Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Neurol., 07 September 2021
Sec. Epilepsy

Early Intervention via Stimulation of the Medial Septal Nucleus Improves Cognition and Alters Markers of Epileptogenesis in Pilocarpine-Induced Epilepsy

  • 1Department of Neurological Surgery, University of California, Davis, Sacramento, CA, United States
  • 2Center for Neuroscience, University of California, Davis, Davis, CA, United States
  • 3Department of Psychology, University of Arizona, Tucson, AZ, United States
  • 4McKnight Brain Institute, University of Arizona, Tucson, AZ, United States

Over one-third of patients with temporal lobe epilepsy are refractory to medication. In addition, anti-epileptic drugs often exacerbate cognitive comorbidities. Neuromodulation is an FDA treatment for refractory epilepsy, but patients often wait >20 years for a surgical referral for resection or neuromodulation. Using a rodent model, we test the hypothesis that 2 weeks of theta stimulation of the medial septum acutely following exposure to pilocarpine will alter the course of epileptogenesis resulting in persistent behavioral improvements. Electrodes were implanted in the medial septum, dorsal and ventral hippocampus, and the pre-frontal cortex of pilocarpine-treated rats. Rats received 30 min/day of 7.7 Hz or theta burst frequency on days 4–16 post-pilocarpine, prior to the development of spontaneous seizures. Seizure threshold, spikes, and oscillatory activity, as well as spatial and object-based learning, were assessed in the weeks following stimulation. Non-stimulated pilocarpine animals exhibited significantly decreased seizure threshold, increased spikes, and cognitive impairments as compared to vehicle controls. Furthermore, decreased ventral hippocampal power (6–10 Hz) correlated with both the development of spikes and impaired cognition. Measures of spikes, seizure threshold, and cognitive performance in both acute 7.7 Hz and theta burst stimulated animals were statistically similar to vehicle controls when tested during the chronic phase of epilepsy, weeks after stimulation was terminated. These data indicate that modulation of the septohippocampal circuit early after pilocarpine treatment alters the progression of epileptic activity, resulting in elevated seizure thresholds, fewer spikes, and improved cognitive outcome. Results from this study support that septal theta stimulation has the potential to serve in combination or as an alternative to high frequency thalamic stimulation in refractory cases and that further research into early intervention is critical.

Introduction

Of the ~3.4 million adults and children in the U.S. diagnosed with epilepsy (1), more than half also experience comorbidities such as cognitive decline (2). Temporal lobe epilepsy (TLE) is a frequently diagnosed form of focal epilepsy and also difficult to treat pharmacologically, as antiepileptic drugs fail to control seizures in nearly 40% of cases (35). Even when successful, anti-seizure medications are not designed to treat cognitive comorbidities, and in many patients, they induce adverse side effects (6). Surgical resection of the seizure focus can be curative in ~70% of patients (7), however, it is often not performed for 20–25 years after refractory classification, reducing the probability of post-operative seizure freedom and increasing the risk of lasting seizure-related comorbidities, temporal lobe pathology, and injury/fatality (2, 811). For patients who do not meet surgical criteria (12), neuromodulation paradigms such as vagal nerve stimulation (VNS), deep brain stimulation (DBS) of the anterior nucleus of the thalamus (ANT), and responsive neurostimulation (RNS) represent reversible and effective alternatives (1315). While these stimulation paradigms result in progressive improvements in responder rates (1316) as compared to pharmacology alone, they do not significantly improve cognitive outcome (17).

Animal models of neural connectivity and TLE, predominantly in rodents, have been critical for the development and optimization of electrical neuromodulation. For example, both RNS (18) and DBS of the thalamus (19) were initially tested in animal models including pilocarpine-treated rats prior to clinical translation. In addition, direct stimulation of the hippocampus (20) and the centromedian thalamic nuclei (21) also reduced seizures in the absence of additional cognitive impairments. Critically, the majority of these studies focused on high frequency stimulation parameters to desynchronize neuronal output and reduce epileptiform activity. There are also a limited number of studies demonstrating that low frequency stimulation of the hippocampus (22) and amygdala (23) can reduce seizure frequency. While each of these pre-clinical and clinical findings demonstrate that feasibility of neuromodulation to reduce spontaneous seizures, there remains a critical need for the development of a novel therapeutic paradigm to treat not only seizures but also the significant cognitive impairments associated with TLE.

Low frequency theta oscillations are implicated in both cognitive processing (2426) and the modulation of seizures (27, 28). We previously demonstrated that 7.7 Hz theta frequency stimulation of the medial septal nucleus (MSN) both increases seizure threshold and improves cognitive function in chronically epileptic rats (29). We now hypothesize that engaging the septohippocampal circuit earlier, in the weeks immediately following pilocarpine injection, will alter the development of epilepsy markers, as measured by spikes and seizure threshold, while concurrently improving long-term cognitive outcome. If early theta stimulation of the septum demonstrates lasting effects, this study would indicate a need to further investigate the potential of closing the gap between diagnosis of refractory epilepsy (~2–3 years) and the consideration for reducing the time from diagnosis to surgical/electrical intervention which frequently takes over 20 years. Moreover, if early stimulation also improves learning, this would represent a single modulatory paradigm that could potentially address both epileptiform and cognitive phenotypes in TLE.

Materials and Methods

Experimental Design

All experiments involving animals complied with the ARRIVE guidelines and were performed in accordance with the National Institutes of Health guide for the care and use of laboratory animals and with an approval from the University of California, Davis Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee. Adult male Sprague-Dawley rats (n = 75; 300–375 g; Envigo, Livermore, CA, USA) were randomly assigned to vehicle control (n = 16) or pilocarpine (n = 59); all animals were housed in a standard institutional vivarium with a 12-h (7) light cycle. Complications related to status epilepticus (n = 20), and failed implants (vehicle n = 2, pilocarpine n = 1) resulted in a final sample size n = 52. For all behavioral experiments, investigators were blind to group (pilocarpine vs. vehicle) but not to stimulation condition (stimulation is triggered). Therefore, all data was recoded after collection and all analyses were performed blind to condition. A timeline of experiments is illustrated in Figure 1A.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Experimental timeline and biological measures post-pilocarpine injection. (A) Timeline of procedures. We allowed 4 weeks, operationally defined as the epileptogenic period, from the time of pilocarpine for rats to develop spontaneous seizures. (B) Stimulation groups were counterbalanced via cycles of status epilepticus and (C) weight following pilocarpine injection. Each of the three pilocarpine-treated groups were statistically different from vehicle across the first 16 days following pilocarpine administration. However, there were no statistical differences between any of the pilocarpine-treated groups. ***p < 0.001.

Electrode Implantation Pre-pilocarpine

Surgical procedures followed previously published work (29). Briefly, animals were anesthetized using 2–4% isoflurane in O2/N2O (1:2) carrier gas, intubated, and mechanically ventilated to maintain a surgical plane of anesthesia. Small craniotomies (2 mm) were made bilaterally over the medial pre-limbic cortex (PFC; AP = +3.2, ML = ±1.3), dorsal hippocampus (dHPC; AP = −3.3, ML = ±2.0), ventral hippocampus (vHPC; AP = −5.2, ML = ±5.3), and MSN (AP = +0.48, ML = ±1.5). Five small stainless steel screws (#0–80) were then embedded into the skull proximal to the craniotomies. Tungsten electrodes (E363T-2-SPC, 0.2 mm diameter; Plastics1, Roanoke VA) were stereotactically implanted in each location including bilateral PFC (DV = −3.5; 9° angle), dHPC and vHPC spanning CA3-CA1 (DV = −3.2, −6.0, respectively), and unilaterally over left MSN (DV = −6.8; 12.8° angle). A pair of electrodes (~1 mm between tips) was also implanted in the right MSN (DV = −7.01; 10° angle) for bipolar stimulation. Electrodes were anchored to the screws using Metabond (Parkell, Inc., Edgewood NY) and non-toxic superglue. Electrodes and screws were subsequently wired (stainless steel) and pinned to separate channels on a Neuralynx electrode interface board (Neuralynx EIB-QC-16). The implant was anchored to the skull with methyl methacrylate and cyanoacrylate adhesive (Yates Motloid, Chicago IL) to form a rigid implant. All animals, including vehicle and pilocarpine-treated rats, underwent identical electrode implantation. Animals recovered for 10–14 days prior to injection of pilocarpine.

Pilocarpine Administration

As detailed in previous work (29), rats received injections (i.p.) of 1 mg/kg scopolamine methyl nitrate followed 30 min later by 350 mg/kg pilocarpine hydrochloride. Four hours later, animals were injected with 8 mg/kg diazepam to terminate convulsions. Vehicle control animals received similar doses of scopolamine and diazepam, however pilocarpine injection was replaced with an equal dose of sterile saline solution. All pilocarpine-induced rats experienced at least one cycle of status epilepticus as defined by generalized motor convulsions including rearing and falling with forelimb clonus. Animal weights were recorded on days 1–16 following pilocarpine, and again on days 32 and 56.

Stimulation (PPD-4–16)

Rats were counterbalanced into treatment groups based on a combination of average weight from post-pilocarpine day 1–3 (PPD1–3; Figure 1B) and the number of cycles of status epilepticus in the first 2 h after pilocarpine injection (Figure 1C). Control animals included vehicle controls for pilocarpine with no stimulation (Vehicle; n = 14) and pilocarpine-treated rats with no stimulation (Pilo; n = 13) rats. Experimental pilocarpine groups received either 7.7 Hz theta (Fixed; n = 12) or theta-burst stimulation (Burst; n = 13). On PPD4–16, animals were placed in a Plexiglas box (28 × 28 × 30 cm), and bipolar MSN stimulation was applied (IZ2 Stimulator; Tucker Davis Technologies) for 30 min/day. Based on previous studies, stimulation parameters were as follows: (Fixed) 7.7 Hz at 80 μA with 1 ms pulse-width (24, 2933); (Burst) 50 ms trains of 200 Hz, 5 trains/s, 60 μA, 100 μs pulse-width (34).

Spike and Seizure Detection, Local Field Potentials (LFPs), and Spectral Analyses

All LFPs were recorded with a head stage pre-amplifier (Neuralynx HS-16-QC) tethered to a data acquisition system (Neuralynx Digital Lynx SX; band-pass filtered 1–500 Hz). Custom MATLAB (MathWorks) scripts were used for signal processing and spike detection. LFP recording sessions were visually inspected, and artifacts were manually rejected prior to analyses. Raw data were then down-sampled to 1,000 Hz and filtered for 60 Hz line noise using a Butterworth bandstop IIR filter.

A custom Matlab script was developed to quantify spikes in PFC, vHPC, and dHPC, as well as the MSN, during novel object recognition sessions. A two-step process was employed to identify epileptiform spikes within each channel for each recording session and each animal. First, amplitude criteria were used to distinguish the positive and negative polarity peaks using the Matlab function findpeaks(); the minimum peak height for selection as a spike was defined as 4.5 standard deviations from the signal mean calculated over the entire 5 min recording session. However, when variance was low within an epoch, most often in vehicle controls, and a resulting value was <0.5 mV, the minimum peak height was then reset to 0.5 to reduce physiologic oscillatory activity being mischaracterized as a spike. To limit the number of false positives, a second criterion using increases in power of the signal, was employed to refine the detection approach. Using spectral decomposition, the power was calculated for each channel and epoch. We then averaged the power (normalized within frequency band) across frequencies to obtain a single value for each time point. Again, the peaks in power were detected using a threshold minimum of the 90th quantile of the average power time-series (In this case, standard deviation was not used due to a non-parametric distribution of power values). Only spikes that met both the amplitude and power criteria were considered as spikes. Overall, while there was slight variation in detection performance by the developed algorithm (i.e., inclusion of some false positives and negatives), using a fully automated method ensured no bias in this quantification. Finally, the rates of spikes (spikes/min) were calculated; if there were multiple recording sessions in a day (e.g., the NOR task), the spike rate was averaged over sessions to obtain a single value per day per animal. Following detection and quantification, spikes were rejected prior to spectral analyses.

We also conducted 24 h (3 × 8 h) of seizure monitoring on PPD70-72 in a subset of animals (n = 17), as equipment necessary to record simultaneously from multiple rats was acquired midway through the experiment. Seizures were manually identified via video and electrophysiology.

LFP recordings occurred throughout the experimental timeline (Figure 1A), including for 5 min on non-behavioral days in a black Plexiglas box (28 × 28 × 30 cm), and for 5 min prior to and 10 min following stimulation sessions (Baseline, PPD4–16), and during the novel object task and Barnes maze.

Analysis of theta power (6–10 Hz) was performed across multiple recording sessions (pre-pilocarpine, PPD4, PPD31). Spectral power estimates across frequency bands (2–30 Hz, 30 logarithmically spaced points) were calculated using Morlet wavelets (cycles = 6) (35). Both baseline (pre-pilocarpine) and PPD4 power spectral densities were normalized to decibels using the ratio of power in a 15 s epoch of ambulation to the entire 5-min baseline recording session (36). For PPD31, the entire PPD4 recording was used for normalization.

Flurothyl Testing (PPD32 and 56)

All animals including Vehicle control rats underwent flurothyl testing (29). Seizure threshold was measured using the volatile GABAA antagonist flurothyl (bis(2,2,2-trifluroethyl) ether, Sigma Aldrich) (37). An initial seizure threshold was determined prior to behavioral assessment on PPD32, and a final measurement was obtained on PPD56, after cognitive evaluation. Flurothyl was dripped directly onto filter paper at a rate of 1.2 mL/h (29). Times to induce Racine stages 1–5 seizures were recorded and used to compare individual thresholds. PPD32 and 56 values were compared to determine effects of time and stimulation.

Novel Object Recognition (NOR) Task (PPD43–45)

On PPD43, animals were habituated for 5 min to a dark, square Plexiglas box (100 × 100 × 40 cm) with cues on each wall. On PPD45, animals were placed in the box for 5 min with two of three potential objects positioned 40 cm apart. Animals were then placed in a neutral cage for 3 h and subsequently returned to the box for a 5 min test phase, in which one of the objects was replaced with a novel object. Objects and object positions were randomized. Recognition index, a comparison of the percentage of time animals interacted with the novel object relative to overall object interaction, was calculated (38).

Barnes Maze (PPD49–53)

The Barnes maze consists of a Plexiglas table (145 cm diameter) with 22 peripheral holes, a goal box in a fixed location under one hole, and four equally spaced distal cues around a dark curtain. Barnes maze habituation occurred on PPD49, and testing spanned PPD50–53 (2 trials/day; 8 total trials). Escape latency and search strategy (spatial, peripheral, or random) were evaluated on trials 2–8. Spatial strategy was defined as the use of a direct path to the goal box; peripheral strategy was defined as circling the periphery of the table; and random search strategy was defined as exploration of non-consecutive holes. Animals tended to forage and explore the Barnes maze on the first trial due to its novelty. As a result, the first trial was omitted from the analysis. Parameters and analyses were based on previous studies (29, 32, 33).

Statistical Analyses

Except where noted, all statistical data are presented as the mean ± SEM. Statistical significance was assigned to values p < 0.05. All statistical analyses were conducted in GraphPad Prism 8.1.

PPD1–16 weight changes across groups were evaluated using a mixed-model ANOVA with a Bonferroni post-hoc analysis. A one-way ANOVA was used to evaluate group differences in cycles of status following pilocarpine.

A one-way ANOVA was used to compare spike counts with a post-hoc Dunnett's multiple comparisons test comparing each of the pilocarpine-treated rats to the Vehicle control. To evaluate the effect of stimulation on seizure threshold, a mixed-effects ANOVA was performed, comparing groups across the two time points (PPD32, 56); a post-hoc Dunnett's multiple comparisons test was utilized to compare all groups to Vehicle.

For analysis of oscillatory activity, including pre-pilocarpine, PPD4, and PPD31, separate mixed model ANOVAs were utilized to compare the theta power spectral data between groups across delta (2–5 Hz), theta (6–10 Hz), and beta (12–30 Hz) frequencies. A post-hoc Dunnett's multiple comparisons comparing all groups to Vehicle was used to determine whether there was a difference between counter-balanced groups prior to stimulation on PPD4 and also whether there was a change in oscillatory power between PPD4 and PPD31. A subset of animals was not included in electrophysiological analyses due to noise resulting from pilocarpine induction as well as channel dropout over time.

To evaluate NOR, a one-way ANOVA with a Dunnett's multiple comparisons test was used to compare all treatment groups to Vehicle. Several animals were excluded due to seizures either immediately before or during the task (n = 4) or for failure to explore the objects during acquisition (n = 5).

Average daily Barnes maze latency was calculated, and a repeated-measures ANOVA was used with a Dunnett's post-hoc test to compare all treatment groups to Vehicle. A non-parametric chi-square analysis was used to compare differences in search strategies (spatial, peripheral, random) across the sum of all trials, with Vehicle performance used as the test proportion. All analyses excluded trial 1, as animal performance was necessarily random/unlearned. Four animals were not included due to seizures.

Separate linear correlation analyses were performed to determine the relationship between: PPD31 power × total Barnes maze latency (trials 2–8); PPD31 power × spikes during NOR. As described above, several trials were removed from the analysis due to seizures as well as channel noise/dropout over time.

Histology

Upon completion of the experiment, animals were euthanized (Euthasol, Virbac Corp., Fort Worth, TX) and transcardially perfused with 4 C 0.1M sodium phosphate buffer (pH 7.4; 100 mL) followed immediately by 4 C 4% paraformaldehyde (pH 7.4; 350 mL). Brains were removed and post-fixed for 24 h in 4% paraformaldehyde at 41 C, then immersed in 10% sucrose solution for 24 h, followed by 48 h in a 30% sucrose solution. After buffer exchange, electrode tract locations were confirmed for all animals in the study using previously described histological methods (29, 32, 33) to visualize NeuN (MAB377 MilliporeSigma, Burlington MA).

Results

Pilocarpine-Induced Seizures and Epileptogenesis

All animals injected with pilocarpine experienced motor limbic seizures followed by status epilepticus. Counterbalanced pilocarpine-treated groups did not significantly differ from each other in terms of cycles of status epilepticus following exposure to pilocarpine (Figure 1B). There was a significant effect of group (F(3, 48) = 2.88, p = 0.046) and an interaction between group and time (F(45, 680) = 2.15, p < 0.0001) on PPD1–16 weight (Figure 1C). Post-hoc Bonferroni analysis demonstrated that each pilocarpine group was statistically different from Vehicle (p < 0.0001 for all comparisons), however there were no differences between Pilo, Fixed, or Burst groups. By days 32 and 56, there were no differences in weight between any of the groups.

Spikes Are Evident Across Groups

We hypothesized that stimulation would reduce spikes. While regular oscillations were observed across the vHPC, dHPC, PFC, and MSN in Vehicle animals (Figure 2A), there was evidence of spikes (Figure 2B) and seizures (Figure 2C) across regions in pilocarpine-treated rats. While all regions were analyzed (Table 1), data presented represent observations in the vHPC as changes in number and amplitude of spikes were greatest in this region. There was a significant main effect of group in vHPC spike rate (F(3, 39) = 2.957, p = 0.044). Dunnett post-hoc analysis indicated that Pilo animals had significantly higher spike rates than Vehicle (7.82 ± 2.07 vs. 1.48 ± 0.56 spikes/min, p = 0.022). Critically, following either Fixed or Burst stimulation (5.50 ± 1.96, p = 0.22 and 3.16 ± 1.51, p = 0.80, respectively), spike rates remained statistically similar to Vehicle (Figure 3A; Table 1). Over the 24 h (3 × 8 h) of seizure monitoring of a small subset of the animals (data not shown), there were no differences in the number of seizures between pilocarpine groups.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. LFP recordings. (A) An example of LFP traces in a Vehicle animal from the ventral (vHPC) and dorsal (dHPC) hippocampus, medial septal nucleus (MSN), and pre-frontal cortex (PFC). (B) Pilocarpine-injected animals exhibited spikes; all detected spikes could be observed in the vHPC, with only a subset detected in the more remote MSN and PFC. (C) Pilocarpine-injected animals also exhibited spontaneous seizures. This electrographic activity represents a visually confirmed seizure with the entire seizure recorded from the vHPC depicted on the top right. Note, while the activity was highly synchronized and observable across channels, seizure amplitude was maximal in the vHPC and was minimal in MSN and PFC. All traces are on the same scale.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. ∫Average spike rate (spikes/min ± SEM) during the novel object task.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Ictal activity in epileptic animals. (A) During the novel object recognition task, there was a significant difference in spike rate when comparing Pilo animals with Vehicle. (B) On PPD32 and 56, Pilo animals demonstrated significantly reduced stage 3 seizure threshold as compared to Vehicle animals. Both stimulation groups had similar seizure thresholds to the Vehicle group. *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01.

Early Stimulation Has Lasting Effects on Seizure Threshold

To determine if there were persistent effects of stimulation on seizure susceptibility, seizure threshold was evaluated on PPD32 and 56 (Figure 3B). As seizure threshold values were co-linear between groups, and statistical differences were similar across seizure stages, Racine stage 3 was selected as an example. A mixed-effects ANOVA comparing latency to Racine stage 3 indicted a significant main effect of group [F(3, 47) = 5.77, p = 0.002]. A Dunnett's test demonstrated that Pilo animals had a significantly reduced stage 3 threshold compared to Vehicle on both PPD32 (316.46 ± 27.74 vs. 513.92 ± 37.05 s, p = 0.002) and PPD56 (332.23 ± 20.94 vs. 496.85 ± 31.19 s, p = 0.014). Similar to what was observed when quantifying spikes, rats receiving Fixed (415.08 ± 49.96 s, p = 0.35 on PPD32; 388.33 ± 35.92, p = 0.25 on PPD56) or Burst (455.62 ± 34.43, p = 0.85 on PPD32; 455.46 ± 54.46 s on PPD56, p = 0.97) stimulation were not statistically different from Vehicle. Analyses of seizures stages (stages 1, 3, and 5) are presented in Table 2.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Seizure thresholds across the Racine scale.

Status Epilepticus Suppresses Broadband Activity

Prior to pilocarpine injection, there were no differences in vHPC oscillatory power between all animals based on eventual grouping (Figure 4A). We hypothesized that reduced theta oscillations would be evident following pilocarpine injection. Four days following pilocarpine but prior to stimulation, there was a significant decrease in vHPC theta power as compared to Vehicle (Figure 4B; F(1, 192) = 14.52, p = 0.0002). There was also significant suppression of the neighboring delta (F(1, 384) = 9.749, p = 0.002) and beta (F(1, 320) = 27.34, p < 0.0001) bands, indicating that the response to pilocarpine over the first days following status epilepticus suppressed broadband spectral activity.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Longitudinal changes in vHPC power. (A) Prior to the administration of pilocarpine (baseline), oscillatory power was similar between the groups. (B) However, theta, delta, and beta power significantly decreased on PPD4 (normalized to baseline) in animals treated with pilocarpine as compared with Vehicle. (C) Animals counterbalanced into stimulation groups demonstrate no difference in 6–10 Hz power prior to the initiation of stimulation (PPD4). (D) When normalized to PPD4, PPD31 power was not significantly different between treatment groups. **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001.

Pilocarpine animals were counterbalanced into Pilo, Fixed, and Burst and there was no difference in vHPC power between the three pilocarpine-treated groups on PPD4 (Figure 4C) prior to stimulation. Comparing power of PPD31 to PPD4, it was clear that theta oscillations remained depressed in animals treated with pilocarpine, and that there were no lasting benefits of stimulation on oscillatory power (Figure 4D).

In the dHPC, significant differences in the theta [F(1, 256) = 13.36, p = 0.0003] and beta [F(1, 352) = 26.22, p < 0.0001] bands, but not delta [F(1, 384) = 3.336, p = 0.069], were apparent between groups on PPD4; similar to the vHPC, there were no differences on PPD31 (data not shown). For PFC, significant group differences were only apparent in the beta [F(1, 280) = 65.80, p < 0.0001] band on PPD4 (data not shown).

Early Stimulation Improves NOR and Barnes Maze Performance

Animals were tested in the NOR and Barnes Maze to identify any persistent effects of early stimulation on object-based and spatial memory performance during the chronic stage of epilepsy.

Novel Object Task

As increased preference for the novel object indicates memory recognition (38), we hypothesized that epilepsy would result in reduced exploration of the novel object. Analysis via one-way ANOVA revealed a significant group difference in performance (Figure 5A, F(3, 40) = 3.630, p = 0.02); importantly, a Dunnett's post-hoc test indicated that Pilo animals had significantly reduced preference for the novel object (41.5% ± 6.0; p = 0.041) compared to Vehicle animals (59.0% ± 4.2). Both Fixed (61.9% ± 4.4, p = 0.96) and Burst (61.1% ± 4.9, p = 0.98) stimulation groups exhibited a clear preference for the novel object similar to Vehicles.

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Group differences on behavioral tasks. (A) On PPD 43–45, Pilo animals demonstrated significantly reduced preference for the novel object as compared to Vehicle. Fixed and Burst stimulation groups were no different than Vehicle. (B) Analysis of the Barnes maze on PPD 50–53 revealed that Pilo animals had a significantly elevated latency to the escape box as compared with Vehicle. Both stimulation groups had statistically similar latencies to the Vehicle group. (C) Pilo animals demonstrated significantly different search strategy utilization as compared with Vehicle. Both stimulation groups also had different strategy utilizations as compared to Vehicle. *p < 0.05, ***p < 0.001.

Barnes Maze

We identified a significant group-by-time interaction on Barnes maze escape latency (Figure 5B; F(9, 132) = 2.316, p = 0.019) indicating a difference in how the groups learned across days. While all the animals performed similarly on days 1 and 2, a Dunnett's post-hoc analysis demonstrated that Pilo animals had significantly longer latencies to the goal box as compared to Vehicle by days 3 (125.86 ± 33.84 vs. 31.64 ± 7.65 s; p = 0.023) and 4 (114.59 ± 36.99 vs. 26.00 ± 3.71 s; p = 0.035). Both Fixed and Burst demonstrated an intermediate phenotype and were statistically similar to Vehicles across all days.

Search strategy is an important indicator of learning during spatial navigation. Pilo animals predominantly used random search strategies (83.1%) compared to Vehicle (50.5%), Fixed (57.1%), and Burst (56%) rats. Therefore, similar to Vehicle, Fixed, and Burst animals utilized more efficient peripheral and spatial strategies (Figure 5C). Using Vehicle as the test proportion, non-parametric chi-square analyses demonstrated significantly worse search strategies in Pilo rats [χ2(2) = 40.06; p < 0.0001].

vHPC Theta-Band Power Is Correlated With Cognitive Performance and Spikes

We predicted that there would be a strong relationship between theta oscillatory power on PPD31 and both epileptiform activity and Barnes maze performance. A correlation matrix identified a significant relationship between theta power and spikes, with reduced theta power in the vHPC associating with elevated spike counts (Figure 6A, r = −0.39, p = 0.027). In addition, we found that higher theta power correlated with shorter latencies to the escape box in the Barnes maze (Figure 6B, r = −0.43, p = 0.015), indicating that theta oscillations in the vHPC and spatial navigation are also directly associated. In the dHPC, a similar relationship between power and spikes (data not shown, r = −0.47, p < 0.0081), and power and latency (data not shown, r = −0.51, p = 0.0018) was also observed.

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. Relationship between vHPC power, behavioral performance, and spikes. (A) There was a significant linear relationship between vHPC power (6–10 Hz) on PPD31 and the development of spikes (p < 0.05). (B) vHPC Power (6–10 Hz) on PPD31 was also significantly correlated with total Barnes maze latency (p < 0.05).

Discussion

In the current study, we evaluated whether theta stimulation during early epileptogenesis could alter the progression of pilocarpine-induced TLE. Critically, our results indicate that 2 weeks of daily stimulation led to lasting changes. Specifically, while Pilo rats had significantly higher spike rates, lower seizure thresholds, and were impaired on the NOR and the Barnes maze, rats receiving Fixed or Burst stimulation performed statistically similar to Vehicle across all measures in the weeks following stimulation. These novel results further corroborate research from other labs (39), and also add to our previous findings that theta-frequency stimulation of the MSN when administered at the time of behavioral testing improves spatial learning in the first 2 weeks following status epilepticus (32) and also in chronically epileptic rats (29). Ultimately, this novel therapeutic paradigm of acute septal theta stimulation represents a paradigm with the potential to not only alter the development epileptiform activity, but also to improve long-term cognitive outcome.

Fixed-Frequency and Theta-Burst Stimulation of the MSN

Theta oscillations facilitate the timely integration of activity from multiple brain regions and are important in the brain's ability to form and recall memories (26, 31). In rodents, lesions of the MSN, or septal suppression with pharmacological agents such as tetracaine, scopolamine, or muscimol, all attenuate hippocampal theta oscillations and are associated with significant cognitive deficits (25, 31, 4042). In epilepsy, we see a similar set of relationships between septohippocampal abnormalities (i.e., reduced theta oscillations), and impaired cognition. Specifically, LFP recordings from epileptic rats reveal a significant reduction in hippocampal theta power and amplitude (27, 29) and demonstrate impaired performance in memory tasks both acutely following induction (32, 43) and after developing SRS (4345). Our data add to these prior findings as we now report statistically significant correlations between a reduction in theta power and an increase in spikes as well as between decreases in theta power and longer latencies on the Barnes maze. While we have previously indicated the benefits of theta MSN stimulation after animals have developed spontaneous seizures, data from this novel paradigm indicate that stimulated animals (Fixed and Burst) performed at Vehicle-level on both the NOR and the Barnes maze when stimulation was applied early, and behavior was assessed weeks after stimulation was terminated. More specifically, on the NOR stimulated animals had similar preference for the novel object (Vehicle 59.0% ± 4.2, Fixed 61.9% ± 4.4, and Burst 61.1% ± 4.9) and when evaluating spatial memory on the Barnes maze, search strategies utilized by stimulated animals indicated significantly more efficient learning, similar to Vehicle (Vehicle 11% Spatial, 38.5% peripheral, Fixed 19.5% spatial, 23.4% peripheral, and Burst 22.6% spatial, 21.4% peripheral).

In addition to fixed theta stimulation, we also chose to evaluate theta burst stimulation of the MSN. While theta burst stimulation is associated with superior long-term potentiation compared to simple fixed-frequency stimulation paradigms (46), we were most interested in the relationship between burst stimulation and intrinsic septohippocampal activity. It is well-established that the MSN is critical for modulating hippocampal excitation and inhibition (47). More specifically, it is hypothesized that bursting septal neurons pace the oscillatory frequency of theta fields in the hippocampus (4850). This is evidenced by both the temporal precedence of MSN bursting neurons and their tight phase locking with the ongoing hippocampal theta fields (4951). In addition, following pilocarpine, changes in hippocampal theta oscillations coincide with altered neural discharges and burst rates in the MSN (27, 5254) resulting in the phase relationship between septal activity and the hippocampal theta rhythm becoming uncoupled (52, 53, 55, 56). For these reasons, we evaluated both 7.7 Hz fixed-frequency and burst stimulation of the MSN. Interestingly, we did not find any significant differences between paradigms as they pertained to spikes or cognitive outcomes in this study.

vHPC Theta Power Is Associated With Spikes and Cognitive Performance

We consistently observed the highest amplitude seizures and the most spikes originated in the vHPC. Moreover, we found that oscillations in the vHPC significantly correlated with outcome. Specifically, chronic oscillatory changes in the vHPC weeks following pilocarpine exposure were significantly associated with spikes and impaired spatial learning.

Observed relationships between vHPC theta and increased spikes are in line with both preclinical and clinical observations, demonstrating the importance of this region in epileptogenesis. For example, a recent study evaluating ictogenesis in 600 pilocarpine-induced seizures across 16 bilaterally distributed electrode locations found that, while seizures usually generalized to all regions, they frequently originated in the vHPC (57). Similarly, in one clinical case series, 80% of patients with TLE had a seizure focus either in the anterior portion of the hippocampus (vHPC in the rat) or amygdala (58, 59). Furthermore, detection of pathological oscillations in temporal regions such as the anterior hippocampus during intraoperative electrocorticography has become a valuable predictor of successful outcome following resection of potentially epileptogenic tissue (60).

The role of vHPC in cognition is also well-described. The vHPC and PFC are monosynaptically connected, and functional communication between these structures is critical for working memory in the rodent (6164). Furthermore, individual neurons in the PFC phase-lock with ongoing theta field activity in the hippocampus via the ventral subregion during spatial, non-spatial, and object-based behavioral tasks (6264). Importantly, disruption of neural activity in the vHPC, whether surgically or resulting from pilocarpine induction, results in impaired working memory and reduced theta oscillations (29, 32, 43, 44, 65). As such, it is not surprising that, in the current study, animals with higher power theta oscillations were consistently better performers on the Barnes maze.

While it is evident that reduced theta power is correlated with increased spikes and impaired cognition, it remains unclear the mechanism with which stimulation results in better cognitive performance. While we were not able to quantify the effects of stimulation on seizure frequency, both Fixed and Burst-treated rats exhibited similar seizure thresholds and rates of spikes compared to Vehicle controls. Because spikes are believed to interfere with learning and memory, fewer spikes or increased seizure threshold as a result of stimulation may in turn have contributed to improved cognition regardless of a change in seizure frequency (66). Conversely, stimulated animals may have also exhibited better performance directly as a result of stimulating critical cognitive circuits functionally connected with the MSN (24, 25, 6769) leading to lasting plasticity. Low frequency stimulation of the MSN may be in fact driving oscillatory activity in both proximal and more distally connected regions such as the hippocampus and PFC, where oscillations may be disturbed as a result of epileptogenesis. Studies from both our lab and others have shown that low frequency stimulation of the MSN, either continuously or immediately before behavioral testing, results in improved outcome in various injury models including pilocarpine, traumatic brain injury, as well as in lesion studies (29, 3133). Interestingly, a recent study of low frequency 1 Hz hippocampal stimulation identified similar cognitive benefits in pilocarpine-treated rats (70). Ultimately, these data highlight the importance of further investigation into the mechanisms of how DBS impacts hyperexcitability and cognition, particularly over extended periods of time.

Argument for Earlier Intervention

TLE is considered refractory after two failed drug treatments (10, 71). Although resection has a high rate of seizure freedom (~70%), surgical options remain a last resort, often occurring more than 20 years after disease onset (7, 7274) and continued failure to respond to medicine (75). TLE, however, can be progressive, resulting in debilitating consequences such as hippocampal sclerosis and death, especially if seizures are poorly controlled (72, 76, 77).

When resection is not an option (12), FDA-approved neuromodulation paradigms, including DBS of the ANT, RNS, and VNS, all result in ~70% seizure reduction over time. In addition, neuromodulation has fewer irreversible consequences than resection (1315). Current epilepsy stimulation paradigms, however, also occur as a “last resort,” decades after diagnosis, and do not effectively resolve TLE-associated cognitive deficits. Several clinical examples in childhood and adolescent epilepsies emphasize the potential for earlier intervention. Younger patients receive surgical intervention more rapidly, due to the greater impact of seizures on development and also based on evidence of high rates of seizure freedom resulting from surgery (78, 79). In a retrospective study comparing surgical outcome in children and adults with intractable TLE, a combined 80% of children, compared to 49% of adults, were seizure free despite similar rates of hippocampal sclerosis (80). Furthermore, the likelihood of seizure freedom following surgery is significantly increased in patients with fewer than 5 years of epilepsy history (80), indicating that not just age but also intervention more proximal to diagnosis increased treatment efficacy. Critically, the results of our study support that (1) Early intervention in rodents can have a lasting effect including a reduction in spikes and an increase in seizure threshold; (2) DBS of the MSN has the potential to concurrently improve cognitive outcome. In sum, data from our lab as well as clinical studies support the potential benefits of early intervention. While current anti-seizure treatment strategies should necessarily continue as the first line of treatment, we argue for additional studies to explore the more expeditious implementation of DBS in refractory patients (i.e., to close the gap between diagnosis and surgical referral) as well to strategies to address epilepsy comorbidities.

Study Limitations and Future Directions

While we were able to utilize seizure threshold and spikes as proxy measures of increased hyperexcitability and epileptogenesis, due to technological constraints, we were unable to continuously record from our animals. With the recordings we did make, we lacked sufficient power to make a claim about the effects of stimulation on the development of seizures. As pilocarpine-induced epilepsy typically results in an average of 2–3 spontaneous seizures per week (81, 82), 24 h of total recording over a 3 day period, during the light cycle only, and in only a subset of animals resulted in a significant under-sampling. Therefore, we do not report any conclusive results regarding the effect of stimulation on seizure occurrence. However, our pilocarpine induction procedures have been well-characterized in previous studies and all of our animals experienced status epilepticus following pilocarpine (29, 32, 81, 8387). It is critical, therefore, that future studies incorporate continuous monitoring to determine whether stimulation-induced changes in spikes and seizure threshold result in a significant reduction in seizure frequency.

We developed a custom Matlab script to identify spikes objectively and in an unbiased manner. This included counting any oscillatory activity that either (1) exceeded a 4.5 standard deviation change from baseline, or, in the case where there was little variance, such as in vehicle controls, (2) exceeding a minimum peak height. The result was that, in several of our vehicle controls, some spiking activity above baseline was detected. Rather than manipulate the code to eliminate spikes in our controls, or not show the vehicle data all together, we elected to keep the unbiased analysis in the manuscript. In addition to allowing us to maintain objectivity, it also allows for the small possibility that triggering status epilepticus with flurothyl 13 days earlier may have led to some lingering ictal activity.

Multiple studies have demonstrated the dynamic nature of theta oscillations during epochs of movement in rodents (24, 88, 89). One additional limitation of our study is that we did not capture specific velocity or acceleration of the rats while recording oscillatory activity. Instead, analyses between groups were made in epochs that included similarly behaving and moving rats. We hope to include tracking software in future studies to address any issues related to movement speed.

One interesting unanswered question remains how stimulation improved outcome. We had initially hypothesized that 2 weeks of stimulation would lead to lasting improvements in neural activity as measured by changes in oscillations. However, we did not observe a long-term benefit of stimulation on theta or broadband power. The effects could not be related directly to the stimulation as in our previous studies (29, 33), as there was a 1-month gap between stimulation and behavior. Perhaps interesting to consider is that the efficacy of both RNS and DBS in epilepsy patients improves with time, even without changes to the stimulation paradigm (13, 14). These clinical data, along with our current data, suggest that repeated stimulation may lead to changes in neuroanatomy or connectivity that may explain the link between stimulation and the clear evidence of plasticity. These neuroanatomical changes could include a reduction in cell death, a reduction in neuroinflammation, or even change in neurotransmission. As tissue from these current experiments has significant artifact due to the number of implanted electrodes and, in many cases damage to the tissue caused by the removal of the implant, it is not suitable for a rigorous analysis of anatomical changes. Future studies should consider including animals implanted only with stimulating electrodes with the express goal of evaluating lasting histological changes that may inform the mechanism of action of septal DBS as it pertains to the long-term benefits of stimulation. In addition, a longitudinal genomics analysis to look at expression of genes related to neuroplasticity and neurotransmission before, during and after periods of neuromodulation could be highly informative.

Finally, animals were stimulated predominantly during the epileptogenic period. Our premise was that the induction of status epilepticus with pilocarpine served as the initial seizure events. However, one might argue that patients would not receive anti-seizure medication until they were diagnosed with recurring spontaneous seizures. Invasive neuromodulation would not be an option until a patient has been deemed refractory. However, the current data serve as a proof-of-concept that early intervention can lead to lasting changes in disease progression and outcome as animals benefitted from neuromodulation. Therefore, to bridge the translational gap, future studies should effects of stimulation occurring at least several weeks following the detection of spontaneous seizures and then to compare whether stimulation in these rats alters the progression of the disease similar to our observations in the current data set. Results from this type of study would enhance translatability to the clinical setting where patients have already experienced several years of seizures, prior to being classified as refractory to treatment.

Conclusion

We now demonstrate in a rodent model of TLE that the MSN and theta rhythm represent a neuromodulation target that have the potential to both reduce markers of epilepsy while also improving cognitive outcome. These data are especially exciting, as neither rats receiving Fixed or Burst stimulation performed differently from Vehicle, whereas untreated Pilo rats had significant increases in spikes, a reduction in seizure threshold and demonstrated impaired performance on both the NOR and Barnes maze tasks. Furthermore, these data provide further support toward growing evidence that early intervention in patients with TLE results in superior outcomes. In the clinic, many patients do not meet criteria for resection, and while RNS and DBS of the ANT can lead to significantly improved seizure control over time, these therapies are not designed to address cognitive comorbidities and are often utilized decades after diagnosis. Our results represent a significant step toward identifying a promising DBS therapy for refractory patients with cognitive impairments, while also supporting the potential benefits of earlier intervention.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The animal study was reviewed and approved by Office of Research Animal Care Program (IACUC).

Author Contributions

AI contributed to all facets of the experiment including experimental design and data collection and analysis as well as writing. AS, KO, and GD each were involved with data collection and analysis as well as manuscript editing. SC, AE, and KS were involved with experimental design, data analysis, and editing. GG was involved in all aspects of the study including design, data collection and analysis, and writing. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This study was supported by the National Institute of Health (NINDS NS084026) and the Bronte Epilepsy Foundation.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

1. Zack MM, Kobau R. National and State Estimates of the numbers of adults and children with active epilepsy - United States, 2015. MMWR Morb Mortal Wkly Rep. (2017) 66:821–5. doi: 10.15585/mmwr.mm6631a1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Keezer MR, Sisodiya SM, Sander JW. Comorbidities of epilepsy: current concepts and future perspectives. Lancet Neurol. (2016) 15:106–15. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(15)00225-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Kwan P, Schachter SC, Brodie MJ. Drug-resistant epilepsy. N Engl J Med. (2011) 365:919–26. doi: 10.1056/NEJMra1004418

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Brodie MJ. Road to refractory epilepsy: the Glasgow story. Epilepsia. (2013) 54:5–8. doi: 10.1111/epi.12175

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Semah F, Picot MC, Adam C, Broglin D, Arzimanoglou A, Bazin B, et al. Is the underlying cause of epilepsy a major prognostic factor for recurrence?. Neurology. (1998) 51:1256–62. doi: 10.1212/WNL.51.5.1256

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Perucca P, Gilliam FG. Adverse effects of antiepileptic drugs. Lancet Neurol. (2012) 11:792–802. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(12)70153-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Wiebe S, Blume WT, Girvin JP, Eliasziw M, Effectiveness and Efficiency of Surgery for Temporal Lobe Epilepsy Study Group. A randomized, controlled trial of surgery for temporal-lobe epilepsy. N Engl J Med. (2001) 345:311–8. doi: 10.1056/NEJM200108023450501

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Kleen JK, Scott RC, Lenck-Santini PP, Holmes GL. Cognitive and behavioral co-morbidities of epilepsy. In: Noebels JL, Avoli M, Rogawski MA, Olsen RW, Delgado-Escueta VA, editors. Jaspers Basic Mechanisms of the Epilepsies. Bethesda, MD: National Center for Biotechnology Information (2012). p. 79.

Google Scholar

9. Kwan P, Sperling MR. Refractory seizures: try additional antiepileptic drugs (after two have failed) or go directly to early surgery evaluation?. Epilepsia. (2009) 50:57–62. doi: 10.1111/j.1528-1167.2009.02237.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Kwan P, Arzimanoglou A, Berg AT, Brodie MJ, Allen Hauser W, Mathern G, et al. Definition of drug resistant epilepsy: consensus proposal by the ad hoc Task Force of the ILAE Commission on Therapeutic Strategies. Epilepsia. (2010) 51:1069–77. doi: 10.1111/j.1528-1167.2009.02397.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Yoon HH, Kwon HL, Mattson RH, Spencer DD, Spencer SS. Long-term seizure outcome in patients initially seizure-free after resective epilepsy surgery. Neurology. (2003) 61:445–50. doi: 10.1212/01.WNL.0000081226.51886.5B

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Ryvlin P, Rheims S. Epilepsy surgery: eligibility criteria and presurgical evaluation. Dialogues Clin Neurosci. (2008) 10:91–103. doi: 10.31887/DCNS.2008.10.1/pryvlin

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Geller EB, Skarpaas TL, Gross RE, Goodman RR, Barkley GL, Bazil CW, et al. Brain-responsive neurostimulation in patients with medically intractable mesial temporal lobe epilepsy. Epilepsia. (2017) 58:994–1004. doi: 10.1111/epi.13740

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Salanova V, Witt T, Worth R, Henry TR, Gross RE, Nazzaro JM, et al. Long-term efficacy and safety of thalamic stimulation for drug-resistant partial epilepsy. Neurology. (2015) 84:1017–25. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000001334

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Fisher RS, Handforth A. Reassessment: vagus nerve stimulation for epilepsy: a report of the Therapeutics and Technology Assessment Subcommittee of the American Academy of Neurology. Neurology. (1999) 53:666–9. doi: 10.1212/WNL.53.4.666

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Revesz D, Frojd V, Rydenhag B, Ben-Menachem E. Estimating long-term vagus nerve stimulation effectiveness: accounting for antiepileptic drug treatment changes. Neuromodulation. (2018) 21:797–804. doi: 10.1111/ner.12775

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Troster AI, Meador KJ, Irwin CP, Fisher RS, Sante Study Group. Memory and mood outcomes after anterior thalamic stimulation for refractory partial epilepsy. Seizure. (2017) 45:133–41. doi: 10.1016/j.seizure.2016.12.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Good LB, Sabesan S, Marsh ST, Tsakalis K, Treiman D, Iasemidis L. Control of synchronization of brain dynamics leads to control of epileptic seizures in rodents. Int J Neural Syst. (2009) 19:173–96. doi: 10.1142/S0129065709001951

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Hamani C, Hodaie MJ, Chiang del Campo M, Andrade DM, et al. Deep brain stimulation of the anterior nucleus of the thalamus: effects of electrical stimulation on pilocarpine-induced seizures and status epilepticus. Epilepsy Res. (2008) 78:117–23. doi: 10.1016/j.eplepsyres.2007.09.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Velasco AL, Velasco F, Velasco M, Trejo D, Castro G, Carrillo-Ruiz JD. Electrical stimulation of the hippocampal epileptic foci for seizure control: a double-blind, long-term follow-up study. Epilepsia. (2007) 48:1895–903. doi: 10.1111/j.1528-1167.2007.01181.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Stavropoulos I, Selway R, Hasegawa H, Hughes E, Rittey C, Jimenez-Jimenez D, et al. Low frequency centromedian thalamic nuclei deep brain stimulation for the treatment of super refractory status epilepticus: A case report and a review of the literature. Brain Stimul. (2021) 14:226–9. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2020.12.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Paschen E, Elgueta C, Heining K, Vieira DM, Kleis P, Orcinha C, et al. Hippocampal low-frequency stimulation prevents seizure generation in a mouse model of mesial temporal lobe epilepsy. Elife. (2020) 9:e54518. doi: 10.7554/eLife.54518

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Weiss SR, Li XL, Rosen JB, Li H, Heynen T, Post RM. Quenching: inhibition of development and expression of amygdala kindled seizures with low frequency stimulation. Neuroreport. (1995) 6:2171–6. doi: 10.1097/00001756-199511000-00018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Gray JA, Ball GG. Frequency-specific relation between hippocampal theta rhythm, behavior, amobarbital action. Science. (1970) 168:1246–8.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

25. Winson J. Loss of hippocampal theta rhythm results in spatial memory deficit in the rat. Science. (1978) 201:160–3. doi: 10.1126/science.663646

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Buzsaki G. Theta rhythm of navigation: link between path integration and landmark navigation, episodic and semantic memory. Hippocampus. (2005) 15:827–40. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20113

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Colom LV, Garcia-Hernandez A, Castaneda MT, Perez-Cordova MG, Garrido-Sanabria ER. Septo-hippocampal networks in chronically epileptic rats: potential antiepileptic effects of theta rhythm generation. J Neurophysiol. (2006) 95:3645–53. doi: 10.1152/jn.00040.2006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Miller JW, Turner GM, Gray BC. Anticonvulsant effects of the experimental induction of hippocampal theta activity. Epilepsy Res. (1994) 18:195–204. doi: 10.1016/0920-1211(94)90040-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Izadi A, Pevzner A, Lee DJ, Ekstrom AD, Shahlaie K, Gurkoff GG. Medial septal stimulation increases seizure threshold and improves cognition in epileptic rats. Brain Stimul. (2019) 12:735–42. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2019.01.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Lee DJ, Gurkoff GG, Izadi A, Berman RF, Ekstrom AD, Muizelaar JP, et al. Medial septal nucleus theta frequency deep brain stimulation improves spatial working memory after traumatic brain injury. J Neurotrauma. (2013) 30:131–9. doi: 10.1089/neu.2012.2646

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. McNaughton N, Ruan M, Woodnorth MA. Restoring theta-like rhythmicity in rats restores initial learning in the Morris water maze. Hippocampus. (2006) 16:1102–10. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20235

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Lee DJ, Izadi A, Melnik M, Seidl S, Echeverri A, Shahlaie K, et al. Stimulation of the medial septum improves performance in spatial learning following pilocarpine-induced status epilepticus. Epilepsy Res. (2017) 130:53–63. doi: 10.1016/j.eplepsyres.2017.01.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Lee DJ, Gurkoff GG, Izadi A, Echeverri A, Melnik M, Berman RF, et al. Septohippocampal neuromodulation improves cognition after traumatic brain injury. J Neurotrauma. (2015) 32:1822–32. doi: 10.1089/neu.2014.3744

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Sweet JA, Eakin KC, Munyon CN, Miller JP. Improved learning and memory with theta-burst stimulation of the fornix in rat model of traumatic brain injury. Hippocampus. (2014) 24:1592–600. doi: 10.1002/hipo.22338

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Cohen MX. Analyzing Neural Time Series Data: Theory and Practice. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press (2014).

Google Scholar

36. Cohen MX, Donner TH. Midfrontal conflict-related theta-band power reflects neural oscillations that predict behavior. J Neurophysiol. (2013) 110:2752–63. doi: 10.1152/jn.00479.2013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Araki H, Kobayashi Y, Hashimoto Y, Futagami K, Kawasaki H, Gomita Y. Characteristics of flurothyl-induced seizures and the effect of antiepileptic drugs on flurothyl-induced seizures in Mongolian gerbils. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. (2002) 74:141–7. doi: 10.1016/S0091-3057(02)00965-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Antunes M, Biala G. The novel object recognition memory: neurobiology, test procedure, its modifications. Cogn Process. (2012) 13:93–110. doi: 10.1007/s10339-011-0430-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Fisher RS. Stimulation of the medial septum should benefit patients with temporal lobe epilepsy. Med Hypotheses. (2015) 84:543–50. doi: 10.1016/j.mehy.2015.02.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Mitchell SJ, Rawlins JN, Steward O, Olton DS. Medial septal area lesions disrupt theta rhythm and cholinergic staining in medial entorhinal cortex and produce impaired radial arm maze behavior in rats. J Neurosci. (1982) 2:292–302. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.02-03-00292.1982

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Givens BS, Olton DS. Cholinergic and GABAergic modulation of medial septal area: effect on working memory. Behav Neurosci. (1990) 104:849–55.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

42. Mizumori SJ, Perez GM, Alvarado MC, Barnes CA, McNaughton BL. Reversible inactivation of the medial septum differentially affects two forms of learning in rats. Brain Res. (1990) 528:12–20. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(90)90188-H

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Chauviere L, Rafrafi N, Thinus-Blanc C, Bartolomei F, Esclapez M, Bernard C. Early deficits in spatial memory and theta rhythm in experimental temporal lobe epilepsy. J Neurosci. (2009) 29:5402–10. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4699-08.2009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Groticke I, Hoffmann K, Loscher W. Behavioral alterations in the pilocarpine model of temporal lobe epilepsy in mice. Exp Neurol. (2007) 207:329–49. doi: 10.1016/j.expneurol.2007.06.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Hort J, Brozek G, Mares P, Langmeier M, Komarek V. Cognitive functions after pilocarpine-induced status epilepticus: changes during silent period precede appearance of spontaneous recurrent seizures. Epilepsia. (1999) 40:1177–83. doi: 10.1111/j.1528-1157.1999.tb00845.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Capocchi G, Zampolini M, Larson J. Theta burst stimulation is optimal for induction of LTP at both apical and basal dendritic synapses on hippocampal CA1 neurons. Brain Res. (1992) 591:332–6. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(92)91715-Q

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Stewart M, Fox SE. Do septal neurons pace the hippocampal theta rhythm?. Trends Neurosci. (1990) 13:163–8. doi: 10.1016/0166-2236(90)90040-H

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Stewart M, Fox SE. Two populations of rhythmically bursting neurons in rat medial septum are revealed by atropine. J Neurophysiol. (1989) 61:982–93. doi: 10.1152/jn.1989.61.5.982

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Varga V, Hangya B, Kranitz K, Ludanyi A, Zemankovics R, Katona I, et al. The presence of pacemaker HCN channels identifies theta rhythmic GABAergic neurons in the medial septum. J Physiol. (2008) 586:3893–915. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2008.155242

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Hangya B, Borhegyi Z, Szilagyi N, Freund TF, Varga V. GABAergic neurons of the medial septum lead the hippocampal network during theta activity. J Neurosci. (2009) 29:8094–102. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5665-08.2009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Serafin M, Williams S, Khateb A, Fort P, Muhlethaler M. Rhythmic firing of medial septum non-cholinergic neurons. Neuroscience. (1996) 75:671–5. doi: 10.1016/0306-4522(96)00349-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Garrido Sanabria ER, Castaneda MT, Banuelos C, Perez-Cordova MG, Hernandez S, Colom LV. Septal GABAergic neurons are selectively vulnerable to pilocarpine-induced status epilepticus and chronic spontaneous seizures. Neuroscience. (2006) 142:871–83. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2006.06.057

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Kitchigina VF, Butuzova MV. Theta activity of septal neurons during different epileptic phases: the same frequency but different significance?. Exp Neurol. (2009) 216:449–58. doi: 10.1016/j.expneurol.2009.01.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Popova IY, Sinelnikova VV, Kitchigina VF. Disturbance of the correlation between hippocampal and septal EEGs during epileptogenesis. Neurosci Lett. (2008) 442:228–33. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2008.07.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Kitchigina V, Popova I, Sinelnikova V, Malkov A, Astasheva E, Shubina L, et al. Disturbances of septohippocampal theta oscillations in the epileptic brain: reasons and consequences. Exp Neurol. (2013) 247:314–27. doi: 10.1016/j.expneurol.2013.01.029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Sinelnikova VV, Popova IY, Kichigina VF. Correlational relationships between the hippocampus and medial septal area and their changes during epileptogenesis. Neurosci Behav Physiol. (2009) 39:619–23. doi: 10.1007/s11055-009-9189-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Toyoda I, Bower MR, Leyva F, Buckmaster PS. Early activation of ventral hippocampus and subiculum during spontaneous seizures in a rat model of temporal lobe epilepsy. J Neurosci. (2013) 33:11100–15. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0472-13.2013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. King D, Bronen RA, Spencer DD, Spencer SS. Topographic distribution of seizure onset and hippocampal atrophy: relationship between MRI and depth EEG. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. (1997) 103:692–7. doi: 10.1016/S0013-4694(97)00090-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Sone D, Sato N, Kimura Y, Watanabe Y, Okazaki M, Matsuda H. Brain morphological and microstructural features in cryptogenic late-onset temporal lobe epilepsy: a structural and diffusion MRI study. Neuroradiology. (2018) 60:635–41. doi: 10.1007/s00234-018-2019-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Roessler K, Heynold E, Buchfelder M, Stefan H, Hamer HM. Current value of intraoperative electrocorticography (iopECoG). Epilepsy Behav. (2019) 91:20–4. doi: 10.1016/j.yebeh.2018.06.053

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Swanson LW. A direct projection from Ammons horn to prefrontal cortex in the rat. Brain Res. (1981) 217:150–4.

Google Scholar

62. Siapas AG, Lubenov EV, Wilson MA. Prefrontal phase locking to hippocampal theta oscillations. Neuron. (2005) 46:141–51. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.02.028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Jones MW, Wilson MA. Theta rhythms coordinate hippocampal-prefrontal interactions in a spatial memory task. PLoS Biol. (2005) 3:e402. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0030402

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. O'Neill PK, Gordon JA, Sigurdsson T. Theta oscillations in the medial prefrontal cortex are modulated by spatial working memory and synchronize with the hippocampus through its ventral subregion. J Neurosci. (2013) 33:14211–24. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2378-13.2013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Persinger MA, Bureau YR, Kostakos M, Peredery O, Falter H. Behaviors of rats with insidious, multifocal brain damage induced by seizures following single peripheral injections of lithium and pilocarpine. Physiol Behav. (1993) 53:849–66. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(93)90261-D

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Kleen JK, Scott RC, Holmes GL, Lenck-Santini PP. Hippocampal interictal spikes disrupt cognition in rats. Ann Neurol. (2010) 67:250–7. doi: 10.1002/ana.21896

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Rawlins JN, Feldon J, Gray JA. Septo-hippocampal connections and the hippocampal theta rhythm. Exp Brain Res. (1979) 37:49–63. doi: 10.1007/BF01474253

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Vertes RP, Kocsis B. Brainstem-diencephalo-septohippocampal systems controlling the theta rhythm of the hippocampus. Neuroscience. (1997) 81:893–926.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

69. Colom LV, Castaneda MT, Reyna T, Hernandez S, Garrido-Sanabria E. Characterization of medial septal glutamatergic neurons and their projection to the hippocampus. Synapse. (2005) 58:151–64. doi: 10.1002/syn.20184

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Wang L, Shi H, Kang Y, Guofeng W. Hippocampal low-frequency stimulation improves cognitive function in pharmacoresistant epileptic rats. Epilepsy Res. (2020) 168:106194. doi: 10.1016/j.eplepsyres.2019.106194

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Kwan P, Brodie MJ. Early identification of refractory epilepsy. N Engl J Med. (2000) 342:314–9. doi: 10.1056/NEJM200002033420503

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

72. Engel J, Jr. Surgical treatment for epilepsy: too little, too late?. JAMA. (2008) 300:2548–50. doi: 10.1001/jama.2008.756

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

73. Berg AT, Vickrey BG, Testa FM, Levy SR, Shinnar S, DiMario F, et al. How long does it take for epilepsy to become intractable? A prospective investigation. Ann Neurol. (2006) 60:73–9. doi: 10.1002/ana.20852

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

74. Engel J Jr, McDermott MP, Wiebe S, Langfitt T, Stern J, et al. Early surgical therapy for drug-resistant temporal lobe epilepsy: a randomized trial. JAMA. (2012) 307:922–30. doi: 10.1001/jama.2012.220

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

75. Perucca E, Tomson T. The pharmacological treatment of epilepsy in adults. Lancet Neurol. (2011) 10:446–56. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(11)70047-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

76. Laxer KD, Trinka E, Hirsch LJ, Cendes F, Langfitt J, Delanty N, et al. The consequences of refractory epilepsy and its treatment. Epilepsy Behav. (2014) 37:59–70. doi: 10.1016/j.yebeh.2014.05.031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

77. Sperling MR. The consequences of uncontrolled epilepsy. CNS Spectr. (2004) 9:98–101. doi: 10.1017/s1092852900008464

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

78. Liu S, An N, Yang H, Yang M, Hou Z, Liu L, et al. Pediatric intractable epilepsy syndromes: reason for early surgical intervention. Brain Dev. (2007) 29:69–78. doi: 10.1016/j.braindev.2006.06.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

79. Sugano H, Arai H. Epilepsy surgery for pediatric epilepsy: optimal timing of surgical intervention. Neurol Med Chir (Tokyo). (2015) 55:399–406. doi: 10.2176/nmc.ra.2014-0369

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

80. Radhakrishnan A, Menon R, Abraham M, Vilanilam G, Sharma S, Thomas B, et al. Predictors of outcome after surgery in 134 children with drug-resistant TLE. Epilepsy Res. (2018) 139:150–6. doi: 10.1016/j.eplepsyres.2017.11.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

81. Cavalheiro EA, Leite JP, Bortolotto ZA, Turski WA, Ikonomidou C, Turski L. Long-term effects of pilocarpine in rats: structural damage of the brain triggers kindling and spontaneous recurrent seizures. Epilepsia. (1991) 32:778–82. doi: 10.1111/j.1528-1157.1991.tb05533.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

82. Lemos T, Cavalheiro EA. Suppression of pilocarpine-induced status epilepticus and the late development of epilepsy in rats. Exp Brain Res. (1995) 102:423–8.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

83. Turski WA, Cavalheiro EA, Schwarz M, Czuczwar SJ, Kleinrok Z, Turski L. Limbic seizures produced by pilocarpine in rats: behavioural, electroencephalographic and neuropathological study. Behav Brain Res. (1983) 9:315–35. doi: 10.1016/0166-4328(83)90136-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

84. Turski WA, Cavalheiro EA, Bortolotto ZA, Mello LM, Schwarz M, Turski L. Seizures produced by pilocarpine in mice: a behavioral, electroencephalographic and morphological analysis. Brain Res. (1984) 321:237–53. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(84)90177-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

85. Turski L, Cavalheiro EA, Sieklucka-Dziuba M, Ikonomidou-Turski C, Czuczwar SJ, Turski WA. Seizures produced by pilocarpine: neuropathological sequelae and activity of glutamate decarboxylase in the rat forebrain. Brain Res. (1986) 398:37–48. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(86)91247-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

86. Turski L, Ikonomidou C, Turski WA, Bortolotto ZA, Cavalheiro EA. Review: cholinergic mechanisms and epileptogenesis. The seizures induced by pilocarpine: a novel experimental model of intractable epilepsy. Synapse. (1989) 3:154–71.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

87. Leite JP, Bortolotto ZA, Cavalheiro EA. Spontaneous recurrent seizures in rats: an experimental model of partial epilepsy. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (1990) 14:511–7. doi: 10.1016/S0149-7634(05)80076-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

88. Buzsaki G. Theta oscillations in the hippocampus. Neuron. (2002) 33:325–40. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(02)00586-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

89. Winson J. Patterns of hippocampal theta rhythm in the freely moving rat. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. (1974) 36:291–301. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(74)90171-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: deep brain stimulation, temporal lobe epilepsy, theta oscillations, cognition, pilocarpine

Citation: Izadi A, Schedlbauer A, Ondek K, Disse G, Ekstrom AD, Cowen SL, Shahlaie K and Gurkoff GG (2021) Early Intervention via Stimulation of the Medial Septal Nucleus Improves Cognition and Alters Markers of Epileptogenesis in Pilocarpine-Induced Epilepsy. Front. Neurol. 12:708957. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2021.708957

Received: 13 May 2021; Accepted: 28 June 2021;
Published: 07 September 2021.

Edited by:

Dinesh Upadhya, Manipal Academy of Higher Education, India

Reviewed by:

Maxime Lévesque, McGill University, Canada
Luiz E. Mello, Federal University of São Paulo, Brazil

Copyright © 2021 Izadi, Schedlbauer, Ondek, Disse, Ekstrom, Cowen, Shahlaie and Gurkoff. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Gene G. Gurkoff, gggurkoff@ucdavis.edu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.