Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Neurol., 09 March 2021
Sec. Dementia and Neurodegenerative Diseases
This article is part of the Research Topic Non-invasive Brain Stimulation for Neurodegenerative Disorders: From Investigation to Therapeutic Application View all 9 articles

The Effect of Transcranial Random Noise Stimulation on Cognitive Training Outcome in Healthy Aging

\nMichela Brambilla,Michela Brambilla1,2Lars Dinkelbach,&#x;Lars Dinkelbach1,3Annelien Bigler,Annelien Bigler1,4Joseph Williams,Joseph Williams1,5Nahid ZokaeiNahid Zokaei1Roi Cohen KadoshRoi Cohen Kadosh1Anna-Katharine Brem,*&#x;Anna-Katharine Brem1,6*
  • 1Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, Oxford, United Kingdom
  • 2Biomedical and Clinical Sciences Department, Center for Research and Treatment on Cognitive Dysfunctions, “Luigi Sacco” Hospital, University of Milan, Milan, Italy
  • 3Department of Neurology, Institute of Clinical Neuroscience and Medical Psychology, Heinrich Heine University, Duesseldorf, Germany
  • 4Department of Experimental Psychology, Ghent University, Ghent, Belgium
  • 5Medical Sciences Division, University of Oxford, Oxford, United Kingdom
  • 6Department of Neurology, Berenson-Allen Center for Noninvasive Brain Stimulation and Division for Cognitive Neurology, Beth Israel Deaconess Medical Center and Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, United States

Background and Objective: Aging is associated with a decline in attentional and executive abilities, which are linked to physiological, structural, and functional brain changes. A variety of novel non-invasive brain stimulation methods have been probed in terms of their neuroenhancement efficacy in the last decade; one that holds significant promise is transcranial random noise stimulation (tRNS) that delivers an alternate current at random amplitude and frequency. The aim of this study was to investigate whether repeated sessions of tRNS applied as an add-on to cognitive training (CT) may induce long-term near and far transfer cognitive improvements.

Methods: In this sham-controlled, randomized, double-blinded study forty-two older adults (age range 60–86 years) were randomly assigned to one of three intervention groups that received 20 min of 0.705 mA tRNS (N = 14), 1 mA tRNS (N = 14), or sham tRNS (N = 19) combined with 30 min of CT of executive functions (cognitive flexibility, inhibitory control, working memory). tRNS was applied bilaterally over the dorsolateral prefrontal cortices for five sessions. The primary outcome (non-verbal logical reasoning) and other cognitive functions (attention, memory, executive functions) were assessed before and after the intervention and at a 1-month follow-up.

Results: Non-verbal logical reasoning, inhibitory control and reaction time improved significantly over time, but stimulation did not differentially affect this improvement. These changes occurred during CT, while no further improvement was observed during follow-up. Performance change in logical reasoning was significantly correlated with age in the group receiving 1 mA tRNS, indicating that older participants profited more from tRNS than younger participants. Performance change in non-verbal working memory was significantly correlated with age in the group receiving sham tRNS, indicating that in contrast to active tRNS, older participants in the sham group declined more than younger participants.

Interpretation: CT induced cognitive improvements in all treatment groups, but tRNS did not modulate most of these cognitive improvements. However, the effect of tRNS depended on age in some cognitive functions. We discuss possible explanations leading to this result that can help to improve the design of future neuroenhancement studies in older populations.

Introduction

“Cognitive health” is consistently cited as an essential factor contributing to functional ability and quality of late life (1, 2). Cognitive decline along with physiological, structural, and functional brain changes represents a crucial component of healthy aging (3, 4). The frontal lobes are particularly vulnerable to age-related deterioration, potentially explaining the most important changes in cognitive performance associated with normal aging; these changes primarily affect cognitive activities that require rapid information processing, such as working memory and other executive functions (5). Age-related decline is thought to be actively counteracted by the brain with compensatory processes, as can be seen, for example, in positive associations between the activation of bilateral frontal regions and cognitive performance (1, 6, 7). The frontal lobes have therefore been singled out as a potential target for early interventions to counteract age-related changes and maintain cognitive function (810).

Neuroenhancement describes the use of neuroscience-based techniques to enhance cognitive function, acting directly on the human cortex to alter its properties and increase performance for a specific cognitive task or a set of tasks (11). A wide variety of neuroenhancement methods have been developed in the last decade, one of them being transcranial electrical stimulation (tES). tES approaches are non-invasive neuromodulatory techniques that use the application of electrical current over the scalp, to facilitate or inhibit spontaneous neuronal activity resulting in altered brain functions. tES with its easy and safe application has the potential to serve as a cognitive enhancer in healthy older populations, as well as a therapeutic intervention to compensate for deficits in individuals with neurological and psychiatric conditions (1214). Recent meta-analyses and systematic reviews provide robust support for enhancing both cognitive and motor performance in healthy aging with direct current stimulation (tDCS) in single and multiple sessions (1517). A more recently developed type of tES is transcranial random noise stimulation (tRNS) that delivers an alternate current at random amplitude and frequency (18). Moliadze et al. showed that tRNS could elicit a more pronounced amplitude elevation of motor evoked potentials than tDCS (19). However, these results have not been universally supported, with others finding no clear difference between the effects of tDCS and tRNS on motor cortex excitability (20). tRNS might lead to more widespread effects via stochastic resonance (21) and the aging brain might react differently to tRNS due to the activation of broader networks (22). Age appears to be associated with more beneficial outcomes in neurostimulation in some studies (23, 24). One possible explanation is that lower performing individuals are thought to profit more from cognitive interventions (25) and neurostimulation (26) and show more consistent stimulation gains (27). However, other studies demonstrated that differences in education level may account for the differing effects of neurostimulation between age groups (28).

Since the pioneering study on CT, the CT ACTIVE trial in older adults by Ball et al. (29), several studies assessing intensive cognitive rehabilitation approaches targeting attention, information processing speed, memory, and executive function reported positive results in older adults. Most reported a behavioral improvement with “transfer” to other tasks and functional ability at the end of a training period; a few studies focusing on the neurophysiological changes underlying this improvement showed increased frontal brain activity after training (3032). However, recent meta-analyses indicate that there is a lack of clear-cut findings regarding the long-term efficacy and generalizability of such programs (3337). Recently, researchers have started to combine CT with neurostimulation. The rationale for combining approaches is derived from the assumption that combinatory approaches, such as neurostimulation and CT, are more effective in achieving long-lasting effects than each approach on its own. The application of tES alone does not lead to neuronal depolarization, but is thought to modulate the resting membrane potential either toward or away from the depolarization threshold. Within this framework, intrinsic fluctuations, e.g., resulting from a concomitant task, are more likely to lead to neuronal firing when the resting membrane potential is shifted toward the threshold via application of an external modulator (i.e., tES) (38). Through this mechanism neurostimulation is thought to enhance ongoing brain processes and therefore improve training-related effects (39).

To date, only few studies investigated the combined effect of repeated tDCS sessions combined with CT on healthy aging participants (4042). In the study by Meinzer et al. (40), subjects acquired a novel vocabulary over 5 consecutive days and received either tDCS over the left posterior temporo-parietal junction or sham tDCS. TDCS yielded steeper learning curves and enhanced learning success at the end of the training and at 1-week follow-up. Park et al. (41) assessed the effects of ten sessions of tDCS over the bilateral prefrontal cortices during CT on attention and executive functions in healthy older adults. Both verbal working memory and digit span forward improved significantly after CT combined with active tDCS vs. sham tDCS. In the study of Jones et al. (42) participants received ten sessions of sham or active (anodal, 1.5 mA) tDCS to the right prefrontal, parietal, or prefrontal/parietal (alternating) cortices and afterwards performed a short verbal and visual working memory training. All groups benefited from working memory training, however, at the 1-month follow-up only participants from the active tDCS groups maintained significant improvement for both trained and transfer tasks. The majority of studies report improvements on near transfer effects (i.e., improvements on tasks that are similar to the trained tasks). To our knowledge, to date only one study reported far transfer effects in older adults. Stephens and Berryhill (43) found far transfer effects on ecologically valid tasks, such as weekly calendar planning and driving knowledge, 1 month after a 5-day intervention involving working memory training and tDCS. Conversely, some studies reported no improvement after tDCS combined with CT (44, 45). Recent studies have attributed the effect of tRNS on cognitive training (CT) in young adults to modulation of neurophysiological mechanisms that are associated with sustained attention (46). Indeed, in children with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder, tRNS with CT over the dorsolateral prefrontal cortices has yielded promising clinical improvement compared to tDCS with CT (47). Moreover, a recent study in juvenile mice revealed that chronic tRNS over the prefrontal cortex, with similar parameters to what has been used in atypically developing children (48), reduced GABAergic activity (49). Single sessions of tRNS showed promise in modulating cognitive functions in aging (22, 28, 50). However, to date, the potential of tRNS as an effective cognitive enhancer in older adults remains to be shown. For example, in a recent study by Fertonani et al. (22), the authors investigated the neurophysiological mechanism underlying tRNS effects applied over the visual cortex in healthy elderly. They found that tRNS modulated visuo-perceptual learning in young, but not older healthy adults. Moreover, tRNS appears to have differential effects on young and older adults. Cappelletti et al. (50) compared tRNS over the motor and parietal cortex with sham stimulation during CT. While both age groups showed similar improvements in numerosity discrimination, these improvements were driven by different mechanisms and led to different transfer effects in the two age groups.

The aim of the present study was to investigate whether repeated sessions of bilateral tRNS applied at one of two different intensities (1 mA or 0.705 mA) over the dorsolateral prefrontal cortices applied as an add-on to executive function training induces immediate and long-term cognitive improvements compared to sham stimulation combined with executive function training. We hypothesized that active stimulation vs. sham would result in increased frontal connectivity during executive function training in older adults resulting in near and far transfer cognitive improvements.

Materials and Methods

Participants

Fifty healthy right-handed participants aged 60 or older were enrolled in the study of which forty-seven underwent all study measures (see Table 1 for demographic characteristics). Two subjects dropped out after baseline measures due to personal commitments. One person (receiving 1 mA tRNS) dropped out after the second intervention visit due to headache. Participants were recruited via the website www.joindementiaresearch.co.uk. Exclusion criteria were contraindication to brain stimulation (51), current history of drug or alcohol abuse or dependence, any past or present psychiatric or neurological disorder, and subjective cognitive complaint. Participants performed the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) (52, 53) and Beck Depression Inventory-II (BDI-II) (54) at the pre-test session to screen for cognitive impairment and depression; participants with a score lower than 24/30 in the MoCA (55) or with a score higher than 18/63 in the BDI-II were excluded from the study. All subjects received detailed information about the study protocol and voluntarily gave their written informed consent prior to study commencement. Participants were naive with respect to the experimental hypothesis and remained unaware of what type of stimulation they received. The study was approved by the ethics committee of the University of Oxford and performed in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. Participants were instructed to sleep at least 6 h each night, abstain from alcohol for the study duration, and refrain from caffeine for 1 h before study visits.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Demographic characteristics of individuals grouped according to experimental condition.

Study Design

The study design is depicted in Figure 1. In this randomized, sham-controlled, double blind trial, CT was combined with tRNS over five sessions. Sessions took place on consecutive workdays in a block of three (Wednesday, Thursday, Friday) and 2 days (Monday, Tuesday) and were therefore spread across 2 weeks. Participants were randomly assigned to one of three intervention groups: (1) 0.705 mA tRNS plus CT, N = 14; (2) 1 mA tRNS plus CT, N = 14; and (3) sham tRNS plus CT, N = 19. Notably, the original study design included two stimulation intervention groups (1 mA tRNS vs. sham tRNS combined with CT). However, due to a technical error five additional subjects received sham stimulation and a number of subjects were stimulated at a 30% lower intensity level. We therefore decided to adapt the original study design to include a third group receiving this lower stimulation intensity (0.705 mA). The research staff administering the intervention remained blinded. All participants underwent a neuropsychological and experimental assessment at baseline and again within 2 days after the intervention. We decided to test participants' cognitive performance not immediately after the last training session in order to avoid possible cognitive benefits due to immediate stimulation after-effects, or cognitive fatigue immediately after CT. One month later, participants were tested again in order to assess long-term effects of the intervention (Figure 1).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Experimental design and intervention. (A) Experimental Design. After baseline testing participants were randomized to receive executive function training combined with real tRNS (1 or 0.705 mA) or sham tRNS. Cognitive tests were repeated immediately after and 1 month after the intervention. (B) Cognitive training. Examples of single (left panel) and complex tasks (right panel). The middle panel depicts the intervention setup. (C) Modeling of the current flow. Stimulation was delivered over the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and stimulation onset coincided with training onset. Electric field calculations (component of the electric field orthogonal to the cortical surface En [V/m]) were performed using a realistic head model (56). Positive values indicate that the field is directed into the cortical surface. Note that the protocol only depicts one instance of stimulation given that polarity changes continually during tRNS.

Cognitive Training

A newly-developed CT program (Flexible, Adaptive, Synergistic Training, FAST, Simcoach Games, Pittsburgh, PA, USA) was used to train participants in a series of gamified isolated and integrated executive function tasks [for a detailed description see (57)]. CT (30 min per session) was presented on a laptop while subjects listened to custom-designed music and game-specific sounds via earphones. Participants were trained in a quiet laboratory environment. The training consisted of a series (15 blocks, each 2 min) of tasks, drawing on working memory (n-back tasks), inhibition, and cognitive flexibility. Each training block was preceded by instructions and followed by a performance feedback. The training was adaptive, i.e., whenever participants passed 80% of a training block, difficulty levels were increased. This way an individually adapted and constant level of challenge was ensured. When 50–80% corrects were reached, the difficulty level was maintained, while difficulty decreased when subjects reached <50% correct answers. The overall training started with exercises of single sub-functions (working memory or inhibition or cognitive flexibility) and advanced to combinations of two or all three sub-functions. Difficulty was furthermore increased by reducing the time available to respond, or by increasing the number of items, while changing training parameters in the opposite direction decreased difficulty.

Transcranial Random Noise Stimulation

Active and sham tRNS was delivered via gel-filled pi-electrodes (3.14 cm2) that were inserted into a neoprene cap (Starstim®, Neuroelectrics, Barcelona, Spain) with predefined localization of the anode (F3) and the cathode (F4). StimWeaver (Neuroelectrics, Barcelona, Spain) was used to optimize the stimulation montage according to the specified targets. Electric field calculations (component of the electric field orthogonal to the cortical surface En [V/m]) were performed using a realistic head model (56). Positive values indicate that the field is directed into the cortical surface. Note that the tRNS protocol only depicts one instance of stimulation given that polarity changes continually during stimulation (Figure 1). According to the 10–20 international EEG system these locations correspond to the left and right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. During each session, 20 min of high-frequency tRNS (100–640 Hz) at 1 or 0.705 mA or sham stimulation was delivered. During the latter, current was ramped up to a stimulation intensity of 1 mA and then down again over a period of 30 s. Stimulation onset coincided with training onset. Neither the subjects nor the study staff administering the training and testing of cognitive functions were aware about the nature of the stimulation.

Neuropsychological Assessment

The neuropsychological test battery included a range of standardized and experimental tasks that were completed in a fixed order, with an average duration of 60 min. All tasks are described below in detail. Trained staff, which was blinded to patient intervention allocations, administered the neuropsychological tests. The same assessor administered all assessments (baseline, post-test, and follow-up) for each single subject in order to minimize bias. Far transfer effects were assessed with a non-verbal logical reasoning task [Sandia (58), primary outcome] as well as attention and memory tasks. Near transfer effects were assessed with executive tasks that were dissimilar from the training tasks.

Inhibitory Control (False Alarms)

To assess changes in inhibitory control we used a computerized Go/Nogo task programmed in Matlab that was previously used (59). Participants were asked to respond to a centrally located target letter that appeared on a screen. Upon the presentation of the letters X (Go) or O (Nogo) they had to press the respective response keys as quickly as possible. The letters were white on a black background and the stimuli size was 0.7 by 0.7 cm. Each stimulus was displayed for 100 ms followed by a random intertrial interval between 1,000 and 2,000 ms. The stimuli were presented randomly in three blocks of 150 trials and consisted of 70% Go trials and 30% NoGo trials. Subjects were instructed to respond as fast as possible with the index finger of the dominant hand.

Working Memory

We used a computerized task (Matlab) to examine spatial working memory. The task measured precision, which provides a more sensitive measure of working memory capacity than traditional span tasks (60). The stimuli were randomly oriented colored bars located centrally on a gray background and always differed at least 10° in orientation from the preceding bar. In this 4-item (high-load) working memory task (Supplementary Figure 1) four bars of random color and orientation were presented. Before each block, participants were instructed to remember the orientation of all bars. After the four stimulus bars were presented, a probe bar of any of the previously presented colors was presented after a 500 ms delay. Participants were asked to rotate the probe bar to the orientation of the same-colored bar from memory. Before starting the main task, participants practiced (30 trials each). The main task included two 30-trial blocks, participants therefore completed 60 trials. For each item, recall performance was defined as the difference between the target and submitted angles (60). Precision was calculated as the circular standard deviation of the error response, with reduced variability showing greater overall precision. Precision is therefore given in 1/radian.

Attention (Reaction Time)

We assessed reaction time with a version of the attention network test (ANT). The details of this task are described elsewhere (61). Stimuli were presented via E-prime software on a 17″ monitor, alerting tones were presented through headphones.

Verbal Fluency (Semantic and Phonemic)

In this task, the subject was asked to produce orally as many words as possible within 2 min beginning with a specific letter (S, F, or A) or from a specific category (fruit, animals, or clothing). Conditions were randomized between visits.

Short-Term Memory and Working Memory

Digit span tasks (span length forward and backward) from the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised (62) as well as blocktapping forward and backward were used to assess verbal and visuospatial short-term memory and working memory.

Non-verbal Logical Reasoning

Sandia matrices overcome the issue of a limited number of stimuli by providing the option to choose from a pool of ~3,000 matrices, obtained through the combination of different stimulus features like shape, color and orientation (58). Experimental matrices were chosen from four different classes based on the type and number of analogical operations required for a correct solution (1-, 2-, 3- relations and logic matrices) and matched in terms of their difficulty for a set of three parallel test versions that were randomized across time-points. Participants were given four practice trials before starting that actual task that required them to solve as many matrices as possible within 15 min. This task measuring far transfer effects was defined as the primary outcome.

Questionnaires

An adapted version of the questionnaire developed by Jennett et al. (63) was administered after the last session of training to register as how pleasant, interesting, rewarding and meaningful the training was perceived. Its thirty-five items are scored on a scale between 0 and 7 (Table 3). Potential side effects, such as perception of itching, burning, etc., were assessed with a questionnaire. Blinding efficacy was determined by asking subjects at the end of the last training session to guess if they had received active or sham stimulation (i.e., “In your opinion, did you receive a real stimulation or a placebo stimulation?”).

Power Considerations and Data Analysis

Power calculations were performed with G*Power 3.1 (64). Assuming an effect size (Cohens'd) of f(V) = 0.6, suggesting a medium effect, a sample size of 42 was estimated to have a 85% power using repeated-measures ANOVA with a 0.05 two-sided significance level. Given that another study with a similar design using tDCS (43) showed a similar effect size, we believe that our estimate was justified. To identify relevant differences between groups in demographic characteristics, univariate ANOVAs were calculated for numerical data (age, MoCA, education). For gender and blinding efficacy, Chi-Square tests were calculated. To identify changes in neuropsychological measures due to neurostimulation, repeated measures ANOVAs for each outcome parameter with time (baseline, post-test, follow-up) as a within-subject and group (sham, 1, 0.705 mA) as a between-subjects variable were calculated and checked for interaction-effects. Violations of the assumption of sphericity where examined with the Mauchly's test and adjusted by the Greenhouse-Geisser correction if necessary. Analyses were conducted using IBM SPSS Statistics 26 (International Business Machines Corporation, Armonk, USA) and JASP.

Results

None of the recruited participants were excluded in the screening phase. At baseline, no significant differences of age, gender, education, or cognitive abilities as measured with the MoCA were observed (see Table 1).

Neuropsychological Assessment

The mean and standard deviation of all neuropsychological assessments for all three timepoints (baseline, post-test, follow-up) separated by groups (sham, 1, 0.705 mA) are being presented in Table 2. None of the interaction effects (time*group) reached the level of significance, no differential effect of neurostimulation on measures of the neuropsychological assessment was observed in the domains of Memory (verbal short-term memory [F(4,86) = 0.184, p = 0.946, ηp2 = 0.008]; non-verbal short-term memory [F(4,86) = 0.980, p = 0.423, ηp2 = 0.044], Executive Functions (phonemic fluency [F(4,88) = 0.428, p = 0.788, ηp2 = 0.019]; semantic fluency [F(4,84) = 1.245, p = 0.298, ηp2 = 0.056]; verbal working memory [F(4,86) = 0.575, p = 0.682, ηp2 = 0.026]; non-verbal working memory [F(4,86) = 0.397, p = 0.810, ηp2 = 0.018]; inhibitory control [F(3.09,64.95) = 1.075, p = 0.367, ηp2 = 0.049, adjusted by Greenhouse-Geisser]), the primary outcome Logical Reasoning (Sandia [F(4,76) = 0.457, p = 0.767, ηp2 = 0.023]), and Attention (reaction time [F(3.24,66.35) = 1.883, p = 0.137, ηp2 = 0.084], adjusted by Greenhouse-Geisser).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Neuropsychological outcomes at baseline, post-testing, and follow-up.

Over time, participants significantly improved in the Sandia test of logical reasoning [F(2,76) = 12.441, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.247], yielded faster reaction times in the attention network test [F(1.62,66.35) = 10.538, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.204, adjusted by Greenhouse-Geisser] and in inhibitory control [F(1.55,64.95) = 5.492, p = 0.011, ηp2 = 0.116, adjusted by Greenhouse-Geisser] (Figure 2). Post-hoc tests using the Tukey HSD test indicated that this was driven by changes during the intervention from baseline to post-test in all three measures [Sandia: p = 0.002, 95% CI (−3.71, −0.92); reaction time: p = 0.002, 95% CI (12.72, 50.96); inhibitory control: p = 0.015, 95% CI (0.005, 0.045)] while no significant changes were observed after the end of the intervention from the post-test to the follow-up test [Sandia: p = 0.158, 95% CI (−2.25, 0.38); reaction time: p = 0.663, 95% CI (−14.77, 9.50); inhibitory control: p = 0.680, 95% CI (−0.009, 0.014)].

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Cognitive performance over time. The main outcome measure non-verbal logical reasoning (Sandia) as well as inhibitory control (GoNogo false alarms) and reaction time (ANT) improved significantly over time across groups, but stimulation did not differentially affect this improvement. These improvements were driven by significant changes during the intervention (from baseline to post-test) in all three measures (mean and standard errors are shown).

To complement the null findings on the interaction between time and group on the different outcome measures, we performed Bayesian analyses using the software, JASP (65) with default priors. First, we ran a mixed Bayesian ANOVA with Time as within-subjects factor and Group as between-subjects factor. We found that the null model was preferred to the interaction between time and group model, as the Bayes factors (BF10) were smaller than 0.173 thus providing at least a moderate support for the null hypothesis. However, in two cases the interaction term was preferred over the null model (Sandia non-verbal logical reasoning and ANT reaction time). Notably, in both cases this was due to the inclusion of the main effect of time that was the best predictor, showing an improvement in performance over time. In other words, the inclusion of the interaction between time and group to the main effect of time, benefited from the variance explained by the factor time, and reduced the strength of this model. Overall, these analyses provided at least moderate evidence in favor of the null hypothesis (Supplementary Results).

In order to explore a possible influence of age, we entered this factor as a covariate. When entering age as a covariate, we found a significant interaction in the main outcome (Sandia) between time*stimulation [F(4,70) = 3.11, p = 0.021, ηp2 = 0.151] and between time*stimulation*age [F(4,70) = 3.03, p = 0.023, ηp2 = 0.148] fitting a linear function. At baseline, the Sandia was significantly correlated with age (r = −0.521, p < 0.001) indicating that older subjects showed lower performance in logical reasoning. The change from pre to post was marginally correlated with age in the group receiving 1 mA tRNS (r = 0.459, p = 0.099), while it was not significant in either the group receiving 0.705 mA tRNS (r = 0.432, p = 0.161) or sham (r = −0.216, p = 0.438) (Figure 3A). Similarly, the change from pre to follow-up was significantly correlated with age in the group receiving 1 mA tRNS (r = 0.709, p = 0.005), while it was not significant in either the group receiving 0.705 mA tRNS (r = 0.054, p = 0.868) or sham (r = −0.048, p = 0.866). The correlation in the 1 mA tRNS group was significantly stronger than in the sham group (p = 0.003) and marginally stronger than in the 0.705 tRNS group (p = 0.064) (Figure 3B).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Correlations between the change in Sandia performance and age in the three intervention groups. Age was associated with improvement in performance in the group receiving 1 mA tRNS, indicating that older participants profited significantly more from tRNS than younger participants. During the intervention (A) this association was marginal, while it was significant from pre to follow-up (B). The correlation in the group receiving 1 mA tRNS was significantly stronger than in the groups receiving 0.705 mA tRNS or sham stimulation.

We furthermore found a significant interaction in non-verbal working memory between time*stimulation [F(4,80) = 3.19, p = 0.018, ηp2 = 0.137] and time*stimulation*age [F(4,80) = 3.16, p = 0.018, ηp2 = 0.136]. This interaction fit a quadratic function indicating a decline from pre to post in all three groups, and improvement from post to follow-up only for the active tRNS conditions (see Table 2 and Supplementary Figure 2). The change from pre to post was significantly correlated with age in the group receiving sham tRNS (r = −0.635, p = 0.005), while it was not significant in either the group receiving 0.705 mA tRNS (r = −0.012, p = 0.968) or 1 mA tRNS (r = 0.181, p = 0.536). The correlation in the sham group was significantly different from the 1 mA tRNS group (p = 0.019) (Supplementary Figure 3A), indicating that in contrast to active tRNS, older participants in the sham group declined more than younger participants. The change from pre to follow-up was not significantly correlated with age in any of the groups (Supplementary Figure 3B).

Engagement Questionnaire

The overall self-reported commitment with the computer-based CT as declared in the engagement questionnaire (Table 3) can be considered as medium. Subjects reported an overall high effort in playing the game and had to invest a lot of cognitive effort to follow instructions. They perceived the game as challenging but did not become so involved that they wanted to speak to the game or felt emotionally attached to it. They nevertheless tried their best while forgetting about everyday concerns during the training and not being distracted by things that were ongoing in their surroundings. However, they did not reach a point where they were able to play the game effortlessly, i.e., to a degree where they were unaware of even using controls. Finally, they were not disappointed when the training was over.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Self-reported commitment with the CT.

Blinding and Side Effects

Apart from one participant who dropped out due to headache after the second intervention session (1 mA), the side effects questionnaire showed that all other participants tolerated the stimulation very well with only minor discomfort reported (i.e., itching and tingling comparable for all groups). The proportion of subjects who reported being stimulated actively did not differ by stimulation group χ(2,N=44)2 = 0.799, p = 0.740. Three subjects (all from the group receiving 0.705 mA) were undecided whether they received real or sham stimulation. Overall, only 40% of participants identified correctly which type of stimulation they received [sham: 47% (9/19), active 1 mA: 43% (6/14), active 0.705 mA: 36% (4/14)]. This response is below chance level; we can therefore assume that blinding was effective. Interestingly, while 40% of all participants received sham stimulation, 51% of all participants believed they received sham stimulation, indicating a global negative expectation toward the protocol they received. This finding is also in accordance with the fact that tRNS is perceived less strongly than tDCS (66).

Discussion

In the present study, conducted in a sample of healthy older adults, we found improvements in non-verbal logical reasoning (primary outcome measure to assess far transfer effects), inhibitory control and in reaction times (as measured with the ANT) in all intervention groups after 5 days of combined neurostimulation and CT targeting executive functions. These changes occurred during the intervention phase, while no further improvement was observed during follow-up. As all participants received CT, far transfer improvements in non-verbal logical reasoning could be due to both training effects (changes due to the treatment) as well as practice effects (changes due to repeated testing). Given that we used parallel tests to assess logical reasoning and did not find practice effects in any of the other cognitive tasks (even for tasks without parallel versions), training effects are likely to play the principal role. We moreover found improvements over time in inhibitory control, which indicates near transfer effects in this executive domain that was trained during the CT. No near transfer effects were found in working memory, an improvement, which we had expected to occur, given that we hypothesized that far transfer effects result from activity in common underlying brain networks. Neurostimulation (tRNS at 1 or 0.705 mA) did not significantly modulate other cognitive functions, although we found that age moderated the effects of tRNS on non-verbal working memory and logical reasoning over time. Importantly, the performance change in logical reasoning was significantly correlated with age in the group receiving 1 mA tRNS, indicating that older participants profited more from tRNS than younger participants. CT combined with tRNS, at least using the current of 1 mA, might be particularly effective in older adults. This effect could be explained via its main mechanism, stochastic resonance, which can mitigate the age-dependent increase in neuronal noise (50). However, other studies have suggested that tRNS might be less effective in older than younger adults (22). We used a scientifically rigorous double-blind, sham-controlled, randomized study design to investigate the questions put forth with as minimal experimental bias as possible and confirmed successful blinding of the subjects. However, several drawbacks likely contributed to the rejection of our main hypothesis. We discuss possible reasons leading to these findings to help improving the design of future tES studies and encourage researchers to consider specific requirements for the aging population. Table 4 contains a graphical representation of the possible factors that might be responsible for our findings.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Possible explanations and solutions for future trials.

General Reason

We might not have been able to detect the effect of active vs. sham brain stimulation in combination with CT due to the large inter-individual variation in the outcome of neuroenhancement interventions (67, 68), an effect which might be even larger in older adults due to the larger variability in age-related structural and functional brain changes.

Reasons Related to the Sample

Our sample consisted of a very highly educated population with an average education duration of over 16 years that was enrolled from the Oxford area via the website www.joindementiaresearch.co.uk. We assume that this population likely operated at a high cognitive level, which could be associated with ceiling effects for both CT and stimulation effects, possibly limits the range for cognitive improvement after training (2527) and is most likely not representative of the general older adult population of the UK. Moreover, a trend difference in baseline cognitive ability (as assessed with the MoCA) (Table 1), might have contributed to the non-significant effect of stimulation. Higher baseline performance is thought to be associated with less profit from cognitive interventions (25) and neurostimulation (26) and to induce less consistent stimulation gains (27).

Reasons Related to the Gamification of the Training

A potential critical issue may have been the medium enjoyment of the training, as reported in the engagement questionnaire. The design and setting of the CT (a robot factory), originally developed for the use in young people, might not have been as appealing for older people, leading to task disengagement (69). Gamified cognitive tasks are supposed to be more motivating than traditional tasks, thus making the participant experience less effortful and potentially reducing drop-outs in longitudinal studies (70, 71). However, it was found that game mechanics might specifically reduce participant motivation in low-performing subjects, for example by presenting negative feedback, i.e., by visualizing low performance, especially when only a single player is present. This might lead to more de-motivation and/or frustration in elderly populations (71). Participants in the current study perceived the game as challenging and reported an overall high effort to succeed with the tasks and follow the instructions, which possibly prevented them from becoming entirely involved and immersed in the game.

Reasons Related to Cognitive Testing

The high level of education and baseline performance might have led to ceiling effects in some of the tasks. However, some of the cognitive tests, particularly computerized tasks that require prolonged practice time, might have partly been too challenging even for this population. For older adults that are not used to using a computer and a mouse this possibly represented a challenge per se, despite providing ample practice time before each task.

Reasons Related to Stimulation

The null effects might also be due to the stimulation protocol and setup used. For older adults an intensity of ≤ 1 mA might not be sufficient to modulate DLPFC activity given that the stimulated brain areas contain more cerebrospinal liquid resulting in a reduced current intensity in the brain and a possible change in the conduction of current vectors (72). Another possible explanation for the negative finding in our study could be the size of the electrodes (3.14 cm2, round electrodes); the behavioral effects of tES appear to be also critically dependent on the position of the return electrode. Our study used smaller active electrodes with a circular surface compared to many previous studies that stimulated the same area of interest using tES with larger or non-circular stimulation electrodes. Moreover, five stimulation sessions might have been insufficient to open a window of plasticity and trigger neurophysiological changes resulting in cognitive enhancement. Indeed, a recent study investigating combined tDCS and decision-making training over 5 days in older adults also failed to find a significant effect (73). In addition, our stimulation sessions were spread across 2 weeks (blocks of 3 and 2 days) and did not take place on 5 consecutive days. This may have affected tRNS efficacy.

Neurobiological Reasons

The neurobiological mechanisms underlying tES effects are complex and involve changes at multiple levels, ranging from the cellular level to the modulation of intrinsic network dynamics across the brain. Functional reorganization due to aging, particularly in frontal areas of the brain, may partly explain why tES effects are generally variable across studies. Frontal regions are core hubs in working memory processing (74) and are known to be subjected to age-related changes in activation patterns as described in the PASA (posterior-anterior shift in aging) model (75). This model postulates that prefrontal over-activity might be a compensatory effort to mask age-related decline. Dependent on the stage of age-related changes, tES applied over frontal areas could therefore be used to support the primary activation pattern or compensatory processes. However, the rather large variability in response to brain stimulation is also increasingly investigated and discussed in young healthy populations (76, 77). Recent studies demonstrated that by using a combination of neurophysiological and neuroimaging methods with neurostimulation we can shed light on interindividual mechanisms that drive response variability (16, 22, 24, 46, 78, 79) and use this knowledge to improve stimulation efficacy (80). Associated with this, we also need to reconsider the practice of investigating questions in another than the target population. The simplistic view that similar protocols for brain-based interventions, which are effective in treating impaired patients or modulate functions in young healthy student populations, can be used to enhance the cognitive performance of healthy older subjects or to prevent future cognitive disorders, cannot be held up.

Finally, we would like to draw attention to ethical concerns when applying stimulation to improve functions in healthy subjects. The current study covered a broad range of cognitive functions besides the executive functions that were targeted with the training. As previously suggested (81), and demonstrated (82, 83), enhancement through NIBS might be a zero-sum proposition, where the enhancement of a cognitive or motor function comes at a cost of other abilities. The possibility that enhancing some cognitive or motor abilities in healthy older individuals could be at the expense of other abilities has clear ethical implications and is an important issue in need of systematic evaluation (84). We attempted to address this issue in the cognitive domain, but did not assess cross-domain functions (e.g., motor functions).

Notably, in this research area we find a strong association between significant results and publication: studies applying brain stimulation reporting positive or significant results are more likely to be published, and have mostly not been pre-registered. The pre-registration of the experimental design and analysis aims to counteract publication bias by anticipating the review process before the data is collected, in order to have reviewers focus on the importance of the research questions without looking at the findings ahead of time (85). Moreover, to date, funding is often not sufficient to investigate full-factorial designs and longer follow up visits.

To conclude, study protocols should acknowledge the differences between target populations, need to be specifically designed to fit the motivational preferences and baseline abilities, and take into account neurophysiological and anatomical features of the target population.

We believe that research on combined tES and CT remains a fundamentally important goal to counteract age-related cognitive decline with non-invasive means. Studies should compare more established stimulation protocols, such as tDCS, with novel protocols in order to clarify factors that determine tES responsiveness in aging and apply longer trainings. We would like to encourage future studies to assess larger sample sizes with training tasks designed in accordance with specific preferences for older people as well as underlying neurophysiological parameters to systematically expand our knowledge in this area to understand the neural mechanism that induce cognitive preservation and enhancement in the elderly.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Ethics committee of the University of Oxford. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

A-KB and RCK designed the study. MB, LD, JW, AB, and A-KB collected the data. MB, RCK, NZ, and A-KB analyzed the data. MB, LD, and JW prepared the initial draft report. A-KB edited the report, which was critically reviewed by all authors.

Funding

This research was supported by a grant from the James S. McDonnell Foundation 21st Century Science Initiative in Understanding Human Cognition.

Conflict of Interest

RC serves on the scientific advisory boards for Neuroelectrics and Innosphere and filed a UK Patent Application Number 2000874.4 (“method for obtaining personalized parameters for transcranial stimulation, transcranial system, method of applying transcranial stimulation”). This patent does not relate to this work.

The remaining authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We wish to thank all participants that took part in the study and www.joindementiaresearch.co.uk for providing us the opportunity to recruit through their website. We thank Luis Fuentes for providing us the ANT task. FAST (RobotFactory) was implemented by Simcoach Games under the scope of a research effort led by Honeywell International. The effort was funded by the IARPA SHARP program under contract 2014-13121700007. FAST (RobotFactory) was created with scientific and technical inputs from researchers at Honeywell Labs, Harvard Medical School, Northeastern University, and Oxford University.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fneur.2021.625359/full#supplementary-material

References

1. Eyler LT, Sherzai A, Kaup AR, Jeste DV. A review of functional brain imaging correlates of successful cognitive aging. Biol Psychiatry. (2011) 70:115–22. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.12.032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Kaup AR, Mirzakhanian H, Jeste DV, Eyler LT. A review of the brain structure correlates of successful cognitive aging. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. (2011) 23:6–15. doi: 10.1176/jnp.23.1.jnp6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Raz N, Lindenberger U, Rodrigue KM, Kennedy KM, Head D, Williamson A, et al. Regional brain changes in aging healthy adults: general trends, individual differences and modifiers. Cereb Cortex. (2005) 15:1676–89. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhi044

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Salthouse TA. Selective review of cognitive aging. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. (2010) 16:754–60. doi: 10.1017/S1355617710000706

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Moscovitch M, Winocur G. Frontal lobes, memory, and aging. Ann N Y Acad Sci. (1995) 769:119–50. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1995.tb38135.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Bangen KJ, Kaup AR, Mirzakhanian H, Wierenga CE, Jeste DV, Eyler LT. Compensatory brain activity during encoding among older adults with better recognition memory for face-name pairs: an integrative functional, structural, and perfusion imaging study. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. (2012) 18:402–13. doi: 10.1017/S1355617712000197

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Rodríguez-Aranda C, Sundet K. The frontal hypothesis of cognitive aging: factor structure and age effects on four frontal tests among healthy individuals. J Genet Psychol. (2006) 167:269–87. doi: 10.3200/GNTP.167.3.269-287

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Greenwood PM. The frontal aging hypothesis evaluated. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. (2000) 6:705–26. doi: 10.1017/s1355617700666092

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Franzmeier N, Hartmann JC, Taylor ANW, Araque Caballero MÁ, Simon-Vermot L, Buerger K, et al. Left frontal hub connectivity during memory performance supports reserve in aging and mild cognitive impairment. J Alzheimers Dis. (2017) 59:1381–92. doi: 10.3233/JAD-170360

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Gawron N, Łojek E, Kijanowska-Haładyna B, Nestorowicz J, Harasim A, Pluta A, et al. Cognitive patterns of normal elderly subjects are consistent with frontal cortico-subcortical and fronto-parietal neuropsychological models of brain aging. Appl Neuropsychol Adult. (2014) 21:195–209. doi: 10.1080/09084282.2013.789965

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Clark VP, Parasuraman R. Neuroenhancement: enhancing brain and mind in health and in disease. Neuroimage. (2014) 85:889–94. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.08.071

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Dayan E, Censor N, Buch ER, Sandrini M, Cohen LG. Noninvasive brain stimulation: from physiology to network dynamics and back. Nat Neurosci. (2013) 16:838–44. doi: 10.1038/nn.3422

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Brunoni AR, Nitsche MA, Bolognini N, Bikson M, Wagner T, Merabet L, et al. Clinical research with transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS): challenges and future directions. Brain Stimul. (2012) 5:175–95. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2011.03.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Habich A, Fehér KD, Antonenko D, Boraxbekk CJ, Flöel A, Nissen C, et al. Stimulating aged brains with transcranial direct current stimulation: opportunities and challenges. Psychiatry Res Neuroimag. (2020) 306:111179. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2020.111179

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Elmasry J, Loo C, Martin D. A systematic review of transcranial electrical stimulation combined with cognitive training. Restor Neurol Neurosci. (2015) 33:263–78. doi: 10.3233/RNN-140473

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Perceval G, Flöel A, Meinzer M. Can transcranial direct current stimulation counteract age-associated functional impairment? Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2016) 65:157–72. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.03.028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Summers JJ, Kang N, Cauraugh JH. Does transcranial direct current stimulation enhance cognitive and motor functions in the ageing brain? A systematic review and meta-analysis. Ageing Res Rev. (2016) 25:42–54. doi: 10.1016/j.arr.2015.11.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Antal A, Herrmann CS. Transcranial alternating current and random noise stimulation: possible mechanisms. Neural Plast. (2016) 2016:3616807. doi: 10.1155/2016/3616807

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Moliadze V, Fritzsche G, Antal A. Comparing the efficacy of excitatory transcranial stimulation methods measuring motor evoked potentials. Neural Plast. (2014) 2014:837141. doi: 10.1155/2014/837141

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Ho KA, Taylor JL, Loo CK. Comparison of the effects of transcranial random noise stimulation and transcranial direct current stimulation on motor cortical excitability. J ECT. (2015) 31:67–72. doi: 10.1097/YCT.0000000000000155

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Curatolo M, La Bianca G, Cosentino G, Baschi R, Salemi G, Talotta R, et al. Motor cortex tRNS improves pain, affective and cognitive impairment in patients with fibromyalgia: preliminary results of a randomised sham-controlled trial. Clin Exp Rheumatol. (2017) 35:100–5.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

22. Fertonani A, Pirulli C, Bollini A, Miniussi C, Bortoletto M. Age-related changes in cortical connectivity influence the neuromodulatory effects of transcranial electrical stimulation. Neurobiol Aging. (2019) 82:77–87. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2019.07.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Manenti R, Brambilla M, Petesi M, Ferrari C, Cotelli M. Enhancing verbal episodic memory in older and young subjects after non-invasive brain stimulation. Front Aging Neurosci. (2013) 5:49. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2013.00049

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Gutchess A. Plasticity of the aging brain: new directions in cognitive neuroscience. Science. (2014) 346:579–82. doi: 10.1126/science.1254604

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Binder JC, Martin M, Zöllig J, Röcke C, Mérillat S, Eschen A, et al. Multi-domain training enhances attentional control. Psychol Aging. (2016) 31:390–408. doi: 10.1037/pag0000081

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Habich A, Klöppel S, Abdulkadir A, Scheller E, Nissen C, Peter J. Anodal tDCS enhances verbal episodic memory in initially low performers. Front Hum Neurosci. (2017) 11:542. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2017.00542

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Hsu WY, Ku Y, Zanto TP, Gazzaley A. Effects of noninvasive brain stimulation on cognitive function in healthy aging and Alzheimer's disease: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Neurobiol Aging. (2015) 36:2348–59. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2015.04.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Mammarella N, Di Domenico A, Palumbo R, Fairfield B. Self-generation and positivity effects following transcranial random noise stimulation in medial prefrontal cortex: a reality monitoring task in older adults. Cortex. (2017) 91:186–96. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2016.11.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Ball K, Berch DB, Helmers KF, Jobe JB, Leveck MD, Marsiske M, et al. Effects of cognitive training interventions with older adults: a randomized controlled trial. JAMA. (2002) 288:2271–81. doi: 10.1001/jama.288.18.2271

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Joubert C, Chainay H. Aging brain: the effect of combined cognitive and physical training on cognition as compared to cognitive and physical training alone–a systematic review. Clin Interv Aging. (2018) 13:1267–301. doi: 10.2147/CIA.S165399

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Bamidis PD, Vivas AB, Styliadis C, Frantzidis C, Klados M, Schlee W, et al. A review of physical and cognitive interventions in aging. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2014) 44:206–20. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.03.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Rebok GW, Ball K, Guey LT, Jones RN, Kim HY, King JW, et al. Ten-year effects of the advanced cognitive training for independent and vital elderly cognitive training trial on cognition and everyday functioning in older adults. J Am Geriatr Soc. (2014) 62:16–24. doi: 10.1111/jgs.12607

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Lampit A, Valenzuela M, Gates NJ. Computerized cognitive training is beneficial for older adults. J Am Geriatr Soc. (2015) 63:2610–2. doi: 10.1111/jgs.13825

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Lampit A, Hallock H, Valenzuela M. Computerized cognitive training in cognitively healthy older adults: a systematic review and meta-analysis of effect modifiers. PLoS Med. (2014) 11:e1001756. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.1001756

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Papp KV, Walsh SJ, Snyder PJ. Immediate and delayed effects of cognitive interventions in healthy elderly: a review of current literature and future directions. Alzheimers Dement. (2009) 5:50–60. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2008.10.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Kelly ME, Loughrey D, Lawlor BA, Robertson IH, Walsh C, Brennan S. The impact of cognitive training and mental stimulation on cognitive and everyday functioning of healthy older adults: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Ageing Res Rev. (2014) 15:28–43. doi: 10.1016/j.arr.2014.02.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Gates NJ, Rutjes AW, Di Nisio M, Karim S, Chong L-Y, March E, et al. Computerised cognitive training for 12 or more weeks for maintaining cognitive function in cognitively healthy people in late life. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. (2020) 2:CD012277. doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD012277.pub3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Philip NS, Nelson BG, Frohlich F, Lim KO, Widge AS, Carpenter LL. Low-intensity transcranial current stimulation in psychiatry. Am J Psychiatry. (2017) 174:628–39. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2017.16090996

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Brem AK, Sensi SL. Towards combinatorial approaches for preserving cognitive fitness in aging. Trends Neurosci. (2018) 41:885–97. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2018.09.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Meinzer M, Jähnigen S, Copland DA, Darkow R, Grittner U, Avirame K, et al. Transcranial direct current stimulation over multiple days improves learning and maintenance of a novel vocabulary. Cortex. (2014) 50:137–47. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2013.07.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Park SH, Seo JH, Kim YH, Ko MH. Long-term effects of transcranial direct current stimulation combined with computer-assisted cognitive training in healthy older adults. Neuroreport. (2014) 25:122–6. doi: 10.1097/WNR.0000000000000080

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Jones KT, Stephens JA, Alam M, Bikson M, Berryhill ME. Longitudinal neurostimulation in older adults improves working memory. PLoS ONE. (2015) 10:e0121904. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0121904

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Stephens JA, Berryhill ME. Older adults improve on everyday tasks after working memory training and neurostimulation. Brain Stimul. (2016) 9:553–9. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2016.04.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Külzow N, Cavalcanti de Sousa AV, Cesarz M, Hanke JM, Günsberg A, Harder S, et al. No effects of non-invasive brain stimulation on multiple sessions of object-location-memory training in healthy older adults. Front Neurosci. (2017) 11:746. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2017.00746

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Nilsson J, Lebedev AV, Rydström A, Lövdén M. Direct-current stimulation does little to improve the outcome of working memory training in older adults. Psychol Sci. (2017) 28:907–20. doi: 10.1177/0956797617698139

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Sheffield JG, Raz G, Sella F, Kadosh RC. How can noise alter neurophysiology in order to improve human behaviour? A combined tRNS and EEG study. bioRxiv. (2020) 2020.01.09.900118. doi: 10.1101/2020.01.09.900118

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Berger I, Dakwar-Kawar O, Grossman ES, Nahum M, Kadosh RC. Scaffolding the attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder brain using random noise stimulation. medRxiv. (2020) 19005983. doi: 10.1101/19005983

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Looi CY, Lim J, Sella F, Lolliot S, Duta M, Avramenko AA, et al. Transcranial random noise stimulation and cognitive training to improve learning and cognition of the atypically developing brain: a pilot study. Sci Rep. (2017) 7:4633. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-04649-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Sanchez-Leon CA, Sanchez-Lopez A, Gomez-Climent MA, Cordones I, Cohen Kadosh R, Marquez-Ruiz J. Impact of chronic transcranial random noise stimulation (tRNS) on GABAergic and glutamatergic activity markers in the prefrontal cortex of juvenile mice. Prog Brain Res. (in press).

Google Scholar

50. Cappelletti M, Pikkat H, Upstill E, Speekenbrink M, Walsh V. Learning to integrate versus inhibiting information is modulated by age. J Neurosci. (2015) 35:2213–25. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1018-14.2015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Bikson M, Grossman P, Thomas C, Zannou AL, Jiang J, Adnan T, et al. Safety of transcranial direct current stimulation: evidence based update 2016. Brain Stimul. (2016) 9:641–61. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2016.06.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Nasreddine ZS, Phillips NA, Bédirian V, Charbonneau S, Whitehead V, Collin I, et al. The Montreal Cognitive Assessment, MoCA: a brief screening tool for mild cognitive impairment. J Am Geriatr Soc. (2005) 53:695–9. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2005.53221.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Nasreddine ZS, Phillips N, Chertkow H. Normative data for the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) in a population-based sample. Neurology. (2012) 78:765–6; author reply 766. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000413072.54070.a3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Beck AT, Steer RA, Ball R, Ranieri W. Comparison of Beck Depression Inventories-IA and -II in psychiatric outpatients. J Pers Assess. (1996) 67:588–97. doi: 10.1207/s15327752jpa6703_13

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Luis CA, Keegan AP, Mullan M. Cross validation of the Montreal Cognitive Assessment in community dwelling older adults residing in the Southeastern US. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. (2009) 24:197–201. doi: 10.1002/gps.2101

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Miranda PC, Mekonnen A, Salvador R, Ruffini G. The electric field in the cortex during transcranial current stimulation. Neuroimage. (2013) 70:48–58. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.12.034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Almquist JNF, Mathan S, Brem AK, Plessow F, McKanna J, Santarnecchi E, et al. FAST: a novel, executive function-based approach to cognitive enhancement. Front Hum Neurosci. (2019) 13:235. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2019.00235

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Matzen LE, Benz ZO, Dixon KR, Posey J, Kroger JK, Speed AE. Recreating Raven's: software for systematically generating large numbers of Raven-like matrix problems with normed properties. Behav Res Methods. (2010) 42:525–41. doi: 10.3758/BRM.42.2.525

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Spronk DB, De Bruijn ERA, van Wel JHP, Ramaekers JG, Verkes RJ. Acute effects of cocaine and cannabis on response inhibition in humans: an ERP investigation. Addict Biol. (2016) 21:1186–98. doi: 10.1111/adb.12274

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Zokaei N, Burnett Heyes S, Gorgoraptis N, Budhdeo S, Husain M. Working memory recall precision is a more sensitive index than span. J Neuropsychol. (2015) 9:319–29. doi: 10.1111/jnp.12052

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Fuentes LJ, Campoy G. The time course of alerting effect over orienting in the attention network test. Exp Brain Res. (2008) 185:667–72. doi: 10.1007/s00221-007-1193-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Wechsler D. WAIS-IV Administration and Scoring Manual. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation (2008).

63. Jennett C, Cox AL, Cairns P, Dhoparee S, Epps A, Tijs T, et al. Measuring and defining the experience of immersion in games. Int J Hum Comput Stud. (2008) 66:641–61. doi: 10.1016/j.ijhcs.2008.04.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Faul F, Erdfelder E, Buchner A, Lang AG. Statistical power analyses using G*Power 3.1: tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behav Res Methods. (2009) 41:1149–60. doi: 10.3758/BRM.41.4.1149

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. JASP Team. JASP (Version 0.14) [Computer Software]. Amsterdam: JASP Team (2020).

66. Ambrus GG, Paulus W, Antal A. Cutaneous perception thresholds of electrical stimulation methods: comparison of tDCS and tRNS. Clin Neurophysiol. (2010) 121:1908–14. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2010.04.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Benwell CSY, Learmonth G, Miniussi C, Harvey M, Thut G. Non-linear effects of transcranial direct current stimulation as a function of individual baseline performance: evidence from biparietal tDCS influence on lateralized attention bias. Cortex. (2015) 69:152–65. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2015.05.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Pellegrini M, Zoghi M, Jaberzadeh S. Biological and anatomical factors influencing interindividual variability to noninvasive brain stimulation of the primary motor cortex: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Rev Neurosci. (2018) 29:199–222. doi: 10.1515/revneuro-2017-0048

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Jones KT, Gözenman F, Berryhill ME. The strategy and motivational influences on the beneficial effect of neurostimulation: a tDCS and fNIRS study. Neuroimage. (2015) 105:238–47. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.11.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Looi CY, Duta M, Brem A-K, Huber S, Nuerk H-C, Cohen Kadosh R. Combining brain stimulation and video game to promote long-term transfer of learning and cognitive enhancement. Sci Rep. (2016) 6:22003. doi: 10.1038/srep22003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text

71. Lumsden J, Edwards EA, Lawrence NS, Coyle D, Munafò MR. Gamification of cognitive assessment and cognitive training: a systematic review of applications and efficacy. JMIR Serious Games. (2016) 4:e11. doi: 10.2196/games.5888

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

72. Mahdavi S, Towhidkhah F, Alzheimer's Disease Neuroimaging Initiative. Computational human head models of tDCS: influence of brain atrophy on current density distribution. Brain Stimul. (2018) 11:104–7. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2017.09.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

73. Horne KS, Filmer HL, Nott ZE, Hawi Z, Pugsley K, Mattingley JB, et al. Evidence against benefits from cognitive training and transcranial direct current stimulation in healthy older adults. Nat Hum Behav. (2020) 15:146–58. doi: 10.1038/s41562-020-00979-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

74. Fakhri M, Sikaroodi H, Maleki F, Ali Oghabian M, Ghanaati H. Age-related frontal hyperactivation observed across different working memory tasks: an fMRI study. Behav Neurol. (2012) 25:351–61. doi: 10.3233/BEN-2012-120280

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

75. Davis SW, Dennis NA, Daselaar SM, Fleck MS, Cabeza R. Que PASA? The posterior-anterior shift in aging. Cereb Cortex. (2008) 18:1201–9. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhm155

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

76. Evans C, Banissy MJ, Charlton RA. The efficacy of transcranial random noise stimulation (tRNS) on mood may depend on individual differences including age and trait mood. Clin Neurophysiol. (2018) 129:1201–8. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2018.03.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

77. López-Alonso V, Cheeran B, Río-Rodríguez D, Fernández-Del-Olmo M. Inter-individual variability in response to non-invasive brain stimulation paradigms. Brain Stimul. (2014) 7:372–80. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2014.02.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

78. Harty S, Cohen Kadosh R. Suboptimal engagement of high-level cortical regions predicts random-noise-related gains in sustained attention. Psychol Sci. (2019) 30:1318–32. doi: 10.1177/0956797619856658

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

79. Yang LZ, Zhang W, Wang W, Yang Z, Wang H, Deng ZD, et al. Neural and psychological predictors of cognitive enhancement and impairment from neurostimulation. Adv Sci (Weinh). (2020) 7:1902863. doi: 10.1002/advs.201902863

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

80. Gomes-Osman J, Indahlastari A, Fried PJ, Cabral DLF, Rice J, Nissim NR, et al. Non-invasive brain stimulation: probing intracortical circuits and improving cognition in the aging brain. Front Aging Neurosci. (2018) 10:177. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2018.00177

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

81. Brem AK, Fried PJ, Horvath JC, Robertson EM, Pascual-Leone A. Is neuroenhancement by noninvasive brain stimulation a net zero-sum proposition? Neuroimage. (2014) 85:1058–68. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.07.038

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

82. Bakulin I, Zabirova A, Lagoda D, Poydasheva A, Cherkasova A, Pavlov N, et al. Combining HF rTMS over the left DLPFC with concurrent cognitive activity for the offline modulation of working memory in healthy volunteers: a proof-of-concept study. Brain Sci. (2020) 10:83. doi: 10.3390/brainsci10020083

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

83. Iuculano T, Cohen Kadosh R. The mental cost of cognitive enhancement. J Neurosci. (2013) 33:4482–6. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4927-12.2013

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

84. Hamilton R, Messing S, Chatterjee A. Rethinking the thinking cap: ethics of neural enhancement using noninvasive brain stimulation. Neurology. (2011) 76:187–93. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e318205d50d

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

85. Dwan K, Gamble C, Williamson PR, Kirkham JJ, Reporting Bias Group. Systematic review of the empirical evidence of study publication bias and outcome reporting bias–an updated review. PLoS ONE. (2013) 8:e66844. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0066844

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: healthy aging, transcranial random noise stimulation, cognitive training, cognitive enhancement, executive functions

Citation: Brambilla M, Dinkelbach L, Bigler A, Williams J, Zokaei N, Cohen Kadosh R and Brem A-K (2021) The Effect of Transcranial Random Noise Stimulation on Cognitive Training Outcome in Healthy Aging. Front. Neurol. 12:625359. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2021.625359

Received: 02 November 2020; Accepted: 09 February 2021;
Published: 09 March 2021.

Edited by:

Francesco Di Lorenzo, Santa Lucia Foundation (IRCCS), Italy

Reviewed by:

Domenica Veniero, University of Nottingham, United Kingdom
Chiara Bagattini, Centro San Giovanni di Dio Fatebenefratelli (IRCCS), Italy

Copyright © 2021 Brambilla, Dinkelbach, Bigler, Williams, Zokaei, Cohen Kadosh and Brem. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Anna-Katharine Brem, katy.brem@gmail.com

Present address: Lars Dinkelbach, Department of Pediatric Oncology, Hematology and Clinical Immunology, Medical Faculty, Centre for Child and Adolescent Health, Heinrich Heine University, Duesseldorf, Germany
Anna-Katharine Brem, University Hospital of Old Age Psychiatry, University of Bern, Bern, Switzerland; Department of Neuropsychology, Lucerne Psychiatry, Lucerne, Switzerland

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.