Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Neurol., 07 March 2022
Sec. Pediatric Neurology
This article is part of the Research Topic Neuronal Plasticity and Neuromodulation in Development and Developmental Disorders View all 13 articles

Electrocorticography to Investigate Age-Related Brain Lateralization on Pediatric Motor Inhibition

\nChao-Hung Kuo,,,
Chao-Hung Kuo1,2,3,4*Kaitlyn CasimoKaitlyn Casimo5Jing WuJing Wu6Kelly Collins,Kelly Collins4,7Patrick RicePatrick Rice8Bo-Wei ChenBo-Wei Chen1Shih-Hung YangShih-Hung Yang9Yu-Chun LoYu-Chun Lo10Edward J. Novotny,Edward J. Novotny11,12Kurt E. Weaver,Kurt E. Weaver13,14You-Yin Chen,
You-Yin Chen1,10*Jeffrey G. Ojemann,,Jeffrey G. Ojemann4,14,15
  • 1Department of Biomedical Engineering, National Yang Ming Chiao Tung University, Taipei, Taiwan
  • 2School of Medicine, National Yang Ming Chiao Tung University, Taipei, Taiwan
  • 3Department of Neurosurgery, Neurological Institute, Taipei Veterans General Hospital, Taipei, Taiwan
  • 4Department of Neurological Surgery, University of Washington, Seattle, WA, United States
  • 5Graduate Program in Neuroscience, Center for Neurotechnology, University of Washington, Seattle, WA, United States
  • 6Department of Bioengineering, Center for Neurotechnology, University of Washington, Seattle, WA, United States
  • 7Department of Neurological Surgery, Oregon Health & Science University, Portland, OR, United States
  • 8Department of Psychology, Institute for Learning and Brain Sciences, University of Washington, Seattle, WA, United States
  • 9Department of Mechanical Engineering, National Cheng Kung University, Tainan, Taiwan
  • 10The Ph.D. Program for Neural Regenerative Medicine, College of Medical Science and Technology, Taipei Medical University, Taipei, Taiwan
  • 11Departments of Neurology and Pediatrics, University of Washington, Seattle, WA, United States
  • 12Center for Integrative Brain Research, Seattle Children's Research Institute, Seattle, WA, United States
  • 13Department of Radiology, Integrated Brain Imaging Center, University of Washington, Seattle, WA, United States
  • 14Center for Neurotechnology, University of Washington, Seattle, WA, United States
  • 15Departments of Surgery, Seattle Children's Hospital, Seattle, WA, United States

Response inhibition refers to the ability to suppress inappropriate actions that interfere with goal-driven behavior. The inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) is known to be associated with inhibition of a motor response by assuming executive control over motor cortex outputs. This study aimed to evaluate the pediatric development of response inhibition through subdural electrocorticography (ECoG) recording. Subdural ECoG recorded neural activities simultaneously during a Go/No-Go task, which was optimized for children. Different frequency power [theta: 4–8 Hz; beta: 12–40 Hz; high-gamma (HG): 70–200 Hz] was estimated within the IFG and motor cortex. Age-related analysis was computed by each bandpass power ratio between Go and No-Go conditions, and phase-amplitude coupling (PAC) over IFG by using the modulating index metric in two conditions. For all the eight pediatric patients, HG power was more activated in No-Go trials than in Go trials, in either right- or left-side IFG when available. In the IFG region, the power over theta and HG in No-Go conditions was higher than those in Go conditions, with significance over the right side (p < 0.05). The age-related lateralization from both sides to the right side was observed from the ratio of HG power and PAC value between the No-Go and Go trials. In the pediatric population, the role of motor inhibition was observed in both IFG, with age-related lateralization to the right side, which was proved in the previous functional magnetic resonance imaging studies. In this study, the evidence correlation of age and response inhibition was observed directly by the evidence of cortical recordings.

Introduction

Motor inhibition refers to the ability to suppress inappropriate or prepotent actions that interfere with goal-driven behavior. Go/No-Go tasks are designed to provide experimental epochs of movement preparation, response execution, and motor inhibition. Consequently, they are widely used to investigate neural responses specifically attributable to motor inhibition (1). Typically, during the task, participants are requested to press a button or otherwise respond to one type of stimuli, such as a set of alphabet letters, a colored dot, or an image (Go stimuli), and withhold or inhibit a response to another type of stimuli, such as a specific single letter, a different color dot, or a contrasting image (No-Go stimuli). Inhibition is a conversion process of motor behavior, reflecting the capacity to selectively withhold voluntary movements (2).

A large accumulation of neuroscientific evidence from functional (fMRI) studies has reported an increase in the concentration of oxygenated hemoglobin during successful No-Go inhibition in the predominantly right-lateralized brain network comprising the orbitofrontal cortex, dorsolateral pre-frontal cortex, supplementary motor areas (SMA)/pre-SMA areas, basal ganglia circuits, and inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) (36). The results indicate that motor inhibition is a largely lateralized process within (generally) the right hemisphere, with the right IFG believed to be particularly sensitive to response suppression (7). Within this inhibition network, the right IFG is predicted to serve as an execution center when inhibition is required (810).

Following a literature review, a power spectrum analysis based on adults' electroencephalogram (EEG) studies demonstrated that the theta frequency band (4–8 Hz) plays an important role during motor inhibitory control (1113). Considering the demand for motor inhibition, theta band activity revealed a correlation with right IFG during inhibition tasks (14). The power change between the Go and No-Go conditions were also observed in electrocorticography (ECoG) studies. A previous ECoG study of 16 patients with intracranial electrodes recording for medically refractory epilepsy found significantly increased gamma band activity in the right IFG after No-Go signal cueing (15). Motor inhibition-evoked gamma-band responses during No-Go trials localize to the right IFG (15), which is compatible with the activation of blood oxygenation level-dependent (BOLD) response in functional MRI (fMRI) studies (3, 8). The aforementioned findings, based on EEG and ECoG studies, show that the right IFG has a role in the motor inhibition of adults.

Age-related functional and anatomical neural development has been observed (16). In a meta-study including 2–12-year-old children from 65 EEG studies, the No-Go-related negative amplitude became progressively negative by age, compared with the Go conditions. The results implied that No-Go-related EEG signals as indexing motor inhibition would change with age (17). In the pediatric population, the findings of frontal activation in No-Go conditions were similar to those in adult populations (2, 18, 19), with some notable differences, including an overall greater engagement of more widespread brain networks (19) and left frontal engagement, which was predicted to contribute to motor inhibition in the immature nervous system (20). Yet, to date, the vast majority of electrophysiological investigations of response in inhibition have been based on adult populations; the dominance of right IFG, the contributions of left IFG during motor inhibition, and the connectivity of frontal and other parts of brain regions between Go and No-Go conditions in children have not been explored using ECoG. Because subdural recordings provide the greatest fidelity of gamma-band dynamics, confirmatory recordings using ECoG in pediatric populations during response-inhibition are warranted.

The processing of motor inhibition included not only IFG, but also other parts of the brain, including the pre-SMA, orbitofrontal cortex, dorsolateral pre-frontal cortex, and basal ganglia (36). Considering the neural network of motor inhibition, power changes across different parts of the cortex between Go and No-Go conditions were proven to be related to cortical connectivity and response accuracy in tasks (11, 21). Thus, taken all together, phase-amplitude coupling (PAC) is a suitable cross-frequency coupling approach used to evaluate cortical coupling and functional connectivity. For example, in a magnetoencephalography (MEG) study, high-gamma power (HG, 30–70 Hz) was phase-locked to alpha neural oscillation (8–13 Hz) with the human eyes closed within occipital channels (22). Furthermore, in ECoG studies, the phase of canonical low-frequency bands, such as theta (4–8 Hz), has been shown to modulate power in HG band (80–150 Hz) signals (23). The task-related coupling effects were also observed between the phase of low-frequency (0–3.5 Hz) and amplitude of gamma band (28–70 Hz) neural signals (24). Comparing the coupling effect between the ECoG and fMRI signals in the resting state of brain connectivity, PAC mimicked comparable patterns in these two measurements (25). Collectively, it is believed that PAC represents a neural gating mechanism where the presence of significant coupling indicates the phase of a low-frequency oscillator, which serves to briefly facilitate the high-frequency activity of a second, distant cortical region (23, 26).

It has been predicted that PAC may serve as a neurophysiological mechanism underlying the maturation of neural communication (26) and as a driving mechanism for orchestrating function, including motor inhibition (27). However, the degree to which this neural network and physiological mechanism of motor inhibition exist within pediatric populations is unclear. The aim of this study is to investigate the neural activity, extracted from in-dwelling ECoG electrodes, during motor inhibition in the pediatric population, and estimate the degree to which PAC is associated with the maturation of motor inhibition.

Materials and Methods

Participants

A total of eight pediatric patients (male:female = 3:5; mean age 9.8 years; range 7–16) underwent neurological surgery at the Seattle Children's Hospital in Seattle, Washington, for the treatment of intractable epilepsy without evidence of anatomical abnormality from the MRI examinations. Corticographic potentials were acquired from four patients with right hemisphere grids, and four patients with left-sided grids, according to the clinical considerations. Five patients completed Go/No-Go testing for both the hands and the other three patients only performed right-hand testing as shown in Table 1. With approval from the Seattle Children's Hospital Institutional Review Board, all patients and guardians provided informed consent, including the use of ECoG recordings and medical records. All patients underwent a two-stage surgery: craniotomy with unilateral subdual grid and strip implantation according to seizure semiology, followed by removal of the electrodes with resection of epileptic foci. Subdural ECoG 8 × 8 grids or 2 × 8 strips (Integra, Princeton, New Jersey, USA) with 4.75-mm diameter platinum electrodes spaced at 10 mm were transiently placed subdurally to localize the epileptic foci according to clinical considerations, and removed after 1 week of ECoG monitoring.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Demographical and clinical characteristics of pediatric patients implanted ECoG grids.

Go/No-Go Task and Signal Recording

The Go/No-Go task was developed in Psychotoolbox-3 with MATLAB software (Mathworks, Natick, Massachusetts, USA) and was optimized for children (2830). During the task, patients were asked to press a button on the appearance of a Go signal (a lion), and not respond on the appearance of a No-Go signal (a bear). The No-Go vs. Go signals were randomly distributed at the ratio of 1:6 with a total of 49 trials in each experimental run. There was a 1 s jittered intertrial interval as shown in Figure 1. Patients were asked to use the right hand during the first run of the task, and the left hand for the 2nd round. However, not all the patients complied. The responses were classified as Go-correct (lion, reaction), Go-wrong (lion, no reaction), No-Go-correct (bear, no reaction), and No-Go-wrong (bear, reaction).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Illustration of the optimized Go/No-Go paradigm (Go vs. No-Go = 1:6) for children. The patients were asked to respond in Go trials (lion) and hold in No-Go trials (bear).

The ECoG signals were recorded at 1.2 kHz by the clinical system with the Xltek® or Natus® Quantum® LTM amplifier (Natus, Pleasanton, California, USA). A portable laptop was utilized for task execution and documenting patients' responses, which were recorded simultaneously with ECoG signals. A digital transistor-transistor logic (TTL) output signal generated through Psychotoolbox-3 time-stamped event boundaries on the ECoG time series data.

Electrocorticography Data Analysis

All the artifact-free ECoG signals were analyzed with MATLAB software (R12, MathWorks, Natick, Massachusetts, USA). The flowchart of ECoG preprocessing was shown in Figure 2. Data were rereferenced within the grid by common average and notch filtered for line noise (at 60, 120, and 180 Hz). A spectral density time series was computed for each channel by (HG, 70–200 Hz) bandpass filtering (4th-order zero-phase Butterworth filter) and the absolute values of the Hilbert transform of the filtered signals were estimated. The bandpass filtered time series were binned into response categories and their corresponding HG powers for each trial type within each epoch were calculated from 1 s before to 1 s after the visual cue. The time-series data was then z-normalized to the first second of (i.e., baseline, precue period of the trial) signal.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Illustration of the flow chart of data analysis. After preprocessing the ECoG signal, high-gamma (HG) activation was applied to define the most activated electrode to represent inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) (Ci) and motor cortex (Cm). Broadband pass power spectrum was analyzed between Go and No-Go conditions. Finally, side-related bandpass power changed, as well as age-related bandpass power and phase-amplitude coupling (PAC) between Go and No-Go conditions were further evaluated.

The coordinates of each electrode were recorded by the intra-operative navigation system and transformed into patient space, which was defined by the preoperative high-resolution T1-weighted MRI volume. The data was further transformed to MNI standard space for the identification of motor and motor areas according to anatomical structure (31). Based on the patients' responses, the results would be Go/Correct, Go/Non-correct, No-Go/Correct, and No-Go/Non-correct. As a screening and selection measure for electrodes over IFG and motor cortex, the maximum value of each trial within an epoch was averaged in Go/Correct and No-Go/Correct conditions. For visualization purposes only, Z-score values in the electrodes over the IFG and motor cortex were plotted on the MNI brain and spatially normalized by a Gaussian distribution. Heat (i.e., color) maps were created to reveal HG brain activity in Go/Correct and No-Go/Correct conditions. All the electrodes within the IFG and motor cortex were ranked according to epoch-based normalized-HG activity. For each patient, the most activated electrodes within Go/Correct and No-Go/Correct conditions were identified as motor cortex cortical signal (Cm) and IFG cortical signal (Ci), respectively, and selected for statistical and cross-frequency coupling analysis.

Broadband power spectra of only Cm and Ci were further calculated. Raw time series data from Cm and Ci were bandpass filtered (2–200 Hz, 4th-order zero-phase Butterworth filter); the Morlet wavelet transform was calculated on the bandpass filtered series (3234) and truncated to each epoch of Go and No-Go conditions from 1 s before to 1 s after the visual cue. For each epoch, the calculations of phase and amplitude of the measured ECoG were determined through the Morlet wavelet transform. Amplitude estimates were again Z-normalized by the first second of power of each 3 Hz frequency step and averaged across all epochs of Go/Correct and No-Go/Correct conditions. The power change across low-to-high frequencies, from rest (1 s before the visual cue) to reaction (1 s after the visual cue), was estimated in Go/Correct and No-Go/Correct conditions.

Single-band power in right and left-side IFG was analyzed. Pre-processed raw time-series form Ci was truncated from 1 s before to 1 s after the visual cue and calculated by the Short-time Fourier transform in each 5 ms window. Power in different frequencies (theta: 4–8 Hz; beta: 12–40 Hz; HG: 70–200 Hz) was filtered and Z-normalized by the same frequency power at rest (1 sec before the visual cue). The peak value in each 5 ms window after the visual cue was chosen. Cross-patient analysis in different frequency bands was compared between right and left IFG.

Age-related analysis was computed using the Ci signal in two parts: the power ratio in different frequency bands between Go/Correct and No-Go/Correct, and PAC. The peak values in each 5 ms window after the visual cue were chosen from the normalized power filtered in different frequency bands (theta: 4–8 Hz; beta: 12–40 Hz; HG: 70–200 Hz). The power ratio of each band between No-Go/Correct and Go/Correct was calculated in each patient by the following equation Equation (1):

power ratio = No - Go/CorrectaveGo/Correctave    (1)

where No-Go/Correctave and Go/Correctave denote the average power of No-Go/Correct trials and Go/Correct in the 1 s reaction period after the visual cue.

Moreover, PAC was computed by modulation index to determine if there was any change over IFG during Go and No-Go epochs. (23) The PAC value, z(t), was is given by Equation (2).

z(t) = AHigh(t)eiϕLow(t)    (2)

whereAHigh(t) is the normalized high frequency (40–200 Hz) envelope amplitude in the time series; and Low(t) is the low-frequency (2–20 Hz; stepped every 3 Hz) phase in the time series. The z(t) in 1 s period after visual was then averaged to calculate the ratio between No-Go/Correct and Go/Correct in each different-age patient.

Statistical Analysis

The peak values in each bandpass power between Go and No-Go conditions were illustrated by mean ± 95% CI and p-values were calculated by unpaired t-test (p = 0.05). The Tukey–Kramer test was used to correct multiple comparisons for the unequal sizes between the Go and No-Go conditions. To test for the statistical significance of PAC between motor and IFG areas, a standard permutation test was applied by conducting 1,000 shuffles of each bin of low-frequency phase and high-frequency amplitude. This method generated a null distribution of modulating index and the 95th percentile CIs. Any real-modulating index values calculated between each bin of frequency and amplitude from Ci revealing greater than null distribution at an alpha level of 0.05 were considered statistically significant. The significance of PAC values of each Go/Correct and No-Go/Correct condition was calculated by a permutation test (significance value = 0.05) (25), and non-significant effects were zero-valued. The results were depicted as the ratio of the average between Go/Correct and No-Go/Correct conditions.

Results

Activation of High-Gamma Band Filtered Signal

High gamma-filtered ECoG power localizes movements from different body parts (35) and fine finger movements (36) across the primary motor cortex. Based on the previous findings, HG power was used to evaluate brain responses over the primary motor cortex and IFG in Go and No-Go conditions (15). For the patients with right-sided grid coverage, activated HG signals over the motor cortex were noted when the contralateral left hand was used in Go conditions (with the exception of one 12-year-old patient with no motor cortex coverage), but no activation when the ipsilateral right hand was used. Independent of which hand was used, right IFG in the No-Go condition exhibited greater HG activation relative to the Go condition (Figure 3). Similar high-frequency response profiles in the left IFG were also observed in the patients with left hemisphere grids. HG power over the motor cortex was activated when the contralateral hand was used, as shown in Figure 4.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Illustration of the mean high-gamma (HG) activation for the patients aged 7, 11, 12, and 15 years with right side grid coverage. Activation of the right IFG in No-Go/Correct trials is greater than that in Go/Correct trials. HG activation over the sensorimotor cortex was observed in Go/Correct trials when the contralateral hand was used (in this case the left hand).

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Illustration of the mean high-gamma (HG) activation for the patients aged 8, 9, 11, and 16 years with left-side grid coverage. Activation of the left IFG in No-Go/Correct trials is higher than that in Go/Correct trials. HG activation over the sensorimotor cortex was observed in Go/Correct trials when the contralateral hand was used (in this case the right hand).

Power Spectrum Over Motor Cortex and IFG

The broadband cortical spectrum (2–200 Hz) was calculated via the wavelet transform from the representative electrodes of Cm and Ci. For the cortical signals from the right Cm, increased high-frequency power (70–200 Hz) and decreased low-frequency power (20–40 Hz) were noted after the visual cue, while the contralateral hand was used in Go/Correct conditions. This power change over the motor cortex was consistent with that of a previously published report (37). For the signal over the right Ci, the activated power over HG (70–150 Hz), beta (12–40 Hz), and theta (4–8 Hz) were observed when either the right or left hand was used in No-Go/Correct conditions as shown in Figure 5. Moreover, the same range of power activation in No-Go/Correct conditions was also noted over the electrode of the left Ci, left IFG region (Figure 6). In the cross-patient power analysis, the mean values of HG, beta, and theta power in the No-Go/Correct condition over Ci were higher than those in the Go/Correct conditions over both sides of the IFG, but statistical significance was only noted in the HG and theta power over the right IFG (p < 0.05, with corrected by Tukey-Kramer test) as shown in Figure 7.

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. The broadband spectral changes revealed the power change over the motor cortex (the blue dot) and the IFG (the right dot) from 1-second before to 1-second after the visual cues (Go or No-Go) by the 11-year-old with a right-side grid implanted. When the left hand was used, the power within the high-gamma band (HG, 70–200 Hz) increased with the decreased beta band (12–40 Hz) over the motor cortex after visual cues in the Go/Correct trials. Increased HG, beta, and theta (4–8 Hz) power was noted when both the right and left hands were used in No-Go/Correct trials.

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. The spectrum revealed the power change over the motor cortex (the blue dot) and the IFG (the right dot) from 1 s before to 1 s after the visual cues (Go or No-Go) by a 9-year-old boy with a left-side grid implanted. When the right hand was used, the power of the high-gamma band (HG, 70–200 Hz) increased with a decreased beta band (12–40 Hz) over the motor cortex after visual cues in Go/Correct trials. Increased HG and theta (4–8 Hz) power were noted when both the right and left hands were used in No-Go/Correct trials.

FIGURE 7
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 7. Illustration of the right and left IFG with cross-patient power analysis in Go/Correct and No-Go/Correct conditions. The normalized power in theta (4–8 Hz), beta (12–40 Hz), and high-gamma (HG, 80–200 Hz) were calculated by mean value, interquartile range (box plot), and maximal/minimal (error bar). In No-Go/Correct conditions, the power of theta, beta, and HG were higher than those in Go/Correct condition, with statistical significance in theta and HG power over the right IFG (marked by an asterisk, p < 0.05, corrected by the Tukey–Kramer Test).

Age-Related ECoG Power and PAC Analysis

In each patient, the theta, beta, and HG band power ratios over the right IFG between No-Go/Correct and Go/Correct conditions were calculated. The bandpass powers among different frequency in the No-Go/Correct conditions were higher than those in the Go/Correct conditions, which mean the ratios were over 1. For the patients with grid-covered right IFG, the ratio of theta, beta, and HG frequency bands showed obvious change by age. However, with grid-covered left IFG, there was a decreasing trend of power ratio in HG by ages, and also an increasing trend in theta, but no age-related pattern was observed in the power ratio of the beta frequency band as shown in Figure 8.

FIGURE 8
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 8. The power ratios between No-Go/Correct and Go/Correct in theta, beta, and HG were illustrated by mean ± 95% CI in each patient. For the patients with the right IFG coverage, there was no obvious change by age. For the left IFG, there was a decreasing trend in HG by age as well as an increasing trend in theta, but no obvious pattern was noted in the ratio of beta.

The PAC values over IFG (Ci) were computed separately for the Go/Correct and No-Go/Correct conditions. Most of the PAC values after the permutation test were non-significant (i.e., set to zero). For the right IFG, the PAC value in the No-Go/Correct conditions was higher than that in the Go/Correct conditions. Although a similar pattern was observed in the younger patients for left IFG, the ratio progressively decreased with age as shown in Figure 9.

FIGURE 9
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 9. The phase-amplitude coupling (PAC) values were calculated by modulating the index between low-frequency phase (2–40 Hz) and high frequency amplitude (70–200 Hz) in Go/Correct and No-Go/Correct conditions. The illustration revealed the calculated ratio between Go/Correct and No-Go/Correct conditions in right and left-side IFGs. For the right IFG, the PAC value in No-Go/Correct conditions is higher than that in Go/Correct conditions, which means the ratio is over 1, and consistent between the low-frequency phase (5–25 Hz) and high frequency amplitude (100–150 Hz). For left IFG, a similar pattern was observed in younger patients, but the ratio decreased with age.

Discussion

In this study, we investigated movement inhibition in the eight pediatric patients who received transient subdural grid implantation for localizing epileptic foci. Our observations highlight two things. First, HG activation in No-Go conditions was higher than that in the Go condition over both the left and right side IFGs. The PAC values in the No-Go condition, calculated between low-frequency (2–40 Hz) phase and high-frequency amplitude (70–200 Hz) from IFG, were higher than those in Go conditions, and we also observed in both left and right cortices. This indicates that activation of IFG in the No-Go condition may modulate the cortical activities to inhibit movements. Second, in the left IFG, the HG and PAC ratio decreased, while the theta ratio increased, with increasing age. In the previous literature, maturational patterns of EEG activity in the resting state revealed that theta-band power decreased while the alpha-band power increased with age (38). The theta-band activity was replaced by the alpha-band first in the occipital regions and progressed later to frontal regions by ages (39). Many studies suggested changes in the white matter (WM) volume was thought to reflect the process of increased myelination, whereas myelination increases the speed of nerve impulse propagation across the brain's region-specific neurocircuitry, especially in the prefrontal cortex, up until 24 years old (40). Thus, growth-related changes in WM might lead decreasing in resting-state theta-band activity with development. In this study, we used the baseline-normalized event-related spectral perturbation (ERSP) (41) to determine the activation of ECoG data under the No-Go trial. There was an increasing power ratio of theta oscillation with stronger theta activity in No-Go trials as lower resting-state theta activity (baseline) by age. The observed age-dependent relationship between theta activity and inhibitory control at the neurophysiological and behavioral level may relate to biophysical properties of theta oscillations and their role in coordinating information processing in a network in the maturation process (42, 43). Critically, relative to adult populations, our results support the hypothesis that the center of motor inhibition in the pediatric populations is not limited to the right IFG, but rather is a product of bilateral contributions from the IFG (20).

The process of motor inhibition is generally assumed to be a multiple step, functional process, mapping onto multiple regions of the cortex (7). In an fMRI study, 26 healthy volunteers (mean age: 23.4 years old) were asked to continuously tap a button with their right index finger, stopping the movement following occasional visual cues, in order to compare the brain activation between the voluntary and forced inhibition of ongoing actions. The results revealed that during the period of inhibition there were greater activations over the SMA, middle cingulate cortex, bilateral insula, and inferior parietal cortex in addition to the right IFG (44). In another fMRI study of 26 healthy subjects, BOLD signals were measured in a Go/No-Go task. Greater activation over the right IFG and pre-SMA regions were noted in No-Go conditions than in Go conditions (45). A 15-participant study (mean age = 27.5 years old) compared different degrees of difficulty of the stop signal task and found that the right IFG and adjacent anterior insula had more activation during more difficult tasks (46). Additional insight regarding the role of the left IFG in Go/No-Go task execution was demonstrated in a 22-participant fMRI study. The study included two variants of the Go/No-Go ratio: a high frequency of Go cues (Go: No-Go = 3: 1), and a high frequency of No-Go cues (Go: No-Go = 1: 3). The results revealed that the left IFG and a dorsal portion of the pre-SMA were more reactive to No-Go cues compared with Go cues, whether the frequency of No-Go cues was high or low (47). Together, these studies indicate that the right IFG participates in a network that orchestrates the process of motor inhibition. The findings of this study support the conclusion that in addition to the right IFG, which is well-established as being involved in motor inhibition in adults, the left IFG also plays a role in motor inhibition within the maturing brain (20).

Considering the whole neural network for movement inhibition, the change of neural activations over extended regions of the cortex, such as motor, premotor, and pre-SMA regions, would also be observed between Go and No-Go conditions. In a stop-event-related study of 12 adult epileptic patients, event-related spectral power was measured by intracranial ECoG to identity the movement-related spectral change over the sensorimotor cortex. The early increased mu band (10–20 Hz) reflected a transient state of motor inhibition over the precentral gyri (48). In another Go/No-Go task, the middle frontal gyrus (MFG) demonstrated transiently increased HG power during stop signals, and the increased HG over MFG was stronger for unsuccessful stop conditions compared to successful stop conditions, which implied the role of MFG in behavioral monitoring (49). The processing of motor inhibition not only included the frontal region, but also the pre-SMA, orbitofrontal cortex, dorsolateral pre-frontal cortex, and basal ganglia (36).

Modulating effects between different brain regions, including the IFG, anterior insula, pre-SMA, and sensorimotor cortex, with interaction with basal ganglia, were demonstrated in a neural network study of movement inhibition (47). The movement context was modulated by the neural activity of the basal ganglia, anterior cingulate cortex, and frontal cortex (50, 51). In an fMRI study, response-related amplitudes were calculated via logistic regression analysis. Covariance was applied to evaluate the coupling effects between two regions. The results revealed the coupling between the fronto-parietal regions and right IFG increased in successful stop signal tasks compared with that of unsuccessful stop signal tasks, suggesting that the right IFG had more neural interaction during movement inhibition (52). The role of the left IFG in movement inhibition was also evaluated by correlation in another fMRI study by a Go/No-Go task. In No-Go trials, the left IFG revealed positive connectivity with the dorsal portion of the pre-SMA, but negative connectivity with regions responding to Go cues (left sensorimotor cortex) (47). In a clinical case report, the fMRI data analysis in the Go/No-Go task revealed left IFG compensated the original right IFG function after brain injury, which may be the reactivation of the original left IFG function (53). These findings are congruent with the results of our study, which revealed that both the right and left IFG serve to modulate focal activity with the motor cortex during No-Go conditions.

Age-related neural development has been widely discussed, including changes in cortical thickness (16), cortical structure (54), the number of synapse formations (55), and functional activity (54). For example, visual acuity has been correlated to the structure and thickness of the visual cortex (16). Most neural development, such as the sensorimotor cortex, matures symmetrically over both sides (56). However, the language function lateralizes to the dominant cortex with age (57). Utilizing structural MRI, lateralization was strongly correlated with volume and thickness over the left IFG (58). For the function of motor inhibition, the presented studies show that right IFG plays an important role (2, 7, 8, 15), but some reports have revealed equal contribution by bilateral IFG (20). In an MRI study, decreasing WM tracts over right IFG was observed in the patients with Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder, compared with the normal population. For the motor inhibition, the results identified the pathogenesis of WM tracts over IFG potentially related to deficient inhibitory control (59). Our study indicated that both IFG played a role in motor inhibition in the pediatric population, but with age, lateralization to the right IFG becomes dominant.

There were some limitations in this study. The brain activity recorded by the ECoG signal was limited by the location of grids and strips, and coverage of the regions of interest was determined entirely by the clinical need. In the future, more cases are needed to investigate the correlation between age and motor inhibition, which may reveal the developmental pattern from bilateral IFG involvement in children to solely right IFG involvement in adults.

Conclusion

In our pediatric patients, both the right and left IFG had roles in motor inhibition. The power ratio between the No-Go and Go conditions revealed age-related lateralization from the bilateral-to-right side IFG. The PAC modulation over IFG was more synchronized in No-Go trials than in Go trials. The correlation of age computed by the PAC ratio between two conditions further supported age-related right side IFG lateralization.

Data Availability Statement

The datasets generated for this study are available on request to the corresponding author.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Seattle Children's Hospital Institutional Review Board. Written informed consent to participate in this study was provided by the participants' legal guardian/next of kin.

Author Contributions

C-HK, KCa, JW, PR, KW, and JO designed the study. C-HK, KCa, JW, KCo, and JO participated in data collection. EN and JO provided the data resource. C-HK, KCa, B-WC, S-HY, Y-CL, and Y-YC analyzed the data. C-HK, KCa, KCo, Y-YC, Y-CL, KW, and JO wrote and reviewed the manuscript. All the authors reviewed and approved the final version of the manuscript.

Funding

We are grateful for support from the Ministry of Science and Technology of Taiwan under Contract numbers of MOST 110-2321-B-010-006, 109-2811-E-010−502, 109-2221-E-010-004-MY2, and 108-2321-B-010-008-MY2. We also are grateful for support from the Higher Education Sprout Project of the National Chiao Tung University and Headquarters of University Advancement at the National Cheng Kung University, Ministry of Education (MOE), Taiwan.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

1. Fujiyama H, Tandonnet C, Summers JJ. Age-related differences in corticospinal excitability during a Go/NoGo task. Psychophysiology. (2011) 48:1448–55. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2011.01201.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Smith JL, Jamadar S, Provost AL, Michie PT. Motor and non-motor inhibition in the Go/NoGo task: an ERP and fMRI study. Int J Psychophysiol. (2013) 87:244–53. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2012.07.185

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Steele VR, Aharoni E, Munro GE, Calhoun VD, Nyalakanti P, Stevens MC, et al. A large scale (N=102) functional neuroimaging study of response inhibition in a Go/NoGo task. Behav Brain Res. (2013) 256:529–36. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2013.06.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Hampshire A, Chamberlain SR, Monti MM, Duncan J, Owen AM. The role of the right inferior frontal gyrus: inhibition and attentional control. Neuroimage. (2010) 50:1313–9. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.12.109

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Hart H, Radua J, Nakao T, Mataix-Cols D, Rubia K. Meta-analysis of functional magnetic resonance imaging studies of inhibition and attention in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: exploring task-specific, stimulant medication, and age effects. JAMA Psychiatry. (2013) 70:185–98. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2013.277

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Simmonds DJ, Pekar JJ, Mostofsky SH. Meta-analysis of Go/No-go tasks demonstrating that fMRI activation associated with response inhibition is task-dependent. Neuropsychologia. (2008) 46:224–32. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.07.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Aron AR, Robbins TW, Poldrack RA. Inhibition and the right inferior frontal cortex: one decade on. Trends Cogn Sci. (2014) 18:177–85. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.12.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Aron AR, Fletcher PC, Bullmore ET, Sahakian BJ, Robbins TW. Stop-signal inhibition disrupted by damage to right inferior frontal gyrus in humans. Nat Neurosci. (2003) 6:115–6. doi: 10.1038/nn1003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Ridderinkhof KR, Ullsperger M, Crone EA, Nieuwenhuis S. The role of the medial frontal cortex in cognitive control. Science. (2004) 306:443–7. doi: 10.1126/science.1100301

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Rubia K, Russell T, Overmeyer S, Brammer MJ, Bullmore ET, Sharma T, et al. Mapping motor inhibition: conjunctive brain activations across different versions of go/no-go and stop tasks. Neuroimage. (2001) 13:250–61. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2000.0685

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Brier MR, Ferree TC, Maguire MJ, Moore P, Spence J, Tillman GD, et al. Frontal theta and alpha power and coherence changes are modulated by semantic complexity in Go/NoGo tasks. Int J Psychophysiol. (2010) 78:215–24. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2010.07.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Funderud I, Lindgren M, Lovstad M, Endestad T, Voytek B, Knight RT, et al. Differential Go/NoGo activity in both contingent negative variation and spectral power. PLoS ONE. (2012) 7:e48504. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0048504

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Harmony T, Alba A, Marroquin JL, Gonzalez-Frankenberger B. Time-frequency-topographic analysis of induced power and synchrony of EEG signals during a Go/No-Go task. Int J Psychophysiol. (2009) 71:9–16. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2008.07.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Adelhofer N, Beste C. Pre-trial theta band activity in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex correlates with inhibition-related theta band activity in the right inferior frontal cortex. Neuroimage. (2020) 219:117052. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.117052

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Bartoli E, Aron AR, Tandon N. Topography and timing of activity in right inferior frontal cortex and anterior insula for stopping movement. Hum Brain Mapp. (2018) 39:189–203. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23835

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Shaw P, Kabani NJ, Lerch JP, Eckstrand K, Lenroot R, Gogtay N, et al. Neurodevelopmental trajectories of the human cerebral cortex. J Neurosci. (2008) 28:3586–94. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5309-07.2008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Hoyniak C. Changes in the NoGo N2 event-related potential component across childhood: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Dev Neuropsychol. (2017) 42:1–24. doi: 10.1080/87565641.2016.1247162

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Albares M, Lio G, Criaud M, Anton JL, Desmurget M, Boulinguez P. The dorsal medial frontal cortex mediates automatic motor inhibition in uncertain contexts: evidence from combined fMRI and EEG studies. Hum Brain Mapp. (2014) 35:5517–31. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22567

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Jonkman LM, Sniedt FL, Kemner C. Source localization of the Nogo-N2: a developmental study. Clin Neurophysiol. (2007) 118:1069–77. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2007.01.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Abdul Rahman A, Carroll DJ, Espy KA, Wiebe SA. Neural correlates of response inhibition in early childhood: evidence from a Go/No-Go task. Dev Neuropsychol. (2017) 42:336–50. doi: 10.1080/87565641.2017.1355917

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Liu L, Rosjat N, Popovych S, Wang BA, Yeldesbay A, Toth TI, et al. Age-related changes in oscillatory power affect motor action. PLoS ONE. (2017) 12:e0187911. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0187911

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Osipova D, Hermes D, Jensen O. Gamma power is phase-locked to posterior alpha activity. PLoS ONE. (2008) 3:e3990. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0003990

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Canolty RT, Edwards E, Dalal SS, Soltani M, Nagarajan SS, Kirsch HE, et al. High gamma power is phase-locked to theta oscillations in human neocortex. Science. (2006) 313:1626–8. doi: 10.1126/science.1128115

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Bruns A, Eckhorn R. Task-related coupling from high- to low-frequency signals among visual cortical areas in human subdural recordings. Int J Psychophysiol. (2004) 51:97–116. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2003.07.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Weaver KE, Wander JD, Ko AL, Casimo K, Grabowski TJ, Ojemann JG, et al. Directional patterns of cross frequency phase and amplitude coupling within the resting state mimic patterns of fMRI functional connectivity. Neuroimage. (2016) 128:238–51. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.12.043

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Voytek B, Knight RT. Dynamic network communication as a unifying neural basis for cognition, development, aging, and disease. Biol Psychiatry. (2015) 77:1089–97. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.04.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Durschmid S, Quandt F, Kramer UM, Hinrichs H, Heinze HJ, Schulz R, et al. Oscillatory dynamics track motor performance improvement in human cortex. PLoS ONE. (2014) 9:e89576. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0089576

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Brainard DH. The psychophysics toolbox. Spat Vis. (1997) 10:433–6. doi: 10.1163/156856897X00357

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Kleiner M, Brainard D, Pelli D, Ingling A, Murray R, Broussard C. What's new in psychtoolbox-3. Perception. (2007) 36:1–16. doi: 10.1068/v070821

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Pelli DG. The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into movies. Spat Vis. (1997) 10:437–42. doi: 10.1163/156856897X00366

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Poliachik SL, Poliakov AV, Jansen LA, McDaniel SS, Wray CD, Kuratani J, et al. Tissue localization during resective epilepsy surgery. Neurosurg Focus. (2013) 34:E8. doi: 10.3171/2013.3.FOCUS1360

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Jmail N, Zaghdoud M, Hadriche A, Frikha T, Ben Amar C, Bénar C. Integration of stationary wavelet transform on a dynamic partial reconfiguration for recognition of pre-ictal gamma oscillations. Heliyon. (2018) 4:e00530. doi: 10.1016/j.heliyon.2018.e00530

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Wang J, Wang J, Xing GG, Li X, Wan Y. Enhanced gamma oscillatory activity in rats with chronic inflammatory pain. Front Neurosci. (2016) 10:489. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2016.00489

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Zhou Y, Sheremet A, Qin Y, Kennedy JP, DiCola NM, Burke SN, et al. Methodological considerations on the use of different spectral decomposition algorithms to study hippocampal rhythms. eNeuro. (2019) 6:ENEURO.0142-19.2019. doi: 10.1523/ENEURO.0142-19.2019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Blakely TM, Olson JD, Miller KJ, Rao RP, Ojemann JG. Neural correlates of learning in an electrocorticographic motor-imagery brain-computer interface. Brain Comput Interfaces. (2014) 1:147–57. doi: 10.1080/2326263X.2014.954183

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Kuo CH, Blakely TM, Wander JD, Sarma D, Wu J, Casimo K, et al. Context-dependent relationship in high-resolution micro-ECoG studies during finger movements. J Neurosurg. (2019) 132:1358–66. doi: 10.3171/2019.1.JNS181840

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Miller KJ, Leuthardt EC, Schalk G, Rao RP, Anderson NR, Moran DW, et al. Spectral changes in cortical surface potentials during motor movement. J Neurosci. (2007) 27:2424–32. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3886-06.2007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Cellier D, Riddle J, Petersen I, Hwang K. The development of theta and alpha neural oscillations from ages 3 to 24 years. Dev Cogn Neurosci. (2021) 50:100969. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2021.100969

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Segalowitz SJ, Santesso DL, Jetha MK. Electrophysiological changes during adolescence: a review. Brain Cogn. (2010) 72:86–100. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2009.10.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Arain M, Haque M, Johal L, Mathur P, Nel W, Rais A, et al. Maturation of the adolescent brain. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat. (2013) 9:449–61. doi: 10.2147/NDT.S39776

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Grandchamp R, Delorme A. Single-trial normalization for event-related spectral decomposition reduces sensitivity to noisy trials. Front Psychol. (2011) 2:236. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00236

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Pscherer C, Bluschke A, Muckschel M, Beste C. The interplay of resting and inhibitory control-related theta-band activity depends on age. Hum Brain Mapp. (2021) 42:3845–57. doi: 10.1002/hbm.25469

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Pscherer C, Muckschel M, Summerer L, Bluschke A, Beste C. On the relevance of EEG resting theta activity for the neurophysiological dynamics underlying motor inhibitory control. Hum Brain Mapp. (2019) 40:4253–65. doi: 10.1002/hbm.24699

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Omata K, Ito S, Takata Y, Ouchi Y. Similar neural correlates of planning and execution to inhibit continuing actions. Front Neurosci. (2018) 12:951. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2018.00951

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Gavazzi G, Orsolini S, Rossi A, Bianchi A, Bartolini E, Nicolai E, et al. Alexithymic trait is associated with right IFG and pre-SMA activation in non-emotional response inhibition in healthy subjects. Neurosci Lett. (2017) 658:150–4. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2017.08.031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Hughes ME, Johnston PJ, Fulham WR, Budd TW, Michie PT. Stop-signal task difficulty and the right inferior frontal gyrus. Behav Brain Res. (2013) 256:205–13. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2013.08.026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Meffert H, Hwang S, Nolan ZT, Chen G, Blair JR. Segregating attention from response control when performing a motor inhibition task: segregating attention from response control. Neuroimage. (2016) 126:27–38. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.11.029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Klopp J, Marinkovic K, Clarke J, Chauvel P, Nenov V, Halgren E. Timing and localization of movement-related spectral changes in the human peri-Rolandic cortex: intracranial recordings. Neuroimage. (2001) 14:391–405. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0828

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Fonken YM, Rieger JW, Tzvi E, Crone NE, Chang E, Parvizi J, et al. Frontal and motor cortex contributions to response inhibition: evidence from electrocorticography. J Neurophysiol. (2016) 115:2224–36. doi: 10.1152/jn.00708.2015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Le TM, Zhang S, Zhornitsky S, Wang W, Li CR. Neural correlates of reward-directed action and inhibition of action. Cortex. (2020) 123:42–56. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2019.10.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Marmor O, Rappel P, Valsky D, Bick AS, Arkadir D, Linetsky E, et al. Movement context modulates neuronal activity in motor and limbic-associative domains of the human parkinsonian subthalamic nucleus. Neurobiol Dis. (2020) 136:104716. doi: 10.1016/j.nbd.2019.104716

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Padmala S, Pessoa L. Moment-to-moment fluctuations in fMRI amplitude and interregion coupling are predictive of inhibitory performance. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. (2010) 10:279–97. doi: 10.3758/CABN.10.2.279

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Gavazzi G, Lenge M, Bartolini E, Bianchi A, Agovi H, Mugnai F, et al. Left inferior frontal cortex can compensate the inhibitory functions of right inferior frontal cortex and pre-supplementary motor area. J Neuropsychol. (2019) 13:503–8. doi: 10.1111/jnp.12170

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Durston S, Casey BJ. What have we learned about cognitive development from neuroimaging? Neuropsychologia. (2006) 44:2149–57. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.10.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Huttenlocher PR, de Courten C. The development of synapses in striate cortex of man. Hum Neurobiol. (1987) 6:1–9.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

56. Karmali F, Whitman GT, Lewis RF. Bayesian optimal adaptation explains age-related human sensorimotor changes. J Neurophysiol. (2018) 119:509–20. doi: 10.1152/jn.00710.2017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Travis KE, Curran MM, Torres C, Leonard MK, Brown TT, Dale AM, et al. Age-related changes in tissue signal properties within cortical areas important for word understanding in 12- to 19-month-old infants. Cereb Cortex. (2014) 24:1948–55. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bht052

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Scheppele M, Evans JL, Brown TT. Patterns of structural lateralization in cortical language areas of older adolescents. Laterality. (2019) 24:450–81. doi: 10.1080/1357650X.2018.1543312

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Tremblay LK, Hammill C, Ameis SH, Bhaijiwala M, Mabbott DJ, Anagnostou E, et al. Tracking inhibitory control in youth with ADHD: a multi-modal neuroimaging approach. Front Psychiatry. (2020) 11:831. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2020.00831

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: electrocorticography, Go/No-Go, high-gamma, inferior frontal gyrus, lateralization, motor inhibition

Citation: Kuo C-H, Casimo K, Wu J, Collins K, Rice P, Chen B-W, Yang S-H, Lo Y-C, Novotny EJ, Weaver KE, Chen Y-Y and Ojemann JG (2022) Electrocorticography to Investigate Age-Related Brain Lateralization on Pediatric Motor Inhibition. Front. Neurol. 13:747053. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2022.747053

Received: 25 July 2021; Accepted: 24 January 2022;
Published: 07 March 2022.

Edited by:

Jean-Pierre Sao-Ming Lin, Guy's and St Thomas' NHS Foundation Trust, United Kingdom

Reviewed by:

Maurizio Elia, IRCCS Oasi Maria SS, Italy
Eishi Asano, Children's Hospital of Michigan, United States
Shih Ming Weng, National Taipei University of Nursing and Health Sciences, Taiwan

Copyright © 2022 Kuo, Casimo, Wu, Collins, Rice, Chen, Yang, Lo, Novotny, Weaver, Chen and Ojemann. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: You-Yin Chen, irradiance@so-net.net.tw; Chao-Hung Kuo, chaohungk@gmail.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.