Skip to main content

REVIEW article

Front. Neurol., 13 March 2018
Sec. Neuro-Ophthalmology
This article is part of the Research Topic Ocular Motor and Vestibular Deficits in Neurometabolic, Neurogenetic, and Neurodegenerative Diseases View all 23 articles

Microsaccade Characteristics in Neurological and Ophthalmic Disease

  • State University of New York (SUNY) Downstate Medical Center, Brooklyn, NY, United States

Microsaccade research has recently reached a critical mass of studies that allows, for the first time, a comprehensive review of how microsaccadic dynamics change in neurological and ophthalmic disease. We discuss the various pathological conditions that affect microsaccades, their impact on microsaccadic and other fixational eye movement dynamics, and the incipient studies that point to microsaccadic features as potential indicators of differential and early diagnoses of multiple clinical conditions, from movement disorders to attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder to amblyopia. We propose that the objective assessment of fixational eye movement parameters may help refine differential diagnostics in neurological disease and assist in the evaluation of ongoing therapy regimes. In addition, determining the effects of ophthalmic disease on fixational eye movement features may help evaluate visual impairment in an objective manner, particularly in young patients or those experiencing communication difficulties.

Introduction

When we attempt to fixate our gaze on a target, our eyes are never still, but produce small “fixational eye movements,” which include tremor, drift, and microsaccades. Microsaccades (also called fixational saccades) occur at a typical rate of 1–2 Hz. Converging research points to a saccadic generation continuum, which extends from the smallest fixational microsaccades to the largest exploratory saccades (15). Drift is a slow (typically less than 2°/s) motion that occurs between microsaccades and saccades, and travels in an erratic pattern that has been modeled as a random walk (6). Tremor (or ocular microtremor) occurs simultaneously with drift, during intersaccadic intervals. This is the smallest fixational eye movement, with amplitudes that approximate the width of a single photoreceptor and dominant frequencies between 70 and 103 Hz (averaging 84 Hz) (7, 8). Tremor studies are much scarcer than those centered on microsaccades and/or drift, due to the technical difficulties inherent to measuring this tiny motion (8, 9). Thus, we do not address tremor in this review.

Because we spend approximately 80% of our waking hours fixating our gaze [not only in a sustained way but also in transient fashion, between large saccades (10)], understanding fixational dynamics is critical to advance current knowledge of oculomotor and visual function. Fixational eye movement assessments may also help further our understanding of central and peripheral pathologies that result in impaired fixation.

Various neurological and ophthalmic disorders produce abnormal fixational eye movement patterns, with distinctive characteristics. Thus, establishing how neurological and ophthalmic disease affects fixational dynamics holds the potential to help in the early and differential diagnosis of such disorders, clarify their pathophysiology, and quantify their progression and response to treatment. Recent research efforts have set out to characterize fixational dynamics in a growing record of neurological and ophthalmological conditions, which we discuss in this review.

A classification of abnormal eye movements in different disorders of fixation was previously published [see Table 1 of Martinez-Conde (10)]. The intervening decade has seen an upsurge in fixational eye movement research, with an emphasis on microsaccade studies. In addition, cross-fertilization between fundamental and translational approaches to fixational dynamics has facilitated the identification of previously unknown links between (micro)saccadic eye movements and saccadic intrusions (the latter formerly relegated to the clinical literature).

Such recent developments have resulted in a critical mass of studies that allows us, for the first time, to offer a comprehensive review of how microsaccadic dynamics change in neurological and ophthalmic pathologies, from movement disorders to attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) to amblyopia.

Microsaccades in Neurological Disease

The balance that fixational eye movement system must achieve in healthy oculomotor function is quite delicate: whereas insufficient eye motion can result in visual losses due to neural adaptation and visual fading, excessive eye motion leads to blurred and unstable vision. This fine calibration is disrupted in patients of various neurological and neurodegenerative disorders who display increased gaze instability during the attempt to fixate (11). Recent research efforts aimed to characterize such fixation instability—with an emphasis on the dynamic of microsaccades and drift—in neurological disease seek not only to improve early and differential diagnosis and help evaluate the efficacy of concurrent treatments but also to gain a deeper understanding of the pathophysiology and pathogenesis of such disorders.

Microsaccades, Saccades, and Saccadic Intrusions in the Healthy Brain and in Neurological Disease

Converging evidence from physiological and behavioral studies conducted over the last decade has led to the current consensus that microsaccades and saccades—though previously considered as two different eye movement types—share a common oculomotor generator [(3); for review see Ref. (12)]. More recently, the proposal of a microsaccade-to-saccade continuum has been expanded to saccadic intrusions (3, 13, 14), defined as involuntary saccades that interrupt, or intrude on, precise fixation. Sacccadic intrusions are prevalent in certain neurodegenerative disorders, although healthy individuals also produce them. The most common saccadic intrusion is the square-wave jerk (SWJ), which consists of a small, horizontal saccade moving away from the fixation target, quickly followed by a corrective return saccade of equivalent amplitude and opposite direction. Though microsaccades and SWJs have most often been studied as two separate types of eye movements, recent work has put forward the notion that they, too, may be fundamentally the same kind of eye movement with different names (4, 5, 13, 14).

Progressive Supranuclear Palsy (PSP) and Other Movement Disorders

Pinnock and colleagues found larger and more frequent saccadic intrusions (including small intrusions due to microsaccades) in patients with Parkinson’s disease, multiple system atrophy, and PSP, than in healthy age-matched controls (15). Otero-Millan et al. subsequently set out to study the characteristics of microsaccades and SWJs in patients with PSP—a parkinsonian disorder that affects the basal ganglia, mesencephalon, and frontal lobe—in which SWJs are a distinctive clinical feature (14) (Figure 1).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Square-wave jerks (SWJs) from three progressive supranuclear palsy (PSP) patients (left) and three age-matched controls (right). In both populations, (micro)saccades with amplitudes equal to or larger than half a degree of visual angle are paired as SWJs. Only the horizontal eye positions are shown. Modified from Otero-Millan et al. (14).

Though normal microsaccades were found to be rare in PSP, microsaccade magnitude was linked to SWJ coupling in both PSP patients and in healthy participants, with large microsaccades being more likely to trigger return saccades (forming SWJs) than small microsaccades (Figure 2). In addition, microsaccades and SWJs were slower in PSP patients than in controls, and they had a diminished vertical component, consistent with the vertical saccadic palsy that sets apart PSP from other parkinsonian patients. The results supported the hypothesis that a common mechanism may account for microsaccade and SWJ generation (13, 14) and explained how the position error from a large first saccade could serve as the trigger for the return saccade in SWJs produced by both PSP patients and healthy participants (16). The study concluded that the apparent distinction between microsaccades and SWJs could be due to two complementary mechanisms, underlying: (1) microsaccade production and (2) correction of gaze fixation errors due to oversized microsaccades (14). These two factors, combined, could explain square-wave coupling, both for microsaccade pairs in healthy subjects and for saccadic intrusions in neurological patients suffering from PSP, Parkinson’s disease, and other movement disorders, including multiple system atrophy, corticobasal syndrome, and spinocerebellar ataxia (5, 14).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Square-wave coupling takes place for large but not small (micro)saccades. In this eye position trace from a healthy participant, red arrows point to pairs of larger microsaccades forming square-wave jerks; green arrows point to smaller unpaired microsaccades. From Otero-Millan et al. (14).

Mild Cognitive Impairment and Alzheimer’s Disease

Kapoula and colleagues recorded the eye movements of Alzheimer’s disease patients, patients with mild cognitive impairment, and healthy age-matched participants during the attempt to fixate. Whereas most microsaccadic features, including magnitude, velocity, duration, and intersaccadic intervals were equivalent across the three groups, oblique microsaccade directions were more prevalent in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease patients than in healthy participants (17). Layfield and colleagues wondered about potential links (positive or negative) between microsaccade dynamics and the amelioration of cognitive deficits in aging adults—following from targeted interventions known as “Speed Processing Training”—but found no relationship (18).

Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD)

The neural system that controls attention and the system that generates (micro) saccadic eye movements overlap extensively. Thus, multiple research studies have examined the connection between microsaccades, attention, and distractors [for review see Ref. (3)]. The superior colliculus, which plays a central role in (micro)saccade triggering, has moreover attracted recent interest as a potential site of dysfunction in ADHD (19, 20). A handful of studies have examined the connection between ADHD and gaze instability during fixation (2123). Most recently, two studies have focused on the characteristics of microsaccades in individuals with ADHD (24) and ADHD traits (20). Fried et al. found a higher microsaccade rate in adult individuals with ADHD who were off medication than in control participants. Methylphenidate medication served to normalize microsaccade rates in the ADHD group. Panagiotidi and colleagues similarly found differing microsaccade rates in non-clinical participants with high and low levels of ADHD-like traits (20), assessed with the Adult ADHD Self-Report Scale (25). These combined results suggested that abnormal fixation behavior is a core deficit in ADHD, which could aid in the development of a biomarker for the disorder (20). Another recent study set out to investigate the impairment of temporal expectations in ADHD, by examining the inhibition of microsaccades prior to the onset of predicted stimuli. The data indicated decreased microsaccade inhibition in participants with ADHD than in controls, suggesting that microsaccade characterization may help enhance current understanding of the range of cognitive deficits that affect ADHD individuals (26).

Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD)

Recent research has found increased drift in autistic individuals (27). Microsaccade sizes and rates during fixation of a small target were comparable in ASD and neurotypical participants (27, 28), but those with ASD presented greater fixation instability, more microsaccades, and larger microsaccades when asked to maintain fixation on a blank screen with no target (28).

Tourette Syndrome

A recent study found patients with Tourette syndrome to have reduced microsaccade amplitudes and increased intersaccadic intervals, along with increased fixation instability and drift velocities (29).

Schizophrenia

Egaña and colleagues (30) found that previously reported decreased oculomotor function—in terms of decreased saccade and fixation rate—in schizophrenic patients (31) no longer differed from that of control participants once they included microsaccades in the analyses. In other words, schizophrenic patients made similar numbers of overall eye movements as healthy individuals, but produced fewer large, exploratory saccades to scan wide regions of the visual field. This study shows that fixational eye movement analyses in neurological and psychiatric disorders can be valuable not only to differentiate across populations but also to reveal previously unknown similarities between groups.

Cerebral Palsy

Kozeis and colleagues proposed that microsaccadic impairment might complicate the acquisition of reading skills in children with cerebral palsy (32), but no studies to date have directly characterized fixational eye movements in this disorder.

Hemianopia and Cortical Blindness

Hemianopia, or blindness in one-half of the visual field, can result from any lesion impairing post-chiasmatic central visual pathways. Reinhard and colleagues found microsaccadic distributions in hemianopic patients to be asymmetrical, with microsaccade directions biased toward the blind hemifield (33).

Gao and Sabel (34) subsequently investigated the characteristics of microsaccades in hemianopic stroke patients, to determine their potential relationship with visual performance and to assess how microsaccadic direction might be related to visual defect topography. They found that hemianopia resulted in enlarged microsaccades with impaired binocular conjugacy. Alterations of microsaccade dynamics worsened over time, being most prominent for older lesions. The data also revealed a microsaccade bias toward the seeing field, which was associated with faster reaction times to super-threshold visual stimuli in areas of residual vision, and suggested greater allocation of attention. Visual acuity was highest in patients with more binocular microsaccades and lower microsaccade velocities. The authors proposed that microsaccades may help compensate visual impairment in hemianopia and provide a basis for vision restoration and plasticity.

Blindsight is a rare phenomenon in which patients who have cortical blindness (due to lesions to the primary visual cortex) produce appropriate behavioral responses to visual stimuli they do not consciously see. Though no studies to date have systematically characterized microsaccadic properties in blindsighted patients, researchers in a recent case report studied microsaccadic inhibition (i.e., the transient suppression of microsaccade production after the presentation of a peripheral stimulus) in a patient who suffered from blindsight due to traumatic brain injury. The investigators observed that the patient’s microsaccade rates dropped briefly after the presentation of high- and low-contrast peripheral stimuli, in both the left (blind) and the right visual fields. In the case of low-contrast stimuli, the release from microsaccadic inhibition was slower in the blind field than in the sighted field, however (35).

Short-Term Hypoxia

Di Stasi and colleagues found that saccadic velocity decreased and intersaccadic drift velocity increased, in connection with short-term hypobaric hypoxia in aviators. The finding that acute hypoxia diminishes eye stability, the authors proposed, may help to better understand the relationship between hypoxia episodes and central nervous system impairments (36).

Microsaccades in Ophthalmic Disease

Vision and eye movements are intrinsically linked. Whereas it may seem intuitive to consider vision primarily in terms of its spatial characteristics, the process of seeing is a spatiotemporal one, where many timing features and constraints that impact our visual experience derive from the timing of eye movement production and targeting. Eye movements shape what we see, and our visual perception, in turn, affects the way we move our eyes. Ophthalmic disease, due to its deleterious effects on visual quality, tends to result in measurable abnormalities in eye movement properties, which extend to the fixational domain.

Because human beings are typically unaware of their fixational eye movements, studying their characteristics in ophthalmic disease may help evaluate the extent of a patient’s visual impairment in an objective manner—particularly in very young patients or those experiencing communication difficulties.

Amblyopia and Strabismus

Most studies of fixational eye movements in ophthalmic disease to date have centered on amblyopia and strabismus. Amblyopia is defined as underdeveloped vision of one eye due to any condition that interferes with focusing during early childhood, including strabismus (in which the two eyes do not align correctly during fixation) and uncorrected refractive error (with anisometropia, or unequal refractive power in the two eyes).

Starting in the late 1970s, Ciuffreda and his colleagues conducted a series of pioneering studies on how amblyopia and strabismus affected fixation behavior (3739). They found that, whereas amblyopic patients produced normal fixational eye movements during binocular fixation (and during monocular fixation with the fellow eye), monocular fixation with the amblyopic eye resulted in increased drift (whether or not strabismus was also present) (11, 3740). If the amblyopia was due to strabismus, or in cases of alternating strabismus, this increase in drift was accompanied by sizable and frequent saccadic intrusions (3739). By contrast, amblyopic fixation in dark-adaptation conditions was found to be normal or close to normal (41, 42).

More recently, Shi and colleagues found less frequent but larger microsaccades during monocular fixation with the amblyopic eye than with the fellow eye (43) and proposed that the objective evaluation of oculomotor function in amblyopia includes a microsaccade assessment. Otero-Millan et al. (44) noted that microsaccades produced during normal binocular fixation of large targets have similar features to those reported by Shi et al. during monocular fixation with the amblyopic eye, which might indicate a common lack of fixation precision in both scenarios. This possibility is consistent with work finding reduced fixation stability in the amblyopic eye, as compared to the fellow eye and to binocular viewing (45) (Figure 3).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Eye movements of a patient with strabismic amblyopia. Horizontal (X) and vertical (Y) positions are plotted for the left eye (gray, amblyopic eye) and the right eye (black, fellow eye). Amblyopic eye viewing results in larger microsaccades in both eyes. Monocular viewing with the fellow eye is tied to increased instability in the amblyopic eye. Monocular viewing with the amblyopic eye produces increased instability in the two eyes. From González et al. (45).

Ghasia et al. found that fixational saccades and ocular drift were more disconjugate for patients with strabismus than for control participants. This disconjugacy was greater for patients with large-angle strabismus and impaired stereopsis (as a result of the misalignment of their eyes) than for patients with small-angle strabismus and preserved stereopsis (Figure 4). This study also found that drift was faster in patients with strabismus than in control subjects (46).

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Visual fixation of a target for 5 s, under monocular viewing conditions. (A) Participant with normal vision: both microsaccades and intersaccadic drift are conjugate. (B) Participant with small-angle strabismus and stereopsis present: disconjugancy across the two eyes is mild for both microsaccades and drift. (C) Participant with large-angle strabismus and absent stereopsis: disconjugancy is pronounced for both microsaccades and drift. (A–C) Horizontal (red: right eye, gray: left eye) and vertical (blue: right eye, gray: left eye) eye positions are presented. Black arrows indicate microsaccades, and gray arrows represent intersaccadic drift. From Ghasia et al. (46).

Fixation stability, usually measured as the eye position dispersion [i.e., bivariate contour elliptical area (BCEA)] during the attempt to fixate, combines the effects of microsaccades and drifts, without making a distinction between the two types of eye motion (44). Subramanian et al. (47) found that the BCEA was larger in the amblyopic eye than in the fellow eye, especially along the horizontal axis, and that patients with larger BCEAs tend to have lower visual acuities.

Increased drift and decreased microsaccade production in severe amblyopia may lead to perceptual fading of large portions of the visual field, including the “small fixation spot, small and large acuity targets, and even portions of the laboratory” during monocular fixation with the amblyopic eye (37, 4850). One patient reportedly “made saccades to revive the faded or blanked-out portions” of the image in such situations (37), suggesting that visual fading in amblyopia might be related to ordinary Troxler fading [i.e., the kind of visual fading that normally sighted individuals can experience during fixation in the absence of microsaccades (10, 51)].

Increased drift in amblyopia could also produce lower visual acuity—and increased variability in visual acuity measurements—by shifting retinal images to more eccentric positions (39, 52). Such links exemplify the tight bond between the motor and sensory aspects of fixational eye movements (44).

Aiming to increase fixation stability for the amblyopic eye (and thus produce bifoveal fixation), Raveendran et al. (53) decreased the contrast of the image viewed by the fellow eye until it was equivalent to the contrast perceived by the amblyopic eye. Fixation stability in the amblyopic eye improved as a result, but bifoveal fixation was nevertheless temporary (due to the amblyopic eye drifting away from foveal alignment).

Loudon and colleagues used a binocularity score to assess how well subjects fixated a target with both eyes and proposed that the presence of fixation instability can help detect amblyopia at an early age (when it otherwise goes undetected up to a third of the time) (54).

Successful orthoptics therapy tends to normalize fixational eye movements in amblyopia [though not all oculomotor and visual functions may improve concurrently (52, 55)]. Thus, Ciuffreda and colleagues have proposed that amblyopic therapies should not be interrupted when patients achieve normal visual acuity and centralized fixation, but should continue until they produced normal or stabilized fixational eye movements. Because critical periods for some aspects of oculomotor plasticity may extend into adulthood, lack of fixational eye movement normalization in amblyopic patients could be a factor in their reverting to the pre-treatment condition once therapy is discontinued (52). Thus, fixational eye movement assessments may help establish the optimal duration of treatments.

Central Scotoma due to Macular Disease or Dysfunction

Macular scotomas, and other pathologies producing prolonged monocular visual deprivation, have also been connected to increased drift, with comparable characteristics to drift in amblyopes (11, 56). A recent study by Kumar and Chung (57) found that patients with macular disease presented not only increased drift amplitudes but also larger microsaccadic amplitudes (Figure 5) than healthy subjects, without a corresponding change in microsaccade rate. The authors concluded that an increase in drift and microsaccade amplitudes—as opposed to changes in velocity or rate—is the strongest predictor of overall fixation instability in macular disease. Moller et al. previously found, in a group of diabetic maculopathy patients, that microsaccade magnitude increased as visual acuity decreased (58). A more recent study set out to determine if saccades in an eye affected by diabetic maculopathy were influenced by the other eye during binocular fixation. The results revealed that microsaccades during monocular fixation with the eye most affected by macular edema were larger, more frequent, and involved a larger retinal area than those produced during binocular fixation. A significant negative correlation was found between area of fixation and visual acuity during monocular, but not binocular, fixation. The authors concluded that binocular fixation can reduce the fixation area and microsaccade amplitude in the “worst eye” of diabetic maculopathy patients and advised that microsaccades in diabetic maculopathy are studied during monocular fixation (59).

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Eye positions during fixation in a patient with macular disease (left) and a healthy control subject (right). From Kumar and Chung (57).

Myopia

Recent work found increased microsaccade amplitude—without a corresponding change in microsaccade velocity or microsaccade rate—in myopic individuals (60). As the severity of uncorrected refractive error increased, so did the sizes of microsaccades. This suggested that the control of microsaccade amplitude relies on the precision of visual information on the fovea, with blurred information leading to fixational instability.

Retinal Implants

Here, we discuss current efforts to characterize fixational eye movements, including microsaccades, in patients with subretinal implants. Because subretinal implants are placed below the retina (unlike epiretinal implants such as the Argus II, where an external camera is used to capture an image), the eye movements of implanted patients have the potential to affect—and be affected by—their visual perception.

In recent research, patients with a subretinal Alpha IMS scanned their visual field to locate a fixation target that appeared at random locations. Upon target fixation, the patients produced fixational eye movements—including microsaccades (with and without square-wave coupling) and drifts—that were analogous to those produced by control participants (61). A previous study by the same group made similar observations (62). The properties of (micro)saccades moreover depended on the shape of the stimulus being viewed. For instance, both patients and control participants made more horizontal eye movements when viewing a rectangle than when viewing a square (61). These data suggest that (micro)saccadic dynamics might help provide an objective measure of the success of an implant, especially in situations where subjective reports are questionable or inviable: if eye movements characteristics change in response to changes in the stimulus, it would indicate that visual inputs have been processed appropriately (61).

One limitation of subretinal implants such as the Alpha IMS is that visible stimuli typically fade from perception within seconds (6163). Understanding the relationship between fixational eye movement dynamics and image fading in prosthetic vision may prove key to improving future subretinal implants. Microsaccades have been shown to counteract, and to help prevent, perceptual fading in natural vision (51, 6468). Similarly, microsaccade occurrence has been connected to fading prevention in patients with subretinal implants (61). It has also been proposed that the characterization of microsaccade patterns in patients could help fine-tune the frequency of stimulation that results in optimal visibility in specific individuals. That is, depending on a particular observer’s fixational eye movement patterns, he/she may need higher or lower stimulation frequencies to maintain visibility (69). Future research may investigate the translational value of this potential relationship.

It remains currently unknown why visual fading is more severe in patients with prosthetic implants than in healthy observers. One possibility is that fixational eye movements counteract and prevent perceptual fading less effectively in implanted patients than in natural vision (70). Recent modeling work has suggested that increased fading in prosthetic vision might be due to a lack of OFF responses and to lower contrast sensitivity than in natural vision (71); thus, the quality of the visual input may be too low for eye movements to refresh retinal images effectively. Fading may also be more or less prevalent depending on the size of electrodes used: stimulation from a single electrode may affect such a large visual region that even when microsaccades shift the stimulus to adjacent electrodes, they may not significantly change the activated neurons (70, 72).

Conclusion

We have reviewed the characteristics of fixational eye movements in neurological and ophthalmic disease, with an emphasis on microsaccades. Though studies addressing microsaccadic impairments in patient populations remain relatively scarce, this has recently become an area of active inquiry, with valuable implications for both clinical and basic research (3). Converging studies have made significant headway vis-à-vis the potential of microsaccade and other fixational eye movement dysfunctions as indicators of ongoing pathologies beyond the oculomotor realm. Thus, the objective assessment of fixational eye movement parameters may help refine differential diagnostics and assist in the evaluation of ongoing therapy regimes (i.e., successful treatments should result in the normalization of previously impaired fixational eye movements). These measures will also help refine current understanding of the pathogenesis of neural disease, as well as place constraints on—and guide the development of—future saccadic generation models, especially with regards to the relationship of (micro)saccades to saccadic intrusions in neurological disease.

Author Contributions

SM-C, RGA, and SLM wrote and edited the manuscript. SM-C and RGA conducted the literature review.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

The authors thank Rosario Malpica and Daniel Cortes-Rastrollo for administrative and technical assistance.

Funding

This work was supported by the Empire Innovation Program (Awards to SM-C and SLM) and the National Science Foundation (Award 1523614 to SLM and Award 1734887 to SLM and SM-C).

References

1. Otero-Millan J, Troncoso XG, Macknik SL, Serrano-Pedraza I, Martinez-Conde S. Saccades and microsaccades during visual fixation, exploration, and search: foundations for a common saccadic generator. J Vis (2008) 8(14):21–21. doi:10.1167/8.14.21

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Rolfs M, Kliegl R, Engbert R. Toward a model of microsaccade generation: the case of microsaccadic inhibition. J Vis (2008) 8(11):5.1–23. doi:10.1167/8.11.5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Martinez-Conde S, Otero-Millan J, Macknik SL. The impact of microsaccades on vision: towards a unified theory of saccadic function. Nat Rev Neurosci (2013) 14(2):83–96. doi:10.1038/nrn3405

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Otero-Millan J, Macknik SL, Langston RE, Martinez-Conde S. An oculomotor continuum from exploration to fixation. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A (2013) 110(15):6175–80. doi:10.1073/pnas.1222715110

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Otero-Millan J, Schneider R, Leigh RJ, Macknik SL, Martinez-Conde S. Saccades during attempted fixation in parkinsonian disorders and recessive ataxia: from microsaccades to square-wave jerks. PLoS One (2013) 8(3):e58535. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0058535

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Engbert R, Kliegl R. Microsaccades keep the eyes’ balance during fixation. Psychol Sci (2004) 15(6):431–6. doi:10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00697.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Bolger C, Bojanic S, Sheahan NF, Coakley D, Malone JF. Dominant frequency content of ocular microtremor from normal subjects. Vision Res (1999) 39(11):1911–5. doi:10.1016/S0042-6989(98)00322-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Martinez-Conde S, Macknik SL, Hubel DH. The role of fixational eye movements in visual perception. Nat Rev Neurosci (2004) 5(3):229–40. doi:10.1038/nrn1348

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Alexander RG, Martinez-Conde S. Fixational eye movements. In: Reuter M, Montag C, Klein C, Ettinger U, editors. Studies in Neuroscience, Psychology and Behavioral Economics: An Introduction to the Scientific Foundations of Eye Movement Research and Its Applications. New York: Springer (Forthcoming).

Google Scholar

10. Martinez-Conde S. Fixational eye movements in normal and pathological vision. Prog Brain Res (2006) 154:151–76. doi:10.1016/s0079-6123(06)54008-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Ciuffreda KJ, Tannen B. Eye Movement Basics for the Clinician. St. Louis: Mosby-Year Book, Inc. (1995).

Google Scholar

12. Rolfs M. Microsaccades: small steps on a long way. Vision Res (2009) 49(20):2415–41. doi:10.1016/j.visres.2009.08.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Gowen E, Abadi RV, Poliakoff E, Hansen PC, Miall RC. Modulation of saccadic intrusions by exogenous and endogenous attention. Brain Res (2007) 1141:154–67. doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2007.01.047

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Otero-Millan J, Serra A, Leigh RJ, Troncoso XG, Macknik SL, Martinez-Conde S. Distinctive features of saccadic intrusions and microsaccades in progressive supranuclear palsy. J Neurosci (2011) 31(12):4379–87. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2600-10.2011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Pinnock RA, McGivern RC, Forbes R, Gibson JM. An exploration of ocular fixation in Parkinson’s disease, multiple system atrophy and progressive supranuclear palsy. J Neurol (2010) 257(4):533–9. doi:10.1007/s00415-009-5356-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Otero-Millan J, Macknik SL, Serra A, Leigh RJ, Martinez-Conde S. Triggering mechanisms in microsaccade and saccade generation: a novel proposal. Ann N Y Acad Sci (2011) 1233:107–16. doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.2011.06177.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Kapoula Z, Yang Q, Otero-Millan J, Xiao S, Macknik SL, Lang A, et al. Distinctive features of microsaccades in Alzheimer’s disease and in mild cognitive impairment. Age (2014) 36(2):535–43. doi:10.1007/s11357-013-9582-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Layfield S, Burge W, Mitchell W, Ross L, Denning C, Amthor F, et al. The effect of speed of processing training on microsaccade amplitude. PLoS One (2014) 9(9):e107808. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0107808

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Clements K, Devonshire I, Reynolds J, Overton P. Enhanced visual responses in the superior colliculus in an animal model of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder and their suppression by D-amphetamine. Neuroscience (2014) 274:289–98. doi:10.1016/j.neuroscience.2014.05.054

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Panagiotidi M, Overton P, Stafford T. Increased microsaccade rate in individuals with ADHD traits. J Eye Mov Res (2017) 10(1):1–9. doi:10.16910/jemr.10.1.6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Gould TD, Bastain TM, Israel ME, Hommer DW, Castellanos FX. Altered performance on an ocular fixation task in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biol Psychiatry (2001) 50(8):633–5. doi:10.1016/S0006-3223(01)01095-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Armstrong I, Munoz D. Attentional blink in adults with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Exp Brain Res (2003) 152(2):243–50. doi:10.1007/s00221-003-1535-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Munoz DP, Armstrong IT, Hampton KA, Moore KD. Altered control of visual fixation and saccadic eye movements in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. J Neurophysiol (2003) 90(1):503–14. doi:10.1152/jn.00192.2003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Fried M, Tsitsiashvili E, Bonneh YS, Sterkin A, Wygnanski-Jaffe T, Epstein T, et al. ADHD subjects fail to suppress eye blinks and microsaccades while anticipating visual stimuli but recover with medication. Vision Res (2014) 101:62–72. doi:10.1016/j.visres.2014.05.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Kessler RC, Adler L, Ames M, Demler O, Faraone S, Hiripi E, et al. The World Health Organization Adult ADHD Self-Report Scale (ASRS): a short screening scale for use in the general population. Psychol Med (2005) 35(2):245–56. doi:10.1017/S0033291704002892

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Dankner Y, Shalev-Mevorach L, Carrasco M, Yuval-Greenberg S. Pre-stimulus inhibition of microsaccades in adults with and without ADHD as an index for temporal expectations. J Vis (2016) 16(12):598–598. doi:10.1167/16.12.598

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Frey HP, Molholm S, Lalor EC, Russo NN, Foxe JJ. Atypical cortical representation of peripheral visual space in children with an autism spectrum disorder. Eur J Neurosci (2013) 38(1):2125–38. doi:10.1111/ejn.12243

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Shirama A, Kanai C, Kato N, Kashino M. Ocular fixation abnormality in patients with autism spectrum disorder. J Autism Dev Disord (2016) 46(5):1613–22. doi:10.1007/s10803-015-2688-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Shaikh AG, Finkelstein SR, Schuchard R, Ross G, Juncos JL. Fixational eye movements in Tourette syndrome. Neurol Sci (2017) 38(11):1977–84. doi:10.1007/s10072-017-3069-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Egaña JI, Devia C, Mayol RV, Parrini J, Orellana G, Ruiz A, et al. Small saccades and image complexity during free viewing of natural images in schizophrenia. Front Psychiatry (2013) 4:37. doi:10.3389/fpsyt.2013.00037

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Beedie SA, Clair DMS, Benson PJ. Atypical scanpaths in schizophrenia: evidence of a trait-or state-dependent phenomenon? J Psychiatry Neurosci (2011) 36(3):150. doi:10.1503/jpn.090169

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Kozeis N, Anogeianaki A, Mitova DT, Anogianakis G, Mitov T, Felekidis A, et al. Visual function and execution of microsaccades related to reading skills, in cerebral palsied children. Int J Neurosci (2006) 116(11):1347–58. doi:10.1080/00207450500514011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Reinhard JI, Damm I, Ivanov IV, Trauzettel-Klosinski S. Eye movements during saccadic and fixation tasks in patients with homonymous hemianopia. J Neuroophthalmol (2014) 34(4):354–61. doi:10.1097/wno.0000000000000146

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Gao Y, Sabel BA. Microsaccade dysfunction and adaptation in hemianopia after stroke. Restor Neurol Neurosci (2017) 35(4):365–76. doi:10.3233/RNN-170749

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Sacher Y, Bonneh Y, Doron R, Fried M, Kadosh O, Lev M, et al. Unilateral blindsight following traumatic brain injury: a case report. Presented at: Rehab Science and Technology Update. Tel Aviv (2016).

Google Scholar

36. Di Stasi LL, Cabestrero R, McCamy MB, Ríos F, Catena A, Quirós P, et al. Intersaccadic drift velocity is sensitive to short-term hypobaric hypoxia. Eur J Neurosci (2014) 39(8):1384–90. doi:10.1111/ejn.12482

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Ciuffreda KJ, Kenyon RV, Stark L. Fixational eye movements in amblyopia and strabismus. J Am Optom Assoc (1979) 50(11):1251–8.

Google Scholar

38. Ciuffreda KJ, Kenyon RV, Stark L. Suppression of fixational saccades in strabismic and anisometropic amblyopia. Ophthalmic Res (1979) 11(1):31–9. doi:10.1159/000264983

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Ciuffreda KJ, Kenyon RV, Stark L. Increased drift in amblyopic eyes. Br J Ophthalmol (1980) 64(1):7–14. doi:10.1136/bjo.64.1.7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Srebro R. Fixation of normal and amblyopic eyes. Arch Ophthalmol (1983) 101(2):214–7. doi:10.1001/archopht.1983.01040010216006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Wald G, Burian HM. The dissociation of form and light perception in strabismus amblyopia. Am J Ophthalmol (1944) 27:950–63. doi:10.1016/S0002-9394(44)92327-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. von Noorden GK, Burian HM. An electro-oculographic study of the behavior of the fixation in amblyopic eyes in light- and dark-adapted state: a preliminary report. Am J Ophthalmol (1958) 46:68. doi:10.1016/0002-9394(58)90037-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Shi XF, Xu LM, Li Y, Wang T, Zhao KX, Sabel BA. Fixational saccadic eye movements are altered in anisometropic amblyopia. Restor Neurol Neurosci (2012) 30(6):445–62. doi:10.3233/RNN-2012-129000

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Otero-Millan J, Macknik SL, Martinez-Conde S. Fixational eye movements and binocular vision. Front Integr Neurosci (2014) 8:52. doi:10.3389/fnint.2014.00052

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. González EG, Wong AMF, Niechwiej-Szwedo E, Tarita-Nistor L, Steinbach MJ. Eye position stability in amblyopia and in normal binocular vision. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci (2012) 53(9):5386–94. doi:10.1167/iovs.12-9941

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Ghasia FF, Otero-Millan J, Shaikh AG. Abnormal fixational eye movements in strabismus. Br J Ophthalmol (2018) 102:253–9. doi:10.1136/bjophthalmol-2017-310346

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Subramanian V, Jost RM, Birch EE. A quantitative study of fixation stability in amblyopiafixation stability in amblyopia. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci (2013) 54(3):1998–2003. doi:10.1167/iovs.12-11054

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Lawwill T. Local adaptation in functional amblyopia. Am J Ophthalmol (1968) 65(6):903–6. doi:10.1016/0002-9394(68)92219-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Hess RF, Campbell FW, Greenhalgh T. On the nature of the neural abnormality in human amblyopia; neural aberrations and neural sensitivity loss. Pflugers Arch (1978) 377(3):201–7. doi:10.1007/BF00584273

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Ciuffreda KJ, Kenyon RV, Stark L. Saccadic intrusions in strabismus. Arch Ophthalmol (1979) 97(9):1673–9. doi:10.1001/archopht.1979.01020020241012

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Martinez-Conde S, Macknik SL, Troncoso XG, Dyar TA. Microsaccades counteract visual fading during fixation. Neuron (2006) 49(2):297–305. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2005.11.033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Ciuffreda KJ, Kenyon RV, Stark L. Different rates of functional recovery of eye movements during orthoptics treatment in an adult amblyope. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci (1979) 18(2):213–9.

Google Scholar

53. Raveendran RN, Babu RJ, Hess RF, Bobier WR. Transient improvements in fixational stability in strabismic amblyopes following bifoveal fixation and reduced interocular suppression. Ophthalmic Physiol Opt (2014) 34(2):214–25. doi:10.1111/opo.12119

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Loudon SE, Rook CA, Nassif DS, Piskun NV, Hunter DG. Rapid, high-accuracy detection of strabismus and amblyopia using the pediatric vision scanner. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci (2011) 52(8):5043–8. doi:10.1167/iovs.11-7503

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Selenow A, Ciuffreda KJ. Vision function recovery during orthoptic therapy in an exotropic amblyope with high unilateral myopia. Am J Optom Physiol Opt (1983) 60(8):659–66. doi:10.1097/00006324-198308000-00003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Leigh RJ, Thurston SE, Tomsak RL, Grossman GE, Lanska DJ. Effect of monocular visual loss upon stability of gaze. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci (1989) 30(2):288–92.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

57. Kumar G, Chung ST. Characteristics of fixational eye movements in people with macular disease. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci (2014) 55(8):5125–33. doi:10.1167/iovs.14-14608

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Moller F, Laursen ML, Sjolie AK. Binocular fixation topography in patients with diabetic macular oedema: possible implications for photocoagulation therapy (3rd revision). Graefes Arch Clin Exp Ophthalmol (2005) 243(9):903–10. doi:10.1007/s00417-004-1120-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Jakobsen NS, Larsen DA, Bek T. Binocular fixation reduces fixational eye movements in the worst eye of patients with center-involving diabetic macular edema. Ophthalmic Res (2017) 58(3):142–9. doi:10.1159/000476038

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Ghasia FF, Shaikh AG. Uncorrected myopic refractive error increases microsaccade amplitude. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci (2015) 56(4):2531–5. doi:10.1167/iovs.14-15882

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Hafed ZM, Stingl K, Bartz-Schmidt K-U, Gekeler F, Zrenner E. Oculomotor behavior of blind patients seeing with a subretinal visual implant. Vision Res (2016) 118:119–31. doi:10.1016/j.visres.2015.04.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Stingl K, Bartz-Schmidt KU, Besch D, Chee CK, Cottriall CL, Gekeler F, et al. Subretinal visual implant Alpha IMS – clinical trial interim report. Vis Res (2015) 111:149–60. doi:10.1016/j.visres.2015.03.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Zrenner E, Bartz-Schmidt KU, Benav H, Besch D, Bruckmann A, Gabel V-P, et al. Subretinal electronic chips allow blind patients to read letters and combine them to words. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci (2011) 278(1711):1489–97. doi:10.1098/rspb.2010.1747

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Troncoso XG, Macknik SL, Martinez-Conde S. Microsaccades counteract perceptual filling-in. J Vis (2008) 8(14):15.1–9. doi:10.1167/8.14.15

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. McCamy MB, Otero-Millan J, Macknik SL, Yang Y, Troncoso XG, Baer SM, et al. Microsaccadic efficacy and contribution to foveal and peripheral vision. J Neurosci (2012) 32(27):9194–204. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0515-12.2012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Costela FM, McCamy MB, Macknik SL, Otero-Millan J, Martinez-Conde S. Microsaccades restore the visibility of minute foveal targets. PeerJ (2013) 1:e119. doi:10.7717/peerj.119

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. McCamy MB, Macknik SL, Martinez-Conde S. Different fixational eye movements mediate the prevention and the reversal of visual fading. J Physiol (2014) 592(19):4381–94. doi:10.1113/jphysiol.2014.279059

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Costela FM, McCamy MB, Coffelt M, Otero-Millan J, Macknik S, Martinez-Conde S. Changes in visibility as a function of spatial frequency and microsaccade occurrence. Eur J Neurosci (2017) 45:433–9. doi:10.1111/ejn.13487

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Stingl K, Bartz-Schmidt KU, Besch D, Braun A, Bruckmann A, Gekeler F, et al. Artificial vision with wirelessly powered subretinal electronic implant Alpha-IMS. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci (2013) 280(1757):20130077. doi:10.1098/rspb.2013.0077

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Weiland JD, Walston ST, Humayun MS. Electrical stimulation of the retina to produce artificial vision. Annu Rev Vis Sci (2016) 2:273–94. doi:10.1146/annurev-vision-111815-114425

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Goetz G, Palanker D. Electronic approaches to restoration of sight. Rep Prog Phys (2016) 79(9):096701. doi:10.1088/0034-4885/79/9/096701

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

72. Behrend MR, Ahuja AK, Humayun MS, Chow RH, Weiland JD. Resolution of the epiretinal prosthesis is not limited by electrode size. IEEE Trans Neural Syst Rehabil Eng (2011) 19(4):436–42. doi:10.1109/TNSRE.2011.2140132

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: microsaccades, fixational eye movements, square-wave jerks, Parkinson’s disease, progressive supranuclear palsy, amblyopia, strabismus, fixational saccades

Citation: Alexander RG, Macknik SL and Martinez-Conde S (2018) Microsaccade Characteristics in Neurological and Ophthalmic Disease. Front. Neurol. 9:144. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2018.00144

Received: 16 August 2017; Accepted: 27 February 2018;
Published: 13 March 2018

Edited by:

Aasef G. Shaikh, Case Western Reserve University, United States

Reviewed by:

Alessandro Serra, Louis Stokes Cleveland VA Medical Center (VHA), United States
Caroline Tilikete, Institut National de la Santé et de la Recherche Médicale (INSERM), France

Copyright: © 2018 Alexander, Macknik and Martinez-Conde. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Susana Martinez-Conde, smart@neuralcorrelate.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.