Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Neurol., 05 December 2018
Sec. Movement Disorders
This article is part of the Research Topic Current State of Postural Research - Beyond Automatic Behavior View all 20 articles

Postural Dynamics Are Associated With Cognitive Decline in Parkinson's Disease

\r\nAnnette Pantall*Annette Pantall1*Piriya Suresparan&#x;Piriya Suresparan1Leanne KapaLeanne Kapa1Rosie Morris,Rosie Morris1,2Alison YarnallAlison Yarnall3Silvia Del DinSilvia Del Din1Lynn Rochester,Lynn Rochester1,3
  • 1Clinical Ageing Research Unit, Institute of Neuroscience, Newcastle University Institute of Ageing, Newcastle upon Tyne, United Kingdom
  • 2Department of Neurology, Oregon Health and Science University, Portland, OR, United States
  • 3The Newcastle upon Tyne NHS Foundation Trust, Newcastle upon Tyne, United Kingdom

Early features of Parkinson's disease (PD) include both motor and cognitive changes, suggesting shared common pathways. A common motor dysfunction is postural instability, a known predictor of falls, which have a major impact on quality of life. Understanding mechanisms of postural dynamics in PD and specifically how they relate to cognitive changes is essential for developing effective interventions. The aims of this study were to examine the changes that occur in postural metrics over time and explore the relationship between postural and cognitive dysfunction. The study group consisted of 35 people (66 ± 8years, 12 female, UPDRS III: 22.5 ± 9.6) diagnosed with PD who were recruited as part of the Incidence of Cognitive Impairment in Cohorts with Longitudinal Evaluation—PD Gait (ICICLE-GAIT) study. Postural and cognitive assessments were performed at 18, 36, and 54 months after enrolment. Participants stood still for 120 s, eyes open and arms by their side. Postural dynamics were measured using metrics derived from a single tri-axial accelerometer (Axivity AX3, York, UK) on the lower back. Accelerometry metrics included jerk (derivative of acceleration), root mean square, frequency, and ellipsis (acceleration area). Cognition was evaluated by neuropsychological tests including the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) and digit span. There was a significant decrease in accelerometry parameters, greater in the anteroposterior direction, and a decline in cognitive function over time. Accelerometry metrics were positively correlated with lower cognitive function and increased geriatric depression score and negatively associated with a qualitative measure of balance confidence. In conclusion, people with PD showed reduced postural dynamics that may represent a postural safety strategy. Associations with cognitive function and depression, both symptoms that may pre-empt motor symptoms, suggest shared neural pathways. Further studies, involving neuroimaging, may determine how these postural parameters relate to underlying neural and clinical correlates.

Introduction

Parkinson's disease (PD) is a common progressive neurodegenerative disease with a UK incidence of 84 per 100,000 in adults over 50 years (1). Clinical characterizations include both motor and non-motor manifestations, indicative of a multisystem neurodegenerative disease (2). Common motor symptoms include resting tremor, bradykinesia, rigidity, and postural instability (3). Postural instability is clinically important as it is a predictor of falls, which impact on quality of life (4). Falls may result in injury, leading to possible loss of functional independence, institutionalization and a poor quality of life (5, 6). Previous studies indicate that 38–68% of people with PD are subject to falls, 25% of which have two or more falls every 6 months (710).

Postural instability is classically defined as the inability to maintain the center of mass within its base of support. Clinically, the retropulsion (pull) test is applied to assess postural instability (11). Postural sway is an indicator of postural instability and a measure of the sensorimotor control loop that regulates standing balance (12). Postural sway is greater in fallers than non-fallers, therefore is an important clinical marker (13). The traditional method of recording postural sway involves tracking the center of pressure using a force platform (14). More recently, body-worn sensors (e.g., accelerometers, gyroscopes, magnetometers, insole pressure sensors) have been developed which permit measurements to be made outside a laboratory setting (12). Strong correlations have been found between accelerometer parameters (postural dynamics) and force-platform derived center of pressure data, thereby validating the accelerometer as a method for assessing postural sway (15, 16). Accelerometry metrics during the first 30 s of standing have been reported to be discriminative of PD (17).

Mild cognitive impairment (MCI) is a non-motor feature present in over 20% of patients at initial diagnosis (18). Cognitive function deteriorates with disease progression, with a >2-fold increase in MCI reported over 3 years (19). Cognitive decline in PD is associated with dysfunction of both dopaminergic and cholinergic pathways as well as increased Lewy bodies and possible vascular pathology (20, 21). Performing a cognitive task while standing has been reported to increase postural parameters and by implication, postural instability in people with PD (2224) compared to healthy older adults. This suggests a “posture second” prioritization, associated with decreased attentional resources available. With cognitive decline, one might anticipate increased postural instability and correlation between cognitive function and postural parameters. Studies have reported a relationship between cognitive changes and postural instability (25, 26) in addition to an association with fall risk (27, 28). This may reflect common shared pathways or adverse events affecting multiple networks. Another common non-motor symptom in PD is depression which has been observed in, on average, 40% of people with PD (29). In PD, Lewy bodies have been found in many subcortical nuclei including the locus coeruleus (30). The locus coeruleus is associated with arousal and also muscle tone critical for postural stability. Patients with depression have been reported to have greater changes in the locus coeruleus compared to non-depressed patients (31). Several studies have reported an association between depression and postural instability (32, 33). Changes in the locus coeruleus and noradrenergic system may partly account for the association between posture and depression. In PD, motor and non-motor features do not exist as separate entities, but rather display interactions, which warrant further investigation.

Information regarding the time-course of postural sway in people with PD is limited as few longitudinal postural studies have been undertaken. Understanding the progression of postural sway may improve our understanding of underlying mechanisms. Exploring the relationship between postural dynamics and cognitive function will inform us of the interaction between the motor and non-motor systems and the effect of diminishing attentional resources on postural stability. Examining the association between depression and postural instability may illuminate the effect of shared pathways on these motor and non-motor features. Clinically, understanding mechanisms underlying postural instability is important given the impact postural control has on falls, gait, and mobility. The main aim of this longitudinal study was to explore how postural dynamics change during quiet standing in people with PD over 36 months. Postural dynamics were determined from accelerometer recordings over the course of a 120 s standing balance test. The hypothesis was that measures of postural dynamics would increase over the 36 months period, indicating increased postural instability. A further hypothesis was that the greatest change in parameters would occur during the first 30 s of the postural task. The second aim was to investigate the relationship between postural instability and global cognition and depression. The hypothesis was that there would be a significant relationship between motor and non-motor features.

Methods

Participants and Clinical Assessments

The study group consisted of 35 people recruited from the Incidence of Cognitive Impairment in Cohorts with Longitudinal Evaluation—Parkinson's disease Gait study (ICICLE-PD GAIT) study (34). The participants underwent a baseline assessment, followed by cognitive and postural assessments at 18, 36, and 54 months. Not all participants underwent a baseline postural assessment; therefore, this study does not include baseline measurements. Participants were assessed at the Clinical Aging Research Unit, Newcastle University. The study was approved by the Newcastle and North Tyneside research ethics committee and conducted according to the declaration of Helsinki. All participants signed an informed consent form prior to testing.

The exclusion criteria included any neurological (other than PD), orthopedic or cardiothoracic condition that may adversely have affected the participant's gait or safety. Additional exclusion factors included cognitive impairment (Mini Mental State Exam (MMSE) ≤ 24) and difficulties comprehending English. Parkinson's disease was diagnosed according to the UK Parkinson's Brain Bank criteria (35).

At each assessment, demographic, clinical, and cognitive information were collected. The Hoehn and Yahr scale was used to measure the motor symptom severity in PD participants (36). The Movement Disorder Society Unified Parkinson's Disease Rating Scale (MDS—UPDRS) Part III (37) assessed motor function in PD (0-no motor symptoms, 132-severe motor symptoms). Balance confidence was assessed using the Activities Balance Self Confidence Scale (ABC), with a score of 0 indicating no confidence and a score of 100 indicating complete confidence in balance when performing various activities (38). Cognitive tests included the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) (39) for global cognition (score range 0–30) with a score of 26–30 indicating normal cognitive function and < 26 suggesting cognitive impairment. The Wechsler Forward Digit Span tested working memory (40), the average number of digits a healthy adult can recall being 7 ± 2 (41). The short Geriatric Depression Scale (GDS) (42) was used as a measure of depressive symptoms (score range 0–15). A score of 0–4 is considered normal, 5–8 indicates mild depression and a score of 12–15 indicates severe depression.

Standing Balance Test

The standing balance test was carried out an hour after medication intake. Participants stood in an upright position with their feet positioned within a predefined area (400 mm wide × 600 mm long), with their hands by their side (43) and looking straight ahead for 120 s. There were no foot placement restrictions and the participants wore their shoes during the test. The recording began 3 s after the participant had understood the instructions of the tests.

Equipment

A tri-axial accelerometer-based monitor (Axivity AX3, York, UK) on the lower back (L5) recorded acceleration at a sampling rate of 100 Hz (17). The accuracy of the accelerometer clock was ±20 parts per million, the resolution was 0.976 mg, the weight of the accelerometer was 9 g with dimensions of 6.0 × 21.5 × 31.5 mm. The Axivity AX3 accelerometer has been validated for recording high resolution movement (44). The accelerometer was attached to the skin with a hydrogel adhesive and Hypafix bandage.

Data Processing

The data processing and analysis have previously been described by Del Din et al. (17). In summary, the data were downloaded to a computer and analyzed by customized MATLAB (R2015a, Mathworks, Natick, MA, USA.) algorithms. Analyses included rotation of the data into anteroposterior (AP), mediolateral (ML), and vertical accelerations using the Moe-Nilssen transformation (45). The following features were then extracted:

a) Jerk (m2.s−5): the rate of change of acceleration (46).

Jerk was calculated for AP and ML and combined axes.

b) Root mean square [RMS (m.s−2)]: a measure of amplitude (46).

RMS was calculated for AP and ML and combined axes.

c) Frequency (Hz): 95% of power of the acceleration power spectrum below frequency.

Frequency was estimated for AP and ML axes (46).

d) Ellipsis (m2.s−4): the area comprising 95% of the AP and ML acceleration trajectories (14).

The four features were selected based on previous studies by Mancini et al. (16) who showed these to be sensitive to disease progression and disease discrimination. Additionally, these features can discriminate between different postural tasks in healthy older adults (47).

All accelerometer features were then determined for the following three phases of standing; the first 30 s, the first 60 s and the entire 120 s.

Statistical Analysis

The data were analyzed using SPSS software (v21; IBM, Chicago, IL, USA). Outliers >2 standard deviations from the mean were removed from the datasets. Linear mixed-effects models were applied to determine the main effects of time-points (18, 36, 54 months), axes (AP, ML, combined) and phase (30, 60, 120 s) and their interaction effects on accelerometry parameters (p < 0.05). The RANDOM subcommand was used to model the covariance between the three axes and between the three phases. Sidak corrections for multiple comparisons were applied. The Friedmann test was applied to non-parametric Levodopa equivalent daily dose (LEDD), Hoehn and Yahr, MDS-UPDRS III, ABC, MoCA, digit span and GDS scores. The Wilcoxon signed-rank test compared different time-points. Spearman's rank correlation was used to examine cross-sectional associations between postural parameters and the ABC, MoCA, and GDS scores. Pearson's product-moment correlation coefficient determined associations between changes in postural parameters and cognitive parameters between time-points. The magnitude of effect of the correlation coefficients was defined by the following: r < 0.10: negligible; 0.10 ≤ r < 0.30: weak; 0.30 ≤ r < 0.50: moderate; r ≥ 0.50: strong (48).

Results

Demographic and Clinical Data

Table 1 lists demographic and clinical information at baseline, 18, 36, and 54 months. There was a greater number of males in the cohort. Although, all participants satisfied the inclusion criterion of MMSE>24, the MoCA at Baseline ranged from 20 to 30 with 10 participants having MoCA scores ≤ 24. There was a significant effect of time for LEDD, Hoehn, and Yahr stage and MDS-UPDRS III [X(3)2 = 78.0, p < 0.001; X(3)2 = 18.1, p < 0.001; X(3)2 = 41.0, p < 0.001, respectively]. H&Y was significantly greater at 18 months compared to baseline (Z = −2.5, p = 0.012). LEDD increased significantly between successive time-points (baseline to 18 months, Z = −5.1, p < 0.001; 18–36 months, Z = −4.8, p < 0.001; 36–54 months, Z = −3.4, p = 0.001). The MDS-UPDRS III score was significantly greater at baseline compared to 18 months (Z = −4.7, p < 0.001), 36 months compared to 18 months (Z = −4.0, p < 0.001) and 54 months compared to baseline (Z = −4.8, p < 0.001). There was a significant time effect for MoCA [X(3)2 = 9.9; p = 0.02], with the score decreasing from 18 to 54 months (Z = −2.5, p = 0.013) and from 36 to 54 months (Z = −2.9, p = 0.004).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Demographic, cognitive, and clinical characteristics of participants at Baseline, 18, 36, and 54 months.

Accelerometer Metrics

Outliers

The metrics of Jerk, RMS, frequency, and ellipsis were analyzed for outliers using the threshold of two standard deviations above or below the mean. Data from these outliers were considered removed from further analysis. Jerk and frequency had the greatest number of outliers across axes, phases, and time-points (1.9%) compared to RMS (1.5%) and ellipsis (1.2%).

Postural Dynamics

a) Axis

The axis had a significant effect on jerk, RMS and frequency (Table 2). Jerk and RMS were greater in the AP direction compared to the ML direction (jerk p = 0.001, effect size = 0.87, power = 65.9%; RMS p < 0.001, effect size = 2.58, power = 100%) (Figure 1). Frequency was however greater in the ML direction (p < 0.001, effect size = 2.60, power = 100%).

b) Time

There was a significant effect of time for ellipsis, which decreased from 18 to 54 months (p = 0.033, effect size = 0.27, power = 45.3%) (Table 2) (Figure 1).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Mixed linear model results for single and interaction effects of time (18, 36, and 54 months), axis (combined, mediolateral, anteroposterior) and phase (30, 60, 120 s) on gait accelerometry parameters.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Changes in jerk (A–C), RMS (D–F), frequency (G,H), and ellipsis (I) at 18, 36, and 54 months. Thirty seconds postural phase; 60 s postural phase; 120 s postural phase. AP, anteroposterior axis; ML, mediolateral axis; a-l—significant difference (p < 0.05) between two timepoints.

Interaction Effect of Phase and Time

The interaction effect of postural phase and time was significant for RMS and ellipsis (Table 2). Pairwise comparisons showed the RMS and ellipsis parameters for the initial 30 s to be significantly lower at 36 months (RMS p = 0.005, effect size = 0.52, power = 63.4%; ellipsis p = 0.032, effect size = 0.44, power = 47.7%) and 54 months (RMS p = 0.001,effect size = 0.52, power = 77.7%; ellipsis p = 0.001, effect size = 0.57, power = 75.6%) compared to 18 months. Additionally, for the initial 60 s there was a decrease from 18 to 54 months for ellipsis (p = 0.023, effect size = 0.41, power = 47.8%) (Figure 1).

Interaction Effect of Axis and Time

There was a significant interaction effect between axis and time for RMS (Table 2). Pairwise comparisons for the RMS parameter showed that, in the combined direction, 18 months was significantly >36 months (p < 0.037, effect size = 0.41, power = 44.1%) and 54 months (p = 0.017, effect size = 0.41, power = 49.4%). Along the AP axis, RMS was greater at 18 months compared to 36 months (p = 0.022, effect size = 0.43, power = 49.6%).

Interaction Effect of Axis, Phase, and Time

The interaction effect of axis, phase, and time was significant for jerk and RMS (Table 2). The 120 s combined jerk parameter was greater at 36 months compared to 18 months (p = 0.036 effect size = 0.24, power = 43.4%) and 36 months compared to 54 months (p = 0.045 effect size = 0.22, power = 41.1%) (Figure 1A). The RMS parameters along the combined axis decreased significantly from 18 to 36 months for 30 s (p = 0.002 effect size = 0.44, power = 70.2%) and 60 s (p = 0.022 effect size = 0.35, power = 48.0%) (Figure 1D). These RMS parameters also decreased from 18 to 54 months (30 s p < 0.001 effect size = 0.46, power = 83.9%, 60 s p = 0.018 effect size = 0.32, power = 50.0%). Additionally, along the AP axis, the 30 s RMS decreased from 18 to 36 months (p < 0.001 effect size = 0.56, power = 86.9%) and 54 months (p < 0.001 effect size = 0.53, power = 89.6%) (Figure 1E). However, for 60 s RMS increased from 36 to 54 months (p = 0.011 effect size = 0.19, power = 51.2%) (Figure 1E).

Association Between Postural Dynamics and Balance Confidence, Cognitive Function and Depression Measures

Table 3 lists cross-sectional correlations between postural measures of jerk, RMS, and ellipsis in the AP direction and ABC, MOCA, and GDS scores at 36 and 54 months. Few correlations were observed for frequency. No relationship was found at 18 months and only few associations for the first 30 and 60 s of standing. No correlations were present between postural measures and the digit span scores.

a) Jerk

At 36 months, moderate negative correlations were found for jerk with ABC, MoCA, and a weak positive correlation with GDS. At 54 months, there was a moderate correlation between jerk and ABC and a weak negative correlation with MoCA.

b) RMS

At 36 months, a moderate negative correlation was observed with ABC, a weak negative correlation between RMS and MoCA and moderate positive correlation with GDS. RMS was moderately negatively correlated with ABC at 54 months and weakly correlated with GDS.

c) Ellipsis

Moderate negative correlations between ellipsis and ABC at 36 and 54 months were found. There was a positive correlation between ellipsis and GDS at 36 months.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Spearman's correlation coefficient for mean postural parameters of jerk, RMS, and ellipsis in the AP direction across 120 s with clinical characteristics at 36 and 54 months.

Correlation Between Change in Postural Dynamics and Change in Balance Confidence, Cognitive Function, and Depression Measures

Correlations between changes in postural parameters for 120 s in the combined direction and ABC, MoCA, and GDS scores are presented in Table 4 and Figure 2. There were no significant correlations from 18 to 36 months between postural parameters and the ABC, MoCA, and GDS scores. The decline in jerk from 36 to 54 months showed a moderate negative correlation with the change in MoCA (Figure 3A) and a moderate positive correlation with change in GDS scale (Figure 3B).

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Pearson's correlation coefficient for change in mean postural dynamic parameters of 120 s jerk, 30 s RMS, and 30 s ellipsis with change in clinical characteristics.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Scatter plots showing the correlation at 36 months between postural parameters and cognitive measures for 120 s phase. (A) Mean root mean square against ABC. (B) Mean jerk against MoCA. (C) Mean ellipsis against GDS. ABC, Activities Balance Self Confidence; MoCA, Montreal Cognitive Assessment; GDS, Geriatric Depression Scale.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Scatter plots showing the correlation between changes in 120 s jerk from 36 to 54 months and (A) changes in MoCA score from 36 to 54 months; (B) changes in GDS from 36 to 54 months. MoCA, Montreal Cognitive Assessment; GDS, Geriatric Depression Scale.

Discussion

In this longitudinal study, we aimed to investigate how postural dynamics change during quiet standing in people with PD over 36 months. Additionally, we investigated the relationship between postural dynamics with balance confidence, global cognition and depression score. We hypothesized that postural dynamics of jerk, RMS, and ellipsis would increase, suggesting increased postural instability, with disease progression. However, our novel findings reported mainly a decline in jerk, RMS, and ellipsis with disease progression. Although most change over time for RMS, frequency and ellipsis was observed for the first 30 s of standing, we found that for jerk, significant changes were present only for the entire 120 s duration. We observed significant relationships between postural parameters with balance confidence, global cognition, and depression score suggesting shared neural pathways.

The decrease in RMS is in partial agreement with Mancini et al. (16). Although Mancini et al. (16) reported an increase over 6–9 months in postural parameters in five individuals not receiving dopaminergic medication, eight subjects on dopaminergic medication displayed trends toward decreased RMS (49). Another study, also reported decreased sway (measured with platform mounted potentiometer) in eight patients with moderate PD (Hoehn and Yahr III–IV) on dopaminergic medication, compared to healthy older adults (50). Interpretation of changes in postural dynamics in people with PD involves not only consideration of changes due to progression of pathology but also concomitant age-related neurodegenerative changes. Duarte and Sternad (51) reported older adults show reduced amplitude of postural sway during prolonged standing compared to younger adults (51). However, in a cross-sectional study, Park et al. (52) reported increased postural accelerometry parameters in older adults, apart from frequency and jerk in the ML direction, which decreased (52). It is unclear to what extent longitudinal changes in RMS in people with PD are the consequence of age-related changes rather than due to progression of PD.

The longitudinal decline in RMS and ellipsis from 18 to 54 months was most prominent for the first 30 s of standing. Del Din et al. (17) have reported that this period was most discriminative between people with PD and healthy older adults (17), suggesting that the initial period requires the greatest sensorimotor integration to achieve balance and is most impacted by PD. Theories to explain postural instability in PD include changes in intermittent and continuous control systems (53), impaired proprioception (54), and alteration in awareness of vertical body position relative to the global axes (55). The initial standing period may highlight more the intermittent and continuous control mechanisms needed to adjust the center of mass position to restore stability. Changes in the body position awareness may have a more prominent effect on postural dynamics with increasing standing duration. Dysfunction of sense of body positioning may result in a greater error in return of center of mass to the position optimal for equilibrium and necessitate faster adjustments (greater jerk) to achieve stability. The increase in jerk over 120 s from 18 to 36 months may be the result of faster corrections due to greater error in positioning of center of mass. The subsequent decrease in jerk over 120 s from to 54 months may reflect the complex interaction of disease progression and dopaminergic treatment on postural control mechanisms. There was approximately a 2-fold increase in LEDD from 36 to 54 months compared to 18 to 36 months, which could account for the decrease in 120 s jerk.

There was a significant effect of direction, with the AP direction showing greater change than ML, with a decrease in RMS value from 18 to 54 months. This finding is supported by a study that reported decreased AP sway during standing in people with PD compared to controls (50). A recent study analyzing postural data from a similar cohort of individuals with PD observed increased regularity of postural dynamics from 18 to 54 months along the AP axis suggesting possible modification of motor control along this axis (56). The reduction in AP postural dynamics may result from greater instability in the AP direction, associated with decreased knee flexion and greater difficulty initiating ankle dorsiflexion to maintain balance (57). Specifically, greater instability has been observed in backward sway when people had normal foot width (57). Axial stiffness increases with PD progression and patients frequently develop a stooped posture (camptocormia) which may also affect AP trunk dynamics (58). However, camptocormia usually presents after 7–8 years of diagnosis, so this is not likely to be a major consideration in our study. The decreased RMS in the AP direction may represent a compensation strategy to maintain stability by keeping movements of the trunk within a smaller, safer range along the AP axis.

We observed negative correlations between postural dynamics with the ABC scale. Greater confidence in standing and a lower fear of falling were associated with lower postural parameters. Reduction in postural dynamics may result from increased lower limb rigidity. Carpenter et al. (59) have reported co-contraction of leg muscles with consequential increased ankle stiffness in people with PD compared to age-matched controls (59). Older adults have also been observed to have increased muscle co-activation compared to young adults with the subgroup of fallers having greater postural sway. Increased co-activation of lower limb agonists and antagonists will result in a more rigid structure, although less reactive to external perturbations. We did not find significant change with disease progression for postural parameters along the ML axis. One study reported increased RMS in the ML direction in people with PD compared to healthy older adults, therefore the RMS value might be expected to increase as the disease progressed (60). However, the observation that there is no longitudinal change may be the result of the interaction of opposing age-related and PD effects on the control of postural dynamics.

We found moderate negative correlations between MoCA and postural dynamics at the 36 months time point with lower cognition associated with increased jerk. Kelly et al. (61) have reported similar findings between lower global cognition and increased postural instability (61). Correlation between postural measures and cognitive tests have also been reported by Nocera et al. (62). Dysfunctions in dopamine networks may to some extent account for this association as impairment in executive function and attention is mediated partially by dopaminergic frontostriatal networks (63). No relationship between postural and cognitive measures was observed at 54 months, which may be due to progression of the pathology, emergence of additional clinical features and effect of medication. Levodopa has been suggested to improve some balance measures but worsen others (64) and in the advanced stages of PD increases postural sway (65). Although both jerk and MoCA decreased on average from 36 to 54 months, we found a moderate negative correlation in the difference between the two time-points for these parameters. The mean change in the MoCA score was 1.23 and reduced the MoCA score at 54 months to 25.54, which is considered clinically to indicate possible cognitive impairment. Our finding suggests there individuals with an increase in postural parameters decrease their cognitive function. This is surprising as mild cognitive impairment is associated with postural instability (66) and increased rates of cognitive decline have been reported in individuals with postural instability and gait disturbance phenotype (67). Possible different effects of disease progression and aging on postural dynamics and cognitive function may explain the results. Further investigation is however needed.

There were moderate positive correlations between GDS score and postural dynamics. The changes in GDS were however small, and even at 54 months the GDS at 3.11 was below the threshold of 5 which has been reported to indicate mild depression. Previous studies have reported a relationship between depression and gait parameters, which was stronger on dopaminergic medication (68, 69). A review of depression in PD reported contradictory findings regarding postural correlates of depression in PD (29). The association between GDS score and postural instability may be related to the physical constraint on activity imposed by postural instability because of the increased falls risk. However, as depression frequently precedes the motor symptoms, the association is more likely to be due to changes in shared neural circuitry (70).

Limitations

The main limiting factor is that we tested patients in the ON medication state. Postural dynamics will differ in the OFF state compared to the ON state, with motor impairment reported to be greater in the OFF state.

Our standing balance test involved participants self-selecting their foot position. This may from 18 to 54 months consequential decrease in postural dynamics. By contrast, many postural studies adopt a standardized foot position, which restricts patients changing their base of support. However, the purpose of our postural analysis was to examine individual postural dynamics by allowing participants to wear their own comfortable footwear and place their feet in a position they considered would provide them with maximum stability.

Conclusion

Postural dynamics decrease over a period of 36 months in people with PD. This may be due to people reducing their postural sway in order to restrict their center of pressure excursions to a smaller “safe” area, as postural instability increases with disease progression. Underlying mechanisms may include co-contraction of agonist and antagonist muscles resulting in increased rigidity. Limiting postural movements may however result in the individual becoming less able to respond to external perturbations and therefore becoming more prone to falls. Postural dynamics are associated at 36 months after diagnosis, with global cognition and depression, emphasizing the interaction between motor and non-motor features, which may reflect shared neural correlates as the locus coeruleus. This study demonstrates the multisystem nature of PD and the need to examine different features as part of a whole unified system.

Ethics Statement

This study was carried out in accordance with the recommendations of the Newcastle and North Tyneside research ethics committee with written informed consent from all subjects. All subjects gave written informed consent in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. The protocol was approved by the Newcastle and North Tyneside research ethics committee.

Author Contributions

AP carried out data analysis, interpreted the data, and wrote the manuscript. PS and LK contributed to data analysis and critically reviewed the manuscript. RM was involved with data collection, reviewed, and commented on manuscript draft. AY was involved with data collection and critically reviewed the manuscript. SD developed code, analyzed data, and critically reviewed the manuscript. LR conceived and designed the study, interpreted the data and critically revised the manuscript.

Funding

ICICLE-PD was funded by Parkinson's UK (J-0802, G-1301, G-1507). The research was supported by the Lockhart Parkinson's Disease Research Fund, the National Institute for Health Research (NIHR) Newcastle Biomedical Research Unit based at Newcastle upon Tyne Hospitals NHS Foundation Trust and Newcastle University and a NIHR Biomedical Research Centre award to the University of Cambridge/Addenbrooke's Hospital.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

1. Horsfall L, Petersen I, Walters K, Schrag A. Time trends in incidence of Parkinson's disease diagnosis in UK primary care. J Neurol. (2013) 260:1351–7. doi: 10.1007/s00415-012-6804-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Magrinelli F, Picelli A, Tocco P, Federico A, Roncari L, Smania N, et al. Pathophysiology of motor dysfunction in Parkinson's disease as the rationale for drug treatment and rehabilitation. Parkinsons Dis. (2016) 2016:9832839. doi: 10.1155/2016/9832839

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Schrag A, Ben-Shlomo Y, Quinn N. How common are complications of Parkinson's disease? J Neurol. (2002) 249:419–23. doi: 10.1007/s004150200032

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Williams DR, Watt HC, Lees AJ. Predictors of falls and fractures in bradykinetic rigid syndromes: a retrospective study. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry (2006) 77:468–73. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.2005.074070

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Frenklach A, Louie S, Koop MM, Bronte-Stewart H. Excessive postural sway and the risk of falls at different stages of Parkinson's disease. Mov Dis. (2009) 24:377–85. doi: 10.1002/mds.22358

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Karachi C, Francois C. Role of the pedunculopontine nucleus in controlling gait and sleep in normal and parkinsonian monkeys. J Neural Transm. (2017) 125:471–83. doi: 10.1007/s00702-017-1678-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Rudzinska M, Bukowczan S, Stozek J, Zajdel K, Mirek E, Chwała W, et al. The incidence and risk factors of falls in Parkinson disease: prospective study. Neurol Neurochir Pol. (2013) 47:431–7. doi: 10.5114/ninp.2013.38223

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Bloem BR, Grimbergen YA, Cramer M, Willemsen M, Zwinderman AH. Prospective assessment of falls in Parkinson's disease. J Neurol. (2001) 248:950–8. doi: 10.1007/s004150170047

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Lord S, Galna B, Yarnall AJ, Coleman S, Burn D, Rochester L. Predicting first fall in newly diagnosed Parkinson's disease: insights from a fall-naive cohort. Mov Disord. (2016) 31:1829–36. doi: 10.1002/mds.26742

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Lord S, Galna B, Yarnall AJ, Morris R, Coleman S, Burn D, et al. Natural history of falls in an incident cohort of Parkinson's disease: early evolution, risk and protective features. J Neurol. (2017) 264:2268–76. doi: 10.1007/s00415-017-8620-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Nonnekes J, Goselink R, Weerdesteyn V, Bloem BR. The retropulsion test: a good evaluation of postural instability in Parkinson's disease? J Parkinson's Dis. (2015) 5:43–7. doi: 10.3233/JPD-140514

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Horak FB, Mancini M. Objective biomarkers of balance and gait for Parkinson's disease using body-worn sensors. Mov Dis. (2013) 28:1544–51. doi: 10.1002/mds.25684

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Shumway-Cook A, Woollacott M, Kerns KA, Baldwin M. The effects of two types of cognitive tasks on postural stability in older adults with and without a history of falls. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. (1997) 52:M232–40. doi: 10.1093/gerona/52A.4.M232

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Palmerini L, Rocchi L, Mellone S, Valzania F, Chiari L. Feature selection for accelerometer-based posture analysis in Parkinson's Disease. Trans Info Tech Biomed. (2011) 15:481–90. doi: 10.1109/TITB.2011.2107916

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Whitney SL, Roche JL, Marchetti GF, Lin CC, Steed DP, Furman GR, et al. A comparison of accelerometry and center of pressure measures during computerized dynamic posturography: a measure of balance. Gait Posture (2011) 33:594–9. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2011.01.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Mancini M, Salarian A, Carlson-Kuhta P, Zampieri C, King L, Chiari L, et al. ISway: a sensitive, valid and reliable measure of postural control. J Neuroeng Rehabil. (2012) 9:59. doi: 10.1186/1743-0003-9-59

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Del Din S, Godfrey A, Coleman S, Galna B, Lord S, Rochester L. Time-dependent changes in postural control in early Parkinson's disease: what are we missing? Med Biol Eng Comput. (2016) 54:401–10. doi: 10.1007/s11517-015-1324-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Lawson RA, Yarnall AJ, Duncan GW, Breen DP, Khoo TK, Williams–Gray CH, et al. Cognitive decline and quality of life in incident Parkinson's disease: the role of attention. Parkinsonism Relat Disord. (2016) 27:47–53. doi: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2016.04.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Uc EY, McDermott MP, Marder KS, Anderson SW, Litvan I, Como PG, et al. Incidence of and risk factors for cognitive impairment in an early Parkinson disease clinical trial cohort. Neurology (2009) 73:1469–77. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3181bf992f

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Mollenhauer B, Rochester L, Chen-Plotkin A, Brooks D. What can biomarkers tell us about cognition in Parkinson's disease? Mov Disord. (2014) 29:622–33. doi: 10.1002/mds.25846

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Yarnall AJ, Rochester L, Baker MR, David R, Khoo TK, Duncan GW, et al. Short latency afferent inhibition: a biomarker for mild cognitive impairment in Parkinson's disease? Mov Disord. (2013) 28:1285–8. doi: 10.1002/mds.25360

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Morris M, Iansek R, Smithson F, Huxham F. Postural instability in Parkinson's disease: a comparison with and without a concurrent task. Gait Posture (2000) 12:205–16. doi: 10.1016/S0966-6362(00)00076-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Ashburn A, Stack E, Pickering RM, Ward CD. A community-dwelling sample of people with Parkinson's disease: characteristics of fallers and non-fallers. Age Ageing (2001) 30:47–52. doi: 10.1093/ageing/30.1.47

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Chen T, Fan Y, Zhuang X, Feng D, Chen Y, Chan P, et al. Postural sway in patients with early Parkinson's disease performing cognitive tasks while standing. Neurol Res. (2018) 40:491–8. doi: 10.1080/01616412.2018.1451017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Kido T, Tabara Y, Igase M, Ochi N, Uetani E, Nagai T, et al. Postural instability is associated with brain atrophy and cognitive impairment in the elderly: the J-SHIPP study. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. (2010) 29:379–87. doi: 10.1159/000255106

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Xu D, Cole MH, Mengersen K, Silburn PA, Qiu F, Graepel C, et al. Executive function and postural instability in people with Parkinson's disease. Parkinson's Dis. (2014) 2014:684758. doi: 10.1155/2014/684758

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Segev-Jacubovski O, Herman T, Yogev-Seligmann G, Mirelman A, Giladi N, Hausdorff JM. The interplay between gait, falls and cognition: can cognitive therapy reduce fall risk? Expert Rev Neurother. (2011) 11:1057–75. doi: 10.1586/ern.11.69

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Rosenberg-Katz K, Herman T, Jacob Y, Mirelman A, Giladi N, Hendler T, et al. Fall risk is associated with amplified functional connectivity of the central executive network in patients with Parkinson's disease. J Neurol. (2015) 262:2448–56. doi: 10.1007/s00415-015-7865-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Cummings JL. Depression and Parkinson's disease: a review. Am J Psychiatry (1992) 149:443–54. doi: 10.1176/ajp.149.4.443

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Vermeiren Y, De Deyn PP. Targeting the norepinephrinergic system in Parkinson's disease and related disorders: the locus coeruleus story. Neurochem Int. (2017) 102:22–32. doi: 10.1016/j.neuint.2016.11.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Chan-Palay V, Asan E. Quantitation of catecholamine neurons in the locus coeruleus in human brains of normal young and older adults and in depression. J Comp Neurol. (1989) 287:357–72. doi: 10.1002/cne.902870307

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Hassan A, Vallabhajosula S, Zahodne LB, Bowers D, Okun MS, Fernandez HH, et al. Correlations of apathy and depression with postural instability in Parkinson disease. J Neurol Sci. (2014) 338:162–5. doi: 10.1016/j.jns.2013.12.040

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Burn DJ, Landau S, Hindle JV, Samuel M, Wilson KC, Hurt CS, et al. Parkinson's disease motor subtypes and mood. Mov Disord. (2012) 27:379–86. doi: 10.1002/mds.24041

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Khoo TK, Yarnall AJ, Duncan GW, Coleman S, O'Brien JT, Brooks DJ, et al. The spectrum of nonmotor symptoms in early Parkinson disease. Neurology (2013) 80:276–81. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e31827deb74

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Gibb WR, Lees AJ. The relevance of the Lewy body to the pathogenesis of idiopathic Parkinson's disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry (1988) 51:745–52. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.51.6.745

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Hoehn MM, Yahr MD. Parkinsonism: onset, progression and mortality. Neurology (1967) 17:427–42. doi: 10.1212/WNL.17.5.427

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text

37. Goetz CG, Tilley BC, Shaftman SR, Stebbins GT, Fahn S, Martinez-Martin P, et al. Movement Disorder Society-sponsored revision of the Unified Parkinson's Disease Rating Scale (MDS-UPDRS): scale presentation and clinimetric testing results. Mov Disord. (2008) 23:2129–70. doi: 10.1002/mds.22340

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Powell LE, Myers AM. The Activities-specific Balance Confidence (ABC) scale. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. (1995) 50A:M28–34. doi: 10.1093/gerona/50A.1.M28

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Dalrymple-Alford JC, MacAskill MR, Nakas CT, Livingston L, Graham C, Crucian GP, et al. The MoCA: well-suited screen for cognitive impairment in Parkinson disease. Neurology (2010) 75:1717–25. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3181fc29c9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Wechsler D. WAIS-III: Administration and Scoring Manual. 3rd ed. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation (1997).

41. Miller GA. The magical number seven plus or minus two: some limits on our capacity for processing information. Psychol Rev. (1956) 63:81–97. doi: 10.1037/h0043158

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Schrag A, Barone P, Brown RG, Leentjens AF, McDonald WM, Starkstein S, et al. Depression rating scales in Parkinson's disease: critique and recommendations. Mov Disord. (2007) 22:1077–92. doi: 10.1002/mds.21333

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Qiu F, Cole MH, Davids KW, Hennig EM, Silburn PA, Netscher H, et al. Effects of textured insoles on balance in people with Parkinson's disease. PLoS ONE (2013) 8:e83309. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0083309

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Ladha C, Jackson D, Ladha K, Olivier P. Shaker table validation of OpenMovement AX3 accelerometer. In: 3rd International Conference on Ambulatory Monitoring of Physical Activity and Movement. Ahmerst (2013).

Google Scholar

45. Moe-Nilssen R. Test-retest reliability of trunk accelerometry during standing and walking. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. (1998) 79:1377–85. doi: 10.1016/S0003-9993(98)90231-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Mancini M, Horak FB, Zampieri C, Carlson-Kuhta P, Nutt JG, Chiari L. Trunk accelerometry reveals postural instability in untreated Parkinson's disease. Parkinsonism Relat Disord. (2011) 17:557–62. doi: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2011.05.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Godfrey A, Lara J, Del Din S, Hickey A, Munro CA, Wiuff C, et al. iCap: instrumented assessment of physical capability. Maturitas (2015) 82:116–22. doi: 10.1016/j.maturitas.2015.04.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Cohen J. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences. 2nd ed. Hillsdale, NJ: L. Erlbaum Associates (1988).

49. Mancini M, Carlson-Kuhta P, Zampieri C, Nutt JG, Chiari L, Horak FB. Postural sway as a marker of progression in Parkinson's disease: a pilot longitudinal study. Gait Posture (2012) 36:471–6. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2012.04.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Horak FB, Nutt JG, Nashner LM. Postural inflexibility in parkinsonian subjects. J Neurol Sci. (1992) 111:46–58. doi: 10.1016/0022-510X(92)90111-W

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Duarte M, Sternad D. Complexity of human postural control in young and older adults during prolonged standing. Exp Brain Res. (2008) 191:265–76. doi: 10.1007/s00221-008-1521-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Park JH, Mancini M, Carlson-Kuhta P, Nutt JG, Horak FB. Quantifying effects of age on balance and gait with inertial sensors in community-dwelling healthy adults. Exp Gerontol. (2016) 85:48–58. doi: 10.1016/j.exger.2016.09.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Perera T, Tan JL, Cole MH, Yohanandan SAC, Silberstein P, Cook R, et al. Balance control systems in Parkinson's disease and the impact of pedunculopontine area stimulation. Brain (2018) 141:3009–22. doi: 10.1093/brain/awy216

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. O'Suilleabhain P, Bullard J, Dewey RB. Proprioception in Parkinson's disease is acutely depressed by dopaminergic medications. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry (2001) 71:607–10. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.71.5.607

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Vaugoyeau M, Viel S, Assaiante C, Amblard B, Azulay JP. Impaired vertical postural control and proprioceptive integration deficits in Parkinson's disease. Neuroscience (2007) 146:852–63. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2007.01.052

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Pantall A, Del Din S, Rochester L. Longitudinal changes over thirty-six months in postural control dynamics and cognitive function in people with Parkinson's disease. Gait Posture (2018) 62:468–74. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2018.04.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Horak FB, Dimitrova D, Nutt JG. Direction-specific postural instability in subjects with Parkinson's disease. Exp Neurol. (2005) 193:504–21. doi: 10.1016/j.expneurol.2004.12.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Doherty KM, van de Warrenburg BP, Peralta MC, Silveira-Moriyama L, Azulay JP, Gershanik OS, et al. Postural deformities in Parkinson's disease. Lancet Neurol. (2011) 10:538–49. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(11)70067-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Carpenter MG, Allum JH, Honegger F, Adkin AL, Bloem BR. Postural abnormalities to multidirectional stance perturbations in Parkinson's disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry (2004) 75:1245–54. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.2003.021147

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Mitchell SL, Collins JJ, De Luca CJ, Burrows A, Lipsitz LA. Open-loop and closed-loop postural control mechanisms in Parkinson's disease: increased mediolateral activity during quiet standing. Neurosci Lett. (1995) 197:133–6. doi: 10.1016/0304-3940(95)11924-L

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Kelly VE, Johnson CO, McGough EL, Shumway-Cook A, Horak FB, Chung KA, et al. Association of cognitive domains with postural instability/gait disturbance in Parkinson's disease. Parkinsonism Relat Disord. (2015) 21:692–7. doi: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2015.04.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Nocera JR, Price C, Fernandez HH, Amano S, Vallabhajosula S, Okun MS, et al. Tests of dorsolateral frontal function correlate with objective tests of postural stability in early to moderate stage Parkinson's disease. Parkinsonism Relat Disord. (2010) 16:590–4. doi: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2010.08.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Lord S, Galna B, Coleman S, Yarnall A, Burn D, Rochester L. Cognition and gait show a selective pattern of association dominated by phenotype in incident Parkinson's disease. Front Aging Neurosci. (2014) 6:249. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2014.00249

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Schoneburg B, Mancini M, Horak F, Nutt JG. Framework for understanding balance dysfunction in Parkinson's disease. Mov Disord. (2013) 28:1474–82. doi: 10.1002/mds.25613

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Rocchi L, Chiari L, Horak FB. Effects of deep brain stimulation and levodopa on postural sway in Parkinson's disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry (2002) 73:267–74. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.73.3.267

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Goldman JG, Weis H, Stebbins G, Bernard B, Goetz CG. Clinical differences among mild cognitive impairment subtypes in Parkinson's disease. Mov Disord. (2012) 27:1129–36. doi: 10.1002/mds.25062

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Arie L, Herman T, Shema-Shiratzky S, Giladi N, Hausdorff JM. Do cognition and other non-motor symptoms decline similarly among patients with Parkinson's disease motor subtypes? Findings from a 5-year prospective study. J Neurol. (2017) 264:2149–57. doi: 10.1007/s00415-017-8605-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Lord S, Baker K, Nieuwboer A, Burn D, Rochester L. Gait variability in Parkinson's disease: an indicator of non-dopaminergic contributors to gait dysfunction? J Neurol. (2011) 258:566–72. doi: 10.1007/s00415-010-5789-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Lord S, Galna B, Coleman S, Burn D, Rochester L. Mild depressive symptoms are associated with gait impairment in early Parkinson's disease. Mov Disord. (2013) 28:634–9. doi: 10.1002/mds.25338

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Borgonovo J, Allende-Castro C, Laliena A, Guerrero N, Silva H, Concha ML. Changes in neural circuitry associated with depression at pre-clinical, pre-motor and early motor phases of Parkinson's disease. Parkinsonism Relat Disord. (2017) 35:17–24. doi: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2016.11.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: posture, Parkinson's, cognition, balance, depression, longitudinal, accelerometer

Citation: Pantall A, Suresparan P, Kapa L, Morris R, Yarnall A, Del Din S and Rochester L (2018) Postural Dynamics Are Associated With Cognitive Decline in Parkinson's Disease. Front. Neurol. 9:1044. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2018.01044

Received: 30 June 2018; Accepted: 19 November 2018;
Published: 05 December 2018.

Edited by:

Joyce Fung, McGill University, Canada

Reviewed by:

Brett W. Fling, Colorado State University, United States
Karen Zown-Hua Li, Concordia University, Canada
Rachel Kizony, University of Haifa, Israel

Copyright © 2018 Pantall, Suresparan, Kapa, Morris, Yarnall, Del Din and Rochester. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Annette Pantall, YW5uZXR0ZS5wYW50YWxsQG5ld2Nhc3RsZS5hYy51aw==

Present Address: Piriya Suresparan, Esteem Care Limited, Southall, United Kingdom

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.