- 1Graduate Program of Medicine (Neurology/Neurosciences), Federal Fluminense University, Niterói, Brazil
- 2Graduate Program of Health Sciences, Montes Claros State University, Montes Claros, Brazil
- 3Universitary Institute of Maia, Maia, Portugal
- 4Norwegian Advisory Unit for Ageing and Health, Vestfold Hospital Trust, Tønsberg, Norway
Background: Spatial navigation is a prodromal dementia marker. Exercise used alongside virtual reality improves many cognitive functions, but effects on spatial navigation are still unclear.
Objective: To investigate the effect of virtual reality-based physical exercise with 2D exergames on spatial navigation in institutionalized non-robust older persons.
Method: A total of 14 older persons (aged ≧ 60) were randomly allocated to the exergame (EG) and active control (ACG) groups. EG performed exercises with 2D exergames, while the ACG used the same movements as the EG, but without the use of virtual reality. Spatial navigation was assessed through the Floor Maze Test, where the immediate maze time (IMT) and delayed maze time (DMT) were recorded.
Results: Spatial navigation was enhanced in EG participants compared to ACG individuals. A significant (p = 0.01) IMT reduction between groups was observed, while DMT time without prior planning was significantly different at the significance threshold (p = 0.07).
Conclusions: Virtual reality-based exercise improves the spatial navigation of institutionalized non-robust older persons. This study should be replicated to confirm the findings reported herein.
Clinical Trial Registration: This study was registered in the Brazilian Registry of Clinical Trials (Protocol RBR-8dv3kg - https://ensaiosclinicos.gov.br/rg/RBR-8dv3kg).
Introduction
The development of chronic diseases in older persons is common, harming their functional capacity and resulting in loss of autonomy (1). Independence reduction is the main reason why older persons dwell in long-term care institutions (LTCIs) (2).
In Brazil, over half of institutionalized older persons have dementia (3), which may occur after institutionalization (4). Spatial navigation is among the cognitive functions that deteriorate in dementia cases and compromises the older person's ability of locomotion (5), which in turn is associated with prodromal dementia (6). Spatial navigation is defined as the ability to integrate cognitive processes and sensorial systems, especially the visual system, into environment data processing and body positioning during spatial displacement (5).
An interesting method used to stimulate spatial navigation ability is immersive virtual reality-based navigation training (VR) (7), which involves the activation of brain zones associated with cognitive processes, such as the hippocampus, caudate nucleus, and frontal cortex (8). However, the use of VR immersive systems is complex and expensive, and difficult to apply in older persons living in LTCIs. Previous studies have indicated that active video games associating two-dimensional virtual environment digital games with physical exercise (2D exergames) improve short-term memory, executive functions, sensorimotor integration, and mobility in older individuals (9, 10). To date, however, no robust evidence that 2D exergames might alter spatial navigation in older individuals is available. In this context, this study aimed to analyze the effect of 2D exergame training on the spatial navigation in institutionalized non-robust older individuals.
Methods
Trial Design
This trial comprised a controlled pilot study with blind randomization of two parallel groups. Consolidated Standards of Reporting Trials (CONSORT) were met. The study protocol was registered in the Brazilian Registration of Clinical Trials (ReBEC), under alphanumeric code number RBR-8dv3kg (https://ensaiosclinicos.gov.br/rg/RBR-8dv3kg).
Participants
The sample was comprised of older persons inhabiting four LTCIs in the Brazilian cities of Montes Claros/MG and Rio de Janeiro/RJ. Both men and women were recruited, aged 60+, totaling 186 eligible participants. The inclusion criteria were as follows: (a) Preserved capacity to communicate with others. (b) absence of medical diagnosis of neurodegenerative diseases or any other disease that may hinder exercise performance; (c) capacity to perform exercise, according to each LTCI physician; (d) no record of severe cardiopathy; (e) absence of acute musculoskeletal injuries that may hamper exercise performance; and (f) no severe sequels of cerebrovascular accident. Participant demographic data was analyzed, and the Brazilian version of the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) was applied (11). NO MMSE cut-off point was applied to exclude participants.
Frailty syndrome was assessed by five objectively measured components, namely non-intentional weight loss, self-reported exhaustion, low physical activity levels, slow walking, and grip strength, leading to classifications of frail, pre-frail, and robust (12).
Participant Randomization and Allocation
Randomization was applied with the division of parallel groups according to similar age. An independent researcher performed the procedure and used an Excel sequence of random numbers, with no participant identification. The codes of each older person's group allocation were sent to the data collection chief researcher to determine the intervention groups.
Interventions
Interventions were performed twice a week, totaling 16 sessions across approximately 2 months. Each session lasted 30–45 min. Participants were randomly allocated to the exergames and active control groups (EG and ACG, respectively). The EG performed exercises with 2D exergames, while the ACG used the same movements as EG without virtual reality. Both intervention programs have been previously published and detailed by our laboratory (13, 14). The frequency, number of sessions, and duration were the same for both groups.
Outcomes
Spatial Navigation Assessment
The Floor Maze Test - FMT (15) was used to assess spatial navigation. This test evaluates planning, allocentric spatial navigation, and episodic memory. FMT consists of a bidimensional white maze drawn in a dark blue carpet (6 m2). Participants must find the exit of the maze as quickly as possible. Course planning time (PT), immediate course performance maze time (IMT), and maze course repetition time without previous planning (Delayed Maze Time - DMT) were assessed. To perform the DMT, the individual remained in a room for 10 min without visual contact with the maze. IMT and DMT trajectory errors were recorded.
Statistical Procedures
The Shapiro-Wilk and Levene tests were used to verify data normality and homoscedasticity, respectively. Descriptive analyses (mean, standard-deviation, median, and 95% confidence interval) were used for sample characterization and result presentation. Differences between post- and pre-intervention data (Δ) were estimated using the spatial navigation results. The independent T test and Kruskal-Wallis were used, when suitable, to compare independent group data (EG Δ vs. ACG Δ). Analyses considered α ≤ 0.05 and β = 0.20 parameters and were performed using the Statistics Package for Social Sciences (SPSS) 24.0. As the multiple comparisons inflate the alpha value, we did not perform paired analyses (within groups), to avoid Type I Errors [See (16)].
Ethical Procedure
This research was approved by the Research Ethics Committee of the State University of Montes Claros under n. 2.398.863/2017. Brazilian Ministry of Health rules were met, according to law n. 466/2012.
Results
From the 186 eligible older persons, 14 were selected for the interventions, as all remained in the study until the end (Figure 1). Groups presented homogeneity regarding age, body weight, height, and global cognition (Table 1). All older persons exhibited frailty criteria, with 12 classified as frailty and two as prefrail.
Spatial navigation improved in EG participants compared to ACG individuals. A significant reduction in IMT performance time was observed between groups (Figure 2A), while the time to perform DMT was significantly different at the significance threshold (Figure 2B).
Figure 2. (A) Δ time to finish the immediate Maze Test. (B) Δ time to finish the Delayed Maze Test. EG: 2D Exergame Group. ACG, Active Control Group. EG IMTbaseline: 388 ± 385 s; IMTpost−intervention: 165 ± 147 s; EG DMTbaseline: 354 ± 329 s; DMTpost−intervention: 117 ± 129 s; ACG IMTbaseline: 146 ± 104 s; IMTpost−intervention: 200 ± 170 s; DMTbaseline: 101 ± 59 s; DMTpost−intervention: 150 ± 117 s.
Discussion
This study indicates that exercise with 2D virtual reality reduces immediate FMT performance time and improves spatial navigation in institutionalized non-robust older persons. These are promising findings regarding spatial navigation decline and speed reduction, which is a predictor of cognitive decline due to dementia disorders (6).
Spatial navigation is considered among the scientific community as a strong predictor of both cognitive impairment and Alzheimer's disease (6, 17, 18). Immediate FMT performance time is associated with executive functions and involves, mainly, planning, mental flexibility, and processing speed (14), functions which might be harmed in dementia cases and are paramount to maintaining independence and postponing institutionalization (4, 17, 19).
The main physical exercise benefits on cognition are associated with neuroplasticity, spatial learning, memory, and executive control (20–23). These benefits justify the improvement of spatial navigation ability found in the present study. Moreover, exercise with virtual reality distinguishes itself as a more attractive strategy for physical exercise practices in older individuals (24).
Other studies corroborating our findings are available, highlighting that the use of training with virtual reality improves the cognitive capacity of institutionalized older persons (10, 12, 13, 24–26). Interactions with virtual environments increase the activation of the frontoparietal cortex network, which contributes to explaining the findings reported herein, as this region is directly connected to spatial navigation (27). Furthermore, a lack of investigation on longitudinal intervention strategies on humans focusing on spatial navigation, as the one conducted in this study, is noted, as most assessments consider only acute effects.
Some studies conducted with animals have highlighted that aerobic and resistance physical exercise may result in positive interferences on brain structures associated with spatial navigation, such as the hippocampus, through the enhancement of trophic factor secretion, brain derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF), and growth factor similar to insulin type 1 (IGF-1), which promote neurogenesis (22, 28).
The most important and main limitation of this study is the sample size. Although p-value and statistical power were statistically significant, this finding should be interpreted with caution, as a replication of the experiment applied herein is required.
Conclusions
Exercise with 2D virtual reality improves the spatial navigation ability of institutionalized, non-robust, older persons. The results reported herein may aid in developing strategies to improve spatial navigation capacity in institutionalized older persons and, therefore, prevent or slow down the development of prodromal dementia in this population.
Data Availability Statement
The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.
Ethics Statement
The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Comitê de Ética em Pesquisa da Universidade Estadual de Montes Claros. Protocol number: no. 2.398.863/2017. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.
Author Contributions
LO wrote the manuscript and collected the data. EE wrote the manuscript. MA participated in the data collection. LC partially wrote, revised the content, and translated the manuscript. DF participated in the data collection. AdP revised the manuscript content and performed the analyses. KE revised the manuscript, the analyses, and the English language. RM-J established the study objective, participated in the data collection, wrote the manuscript, and analyzed the data. ON revised the manuscript, language and analyses. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.
Funding
The authors acknowledge funding support from the Coordenação de Aperfeiçoamento de Pessoal de Nível Superior (CAPES) and Fundação de Amparo à Pesquisa do Estado de Minas Gerais (FAPEMIG).
Conflict of Interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Acknowledgments
The authors thank the participants of the study and the following long-term care institutions: Asilo São Vicente de Paula, Lar Sérgio Falcão, and Lar da Amizade.
References
1. Park J, Yim J. A new approach to improve cognition, muscle strength, and postural balance in community-dwelling elderly with a 3-D virtual reality Kayak program. Tohoku J Exp Med. (2016) 238:1–8. doi: 10.1620/tjem.238.1
2. Yamada Y, Denkinger MD, Onder G, Henrard JC, van der Roest HG, Finne-Soveri H, et al. Dual sensory impairment and cognitive decline: the results from the Shelter study. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. (2016) 71:117–23. doi: 10.1093/gerona/glv036
3. Fagundes DF, Costa MT, Alves BBdS, Benício MMS, Vieira LP, Carneiro LSF, et al. Prevalence of dementia in long-term care institutions: a meta-analysis. J Bras Psiquiatr. (2020). [Epub ahead of print].
4. Gonzalez-Colaco Harmand M, Meillon C, Rullier L, Avila-Funes JA, Bergua V, Dartigues JF, et al. Cognitive decline after entering a nursing home: a 22-year follow-up study of institutionalized and noninstitutionalized elderly people. J Am Med Dir Assoc. (2014) 15:504–8. doi: 10.1016/j.jamda.2014.02.006
5. van Asselen M, Kessels RP, Kappelle LJ, Neggers SF, Frijns CJ, Postma A. Neural correlates of human wayfinding in stroke patients. Brain Res. (2006) 1067:229–38. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2005.10.048
6. Verghese J, Lipton R, Ayers E. Spatial navigation and risk of cognitive impairment: a prospective cohort study. Alzheimers Dement. (2017) 13:985–92. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2017.01.023
7. Riva G, Davide F, IJsselsteijn WA. Being There: Concepts, Effects and Measurements of User Presence in Synthetic Environments. Amsterdam: Ios Press (2003).
8. Maguire EA, Burgess N, Donnett JG, Frackowiak RS, Frith CD, O'Keefe J. Knowing where and getting there: a human navigation network. Science. (1998) 280:921–4. doi: 10.1126/science.280.5365.921
9. Chao YY, Scherer YK, Montgomery CA. Effects of using Nintendo Wii exergames in older adults: a review of the literature. J Aging Health. (2015) 27:379–402. doi: 10.1177/0898264314551171
10. Monteiro-Junior RS, Figueiredo L, Maciel-Pinheiro PT, Abud ELR, Engedal K, Barca ML, et al. Virtual reality-based physical exercise with exergames (PhysEx) improves mental and physical health of institutionalized older adults. J Am Med Dir Assoc. (2017) 18:454.e1–9. doi: 10.1016/j.jamda.2017.01.001
11. Bertolucci PH, Brucki S, Campacci SR, Juliano Y. O mini-exame do estado mental em uma população geral: impacto da escolaridade. Arq Neuropsiquiatr. (1994) 52:1–7. doi: 10.1590/S0004-282X1994000100001
12. Fried LP, Tangen CM, Walston J, Newman AB, Hirsch C, Gottdiener J, et al. Frailty in older adults: evidence for a phenotype. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. (2001) 56:M146–56. doi: 10.1093/gerona/56.3.M146
13. Monteiro-Junior RS, da Silva Figueiredo LF, de Tarso Maciel-Pinheiro P, Abud ELR, Braga AEMM, Barca ML, et al. Acute effects of exergames on cognitive function of institutionalized older persons: a single-blinded, randomized and controlled pilot study. Aging Clin Exp Res. (2017) 29:387–94. doi: 10.1007/s40520-016-0595-5
14. Monteiro-Junior RS, Vaghetti CAO, Nascimento OJM, Laks J, Deslandes AC. Exergames: neuroplastic hypothesis about cognitive improvement and biological effects on physical function of institutionalized older persons. Neural Regen Res. (2016) 11:201. doi: 10.4103/1673-5374.177709
15. Sanders AE, Holtzer R, Lipton RB, Hall C, Verghese J. Egocentric and exocentric navigation skills in older adults. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. (2008) 63:1356–63. doi: 10.1093/gerona/63.12.1356
16. Hopkins WG. A New View of Statistics. (2016). Available online at: https://www.sportsci.org/resource/stats/index.html (accessed August 25, 2020).
17. Almeida CABD, Figueiredo LFDS, Plácido J, Silva FDO, Maciel-Pinheiro PDT, Monteiro-Junior RS, et al. Floor Maze test as a predictor of cognitive decline in older adults living in nursing homes. J Bras Psiquiatr. (2020) 69:88–92. doi: 10.1590/0047-2085000000271
18. Tangen GG, Engedal K, Bergland A, Moger TA, Hansson O, Mengshoel AM. Spatial navigation measured by the Floor Maze test in patients with subjective cognitive impairment, mild cognitive impairment, and mild Alzheimer's disease. Int Psychogeriatr. (2015) 27:1401–9. doi: 10.1017/S1041610215000022
19. Tangen GG, Engedal K, Bergland A, Moger TA, Mengshoel AM. Relationships between balance and cognition in patients with subjective cognitive impairment, mild cognitive impairment, and Alzheimer disease. Phys. Ther. (2014) 94:1123–34. doi: 10.2522/ptj.20130298
20. Colcombe S, Kramer AF. Fitness effects on the cognitive function of older adults: a meta-analytic study. Psychol Sci. (2003) 14:125–30. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.t01-1-01430
21. Etnier JL, Chang Y-K. The effect of physical activity on executive function: a brief commentary on definitions, measurement issues, and the current state of the literature. J Sport Exerc Psychol. (2009) 31:469–83. doi: 10.1123/jsep.31.4.469
22. Cassilhas RC, Tufik S, de Mello MT. Physical exercise, neuroplasticity, spatial learning and memory. Cell Mol Life Sci. (2016) 73:975–83. doi: 10.1007/s00018-015-2102-0
23. Sobral-Monteiro-Junior R, Maillot P, Gatica-Rojas V, Avila WRM, de Paula AMB, Guimaraes ALS, et al. Is the “lactormone” a key-factor for exercise-related neuroplasticity? A hypothesis based on an alternative lactate neurobiological pathway. Med Hypotheses. (2019) 123:63–6. doi: 10.1016/j.mehy.2018.12.013
24. Anderson-Hanley C, Arciero PJ, Brickman AM, Nimon JP, Okuma N, Westen SC, et al. Exergaming and older adult cognition. Am J Prev Med. (2012) 42:109–19. doi: 10.1016/j.amepre.2011.10.016
25. Moreno A, Wall KJ, Thangavelu K, Craven L, Ward E, Dissanayaka NN. A systematic review of the use of virtual reality and its effects on cognition in individuals with neurocognitive disorders. Alzheimers Dement. (2019) 5:834–50. doi: 10.1016/j.trci.2019.09.016
26. Thapa N, Park HJ, Yang JG, Son H, Jang M, Lee J, et al. The effect of a virtual reality-based intervention program on cognition in older adults with mild cognitive impairment: a randomized control trial. J Clin Med. (2020) 9:1283. doi: 10.3390/jcm9051283
27. Kober SE, Kurzmann J, Neuper C. Cortical correlate of spatial presence in 2D and 3D interactive virtual reality: an EEG study. Int J Psychophysiol. (2012) 83:365–74. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2011.12.003
Keywords: dementia, spatial orientation, frailty, physical activity, video games
Citation: Oliveira LM, Evangelista e Souza EH, Alves MR, Carneiro LSF, Fagundes DF, de Paula AMB, Engedal K, Nascimento OJM and Monteiro-Junior RS (2021) 2D Virtual Reality-Based Exercise Improves Spatial Navigation in Institutionalized Non-robust Older Persons: A Preliminary Data Report of a Single-Blind, Randomized, and Controlled Study. Front. Neurol. 11:609988. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2020.609988
Received: 24 September 2020; Accepted: 15 December 2020;
Published: 18 January 2021.
Edited by:
Carlos Bandeira de Mello Monteiro, University of São Paulo, BrazilReviewed by:
Ning Cao, Moss Rehabilitation Hospital, United StatesSantos Villafaina, University of Extremadura, Spain
Copyright © 2021 Oliveira, Evangelista e Souza, Alves, Carneiro, Fagundes, de Paula, Engedal, Nascimento and Monteiro-Junior. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Renato Sobral Monteiro-Junior, monteirojuniorms@gmail.com