatric patients with end-stage renal disease. Am J Kidney Dis. (1983) 2:645–50. doi: 10.1016/S0272-6386(83)80046-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Dettori P, La Greca G, Biasioli S, Chiaramonte S, Fabris A, Feriani M, et al. Changes of cerebral density in dialyzed patients. Neuroradiology. (1982) 23:95–9. doi: 10.1007/BF00367245

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Kretzschmar K, Nix W, Zschiedrich H, Philipp T. Morphologic cerebral changes in patients undergoing dialysis for renal failure. AJNR Am J Neuroradiol. (1983) 4:439–41.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

36. La Greca G, Biasioli S, Chiaramonte S, Dettori P, Fabris A, Feriani M, et al. Studies on brain density in hemodialysis and peritoneal dialysis. Nephron. (1982) 31:146–50. doi: 10.1159/000182634

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Hartung EA, Erus G, Jawad AF, Laney N, Doshi JJ, Hooper SR, et al. Brain magnetic resonance imaging findings in children and young adults with CKD. Am J Kidney Dis. (2018) 72:349–59. doi: 10.1053/j.ajkd.2017.11.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Matsuda-Abedini M, Fitzpatrick K, Harrell WR, Gipson DS, Hooper SR, Beiger A, et al. Brain abnormalities in children and adolescents with chronic kidney disease. Pediatr Res. (2018) 84:387–92. doi: 10.1038/s41390-018-0037-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Solomon MA, van der Plas E, Langbehn KE, Novak M, Schultz JL, Koscik TR, et al. Early pediatric chronic kidney disease is associated with brain volumetric gray matter abnormalities. Pediatr Res. (2021) 89:526–32. doi: 10.1038/s41390-020-01203-w

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. van der Plas E, Lullmann O, Hopkins L, Schultz JL, Nopoulos PC, Harshman LA. Associations between neurofilament light-chain protein, brain structure, and chronic kidney disease. Pediatric Res. (2021). doi: 10.1038/s41390-021-01649-6. [Epub ahead of print].

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Hartung EA, Laney N, Kim JY, Ruebner RL, Detre JA, Liu HS, et al. Design and methods of the NiCK study: neurocognitive assessment and magnetic resonance imaging analysis of children and young adults with chronic kidney disease. BMC Nephrol. (2015) 16:66. doi: 10.1186/s12882-015-0061-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Jiang XL, Wen JQ, Zhang LJ, Zheng G, Li X, Zhang Z, et al. Cerebral blood flow changes in hemodialysis and peritoneal dialysis patients: an arterial-spin labeling MR imaging. Metab Brain Dis. (2016) 31:929–36. doi: 10.1007/s11011-016-9829-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Gerson AC, Butler R, Moxey-Mims M, Wentz A, Shinnar S, Lande MB, et al. Neurocognitive outcomes in children with chronic kidney disease: current findings and contemporary endeavors. Ment Retard Dev Disabil Res Rev. (2006) 12:208–15. doi: 10.1002/mrdd.20116

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Li F, Liu X, Su Z, Sun R. Acidosis leads to brain dysfunctions through impairing cortical GABAergic neurons. Biochem Biophys Res Commun. (2011) 410:775–9. doi: 10.1016/j.bbrc.2011.06.053

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Kim DM, Lee IH, Song CJ. Uremic encephalopathy: MR imaging findings and clinical correlation. AJNR Am J Neuroradiol. (2016) 37:1604–9. doi: 10.3174/ajnr.A4776

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Song R, Zhang L, Yang Z, Tian X. Acidosis and alkalosis impair brain functions through weakening spike encoding at cortical GABAergic neurons. J Neurol Sci. (2011) 304:122–6. doi: 10.1016/j.jns.2011.01.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Weiner DE, Gaussoin SA, Nord J, Auchus AP, Chelune GJ, Chonchol M, et al. Cognitive function and kidney disease: Baseline data from the systolic blood pressure intervention trial (SPRINT). Am J Kidney Dis. (2017) 70:357–67. doi: 10.1053/j.ajkd.2017.04.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Chu NM, Chen X, Gross AL, Carlson MC, Garonzik-Wang JM, Norman SP, et al. Cognitive impairment burden in older and younger adults across the kidney transplant care continuum. Clin Transplant. (2021) 35:e14425. doi: 10.1111/ctr.14425

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Flynn JT, Kaelber DC, Baker-Smith CM, Blowey D, Carroll AE, Daniels SR, et al. Clinical practice guideline for screening and management of high blood pressure in children and adolescents. Pediatrics. (2017) 140:e20171904. doi: 10.1542/peds.2017-1904

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Hartung EA, Kim JY, Laney N, Hooper SR, Radcliffe J, Port AM, et al. Evaluation of neurocognition in youth with CKD using a novel computerized neurocognitive battery. Clin J Am Soc Nephrol. (2016) 11:39–46. doi: 10.2215/CJN.02110215

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Verbitsky M, Sanna-Cherchi S, Fasel DA, Levy B, Kiryluk K, Wuttke M, et al. Genomic imbalances in pediatric patients with chronic kidney disease. J Clin Invest. (2015) 125:2171–8. doi: 10.1172/JCI80877

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Verbitsky M, Kogon AJ, Matheson M, Hooper SR, Wong CS, Warady BA, et al. Genomic disorders and neurocognitive impairment in pediatric CKD. J Am Soc Nephrol. (2017) 28:2303–9. doi: 10.1681/ASN.2016101108

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Kuro-o M, Matsumura Y, Aizawa H, Kawaguchi H, Suga T, Utsugi T, et al. Mutation of the mouse klotho gene leads to a syndrome resembling ageing. Nature. (1997) 390:45–51. doi: 10.1038/36285

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Hu MC, Kuro-o M, Moe OW. Klotho and chronic kidney disease. Contrib Nephrol. (2013) 180:47–63. doi: 10.1159/000346778

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Clinton SM, Glover ME, Maltare A, Laszczyk AM, Mehi SJ, Simmons RK, et al. Expression of klotho mRNA and protein in rat brain parenchyma from early postnatal development into adulthood. Brain Res. (2013) 1527:1–14. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2013.06.044

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. German DC, Khobahy I, Pastor J, Kuro OM, Liu X. Nuclear localization of Klotho in brain: an anti-aging protein. Neurobiol Aging. (2012) 33:1483 e25–30. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2011.12.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Kuro-o M. Klotho in health and disease. Curr Opin Nephrol Hypertens. (2012) 21:362–8. doi: 10.1097/MNH.0b013e32835422ad

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Hu MC, Shi M, Zhang J, Quinones H, Griffith C, Kuro-o M, et al. Klotho deficiency causes vascular calcification in chronic kidney disease. J Am Soc Nephrol. (2011) 22:124–36. doi: 10.1681/ASN.2009121311

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Deary IJ, Harris SE, Fox HC, Hayward C, Wright AF, Starr JM, et al. KLOTHO genotype and cognitive ability in childhood and old age in the same individuals. Neurosci Lett. (2005) 378:22–7. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2004.12.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Dubal DB, Yokoyama JS, Zhu L, Broestl L, Worden K, Wang D, et al. Life extension factor klotho enhances cognition. Cell Rep. (2014) 7:1065–76. doi: 10.1016/j.celrep.2014.03.076

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Portale AA, Wolf MS, Messinger S, Perwad F, Jüppner H, Warady BA, et al. Fibroblast growth factor 23 and risk of CKD progression in children. Clin J Am Soc Nephrol. (2016) 11:1989–98. doi: 10.2215/CJN.02110216

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Komaba H. [CKD-MBD (chronic kidney disease-mineral and bone disorder). Role of FGF23-Klotho axis in CKD-MBD]. Clin Calcium. (2010) 20:1028–36.

Google Scholar

63. Kovalčíková A, Gyurászová M, Vavrincová-Yaghi D, Vavrinec P, Tóthová L, Boor P, et al. Oxidative stress in the brain caused by acute kidney injury. Metab Brain Dis. (2018) 33:961–7. doi: 10.1007/s11011-018-0204-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Calabrese V, Scuto M, Salinaro AT, Dionisio G, Modafferi S, Ontario ML, et al. Hydrogen sulfide and carnosine: modulation of oxidative stress and inflammation in kidney and brain axis. Antioxidants (Basel). (2020) 9:1303. doi: 10.3390/antiox9121303

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: chronic kidney disease, magnetic resonance imaging (MRI), pediatric chronic kidney disease, neurocognition and behavior, brain structural abnormalities, neuroimaging, disease and development

Citation: Steinbach EJ and Harshman LA (2022) Impact of Chronic Kidney Disease on Brain Structure and Function. Front. Neurol. 13:797503. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2022.797503

Received: 18 October 2021; Accepted: 24 January 2022;
Published: 25 February 2022.

Edited by:

Dearbhla Kelly, Trinity College Dublin, Ireland

Reviewed by:

Roberto Chimenz, University of Messina, Italy
(EE) Marius Miglinas, Vilnius University, Lithuania

Copyright © 2022 Steinbach and Harshman. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Lyndsay A. Harshman, lyndsay-harshman@uiowa.edu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.