Skip to main content

REVIEW article

Front. Neurol., 04 September 2020
Sec. Neurotrauma

Behavioral Deficits in Animal Models of Blast Traumatic Brain Injury

  • Department of Biomedical Engineering, Center for Injury Biomechanics, Materials and Medicine, New Jersey Institute of Technology, Newark, NJ, United States

Blast exposure has been identified to be the most common cause for traumatic brain injury (TBI) in soldiers. Over the years, rodent models to mimic blast exposures and the behavioral outcomes observed in veterans have been developed extensively. However, blast tube design and varying experimental parameters lead to inconsistencies in the behavioral outcomes reported across research laboratories. This review aims to curate the behavioral outcomes reported in rodent models of blast TBI using shockwave tubes or open field detonations between the years 2008–2019 and highlight the important experimental parameters that affect behavioral outcome. Further, we discuss the role of various design parameters of the blast tube that can affect the nature of blast exposure experienced by the rodents. Finally, we assess the most common behavioral tests done to measure cognitive, motor, anxiety, auditory, and fear conditioning deficits in blast TBI (bTBI) and discuss the advantages and disadvantages of these tests.

Introduction

Traumatic Brain Injury (TBI) is one of the most prevalent causes for disability and a reduced quality of life among military personnel. About 380,000 military personnel have been diagnosed with TBI since 2000 according to the Defense and Veterans Brain Injury Center (DVBIC) (1). Of these, more than 50% of combat TBI are reported to occur due to blast exposures (2). Most of the soldiers with blast TBI (bTBI) develop cognitive, behavioral and psychological deficits such as PTSD, attention deficits, headaches, anxiety, depression, memory loss, suicidal ideation, tinnitus and vestibular deficiencies (36). The etiology of these deficits following a blast injury is unclear. One of the major challenges over the years of bTBI research has been to design a blast injury model in rodents that mimics the primary blast wave and correlates to the clinical outcome of human TBI.

Blast loading is typically classified into four categories: (1) Primary effect caused by the blast wave propagating through the skull and brain, (2) Secondary effects caused by the penetration of shrapnel, (3) Tertiary effects caused by the blast wind resulting in impact or acceleration-deceleration injuries and (4) Quaternary effects caused by toxic gases from the blast exposure (7). The primary effect of a blast exposure caused by the propagation of the blast wave through the brain has been the main area of focus in the area of blast induced neurotrauma. Linking blast exposure mechanics to the behavioral outcome is crucial in validating rodent blast models in order to the study the underlying cellular mechanisms and to further drug development, as exposure level determines injury severity and hence the mechanisms. Shock tubes have been designed to induce bTBI in rodent models since 2007 (8). Several variants of shock tube models have been used in the past 10 years to study behavioral outcome of bTBI in rodent models. A recent review from our group has listed the types of shock tubes used in bTBI with some of the geometric, process and test parameters (9). The variations in those parameters result in inconsistent findings of behavioral outcome (9).

The objective of this review is (1) to outline and evaluate the behavioral tests currently being used in bTBI (2) to summarize the parameters used in testing and the deficits observed in rodents subjected to blast injury between 2008 and 2019. (3) to highlight the crucial features in the blast testing that can lead to the variability in behavioral deficits seen throughout the bTBI literature.

Overview of Blast Apparatus in rodent Models

The major criterion for a blast tube is to reproduce the biomechanical loading of an open field blast exposure and mimic the clinical outcome observed in TBI patients. A blast overpressure wave propagates as a sharp positive pressure rise compressing the surrounding medium of air or water moving radially outward followed by a negative under pressure wave before returning to baseline or ambient pressure values. The primary blast pressure wave generated in open field explosions has been best described by the Friedlander waveform (10). Pressure waves of explosions in confined spaces are more complex due to reflection of the wave from objects, ground, diffraction, and interaction with the incident shockwave. Most shock tubes used to study rodent models of bTBI have attempted to recreate the Friedlander waveform in order to allow for better reproducibility and comparison across laboratories (10, 11).

One of the most direct forms of bTBI exposure in rodent models has been subjecting the animals to open field blast explosions using TNT or other explosives. This model helps to mimic real life blast exposures containing the primary, reflected waveforms and blast wind. The open field explosions expose the rodents to primary, secondary, tertiary, and quaternary effects of the blast. As a result, these models are associated with higher mortality and difficulty in controlling the clinical outcome in the animals. Open field experiments also require large number of explosives to generate the desired blast pressures when compared to recreating the same blast pressures in shock tubes (1214). In the 1950s, Clemedson and Criborn (15), proposed a cone shaped shock tube using pentaerythritol tetranitrate (PETN) explosives as a blast injury model for rats. The rats were placed 1 m away from the source of detonation and were fixed with the help of metallic nets to limit the tertiary effects of the blast. This, however, does not limit the effects of reflective wave on the rodent head. This model also did not negate the quaternary effects of the blast but required less explosives than the open field experiments to produce the desired blast overpressure (8, 11, 13, 15).

In order to study the effects of the primary blast wave, shockwave tubes using compressed gases that disrupt membranes in order to produce a planar Friedlander waveform have been used (10, 16). Such shock tubes typically contain a driver section that is filled with a compressed gas usually helium, nitrogen or compressed air separated by membranes from the transition and extension section, where the animal is placed, followed by an end plate as shown in Figure 1. The compressed gases fill the driver section leading to the disruption of the membranes. The rapidly expanding gas propagates as a planar shock wave front down the test section. The number, thickness of the membranes and length of the test section controls the intensity, duration, and impulse of the blast overpressure wave produced. Several variations of these models are currently in use for investigating bTBI in rodents (10, 1719).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Variability in the design and process variables of the shock tube and its effect on shock wave profile, bTBI injury and behavior in animal models. The compressed gas and number of membranes used affect the incident pressure generated in shock tubes. The geometry of the shock tube, presence or absence of a transition section affect variables of the shockwave profile such as rise time, peak pressure, duration, and impulse. The animal locations inside, near exit and far from exit of the blast tube play a role in the injury severity and behavioral outcomes. P1 and P2 represent the side on blast pressure measurement sensors.

Some shock tube variations that can alter behavioral outcomes include position of the animal in the blast tube, orientation of the animal, use of head restraint, end conditions of the blast tube (open vs. closed) and length of the blast tube (20). The animal's position in the blast tube plays a crucial role in determining the type of blast injury it is exposed to. Several studies have shown that animal placement at the exit of the blast tube leads to significant changes of the incident pressure waveform from the ideal Friedlander waveform (10, 21). At the outer edges of the blast tube near the exit end, an expansion wave is created that decreases the peak pressure and impulse of the blast overpressure wave, exposing the rat brain to a subsonic jet wind resulting in an acceleration-deceleration induced tertiary blast injury. The animals that were constrained and placed at the exit end of the blast tube experienced high amounts of compression pressure in the head and neck regions (10, 2124).

Another criterion that is crucial in the design of the blast tube is the closed or open exit end of shock tubes. A closed ended shock tube results in the propagation of a reflected wave back into the shock tube when the incident pressure wave reaches the end of the tube. This results in the rodent being exposed to a complex blast waveform with the gases unable to dissipate from the blast tube. An open-ended shock tube, on the other hand, results in the formation of an expansion wave at the end of the shock tube which propagates back into the tube (25). Kuriakose et al., indicated that an end plate reflector placed at an optimum distance to the exit of the blast tube can reduce the reflective waveforms and thereby prevent secondary loading (7). Absence of head restraint during the blast exposure also results in tertiary effects of blast leading to increased behavioral deficits (18, 26). The blast overpressure waves decays over the length of the blast tube. The optimal placement of the animal in the blast tube would be where the Friedlander wave is fully formed (10, 27). The amplitude of the incident blast overpressure wave is also critical in determining the severity in terms of mild, moderate or severe TBI. Previous work in our lab has classified the severity of bTBI overpressure wave based on animal mortality and presence of oxidative markers (28). Due to different blast models used across labs, varying lengths of blast tube and thereby incident blast overpressures corresponding to mild, moderate, and severe bTBI have been reported in literature (2831).

Behavioral Deficits in Blast TBI

Cognitive Deficits

Cognitive deficits have been reported in patients with mild TBI which often resolve in 6 months while moderate and severe TBI cause long term deficits including dementia or other neurodegenerative disorders (32). Soldiers exposed to bTBI have been diagnosed with retrograde or anterograde amnesia and decreased executive function (33, 34). Cognitive deficits, however, have been more associated with blunt TBI than with a primary blast exposure (32). In a study by Barnes et al., it was reported that older veterans diagnosed with a TBI were associated with a 60% increase in the risk of developing dementia over a period of 9 years (35).

bTBI in animal models have been reported to affect prefrontal and hippocampal regions involved in cognition (36). Recognition memory in rodents following bTBI has been evaluated using the novel object recognition test while spatial working memory has been studied using Morris water maze (MWM), Barnes maze and Y maze tests. Novel object recognition test (NOR) measures deficits in both the perirhinal cortex which plays a major role in object recognition and the parahippocampal region involved with visual object recognition memory. NOR measures the ability of the rodent to recognize novelty and is measured by the difference in time spent by the animal in exploring a novel object when compared to a familiar object. The main advantage of the NOR test is its simplicity and lack of training or habituation required prior to conducting the test. The NOR test can also be modified to study short or long term memory deficits by increasing or decreasing the retention time before introducing a novel object (37, 38).

Allocentric spatial memory involving the hippocampus and entorhinal cortex has been studied using several maze tests with the most popular form of assessment being the Morris Water Maze test (39). The other alternatives heavily used in the blast literature have been the Barnes Maze, radial arm maze and Y maze tests. Morris water maze tests the ability of a rodent to use spatial visual cues to identify the location of a rescue platform submerged in water. Place cells present in the hippocampus and entorhinal cortex have been identified to have increased firing rates when the rodent nears the submerged platform location in the MWM test (40). Further, lesions in the hippocampus have been identified to eliminate water maze learning (41).

The Barnes Maze test is similar to the MWM in identifying hippocampal related deficits in cognition. Barnes maze test consists of a circular platform with evenly spaced holes around the circumference with an escape platform placed under one of these holes. The presence of brightly lit open space, use of an aversive sound or blowing air above the maze are some of the methods used to motivate the rats to seek the escape platform (20, 39). The advantage of the Barnes maze test over MWM is that it does not induce the stress of swimming in the animals as measured by the increased plasma cortisone levels in the rats post MWM test (42).

The Y maze test is also used to measure deficits in the hippocampus and the prefrontal cortex and can be used to measure the spatial working and reference memory. The Y maze consists of 3 arms, 120° to each other. Similar to the RA, the number of entries to each arm is measured with greater alternation in the arms indicating good working memory. An alternative to this approach is closing one of the arms during training and measuring the spatial reference memory of the rats to visit the novel unvisited arm during the testing phase. Recognizing the previously visited arms with spatial cues and increased visits to the novel arm indicates intact hippocampal function (43).

Findings of cognitive deficits recorded by researchers across different models of shockwave tubes and blast pressures are summarized in Table 1. Literature review was done using advanced search in Google Scholar for articles with all of the words “rodents, shockwave tube, cognitive memory deficit” and the exact phrase “blast TBI,” between the years 2008–2019. Studies generating blast TBI conditions using open field detonations or shockwave tubes were only incorporated in this review.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Summary of experimental variable and cognitive deficits observed in rodent models of blast TBI.

Based on observations from Table 1, deficits in object recognition memory has been consistently reported in bTBI using the novel object recognition test. Deficits in recognition memory are reported in mild blast exposures of 74–129 kPa and last from 7 days up to 33 weeks following bTBI. Interestingly, Sajja et al. (64), did not observe novel object recognition deficits on day 3 following blast but the animals were observed to develop the deficits on day 7 post injury. This could indicate that the recognition memory deficit in bTBI is not immediate but occurs over time.

Spatial working memory deficits have not been consistently reported across bTBI models. Some factors that contribute to the difference in outcome are the positioning of the animal in the blast tube, blast pressures exposed, use of head restraint and the overall design of the blast tube. Working memory deficits measured by Y maze test has been reported only in animals subjected to open detonations and the duration of deficits directly correlate with the distance from the detonation source (12). Similarly, Barnes maze test has only been reported in literature for injured animals without head restraint or animals kept outside the exit end of the blast tube. These deficits were observed in low bTBI pressures and lasted up to 11 days post blast. Memory deficits in open detonation experiments persisted for a month (58). The working memory deficits in Barnes maze were resolved when animals were provided with head restraint (26). Most deficits that lasted weeks post injury observed in the Morris Water maze test also involves animals placed at the exit end of the blast tube. When the animals are placed inside the blast tube only transient deficits observed at 24 or 48 h post injury were observed (17, 18).

Anxiety and Depression

Veterans diagnosed with bTBI also report developing anxiety and depression disorders over time. In a sample population of veterans exposed to bTBI, it was identified that 50% of individuals exposed to bTBI developed anxiety disorders, 88% developed depression and 60% showed Post Traumatic Stress Disorder (PTSD) traits (68). Treating military personnel with PTSD and depression has been estimated to cost ~$6.2 billion in the first 2 years post deployment (69).

Depression is a complex mental illness which causes persistent feeling of sadness and loss of interest which affects person's quality of life negatively. Severe post-concussive symptoms such as headache, blurred vision, dizziness, and memory impairment manifests due to depression following TBI. Depression following TBI causes disturbances in neuronal circuits such as frontal lobe -basal ganglia circuits and anterior ascending monoaminergic pathways (70, 71). The forced swim test is one of the popular behavior tasks successfully used for assessing depression behavior. Mice or rats are placed in an inescapable transparent tank that is filled with water and their escape related mobility behavior is measured in order to evaluate depression. Increased time spent immobile in the tank correlates with depression traits such as behavioral despair and disengagement from stress coping (72). Can et al. (72), Slattery et al. (73), and Yankelevitch-Yahav et al. (74) have provided detailed protocols for the test parameters for forced swim test for rats and mice (7274).

Anxiety is a psychological, physiological and behavioral state induced in animal or humans by a threat to well-being. Anxiety is characterized by an increased arousal, expectancy, autonomic and neuroendocrinal activation which results in manifestation of specific pattern of behaviors such as exploration, feeding, flight or defensive behavior to the specific stimulus (75). Anxiety behavior is regulated by forebrain and hindbrain regions involving the septo-hippocampal system, entorhinal cortex, medial prefrontal cortex, basolateral amygdaloid complex, and midbrain raphe system (7680).

Anxiety in rodent models of bTBI has been evaluated using tests such as the elevated plus maze (EPM), open field test, elevated zero maze and light/dark box tests as described in Table 2. The EPM test is widely used to study anxiety and has been used to identify deficits in regions such as the limbic regions, hippocampus, amygdala and dorsal raphe nucleus (31, 47, 48, 56, 83, 90). Briefly, rats or mice are placed at the junction of the four arms of the maze, two of which are closed by walls and two are open. The animal is placed facing an open arm and the time spent or number of entries in the open or closed arms of the maze is recorded. The time spent in the closed arms correlates to anxiety, since the animal lacks the drive to explore the open arm that is less safe. The elevated zero maze works on a similar principle as the elevated plus maze with closed and open arms. The elevated zero maze is circular with alternating open and closed arms.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Summary of experimental variables and anxiety and depression related behavior test outcomes in bTBI.

The open field test (OFT) assesses the animal's locomotor activity, exploratory behavior and anxiety. Briefly, the test involves placing the animal in a square, rectangle or circular box with set spacing requirements as described by Gould et al. (91), Seibenhener et al. (92), and recording the animal's exploratory behavior (19, 46, 91, 92). Two factors are known to influence anxiety-like behavior in the open field. The first is the social isolation resulting from the physical separation from cage mates when performing the test. The second is the stress/aversion created by the brightly lit, unprotected, novel test environment (92). Rodents exposed to the novel experimental arena typically spend greater time exploring the periphery rather than the center area. This tendency of animals in known as thigmotaxis. Mice or rats exploring more time at periphery of the arena than the center represents anxious behavior. Besides exploratory activity other anxiety behaviors such as grooming, rearing and defecation can be recorded in OFT task.

The light/dark box test consists of a dark box which represents the “safe area,” a light box which represents the aversive environment and utilizes the rodent's innate aversion to light and exploratory behavior to measure anxiety. The animal is habituated in a dark room and placed in the center of the light box and recorded. The time taken by the animal to explore and reach the dark box is measured. Animals with anxiety are generally observed to take more time to reach the dark box than control animals. This test has been identified to be more effective when done in the dark or night cycle of rodents as their plasma corticosterone levels which are higher in their dark cycle play a significant role in their exploratory behavior (93).

One of the major pitfalls involved in anxiety testing is habituation. Testing done over multiple timepoints leads to habituation of the new/aversive environment leading to a decline in the exploratory behavior of the rats. The weight of the animals and motor deficits have also been identified to play a negative role in their exploratory behavior (37, 73, 90).

Table 2 summarizes the work done in literature regarding anxiety and depression behavior testing in rodents subjected to bTBI. Literature review was done using advanced search in Google Scholar with keywords “rodents, shockwave tube, anxiety, depression” and the exact phrase “blast TBI,” between the years 2008-2019. In most published works in Table 2, bTBI has been correlated with positive anxiety deficits in rodents.

Anxiety measured by the elevated plus maze, light/dark emergence test were identified in most studies at chronic timepoints of 7, 15 days post bTBI resulting in long term deficits irrespective of animal location in the blast tube (36, 48, 56, 65). However, no deficits were observed in animals placed 7 m away from detonation source in open field explosions (59, 60). Animals exposed to multiple blasts analyzed with the elevated zero maze were identified with deficits that lasted months post exposure. Deficits in the open field test were observed 1–7 days post injury in animals placed outside the blast tube (66, 85). Chronic deficits lasting a month have not been reported with the open field test except in a study involving moderate bTBI overpressure (16). These results implicate the importance of experimental parameters and choice of behavior tests in measuring deficits following bTBI.

Fear Conditioning

PTSD and bTBI have symptoms that often overlap with each other. These include attention deficits, irritability, increased startle response, sleep disturbance, emotional numbness and anger (94, 95). However, there is also increasing evidence indicating the development of chronic PTSD as a result of secondary neuronal damage following a bTBI (9698). PTSD is known to manifest in the following stages: an individual is first exposed or witness to a life-threatening situation leading to re-experiencing symptoms such as nightmares and sense of reliving the trauma. This then leads to active avoidance of the trauma, inability to recall the trauma, emotional numbing resulting in irritability, insomnia, hyper vigilance, or an increased startle response (95). PTSD is associated with medial and orbitofrontal cortices, amygdala, and hippocampal regions of the brain (97). Fear conditioning is one of the tests that is used to measure PTSD traits in rodent models of bTBI. Contextual and cued fear conditioning test involve the amygdala, hippocampus, frontal, and cingulate cortex brain regions (99).

Contextual and cued fear conditioning is done by training the rodent in a chamber (context) and providing a conditional stimulus (CS) typically an aversive noise followed by an unconditioned stimulus (US) such as a foot shock. The pairing of the CS- US generates the freezing behavior in rats. When this pairing is repeated, the animal creates a stronger negative association to the testing environment and the auditory cue. This then generates a freezing response when the animal is returned either to the testing chamber or on hearing the auditory cue. The contextual fear memory is tested by placing the rat in the same chamber and measuring the freezing behavior in the animal without the presence of the CS or US. In order to test the cued conditioning, the animal is placed in a different chamber and its freezing response to the CS is now measured. This enables to differentiate the freezing effect observed due to the context and the CS (50, 99).

Table 3 summarizes the fear conditioning tests done in bTBI literature using Google scholar and keywords “shockwave tube,” “rodents,” “fear conditioning,” and the exact phrase “blast TBI” between the years 2008–2019. Most of the published studies in bTBI measuring fear conditioning post bTBI were carried out with repetitive low-level blasts and have consistently reported an increase in freezing or cued fear response following bTBI. The duration of deficits have been reported from 1 to 25 weeks post blast. Fear conditioning deficits measured in a single blast exposure by Perez Gracia et al. (65), were observed 3 weeks post injury further implicating the long-term consequence of blast exposure on fear conditioning (65). These results indicate the development of PTSD related deficits following bTBI in rodent models.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Fear conditioning behavioral testing in bTBI.

Motor Deficits

Veterans diagnosed with bTBI have been known to manifest balance and vestibular motor co-ordination issues with symptoms ranging from dizziness, vertigo, postural instability and impaired tandem gait (101103). Further, balance deficits are found to be amplified in veterans with a combined diagnosis of TBI and amnesia or PTSD (104). Balance and gait disorders in rodent bTBI models are measured using staircase test, ladder test, rotarod, and beam balance test as described in Table 4. Additionally, anxiety assessment apparatus such as elevated plus maze and open field test have also been used to measure locomotor deficits in rodents.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Summary of experimental variables and motor deficit outcomes in bTBI.

The Rotarod test originally described by Dunham and Miya is often used for testing the neurological effects of drugs or trauma on the motor coordination of rodents (50, 108110). The apparatus consists of a rotating rod of constant or accelerating speed on which the rodent is placed and variables such as the time and the speed of rotation for the animal to fall from the apparatus is noted and evaluated for motor deficits. The Rotarod apparatus was identified to be more sensitive in identifying motor deficits in mild TBI than the beam balance or beam walk test (108). The disadvantages of this model are that the deficits observed in the rotarod test can also be influenced by endurance and motor learning in the animals. Motor learning refers to the rodent's ability to develop a strategy to remain on the rotating rod rather than as a result of improved locomotor skills (111, 112). Stress has also been identified to play a role in rotarod deficits (113).

The beam walk, ladder rung tests measure the fine motor skills of the rat unlike the gross motor skills assessed by the rotarod test (99). The beam walk or beam balance apparatus consists of a narrow-elevated beam that the rodent is made to walk on from one end to the other. The number of foot faults of the rodent is measured to assess its motor performance. The advantages of the test are the low cost, the relative ease of setup and very little motor learning associated with recovery of function. However, the weight of the animal can affect their performance on this apparatus. Beam walking test is more suited to identify deficits in moderate and severe TBI than in mild TBI (112). The ladder rung test, similar to the beam balance test, measures the foot faults of rodents as they walk across a horizontal ladder to reach a platform on the other side. The ladder rung test, additionally, allows to assess the grasping ability and motor deficits in each limb of the rodent. The difficulty of the test can be varied by altering the distance between the rungs. An irregular pattern of rung placement for each trial prevents familiarity and learning in the rodents (114). One disadvantage of the beam walk and ladder rung tests is the manual assessment of foot fault scoring which can be tedious and may differ from one individual to another. Anxiety tests such as open field test has also been utilized to study locomotor deficits. Ambulation, latency and rearing of the rodents can be assessed through these tests in addition to anxiety and depression traits (92).

Table 4 summarizes literature review done using Google scholar and keywords “shockwave,” “rodents,” “motor deficits,” and the exact phrase “blast TBI” between the years 2008–2019. Motor deficits, similar to the spatial working memory deficits, have not been consistently reported in bTBI. Most of the deficits observed following bTBI were in animals placed at the exit end of the blast tube implicating the role of reflective pressures and tertiary loading of the blast (30, 46, 86). Majority of the studies with animals located inside the blast tube have reported no motor deficits (17, 18, 50). However, animals located inside the blast tube subjected to multiple blasts have been identified to develop motor deficits (107).

Auditory Deficits

Blast TBI has been well-associated with tinnitus and hearing loss deficits in the veteran population (115117). In a study done by Oleksiak et al. (118), with a sample population of veterans with mild TBI, 76% of veterans were associated with tinnitus while 60% developed hearing loss. About 92.5% of personnel exposed to blast TBI reported some form of hearing loss (118). Blast exposure can cause both peripheral and central auditory system damage by rupture of the tympanic membrane, ossicular disruption, structural damage of inner and outer hair cells and impairment of central auditory processing (CAP). Blast related auditory deficits include CAP deficits, peripheral hearing loss, tinnitus, and vestibular deficits. Tinnitus is defined as head or ear noise that lasts 5 min or longer while CAP deficits causes difficulty in hearing background noises, speech perception, sound localization, and lateralization (4).

Auditory deficits in bTBI rodent models have been evaluated using acoustic startle response (ASR) and various modifications of the test to include prepulse inhibition for identifying hearing loss and gap detection deficits for evaluating tinnitus. The acoustic startle response is described as a rapid contraction of facial and skeletal muscles in response to a loud and unexpected auditory stimulus usually 80 dB above the auditory threshold for a rat (119). The acoustic signaling pathway consists of the auditory nerve, the ventral cochlear nucleus, the cochlear root neurons, the caudal pontine reticular nucleus (PnC), spinal interneurons and finally the motor neurons which produce the startle response (119121). The PnC neurons, specifically, play a crucial role in the startle response elicited (122, 123).

The acoustic startle experiments are generally conducted in a chamber mounted with a platform containing an accelerometer or other voltage motion sensors to assess the startle response in rats. Briefly, the test involves acclimatizing rats to background noise (65–68 dB) before introducing the animal to increasing intensity of the startle stimuli. The startle amplitude of rats is then measured as an output function of the startle intensity provided. For the prepulse inhibition, a sound of lower intensity than the startle stimulus is presented before the startle stimuli. This prepulse stimulus provided in anticipation of the startle stimulus is expected to lower the startle amplitude in control subjects but not in subjects with hearing loss (124). A modification of this paradigm is the gap prepulse inhibition which has recently gained prominence in evaluating tinnitus in human and animal models. The gap prepulse inhibition involves a gap trial in which a silent gap is provided prior to the startle stimuli. Since subjects with tinnitus are expected to fail to hear the silent gap, there would be no reduction in startle amplitude in the gap trials when compared to control subjects. The gap detection is measured as the ratio between the magnitude of the startle stimulus presented alone without a gap trial and trials in which a gap preceded the startle stimulus (125).

The main advantage of the acoustic startle test is since it measures the reflex of the rodent, there is no training involved prior to conducting the test. However, there is considerable variability in the experimental protocol and data interpretation for the gap detection experiments across laboratories. One of the major discrepancies is in comparing the “no gap” vs. “gap trials” of individual rats and determining the presence of tinnitus than comparing the overall performance across groups. Since it is possible that only a percentage of blast exposed rats may develop tinnitus, it would be beneficial to compare individual rat performances than performances across groups (125). Further, there has been contradicting evidence in the assumption that subjects with tinnitus would “fill in” the silent gap trial (126, 127). However, there is significant evidence to support that the gap detection ratio measured is affected in rats with tinnitus (127, 128).

Blast induced tinnitus and hearing loss investigations done in rodent models are summarized in Table 5 using Google Scholar and keywords such as “blast TBI,” “rodents,” “hearing loss,” “tinnitus,” and the exact phrase “blast TBI” between the years 2008–2019. Most of the studies at different blast overpressures observe consistent findings of hearing loss and tinnitus in blast exposed animals indicating the prevalence of auditory deficits in bTBI and the efficacy of the behavioral test.

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Auditory deficits in bTBI.

Summary and Discussion

The primary effects of blast TBI have been replicated and studied in rodent models by many researchers. We have attempted in this review to summarize the various behavioral models used to study common behavioral outcomes of blast TBI such as cognition, anxiety, depression, PTSD, motor, and auditory deficits. The advantages and shortcomings of the tests most commonly used in literature have been summarized in Table 6.

TABLE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Table 6. Behavior tests used in blast TBI with their advantages and disadvantages.

Cognitive deficits in blast induced TBI have been studied using spatial memory tasks such as Morris Water maze, Y maze, Barnes Maze, and object recognition tasks such as the novel object recognition test. From Table 1, it can be inferred that animals exposed to the reflective blast pressure or blast wind manifest spatial memory deficits. In the studies where the animals were placed inside the blast tube, a few researchers observed deficits with Morris Water Maze test at 24 h post impact but not at longer timepoints. No cognitive deficit was observed with the eight arm radial maze. Multiple blast studies done with the Morris Water Maze test also indicated similar transient deficits at 24 h but not at later timepoints (49, 50). Studies done with animals placed outside the blast tube, on the other hand, consistently reported cognitive deficits lasting for weeks indicating these animals may be receiving a higher severity of TBI (30, 31, 57). Investigations of bTBI with animals placed outside the shock tube have demonstrated that the animals are not only subject to the pure effects of a primary blast wave but can be subject to the reflective pressures and blast wind (10, 22). Similarly, spatial memory deficits were also observed to last for weeks or months in animals subject to open field detonations (12, 58). Stemper et al. (48), reported no cognitive deficits up to 60 days post impact when animals were placed at an angle to the opening of the blast tube to minimize the blast wind effects (48). These results indicate that chronic spatial memory deficits observed due to a blast impact are not majorly manifested due to the primary blast wave and could be a result of the secondary or tertiary effects of a blast impact. This was further demonstrated by Goldstein et al. (26), when spatial memory deficits were observed in animals subject to bTBI without head restraint but not in animals with head restraint (26). Thus, it is possible that the spatial memory deficits are more likely to be produced due to tertiary effect of head and body motion and not primary loading effect when the animal is relatively motionless. However, this hypothesis has not been carefully tested.

Object recognition memory, however, measured by the novel object recognition test showed consistent deficits in animals placed both inside and outside the blast pressure tube indicating that primary blast pressure does alter the recognition memory in rodents. Interestingly, spatial object recognition deficits were not affected by the presence or absence of head restraint unlike the spatial working memory deficits (66). This further supports that primary blast loading affects recognition memory while the effects of tertiary loading may affect the working memory. Fear based learning tests such as the active avoidance test which is associated with deficits in the basal forebrain also showed deficiencies following the primary blast injury (18, 29, 131).

Anxiety due to bTBI has most commonly been studied using elevated plus maze, zero maze, open field test and light/dark box emergence tests. In Table 2, most of the anxiety deficits measured by these tests are seen to be manifested at chronic timepoints with the quickest onset being 4 days post blast in the OFT test and not as immediate deficits post blast exposure. Most of the studies using EPM were done with the animals placed at the mouth of the blast tube manifesting chronic anxiety deficits. However, studies by Perez-Garcia et al. (120), and Russell et al. (83), have also demonstrated chronic anxiety deficits in animals subjected to bTBI placed inside the shock tube (65, 83). Statz et al. (84), subjected rats to a repetitive mild blast TBI of 74.5 kPa over 3 consecutive days and found that a part of the blast cohort, but not all animals, manifested anxiety measured by elevated zero maze, light/dark emergence test and fear conditioning, 3 weeks and 6 months post blast impact (84). Increased anxiety behavior at 24 hrs to 3 days post blast exposure was observed in the open field test only when the animals were placed at the exit end of the blast tube or were subjected to multiple blast exposures over days (19, 54, 66, 85, 86). EPM and light /dark emergence tests showed long term deficits of anxiety following bTBI and may be more accurate tests for anxiety. Open field tests are also dependent on exploratory behavior of rodents but deficits in this model were observed only at early timepoints of few days post blast and no deficits were observed at chronic timepoints of a month post blast.

Increased fear conditioning has been observed in blast animals at chronic time points from single blast or multiple blast exposure (Table 3). Interestingly, a decrease in fear conditioning was observed in repetitive bTBI in a study by Genovese et al. (100). Auditory deficits that could play a role suppressing the ability to hear the aversive cue and thereby the performance of the animals in the fear conditioning test were negated as there were no auditory deficits observed in the prepulse inhibition test. However, in this study conditioned suppression was measured and not the fear conditioning. Conditioned suppression is the reduction of the conditioned response to a positive stimulus, which in this study was a lever press to obtain food when the aversive stimulus was presented. Hence, the conditioned suppression parameter measured in this study might produce different results than the typical fear conditioning measured in other studies (100). Whether conditioned fear and conditioned suppression are correlated or different remains disputable and hence the results may still indicate deficits to the conditioned fear in the blast animals (100, 132134).

Motor deficits following bTBI were measured by Rotarod, beam walk/balance tests, elevated plus maze and open field tests. Motor deficits in bTBI showed a similar trend to the cognitive deficits, with increased deficits in animals exposed to the reflective pressures, placed at the mouth of the blast tube or placed inside an open-ended shockwave tube. Most of the studies with animals placed inside the tube indicated little to no motor deficits.

Auditory deficits in blast TBI have been measured using the acoustic startle test with prepulse inhibition for identifying hearing loss and gap detection deficits for evaluating tinnitus. bTBI induced hearing loss and tinnitus have consistently been reported as chronic deficits. However, some studies report only a part of the cohort undergoing blast to be associated with hearing deficits and tinnitus (Table 5). It is critical to make this distinction between animals with and without auditory deficits within the injury group to avoid possible dilution of results.

Overall, it is observed from Tables 15, that the blast pressures used in most studies investigating behavioral deficits of bTBI are mild blasts ranging from 70 to 153 kPa. The most consistently reported chronic behavioral deficits following primary blast TBI in literature between the years 2008–2019 were anxiety, auditory deficits, recognition memory deficits, and fear conditioning often observed between a week to 1-month post blast exposure. This indicates that exposure to primary mild bTBI can lead to persistent anxiety, auditory, fear conditioning and recognition memory deficits. Additionally, animal location in the blast tube did not play a significant role in the development of these deficits. Spatial working memory and motor deficits following bTBI, on the other hand, depend heavily on experimental parameters. Exposure to a primary blast wave where the animal is kept inside the shock tube seemed to cause only transient effects on working memory measured by the Morris Water maze lasting 24 h post injury while exposure to tertiary effects of blast, reflective pressure wave leads to long lasting deficits. Similarly, motor deficits in a primary blast exposure causes little to no deficits while manifesting long term deficiencies in open field experiments and in studies where animals are placed at the exit end of the blast tube. This suggests that the primary blast wave exposure has a significant role in the manifestation of recognition memory deficits, auditory, fear conditioning and anxiety deficits while tertiary effects of loading experienced by the animal placed outside the blast tube may have a role in the manifestation of spatial working memory and motor deficits. Thus, position of the animal in the blast tube and use of head restraint plays a significant role in bTBI behavioral deficits and have to be considered while studying the effects of a primary blast wave.

Author Contributions

AA carried out the literature research, acquisition of data for the tables, and prepared the manuscript. AR aided in data acquisition for the tables and wrote the subheading Anxiety and Depression in the manuscript. NC and BP procured funding, supervised, provided critical revisions, and edited the manuscript. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This work was funded by New Jersey Commission on Brain Injury Research [grant no. CBIR16PIL021] and U.S. Army Medical Research and Materiel Command [grant no. 14059001 and W81XWH-15-1-0303].

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

1. DoD Worldwide Numbers for TBI. Available online at: https://dvbic.dcoe.mil/dod-worldwide-numbers-tbi (accessed June 19, 2019).

2. Butler D, Buono J, Erdtmann F, Reid P. Traumatic brain injury and the military health system in Systems Engineering to Improve Traumatic Brain Injury Care in the Military Health System: Workshop Summary. (2009) National Academies Press.

Google Scholar

3. Lindquist LK, Love HC, Elbogen EB. Traumatic brain injury in iraq and afghanistan veterans: new results from a national random sample study. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. (2017) 29:254–9. doi: 10.1176/appi.neuropsych.16050100

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Fausti SA, Wilmington DJ, Gallun FJ, Myers PJ, Henry JA. Auditory and vestibular dysfunction associated with blast-related traumatic brain injury. J Rehabil Res Dev. (2009) 46:797–810. doi: 10.1682/JRRD.2008.09.0118

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Scherer MR, Schubert MC. Traumatic brain injury and vestibular pathology as a comorbidity after blast exposure. Phys Ther. (2009) 89:980–92. doi: 10.2522/ptj.20080353

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Trudeau DL, Anderson J, Hansen LM, Shagalov DN, Schmoller J, Nugent S, et al. Findings of mild traumatic brain injury in combat veterans with PTSD and a history of blast concussion. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. (1998) 10:308–13. doi: 10.1176/jnp.10.3.308

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Kuriakose M, Skotak M, Misistia A, Kahali S, Sundaramurthy A, Chandra N. Tailoring the blast exposure conditions in the shock tube for generating pure, primary shock waves: the end plate facilitates elimination of secondary loading of the specimen. PLoS ONE. (2016) 11:e0161597. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0161597

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Chen Y, Constantini S. Caveats for using shock tube in blast-induced traumatic brain injury research. Front Neurol. (2013) 4:117. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2013.00117

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Kahali S, Townsend M, Mendez Nguyen M, Kim J, Alay E, Skotak M, et al. The evolution of secondary flow phenomena and their effect on primary shock conditions in shock tubes: experimentation and numerical model. PLoS ONE. (2020) 15:e0227125. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0227125

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Sundaramurthy A, Alai A, Ganpule S, Holmberg A, Plougonven E, Chandra N. Blast-induced biomechanical loading of the rat: an experimental and anatomically accurate computational blast injury model. J Neurotrauma. (2012) 29:2352–64. doi: 10.1089/neu.2012.2413

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Ning YL, Zhou YG. Shock tubes and blast injury modeling. Chin J Traumatol. (2015) 18:187–93. doi: 10.1016/j.cjtee.2015.04.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Rubovitch V, Ten-Bosch M, Zohar O, Harrison CR, Tempel-Brami C, Stein E, et al. A mouse model of blast-induced mild traumatic brain injury. Exp Neurol. (2011) 232:280–9. doi: 10.1016/j.expneurol.2011.09.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Risling M, Davidsson J. Experimental animal models for studies on the mechanisms of blast-induced neurotrauma. Front Neurol. (2012) 3:30. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2012.00030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Kovacs SK, Leonessa F, Ling GS. Blast TBI models, neuropathology, and implications for seizure risk. Front Neurol. (2014) 5:47. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2014.00047

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Clemedson CJ, Criborn CO. A detonation chamber for physiological blast research. J Aviat Med. (1955) 26:373–81.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

16. Cernak I, Merkle AC, Koliatsos VE, Bilik JM, Luong QT, Mahota TM, et al. The pathobiology of blast injuries and blast-induced neurotrauma as identified using a new experimental model of injury in mice. Neurobiol Dis. (2011) 41:538–51. doi: 10.1016/j.nbd.2010.10.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Vandevord PJ, Bolander R, Sajja VS, Hay K, Bir CA. Mild neurotrauma indicates a range-specific pressure response to low level shock wave exposure. Ann Biomed Eng. (2012) 40:227–36. doi: 10.1007/s10439-011-0420-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Ahlers ST, Vasserman-Stokes E, Shaughness MC, Hall AA, Shear DA, Chavko M, et al. Assessment of the effects of acute and repeated exposure to blast overpressure in rodents: toward a greater understanding of blast and the potential ramifications for injury in humans exposed to blast. Front Neurol. (2012) 3:32. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2012.00032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Kamnaksh A, Kwon SK, Kovesdi E, Ahmed F, Barry ES, Grunberg NE, et al. Neurobehavioral, cellular, and molecular consequences of single and multiple mild blast exposure. Electrophoresis. (2012) 33:3680–92. doi: 10.1002/elps.201200319

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Pitts MW. Barnes maze procedure for spatial learning and memory in mice. Bio Protoc. (2018). 8:e2744. doi: 10.21769/BioProtoc.2744

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Gullotti DM, Beamer M, Panzer MB, Chen YC, Patel TP, Yu A, et al. Significant head accelerations can influence immediate neurological impairments in a murine model of blast-induced traumatic brain injury. J Biomech Eng. (2014) 136:091004. doi: 10.1115/1.4027873

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Svetlov SI, Prima V, Kirk DR, Gutierrez H, Curley KC, Hayes RL, et al. Morphologic and biochemical characterization of brain injury in a model of controlled blast overpressure exposure. J Trauma. (2010) 69:795–804. doi: 10.1097/TA.0b013e3181bbd885

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Chang, K.-S., J.-Kim K. Numerical investigation of inviscid shock wave dynamics in an expansion tube. Shock Waves. (1995) 5:33–45. doi: 10.1007/BF02425034

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Haselbacher AC, Balachandar S, Kieffer S. Open-ended shock tube flows: influence of pressure ratio and diaphragm position. AIAA J. (2007) 45:1917–29. doi: 10.2514/1.23081

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Bass CR, Panzer MB, Rafaels KA, Wood G, Shridharani J, Capehart B. Brain injuries from blast. Ann Biomed Eng. (2012) 40:185–202. doi: 10.1007/s10439-011-0424-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Goldstein LE, Fisher AM, Tagge CA, Zhang XL, Velisek L, Sullivan JA, et al. Chronic traumatic encephalopathy in blast-exposed military veterans and a blast neurotrauma mouse model. Sci Transl Med. (2012) 4:134ra60. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2012.05.592

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Chandra N, Ganpule S, Kleinschmit N, Feng R, Holmberg A, Sundaramurthy A, et al. Evolution of blast wave profiles in simulated air blasts: experiment and computational modeling. Shock Waves. (2012) 22:403–15. doi: 10.1007/s00193-012-0399-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Mishra V, Skotak M, Schuetz H, Heller A, Haorah J, Chandra N. Primary blast causes mild, moderate, severe and lethal TBI with increasing blast overpressures: experimental rat injury model. Sci Rep. (2016) 6:26992. doi: 10.1038/srep26992

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Cernak I, Wang Z, Jiang J, Bian X, Savic J. Cognitive deficits following blast injury-induced neurotrauma: possible involvement of nitric oxide. Brain Injury. (2001) 15:593–612. doi: 10.1080/02699050010009559

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Long JB, Bentley TL, Wessner KA, Cerone C, Sweeney S, Bauman RA. Blast overpressure in rats: recreating a battlefield injury in the laboratory. J Neurotrauma. (2009) 26:827–40. doi: 10.1089/neu.2008.0748

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Awwad HO, Gonzalez LP, Tompkins P, Lerner M, Brackett DJ, Awasthi V, et al. Blast overpressure waves induce transient anxiety and regional changes in cerebral glucose metabolism and delayed hyperarousal in rats. Front Neurol. (2015) 6:132. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2015.00132

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Elder GA. Update on TBI and cognitive impairment in military veterans. Curr Neurol Neurosci Rep. (2015) 15:68. doi: 10.1007/s11910-015-0591-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Taber KH, Warden DL, Hurley RA. Blast-related traumatic brain injury: what is known? J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. (2006) 18:141–5. doi: 10.1176/jnp.2006.18.2.141

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Hicks RR, Fertig SJ, Desrocher RE, Koroshetz WJ, Pancrazio JJ. Neurological effects of blast injury. J Trauma. (2010) 68:1257–63. doi: 10.1097/TA.0b013e3181d8956d

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Barnes DE, Kaup A, Kirby KA, Byers AL, Diaz-Arrastia R, Yaffe K. Traumatic brain injury and risk of dementia in older veterans. Neurology. (2014) 83:312–9. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000000616

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Sajja VSSS, Hubbard WB, Hall CS, Ghoddoussi F, Galloway MP, et al. Enduring deficits in memory and neuronal pathology after blast-induced traumatic brain injury. Scient Rep. (2015) 5:15075. doi: 10.1038/srep15075

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Antunes M, Biala G. The novel object recognition memory: neurobiology, test procedure, its modifications. Cogn Process. (2012) 13:93–110. doi: 10.1007/s10339-011-0430-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Cohen SJ, Stackman RW Jr. Assessing rodent hippocampal involvement in the novel object recognition task. A review. Behav Brain Res. (2015) 285:105–17. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2014.08.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Vorhees CV, Williams MT. Assessing spatial learning and memory in rodents. ILAR J. (2014) 55:310–32. doi: 10.1093/ilar/ilu013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Hollup SA, Molden S, Donnett JG, Moser MB, Moser EI. Accumulation of hippocampal place fields at the goal location in an annular watermaze task. J Neurosci. (2001) 21:1635–44. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-05-01635.2001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Logue SF, Paylor R, Wehner JM. Hippocampal lesions cause learning deficits in inbred mice in the Morris water maze and conditioned-fear task. Behav Neurosci. (1997) 111:104–13. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.111.1.104

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Harrison FE, Hosseini AH, McDonald MP. Endogenous anxiety and stress responses in water maze and Barnes maze spatial memory tasks. Behav Brain Res. (2009) 198:247–51. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2008.10.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Kraeuter AK, Guest PC, Sarnyai Z. The Y-maze for assessment of spatial working and reference memory in mice. Methods Mol Biol. (2019) 1916:105–11. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4939-8994-2_10

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Saljo A, Svensson B, Mayorga M, Hamberger A, Bolouri H. Low-level blasts raise intracranial pressure and impair cognitive function in rats. J Neurotrauma. (2009) 26:1345–52. doi: 10.1089/neu.2008.0856

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Saljo A, Bolouri H, Mayorga M, Svensson B, Hamberger A. Low-level blast raises intracranial pressure and impairs cognitive function in rats: prophylaxis with processed cereal feed. J Neurotrauma. (2010) 27:383–9. doi: 10.1089/neu.2009.1053

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Budde MD, Shah A, McCrea M, Cullinan WE, Pintar FA, Stemper BD. Primary blast traumatic brain injury in the rat: relating diffusion tensor imaging and behavior. Front Neurol. (2013) 4:154. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2013.00154

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Zuckerman A, Ram O, Ifergane G, Matar MA, Sagi R, Ostfeld I, et al. Controlled low-pressure blast-wave exposure causes distinct behavioral and morphological responses modelling mild traumatic brain injury, post-traumatic stress disorder, and comorbid mild traumatic brain injury-post-traumatic stress disorder. J Neurotrauma. (2017) 34:145–64. doi: 10.1089/neu.2015.4310

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Stemper BD, Shah AS, Budde MD, Olsen CM, Glavaski-Joksimovic A, Kurpad SN, et al. Behavioral outcomes differ between rotational acceleration and blast mechanisms of mild traumatic brain injury. Front Neurol. (2016) 7:31. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2016.00031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Hall AA, Mendoza MI, Zhou H, Shaughness M, Maudlin-Jeronimo E, McCarron RM, et al. Repeated low intensity blast exposure is associated with damaged endothelial glycocalyx and downstream behavioral deficits. Front Behav Neurosci. (2017) 11:104. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2017.00104

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Elder GA, Dorr NP, De Gasperi R, Gama Sosa MA, Shaughness MC, Maudlin-Jeronimo E, et al. Blast exposure induces post-traumatic stress disorder-related traits in a rat model of mild traumatic brain injury. J Neurotrauma. (2012) 29:2564–75. doi: 10.1089/neu.2012.2510

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Tompkins P, Tesiram Y, Lerner M, Gonzalez LP, Lightfoot S, Rabb CH, et al. Brain injury: neuro-inflammation, cognitive deficit, and magnetic resonance imaging in a model of blast induced traumatic brain injury. J Neurotrauma. (2013) 30:1888–97. doi: 10.1089/neu.2012.2674

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Ning YL, Yang N, Chen X, Zhao ZA, Zhang XZ, Chen XY, et al. Chronic caffeine exposure attenuates blast-induced memory deficit in mice. Chin J Traumatol. (2015) 18:204–11. doi: 10.1016/j.cjtee.2015.10.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Ouyang J, Pace E, Lepczyk L, Kaufman M, Zhang J, Perrine SA, et al. Blast-induced tinnitus and elevated central auditory and limbic activity in rats: a manganese-enhanced MRI and behavioral study. Sci Rep. (2017) 7:4852. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-04941-w

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Lucke-Wold BP, Logsdon AF, Turner RC, Huber JD, Rosen CL. Endoplasmic reticulum stress modulation as a target for ameliorating effects of blast induced traumatic brain injury. J Neurotrauma. (2017) 34:S62–S70. doi: 10.1089/neu.2016.4680

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Yin TC, Voorhees JR, Genova RM, Davis KC, Madison AM, Britt JK, et al. Acute axonal degeneration drives development of cognitive, motor, and visual deficits after blast-mediated traumatic brain injury in mice. eNeuro. (2016) 3. doi: 10.1523/ENEURO.0220-16.2016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Kovesdi E, Kamnaksh A, Wingo D, Ahmed F, Grunberg NE, Long JB, et al. Acute minocycline treatment mitigates the symptoms of mild blast-induced traumatic brain injury. Front Neurol. (2012) 3:111. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2012.00111

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Kovesdi E, Gyorgy AB, Kwon SK, Wingo DL, Kamnaksh A, Long JB, et al. The effect of enriched environment on the outcome of traumatic brain injury; a behavioral, proteomics, histological study. Front Neurosci. (2011) 5:42. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2011.00042

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Song H, Konan LM, Cui J, Johnson CE, Langenderfer M, Grant D, et al. Ultrastructural brain abnormalities and associated behavioral changes in mice after low-intensity blast exposure. Behav Brain Res. (2018) 347:148–57. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2018.03.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Tweedie D, Rachmany L, Rubovitch V, Zhang Y, Becker KG, Perez E, et al. Changes in mouse cognition and hippocampal gene expression observed in a mild physical- and blast-traumatic brain injury. Neurobiol Dis. (2013) 54:1–11. doi: 10.1016/j.nbd.2013.02.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Tweedie D, Rachmany L, Rubovitch V, Li Y, Holloway HW, Lehrmann E, et al. Blast traumatic brain injury-induced cognitive deficits are attenuated by preinjury or postinjury treatment with the glucagon-like peptide-1 receptor agonist, exendin-4. Alzheimers Dement. (2016) 12:34–48. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2015.07.489

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Cho HJ, Sajja VS, Vandevord PJ, Lee YW. Blast induces oxidative stress, inflammation, neuronal loss and subsequent short-term memory impairment in rats. Neuroscience. (2013) 253:9–20. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2013.08.037

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Perez-Garcia G, De Gasperi R, Gama Sosa MA, Perez GM, Otero-Pagan A, Tschiffely A, et al. PTSD-related behavioral traits in a rat model of blast-induced mTBI are reversed by the mGluR2/3 receptor antagonist BCI-838. eNeuro. (2018) 5. doi: 10.1523/ENEURO.0357-17.2018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Baalman KL, Cotton RJ, Rasband SN, Rasband MN. Blast wave exposure impairs memory and decreases axon initial segment length. J Neurotr. (2013) 30:741–51. doi: 10.1089/neu.2012.2478

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Sajja VS, Perrine SA, Ghoddoussi F, Hall CS, Galloway MP, VandeVord PJ. Blast neurotrauma impairs working memory and disrupts prefrontal myo-inositol levels in rats. Mol Cell Neurosci. (2014) 59:119–26. doi: 10.1016/j.mcn.2014.02.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Perez-Garcia G, Gama Sosa MA, De Gasperi R, Lashof-Sullivan M, Maudlin-Jeronimo E, Stone JR, et al. Chronic post-traumatic stress disorder-related traits in a rat model of low-level blast exposure. Behav Brain Res. (2018) 340:117–25. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2016.09.061

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Beamer M, Tummala SR, Gullotti D, Kopil C, Gorka S, Ted A, et al. Primary blast injury causes cognitive impairments and hippocampal circuit alterations. Exp Neurol. (2016) 283(Pt A):16–28. doi: 10.1016/j.expneurol.2016.05.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Muelbl MJ, Slaker ML, Shah AS, Nawarawong NN, Gerndt CH, Budde MD, et al. Effects of mild blast traumatic brain injury on cognitive- and addiction-related behaviors. Sci Rep. (2018) 8:9941. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-28062-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Collen J, Orr N, Lettieri CJ, Carter K, Holley AB. Sleep disturbances among soldiers with combat-related traumatic brain injury. Chest. (2012) 142:622–30. doi: 10.1378/chest.11-1603

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Rosenfeld JV, Ford NL. Bomb blast, mild traumatic brain injury and psychiatric morbidity: a review. Injury. (2010) 41:437–43. doi: 10.1016/j.injury.2009.11.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Fann JR, Hart T, Schomer KG. Treatment for depression after traumatic brain injury: a systematic review. J Neurotr. (2009) 26:2383–402. doi: 10.1089/neu.2009.1091

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Rosenthal M, Christensen BK, Ross TP. Depression following traumatic brain injury. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. (1998) 79:90–103. doi: 10.1016/S0003-9993(98)90215-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

72. Can A, Dao DT, Arad M, Terrillion CE, Piantadosi SC, Gould TD. The mouse forced swim test. JoVE. (2012) 2012:e3638. doi: 10.3791/3638

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

73. Slattery DA, Cryan JF. Using the rat forced swim test to assess antidepressant-like activity in rodents. Nat Protoc. (2012) 7:1009–14. doi: 10.1038/nprot.2012.044

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

74. Yankelevitch-Yahav R, Franko M, Huly A, Doron R. The forced swim test as a model of depressive-like behavior. J Vis Exp. (2015) 52587. doi: 10.3791/52587

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

75. Steimer T Animal models of anxiety disorders in rats and mice: some conceptual issues. Dialog Clin Neurosci. (2011) 13:495–506.

76. Hale MW, Hay-Schmidt A, Mikkelsen JD, Poulsen B, Shekhar A, Lowry CA. Exposure to an open-field arena increases c-Fos expression in a distributed anxiety-related system projecting to the basolateral amygdaloid complex. Neuroscience. (2008) 155:659–72. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2008.05.054

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

77. Gray JA. Précis of The neuropsychology of anxiety: an enquiry into the functions of the septo-hippocampal system. Behav Brain Sci. (1982) 5:469–84. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X00013066

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

78. Duncan GE, Knapp DJ, Breese GR. Neuroanatomical characterization of Fos induction in rat behavioral models of anxiety. Brain Res. (1996) 713:79–91. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(95)01486-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

79. Campbell BM, Merchant KM. Serotonin 2C receptors within the basolateral amygdala induce acute fear-like responses in an open-field environment. Brain Res. (2003) 993:1–9. doi: 10.1016/S0006-8993(03)03384-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

80. Abrams JK, Johnson PL, Hay-Schmidt A, Mikkelsen JD, Shekhar A, Lowry CA. Serotonergic systems associated with arousal and vigilance behaviors following administration of anxiogenic drugs. Neuroscience. (2005) 133:983–97. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2005.03.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

81. Divani AA, Murphy AJ, Meints J, Sadeghi-Bazargani H, Nordberg J, Monga M, et al. A novel preclinical model of moderate primary blast-induced traumatic brain injury. J Neurotrauma. (2015) 32:1109–16. doi: 10.1089/neu.2014.3686

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

82. Kamnaksh A, Kovesdi E, Kwon SK, Wingo D, Ahmed F, Grunberg NE, et al. Factors affecting blast traumatic brain injury. J Neurotrauma. (2011) 28:2145–53. doi: 10.1089/neu.2011.1983

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

83. Russell AL, Handa RJ, Wu TJ. Sex-dependent effects of mild blast-induced traumatic brain injury on corticotropin-releasing factor receptor gene expression: potential link to anxiety-like behaviors. Neuroscience. (2018) 392:1–12. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2018.09.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

84. Statz JK, Ciarlone SL, Goodrich JA, McCarron RM, Walker PB, Norris JN, et al. Affective profiling for anxiety-like behavior in a rodent model of mTBI. Behav Brain Res. (2019) 368:111895. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2019.04.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

85. Huang E, Ngo M, Yee S, Held L, Norman K, Scremin AM, et al. Repeated blast exposure alters open field behavior recorded under low illumination. Brain Res. (2013) 1529:125–33. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2013.06.042

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

86. Park E, Eisen R, Kinio A, Baker AJ. Electrophysiological white matter dysfunction and association with neurobehavioral deficits following low-level primary blast trauma. Neurobiol Dis. (2013) 52:150–9. doi: 10.1016/j.nbd.2012.12.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

87. Hubbard WB, Lashof-Sullivan M, Greenberg S, Norris C, Eck J, Lavik E, et al. Hemostatic nanoparticles increase survival, mitigate neuropathology and alleviate anxiety in a rodent blast trauma model. Sci Rep. (2018) 8:10622. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-28848-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

88. Kawa L, Arborelius UP, Yoshitake T, Kehr J, Hokfelt T, Risling M, et al. Neurotransmitter systems in a mild blast traumatic brain injury model: catecholamines and serotonin. J Neurotrauma. (2015) 32:1190–9. doi: 10.1089/neu.2014.3669

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

89. Sajja VSSS, Hubbard WB, VandeVord PJ. Subacute oxidative stress and glial reactivity in the amygdala are associated with increased anxiety following blast neurotrauma. Shock (Augusta, Ga.). (2015) 44 (Suppl. 1):71–8. doi: 10.1097/SHK.0000000000000311

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

90. Walf AA, Frye CA. The use of the elevated plus maze as an assay of anxiety-related behavior in rodents. Nat Prot. (2007) 2:322–8. doi: 10.1038/nprot.2007.44

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

91. Gould TD, Dao DT, Kovacsics CE. The Open Field Test, in Mood and Anxiety Related Phenotypes in Mice. New York, NY: Springer. (2009). p. 1–20. doi: 10.1007/978-1-60761-303-9_1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

92. Seibenhener ML, Wooten MC. Use of the Open Field Maze to measure locomotor and anxiety-like behavior in mice. J Vis Exp. (2015) 2015:e52434. doi: 10.3791/52434

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

93. Bourin M, Hascoet M. The mouse light/dark box test. Eur J Pharmacol. (2003) 463:55–65. doi: 10.1016/S0014-2999(03)01274-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

94. Chen Y, Huang W. Non-impact, blast-induced mild TBI and PTSD: concepts and caveats. Brain Inj. (2011) 25:641–50. doi: 10.3109/02699052.2011.580313

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

95. Bryant R Post-traumatic stress disorder vs traumatic brain injury. Dialogues Clin Neurosci. (2011) 13:251–62.

96. Sinz EH, Kochanek PM, Dixon CER, Clark SB, Carcillo JA, Schiding JK, et al. Inducible nitric oxide synthase is an endogenous neuroprotectant after traumatic brain injury in rats and mice. J Clin Invest. (1999) 104:647–56. doi: 10.1172/JCI6670

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

97. McAllister TW Neurobiological consequences of traumatic brain injury. Dialogues Clin Neurosci. (2011) 13:287–300. doi: 10.1176/appi.books.9781585624201.js15

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

98. Schneiderman AI, Braver ER, Kang HK. Understanding sequelae of injury mechanisms and mild traumatic brain injury incurred during the conflicts in Iraq and Afghanistan: persistent postconcussive symptoms and posttraumatic stress disorder. Am J Epidemiol. (2008) 167:1446–52. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwn068

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

99. Curzon P, Rustay NR, Browman KE. Cued and contextual fear conditioning for rodents. In: Buccafusco JJ, editor. Methods of Behavior Analysis in Neuroscience. Chapter 2. Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press/Taylor & Francis (2009).

Google Scholar

100. Genovese RF, Simmons LP, Ahlers ST, Maudlin-Jeronimo E, Dave JR, Boutte AM. Effect of mild TBI from repeated blast overpressure on the expression and extinction of conditioned fear in rats. Neuroscience. (2013) 254:120–9. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2013.09.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

101. Gattu R, Akin FW, Cacace AT, Hall CD, Murnane OD, Haacke EM. Vestibular, balance, microvascular and white matter neuroimaging characteristics of blast injuries and mild traumatic brain injury: Four case reports. Brain Inj. (2016) 30:1501–14. doi: 10.1080/02699052.2016.1219056

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

102. Akin FW, Murnane OD. Head injury and blast exposure: vestibular consequences. Otolaryngol Clin North Am. (2011) 44:323–34, viii. doi: 10.1016/j.otc.2011.01.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

103. Vanderploeg RD, Curtiss G, Luis CA, Salazar AM. Long-term morbidities following self-reported mild traumatic brain injury. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. (2007) 29:585–98. doi: 10.1080/13803390600826587

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

104. Wares JR, Hoke KW, Walker W, Franke LM, Cifu DX, Carne W, et al. Characterizing effects of mild traumatic brain injury and posttraumatic stress disorder on balance impairments in blast-exposed servicemembers and Veterans using computerized posturography. J Rehabil Res Dev. (2015) 52:591–603. doi: 10.1682/JRRD.2014.08.0197

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

105. Rodriguez UA, Zeng Y, Deyo D, Parsley MA, Hawkins BE, Prough DS, et al. Effects of mild blast traumatic brain injury on cerebral vascular, histopathological, and behavioral outcomes in rats. J Neurotrauma. (2018) 35:375–92. doi: 10.1089/neu.2017.5256

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

106. Turner RC, Naser ZJ, Logsdon AF, DiPasquale KH, Jackson GJ, Robson MJ, et al. Modeling clinically relevant blast parameters based on scaling principles produces functional & histological deficits in rats. Exp Neurol. (2013) 248:520–9. doi: 10.1016/j.expneurol.2013.07.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

107. Wang Y, Wei YL, Oguntayo S, Wilkins W, Arun P, Valiyaveettil M, et al. Tightly coupled repetitive blast-induced traumatic brain injury: development and characterization in mice. J Neurotr. (2011) 28:2171–83. doi: 10.1089/neu.2011.1990

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

108. Hamm RJ, Pike BR, O'Dell DM, Lyeth BG, Jenkins LW. The rotarod test: an evaluation of its effectiveness in assessing motor deficits following traumatic brain injury. J Neurotr. (1994) 11:187–96. doi: 10.1089/neu.1994.11.187

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

109. Dunham NW, Miya TS. A note on a simple apparatus for detecting neurological deficit in rats and mice. J Am Pharm Assoc Am Pharm Assoc. (1957) 46:208–9. doi: 10.1002/jps.3030460322

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

110. Pearl J, Stander H, McKean DB. Effects of analgesics and other drugs on mice in phenylquinone and rotarod tests. J Pharmacol Exp Ther. (1969) 167:9–13.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

111. Buitrago MM, Schulz JB, Dichgans J, Luft AR. Short and long-term motor skill learning in an accelerated rotarod training paradigm. Neurobiol Learn Mem. (2004) 81:211–6. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2004.01.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

112. Scott D, Saatman KE. Cognitive and Motor Function Assessments in Rodent Models of Traumatic Brain Injury, in Pre-Clinical and Clinical Methods in Brain Trauma Research. Springer. (2018). p. 139–54. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4939-8564-7_9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

113. Mizoguchi K, Yuzurihara M, Ishige A, Sasaki H, Tabira T. Chronic stress impairs rotarod performance in rats: implications for depressive state. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. (2002) 71:79–84. doi: 10.1016/S0091-3057(01)00636-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

114. Metz GA, Whishaw IQ. The ladder rung walking task: a scoring system and its practical application. J Vis Exp. (2009) 1204. doi: 10.3791/1204

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

115. Yurgil KA, Clifford RE, Risbrough VB, Geyer MA, Huang M, Barkauskas DA, et al. Prospective associations between traumatic brain injury and postdeployment tinnitus in active-duty marines. J Head Trauma Rehabil. (2016) 31:30–9. doi: 10.1097/HTR.0000000000000117

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

116. Clifford RE, Baker D, Risbrough VB, Huang M, Yurgil KA. Impact of TBI, PTSD, and hearing loss on tinnitus progression in a US marine cohort. Mil Med. (2019) 184:839–46. doi: 10.1093/milmed/usz016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

117. Swan AA, Nelson JT, Swiger B, Jaramillo CA, Eapen BC, Packer M, et al. Prevalence of hearing loss and tinnitus in Iraq and Afghanistan Veterans: A Chronic Effects of Neurotrauma Consortium study. Hear Res. (2017) 349:4–12. doi: 10.1016/j.heares.2017.01.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

118. Oleksiak M, Smith BM, St Andre JR, Caughlan CM, Steiner M. Audiological issues and hearing loss among Veterans with mild traumatic brain injury. J Rehabil Res Dev. (2012) 49:995–1004. doi: 10.1682/JRRD.2011.01.0001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

119. Koch M, Schnitzler HU. The acoustic startle response in rats–circuits mediating evocation, inhibition and potentiation. Behav Brain Res. (1997) 89:35–49. doi: 10.1016/S0166-4328(97)02296-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

120. Fitch RH, Threlkeld SW, McClure MM, Peiffer AM. Use of a modified prepulse inhibition paradigm to assess complex auditory discrimination in rodents. Brain Res Bull. (2008) 76:1–7. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2007.07.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

121. Davis M, Gendelman DS, Tischler MD, Gendelman PM. A primary acoustic startle circuit: lesion and stimulation studies. J Neurosci. (1982) 2:791–805. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.02-06-00791.1982

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

122. Krase W, Koch M, Schnitzler HU. Glutamate antagonists in the reticular formation reduce the acoustic startle response. Neuroreport. (1993) 4:13–6. doi: 10.1097/00001756-199301000-00003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

123. Koch M, Lingenhohl K, Pilz PK. Loss of the acoustic startle response following neurotoxic lesions of the caudal pontine reticular formation: possible role of giant neurons. Neuroscience. (1992) 49:617–25. doi: 10.1016/0306-4522(92)90231-P

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

124. Valsamis B, Schmid S. Habituation and prepulse inhibition of acoustic startle in rodents. J Vis Exp. (2011) 2011:e3446. doi: 10.3791/3446

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

125. Galazyuk A, Hebert S. Gap-prepulse inhibition of the acoustic startle Reflex (GPIAS) for tinnitus assessment: current status and future directions. Front Neurol. (2015) 6:88. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2015.00088

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

126. Campolo J, Lobarinas E, Salvi R. Does tinnitus “fill in” the silent gaps? Noise Health. (2013) 15:398–405. doi: 10.4103/1463-1741.121232

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

127. Fournier P, Hebert S. Gap detection deficits in humans with tinnitus as assessed with the acoustic startle paradigm: does tinnitus fill in the gap? Hear Res. (2013) 295:16–23. doi: 10.1016/j.heares.2012.05.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

128. Mahmood G, Mei Z, Hojjat H, Pace E, Kallakuri S, Zhang JS. Therapeutic effect of sildenafil on blast-induced tinnitus and auditory impairment. Neuroscience. (2014) 269:367–82. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2014.03.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

129. Luo H, Pace E, Zhang XG, Zhang JS. Blast-induced tinnitus and spontaneous firing changes in the rat dorsal cochlear nucleus. J Neurosc Res. (2014) 92:1466–77. doi: 10.1002/jnr.23424

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

130. Mao JC, Pace E, Pierozynski P, Kou Z, Shen Y, VandeVord P, et al. Blast-induced tinnitus and hearing loss in rats: behavioral and imaging assays. J Neurotrauma. (2012) 29:430–44. doi: 10.1089/neu.2011.1934

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

131. Miyamoto M, Shintani M, Nagaoka A, Nagawa Y. Lesioning of the rat basal forebrain leads to memory impairments in passive and active avoidance tasks. Brain Res. (1985) 328:97–104. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(85)91327-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

132. Pickens CL, Navarre BM, Nair SG. Incubation of conditioned fear in the conditioned suppression model in rats: role of food-restriction conditions, length of conditioned stimulus, generality to conditioned freezing. Neuroscience. (2010) 169:1501–10. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2010.06.036

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

133. McDannald MA. Contributions of the amygdala central nucleus and ventrolateral periaqueductal grey to freezing and instrumental suppression in Pavlovian fear conditioning. Behav Brain Res. (2010) 211:111–7. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2010.03.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

134. Amorapanth P, Nader K, LeDoux JE. Lesions of periaqueductal gray dissociate-conditioned freezing from conditioned suppression behavior in rats. Learn Mem. (1999) 6:491–9. doi: 10.1101/lm.6.5.491

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: blast TBI, behavior deficits, cognitive deficits, anxiety and depression, motor deficits, auditory deficits, fear conditioning

Citation: Aravind A, Ravula AR, Chandra N and Pfister BJ (2020) Behavioral Deficits in Animal Models of Blast Traumatic Brain Injury. Front. Neurol. 11:990. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2020.00990

Received: 18 March 2020; Accepted: 29 July 2020;
Published: 04 September 2020.

Edited by:

Denes V. Agoston, Karolinska Institutet (KI), Sweden

Reviewed by:

Samar Abdelhady, Faculty of Medicine, Alexandria University, Egypt
Alana Conti, Wayne State University, United States

Copyright © 2020 Aravind, Ravula, Chandra and Pfister. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Namas Chandra, bmFtYXMuY2hhbmRyYSYjeDAwMDQwO25qaXQuZWR1; Bryan J. Pfister, YnJ5YW4uai5wZmlzdGVyJiN4MDAwNDA7bmppdC5lZHU=

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.