Corrigendum: Multivariate Analyses of Balance Test Performance, Vestibular Thresholds, and Age
- 1Jenks Vestibular Physiology Laboratory, Mass Eye and Ear Infirmary, Boston, MA, United States
- 2Otolaryngology, Harvard Medical School, Harvard University, Boston, MA, United States
- 3Smead Aerospace Engineering Sciences, University of Colorado, Boulder, CO, United States
- 4Division of Sleep Medicine, Brigham and Women’s Hospital, Boston, MA, United States
We previously published vestibular perceptual thresholds and performance in the Modified Romberg Test of Standing Balance in 105 healthy humans ranging from ages 18 to 80 (1). Self-motion thresholds in the dark included roll tilt about an earth-horizontal axis at 0.2 and 1 Hz, yaw rotation about an earth-vertical axis at 1 Hz, y-translation (interaural/lateral) at 1 Hz, and z-translation (vertical) at 1 Hz. In this study, we focus on multiple variable analyses not reported in the earlier study. Specifically, we investigate correlations (1) among the five thresholds measured and (2) between thresholds, age, and the chance of failing condition 4 of the balance test, which increases vestibular reliance by having subjects stand on foam with eyes closed. We found moderate correlations (0.30–0.51) between vestibular thresholds for different motions, both before and after using our published aging regression to remove age effects. We found that lower or higher thresholds across all threshold measures are an individual trait that account for about 60% of the variation in the population. This can be further distributed into two components with about 20% of the variation explained by aging and 40% of variation explained by a single principal component that includes similar contributions from all threshold measures. When only roll tilt 0.2 Hz thresholds and age were analyzed together, we found that the chance of failing condition 4 depends significantly on both (p = 0.006 and p = 0.013, respectively). An analysis incorporating more variables found that the chance of failing condition 4 depended significantly only on roll tilt 0.2 Hz thresholds (p = 0.046) and not age (p = 0.10), sex nor any of the other four threshold measures, suggesting that some of the age effect might be captured by the fact that vestibular thresholds increase with age. For example, at 60 years of age, the chance of failing is roughly 5% for the lowest roll tilt thresholds in our population, but this increases to 80% for the highest roll tilt thresholds. These findings demonstrate the importance of roll tilt vestibular cues for balance, even in individuals reporting no vestibular symptoms and with no evidence of vestibular dysfunction.
Introduction
Deficits in postural control and resulting falls have profound public health implications (>31,000 deaths and >800,000 hospitalizations/year) (2). Sensory feedback plays a critical role in postural control (3–10). A number of studies have linked vestibular function and falls. One epidemiological study (11) showed that that failure to complete the condition 4 of the Modified Romberg foam test (12–14), which is commonly considered a balance assay of vestibular function, was highly correlated with “difficulty with falling” in the past 12 months. Another epidemiological study reported that 80% of fallers admitted to an Emergency department in the UK had symptoms of vestibular impairment (15). Most recently, we reported significant correlations between failure to complete the Modified Romberg foam balance test and roll tilt perceptual thresholds in the dark (1). These roll tilt perceptual thresholds were previously shown to primarily be a measure of vestibular function (16). These findings complement previous studies showing that vestibular dysfunction negatively impacts clinical balance test performance (3, 4, 6, 17–20). While speculative, a previous manuscript (1) also provided statistical calculations that emphasize the significance of the problem, suggesting that falls associated with vestibular function might cause somewhere between 50,000 and 150,000 deaths in America each year. This would rank vestibular dysfunction somewhere between number 10 and number 3 on the list of leading causes of death in the United States1 (21).
The aforementioned studies emphasize the importance of understanding the connections between vestibular function (and dysfunction), age, and falls. Therefore, we decided to perform another set of analyses on our previously published data set (1) of 105 subjects between 18 and 80 year of age who had been prescreened to be suffering no vestibular symptoms. There was no evidence for differences in the thresholds of males and females, but statistically significant threshold increases above the age of 40 were reported for all five motions investigated: roll tilt about an earth-horizontal axis at 0.2 and 1 Hz, yaw rotation about an earth-vertical axis at 1 Hz, y-translation (interaural/lateral) at 1 Hz, and z-translation (vertical) at 1 Hz. These threshold data were best modeled by a two-segment age model having a constant baseline below an age cutoff around 40 years of age with thresholds increasing above the age of 40. Building on these findings, this manuscript focuses on correlations—both correlations in the threshold measures themselves and correlations between thresholds, age, and failing the vestibular part (condition 4) of the Modified Romberg foam test.
Correlations between thresholds and balance are important because they might help us intervene to prevent falls via (a) warnings (e.g., verbal) from a clinician informing patients about their individual fall risk, (b) rehabilitation/training (22, 23), or (c) via the use of prosthetics/aids like canes, balance feedback devices (24), vestibular prosthesis (25–28), vibro-tactile shoes (29), stochastic resonance of the vestibular system (30–33), and/or orthotic devices (34).
Understanding the correlations between thresholds for different types of motion (e.g., yaw rotation and interaural translation) is also important as it could provide clues regarding the mechanism that causes increased thresholds with age (1). To further explore this, we used principal component analysis (PCA) to understand the structure underlying intersubject variability in thresholds. Moreover, given the vital importance that thresholds might play in predicting fall risk, understanding correlations between thresholds may improve fall risk evaluation.
As detailed in Section “Discussion,” these multivariate analyses yield new findings that were not presented in our previous study. First, while our previous univariate analysis found that all five threshold measures were correlated with chance of failing condition 4, these new multivariate analyses show that only roll tilt 0.2 Hz has a statistically significant correlation after taking into account the relationship between the threshold measures. While the previous analyses found that thresholds are correlated with chance of failing condition 4 even after adjusting for age effects, the new analyses show how the chance of failing depends on the combination of age and roll tilt 0.2 Hz thresholds. We provide correlation coefficients between all threshold measures both before and after age adjustment. Furthermore, new analyses determine the structure of variation in thresholds, including the effects of aging and individual differences.
Materials and Methods
Subjects
As previously described (1), 105 subjects (54 females, 51 males) participated, ranging from 18 to 80 years old. Subjects were excluded if they reported, via a questionnaire, any major health problems (e.g., a history of neurological, otologic, vestibular, and chronic uncontrolled diseases) or any history of vestibular symptoms. The study was approved by the MEEI Human Studies Committee, and written informed consent was obtained from all subjects as dictated by the Declaration of Helsinki.
Balance Testing
The modified Romberg test of standing balance on firm and compliant support surfaces (14) was used to assess balance function (12, 13). Subjects stand with arms crossed and feet together. The test has four conditions, each of which was scored as pass/fail. Each condition must be passed to progress to the next, with two attempts permitted for each condition, according to the following criteria. In condition 1, the subject must stand on the floor for 15 s with eyes open. In condition 2, the subject must stand on the floor for 15 s with eyes closed. In condition 3, the subject must stand on memory foam with eyes open for 30 s. In condition 4, the subject must stand on the foam with eyes closed for 30 s. A fail occurred when subjects did any of the following before the allotted time for each trial: move their feet for stability, open their eyes, or release their arms. Condition 4 primarily assesses vestibular function (11, 14), since visual contributions are eliminated and the foam makes kinesthetic cues unreliable. Six subjects did not perform the balance test and were not included in the balance analyses in this study.
Vestibular Threshold Measurements
The detailed methods used to assay perceptual threshold have been published (1, 16, 35, 36). Upright subjects in the dark were seated in a chair with a helmet to reduce head movement and a five-point harness. The chair was mounted on a Moog 6DOF motion platform that delivered single-cycle acceleration motion stimuli. Subjects listened to white noise through noise-canceling headphones, both to indicate when motion was occurring and to mask other auditory cues. To improve efficiency (37), stimuli magnitudes were selected using an adaptive three-down, one-up staircase (37, 38), in which stimuli become smaller after three consecutive correct responses, and larger after one incorrect response. The staircase followed parameter estimation by sequential testing (PEST) rules (39). The direction (e.g., left/right) was determined randomly for each trial. Testing occurred in blocks of 100 trials, and each trial consisted of a motion stimulus followed by a response. Subjects reported their perception of motion direction using buttons in their left or right hand and were required to report a perception for every trial.
Each subject participated in five blocks of testing lasting approximately 3 h—one block for each of the five types of motion—with the following “motion type” conditions: (1) “yaw 1 Hz”—yaw rotations about an earth-vertical axis at 1 Hz (i.e., a motion duration of 1 s), with an initial stimulus of 4°/s; (2) “Y 1 Hz”—y-translations (lateral/interaural) along an earth-horizontal axis at 1 Hz, with an initial stimulus of 4 cm/s; (3) “Z 1 Hz”—z-translations (superior–inferior) along an earth-vertical axis at 1 Hz, with an initial stimulus of 16 cm/s; (4) “Roll tilt 1 Hz”—ear-down tilts about a head-centered earth-horizontal axis at 1 Hz, with an initial stimulus of 3°/s; and (5) “Roll tilt 0.2 Hz”—the same at 0.2 Hz, with an initial stimulus of 2°/s. Motions at 1 Hz were selected to focus on either the semicircular canals (SCCs) or otolith organs and because 100 trials can be completed in less than 10 min; roll tilt 0.2 Hz was used to study integration of otolith and SCC cues (40).
Threshold (σ) was determined for each block by fitting a Gaussian cumulative distribution psychometric function (41, 42) to the binary responses. The mean of the Gaussian (μ) is often called the “vestibular bias” and corresponds to the stimulus for which there is an equal likelihood of a left vs. right response (or up vs. down).
Data Analysis
All analyses were performed using Matlab R2014b (Mathworks, MA, USA). All statistical analyses were conducted using log-transformed thresholds (e.g., geometric means for across subject averages) because vestibular thresholds have been shown to demonstrate a lognormal distribution across subjects (1, 43, 44).
Correlation coefficients were calculated using Pearson’s correlation. When statistical tests were performed on correlations across the thresholds for different motion types, yielding ten statistical tests, a Bonferroni correction was used for multiple comparisons, using a critical value (pc) of 0.05/10 = 0.005. PCA was performed after standardizing log-transformed thresholds by subtracting the mean from all values and dividing by the SD.
Multiple variable logistic regression was used to model the relationship between the chance of failing condition 4 of the balance test and thresholds, age and sex; logistic regression was used because of the binary pass/fail nature of the dependent variable. The Matlab command fitglm with a binomial distribution was used to perform logistic regressions. Stepwise regression (45) was used for variable selection using the Matlab command stepwiseglm, which uses a forward and backward stepwise procedure and a Bayesian information criteria (BICs). All regressions included an intercept term, which was statistically significant in all analyses.
Analyses were also performed on age-adjusted thresholds. Our previous publication (1) determined that a piecewise model best described the effect of aging on vestibular thresholds, with a constant baseline below 42.5 years, followed by a linear increase above this age cutoff. Each motion type had unique baseline and slope values. For subjects younger than the age cutoff, the age-adjusted threshold was simply their threshold. For subjects older than the age cutoff, we determine each age-adjusted threshold using this equation:
Effectively, this factored out known changes with age by modifying any threshold above the age cutoff—leaving only threshold variations after removing the effect of age. Note that subtraction of the threshold from the model is done after log transformation because of the lognormal threshold distribution.
Results
Threshold Correlations
Thresholds
We applied the published age fitting model (1) to compute the age-adjusted thresholds (Figure 1) which are simply the thresholds after removing the average age effect. Results are segmented by sex to illustrate that there is no effect of sex even after age adjustment, which confirms our previous report of no statistically significant differences between females and males in any threshold measure. Results are also segmented by subjects who passed (⚪) and failed (×) condition 4 of the balance test and show that subjects who failed condition 4 tended to have higher roll tilt thresholds. Older subjects also tended to fail condition 4 more than younger subjects (i.e., more failures on right side of plots).
Figure 1. Age-adjusted thresholds for each subject and each of the five motion types. The published aging regression (1) found that thresholds had a constant baseline until 42.5 years, then increased at different rates for each of the five threshold measures. Thresholds for each subject above 42.5 years were adjusted to leave only variations independent of age. (A) The gray lines show the decrease in yaw threshold applied to each subject, with the top of the line showing the uncorrected threshold and the symbol at the bottom of the line showing the age-adjusted threshold. (B–E) Results for the four other threshold measures.
To further explore the relationship between age, thresholds and balance, we determined the average thresholds for different age groups segmented by results of condition 4 of the balance test (Table 1; Figure 2). While detailed statistical analyses of this relationship will follow, we note a tendency toward higher thresholds for subjects who failed condition 4 in comparison to those who passed condition 4.
Table 1. Thresholds segmented by age group and results of condition 4 of the balance test, with 95% confidence intervals.
Figure 2. Thresholds segmented by age group and results of condition 4 of the balance test, with error bars showing SD.
Correlations between Vestibular Threshold Measures
We examined correlations between threshold measures for each motion type, since these correlations have important practical implications and also provide some insight into the shared organs and mechanisms underlying vestibular sensation. Figure 3 shows the relationship between thresholds across subjects. For example, Figure 3A shows the relationship between roll tilt 0.2 Hz thresholds and roll tilt 1 Hz thresholds. In this case, the correlation coefficient was ra = 0.63 for all subjects, and rp = 0.50 for only subjects who passed condition 4 of the balance test. Each threshold pair had a positive, statistically significant (multiple comparison, p < 0.005) correlation.
Figure 3. Scatterplots showing the relationship between each of the threshold measures across subjects. Each subject is shown, and segmented into those who passed (dots) and failed (×) condition 4 of the balance test. Correlation coefficients were calculated using log-transformed thresholds, including the correlation across all subjects (ra) and only those who passed condition 4 (rp). All correlations were significant at the p < 0.005 level (indicated by *). (A) The relationship between roll tilt 0.2 Hz thresholds and roll tilt 1 Hz thresholds across subjects. (B–J) The relationships between other corresponding pairs of thresholds.
Since all thresholds increase with age, some correlations may occur because of the aging process. To focus on correlation due to factors other than aging, we performed the same analysis on age-adjusted thresholds (Figure 1). Figure 4 shows the relationships between these age-adjusted thresholds. All correlation coefficients decreased somewhat (Table 2) compared with unadjusted thresholds, but all remained statistically significant (multiple comparison, p < 0.005) when calculated for all subjects. Thus, age alone does not explain these correlations. Section “Discussion” provides our interpretation of these correlation coefficients, including the observation that the lowest correlation coefficients were between yaw thresholds and the other four thresholds. When calculated for subjects who passed condition 4, most correlation coefficients were statistically significant, with a few exceptions (p = 0.01, p = 0.02, p = 0.02) that did not reach statistical significance after multiple comparisons correction (pc of 0.05/10 = 0.005).
Figure 4. Scatterplots showing the relationship between each of the age-adjusted threshold measures across subjects. This repeats that analysis shown in Figure 3 in a way that focuses on correlation due to factors other than aging. Each subject is shown, and segmented into those who passed (dots) and failed (×) condition 4 of the balance test. Correlation coefficients were calculated using log-transformed, age-adjusted thresholds, including the correlation across all subjects (ra) and only those who passed condition 4 (rp). Most correlations were statistically significant at the p < 0.005 level (indicated by *), with exceptions noted by the actual p value. (A) The relationship between roll tilt 0.2 Hz thresholds and roll tilt 1 Hz thresholds across subjects. (B–J) The relationships between other corresponding pairs of thresholds.
Structure Underlying Variation in Vestibular Thresholds
We examined the underlying structure of the relationship between thresholds. This was done using PCA, which determines principal components that are a weighted combination of each of the other measures such that they explain most of the variation within the data. The PCA analysis yielded similar results for unadjusted thresholds (Table 3) and age-adjusted threshold (Table 4). The contribution shows the variation in the data explained by each component. Since age adjustment explains 20.7% of the variation in the data (1), we also determined the variation explained by each component as a fraction of the remaining 79.3% of the variation (Table 5, second row). For comparison, the variation explained by each component for the unadjusted thresholds is shown (Table 5, first row).
Table 5. Variance explained by each principal component after considering the variance explained by age adjustment.
More than half of intersubject variability is explained by the first component, suggesting a relatively low-dimensional structure underlying thresholds and vestibular sensory precision. Its projection remains (i.e., the weights remain) surprisingly similar after age adjustment, suggesting that lower or higher thresholds across all measures are an important individual trait. For unadjusted thresholds, the first component has a contribution of 60% (Table 3). After age adjustment, the first component has a contribution of 52% (Table 4), which is 41% (Table 5) of the total variance (52%⋅79.3% = 41%). This shows that much of the variation due to age was included in the first principal component. The remaining components are more difficult to interpret, but the second component may allow a decoupling of yaw and roll thresholds, concomitant with their low correlation.
Correlations with Condition 4 of the Modified Romberg Foam Test
The Relationship between Vestibular Thresholds and Condition 4 of the Modified Romberg Foam Test
Our previous study (1) examined the basic relationship between failing condition 4 of the Modified Romberg test and vestibular thresholds. However, those analyses only looked at the relationship between each individual threshold and the chance of failing the test, without performing multivariate analyses. Specifically, we performed single-variable logistic regressions between failures and each age-adjusted threshold. There were statistically significant correlations with roll 0.2 Hz (p = 0.003) and roll 1 Hz (p = 0.02) thresholds, a suggestion of possible correlation with yaw 1 Hz (p = 0.09) and z-translation 1 Hz (p = 0.09) thresholds, and a non-significant correlation for y-translation (p = 0.50) thresholds. Given that thresholds may be correlated with each other, a weakness of this analysis was that it did not determine if the covariation between thresholds described above could have resulted in an artifact of some thresholds being correlated with the chance of failing condition 4.
Here, we further explore this relationship using multivariate analyses. The 99 subjects who did balance testing all passed conditions 1, 2, and 3. 79 subjects passed condition 4, while 20 failed. Table 6 shows the results of a multiple variable logistic regression to predict the chance of failing condition 4 based on age and vestibular thresholds. This analysis found that only roll tilt 0.2 Hz thresholds had a statistically significant (p = 0.046) relationship to the chance of failing condition 4. Sex and all other thresholds were not significant, with p > 0.47. In particular, roll tilt 1 Hz thresholds were not significantly associated with the chance of failing condition 4, even though our previous single-variable regression found a statistically significant relationship. This was likely due to the correlations between roll tilt 0.2 Hz and roll tilt 1 Hz thresholds described above, which demonstrates the importance of the multiple variable analysis. Figure 5 shows logistic regression curves depicting the dependency of failing condition 4 on age and each threshold measure. Figure 5A shows that younger subjects tend to pass (⚪) the balance test while older subjects tend to fail (×), with the logistic regression curve showing that the chance of failing the balance test is 4% for an 18 years old, and increases to 45% for an 80 years old. Each curve is generated by holding the other five variables at the median value of their sample, so it does not show the combined influence of multiple variables. Figure 5B shows that subjects with low roll tilt 0.2 Hz thresholds tend to pass (⚪) the balance test while subjects with higher thresholds tend to fail (×), with the logistic regression curve showing that the chance of failing the balance test is near 0% for the lowest roll tilt 0.2 Hz thresholds in our population, and increases to 61% for the highest thresholds. Figures 5C–F show the results for other thresholds, confirming the weak relationship between these variables and the chance of failing condition 4.
Table 6. Results of a multiple logistic regression to predict the chance of failing condition 4 of the balance test based on age and log-transformed vestibular thresholds.
Figure 5. Logistic regression curves showing the dependency of failing condition 4 on age and each threshold measure. (A) Circles (○) indicate individual subjects who passed condition 4, while crosses (×) indicate subjects who failed condition 4. A logistic curve showing the dependency of the chance of failing condition 4 on age, with the shaded area showing the 95% confidence intervals. Each curve is generated by holding the other five variables at the median value of their sample, so it does not show the combined influence of multiple variables. (B–F) Similar analyses for each of the five threshold measurements.
Since the regression revealed that many variables had little or no effect on the chance of failing condition 4, we aimed to create a simplified model that included only relevant variables. We used a stepwise procedure (45) to determine which variables to include in the model. Table 7 shows the results for the simplified model, which includes only age and roll tilt 0.2 Hz thresholds, both of which were statistically significant. This model had a lower BIC value than the complete model (85.4 vs. 106). To further confirm this model selection, we also investigated models that included: (1) each threshold without age, (2) age + each threshold, and (3) age + roll tilt 0.2 Hz thresholds + each other threshold. The selected model provided better fits than each of these, according to BIC values (these models are detailed in Section “Model Comparisons” in Appendix). We also considered a model that included age, roll tilt 0.2 Hz threshold and an interaction term between the two, but the interaction term was not significant (p = 0.054), and had slightly worse fit quality (BIC 85.8 vs. 85.4) so we focus on the simpler model.
Table 7. Results of a multiple logistic regression after application of a stepwise algorithm to provide a simplified model.
Both age and roll tilt 0.2 Hz thresholds had a statistically significant contribution to the chance of failing condition 4. Figure 6 shows the chance of failing condition 4 vs. both age and roll tilt 0.2 Hz thresholds. Generally it shows that the chance of failing condition 4 increases with both age and roll tilt 0.2 Hz thresholds. It emphasizes that for older subjects with high thresholds, the chance of failing is very high, approaching 90%. Figure 7A shows the dependency of failing condition 4 on age for different roll tilt thresholds. For example, for subjects whose roll tilt 0.2 Hz thresholds are in the 75th percentile, the chance of failing increases from 6% at age 18 to 68% at age 80. Figure 7B shows the dependency of failing condition 4 on roll tilt thresholds for subjects of different ages. For example, at 60 years old, the chance of failing is roughly 5% for the lowest roll tilt thresholds, which does not differ much from younger subjects. However, this increases to 80% for the highest roll tilt thresholds.
Figure 6. Logistic regression results showing the chance of failing condition 4 vs. both age and roll tilt 0.2 Hz thresholds determined using the model in Table 7. Symbols show subjects who passed (○) and failed (×) condition 4.
Figure 7. Logistic curves showing the chance of failing condition 4 vs. both age and roll tilt 0.2 Hz thresholds determined using the model in Table 7. (A) The dependency of failing condition 4 on age is shown for different roll tilt thresholds, which have been determined for the 5th, 25th, 50th, 75th, and 95th percentile within our sample. (B) The dependency of failing condition 4 on roll tilt 0.2 Hz thresholds for different ages (25, 40, 50, 60, and 75 years old). These curves are also shown in gray in Figure 6.
We also examined the relationship between the chance of failing condition 4 and age-adjusted thresholds shown in Figure 1. Table 8 shows the results of stepwise logistic regression using age-adjusted, log-transformed thresholds. Roll tilt 0.2 Hz thresholds and age were the only selected contributors and both were statistically significant, as for the unadjusted thresholds. While the coefficient for roll tilt 0.2 Hz thresholds was roughly the same for the age-adjusted and unadjusted thresholds, the coefficient of age was larger when age-related changes were removed from the threshold data. As detailed in Section “Discussion,” this is likely due to age effects, both vestibular and non-vestibular, being removed from the threshold measure, causing a stronger effect of age on the chance of failing condition 4. We compared regression models that included various combinations of age-adjusted thresholds and found that the selected model had the lowest BIC value (these models are detailed in Section “Model Comparisons” in Appendix).
Table 8. Results of a multiple logistic regression after application of a stepwise algorithm to provide a simplified model.
We also examined whether a relationship existed between failing condition 4 and vestibular bias. Since a large bias in any direction may indicate the presence of erroneous sensory information, analyses were performed using the absolute value of bias, normalized by the subject’s threshold. No significant contribution was found from bias in any of the five motion axes (p > 0.1), and only age was predictive of the chance of failing (p = 0.0006).
The Relationship between Principal Components and Modified Romberg Foam Test Performance
Since principal components incorporate contributions from all threshold measures, there is a possibility that a latent variable created by projecting all thresholds into a principal component would be a better predictor of the chance of failing condition 4 than using only the roll 0.2 Hz threshold. To evaluate this, we performed a logistic regression using latent variables created using the first and second principal components. For unadjusted, log-transformed thresholds, we found that the first component alone had a statistically significant relationship with the chance of failing condition 4 (p = 0.013), while age (p = 0.12) and the second component (p = 0.77) did not. Interestingly, a model that included only the first principal component (BIC 85.2) provided a similar fit quality to age + roll tilt 0.2 Hz thresholds (BIC 85.4), and slightly better fit quality than roll tilt 0.2 Hz thresholds alone (BIC 88.0) or age + first component (BIC 86.9). These results suggest that the first principal component includes both vestibular and age effects. We performed a similar analysis for age-adjusted, log-transformed thresholds. We found that age (p = 0.00033) and the first component (p = 0.010) had a statistically significant relationship with the chance of failing condition 4, while the second component (p = 0.65) did not. In this case, a model that included age + first component (BIC 86.1) provided a similar fit quality to age + roll tilt 0.2 Hz thresholds (BIC 84.9), and a better fit quality than the first component alone (BIC 99.8).
Discussion
We report correlations among the five thresholds measured, and between thresholds, age and the chance of failing condition 4 of the Modified Romberg balance test. We found moderate correlations (0.30–0.51) between vestibular thresholds, even while using age-adjusted thresholds. PCA suggest that lower or higher thresholds across all threshold measures are an individual trait which account for roughly 60% of the variation in the population; this can be further portioned into about 20% of variation being explained by aging and about 40% of the variation being explained by the first component that represents common variations across the five vestibular thresholds measured.
When only roll tilt 0.2 Hz thresholds and age were analyzed together, we found that the chance of failing condition 4 depends significantly on both (p = 0.006 and p = 0.013, respectively). An analysis incorporating more variables found that the chance of failing condition 4 depended significantly only on roll tilt 0.2 Hz thresholds (p = 0.046) and not age (p = 0.10), sex, nor any of the other four threshold measures, suggesting that some of the age effect might be captured by the fact that vestibular thresholds increase with age. This contrasts with our published univariate analyses which found a significant correlation with all five thresholds, and did not examine their combined contributions.
Illustrating the importance of considering both age and vestibular thresholds, at 60 years of age the average chance of failing condition 4 is 43%, but this ranges from roughly 5% for the lowest roll tilt thresholds in our population to 80% for the highest roll tilt thresholds. As a second illustration, at the median 0.2 Hz roll tilt threshold, the chance of failing condition 4 is 12%, but this ranges from roughly 3% at 18 years to 50% at 80 years.
We emphasize that these were subjects who qualified as healthy normals. Subjects who reported dizziness, imbalance, or other vestibular symptoms on the mandatory healthy questionnaire were excluded. Thus, we are characterizing how normal variability in human balance depends on “subclinical” intersubject differences in vestibular precision. In this way, our study complements previous work showing that vestibular dysfunction negatively impacts clinical balance test performance (3, 4, 6, 17–20).
New Contributions of This Study Compared with Our Previous Study
While our previous study (1) investigated some of the same topics as this study, several new and significant results are described herein. The previous study briefly stated that statistically significant correlations existed between threshold measures; this study visualizes these relationships and provides correlation coefficients both for unadjusted and age-adjusted thresholds. The PCA results are new.
The previous study, using single-variable logistic regression, reported that without age adjustment, all five threshold measures had a statistically significant correlation with the chance of failing condition 4 (p < 0.007). Even after age adjustment, the previous study reported statistically significant correlations for roll tilt 0.2 Hz thresholds (p = 0.003) and roll tilt 1 Hz thresholds (p = 0.02), a suggestion of possible correlation with yaw rotation thresholds (p = 0.09) and z-translation thresholds (p = 0.09), and not significant results for y-translation thresholds (p = 0.50). By contrast, the new multiple variable analysis found that only roll tilt 0.2 Hz thresholds and age had a statistically significant contribution to the chance of failing condition 4. The difference between the two results arises because the multiple variable analysis adjusts for the correlations between thresholds, while the single-variable regression fits do not. This study also shows the chance of failing condition 4 for different combinations of age and roll tilt 0.2 Hz thresholds, while the previous study provided an odds ratio for the chance of failing as a function of thresholds only. While the previous analyses showed that threshold was a significant contributor to chance of failing condition 4, even after age adjustment, it did not show the relative contribution of age and roll tilt 0.2 Hz thresholds (Figure 6).
Interpretation of Correlation Coefficients
Even after age adjustment, the correlation coefficients between different threshold measures ranged from 0.30 to 0.51, which are moderate correlations. We now consider three possibilities for how these correlations may arise from: (1) anatomical variations that impact all peripheral organs similarly, (2) shared peripheral organs, and/or (3) shared processing of cues.
The first principal component included relatively similar contributions from all thresholds, consistent with anatomical variation that impacts all organs or a common central cause. However, this does not explain the differences in correlation coefficients between threshold measures for different motion directions.
Correlations may arise from shared peripheral sensory organs. The largest coefficient (0.51) was between roll tilt 0.2 Hz and roll tilt 1 Hz thresholds, which may be explained by the shared superior and posterior SCC cues. Shared otolith cues may explain the next largest correlations, between roll tilt (both 0.2 and 1 Hz) and Z thresholds (0.46, 0.48), between Y and Z thresholds (0.44) and between roll tilt (both 0.2 and 1 Hz) and Y thresholds (0.41, 0.37). Finally, the lowest coefficients were between yaw thresholds and all others, which may be explained by separate yaw cues from the horizontal SCC. In particular, yaw and roll thresholds had the lowest coefficients (0.30, 0.31), which indicates that even within the SCCs, each canal plane has some unique characteristics. This supports the view that peripheral changes are either a less predominant cause of the variations or may be somewhat different for different end organs.
Correlations may arise from shared processing of vestibular cues. There is substantial evidence for spatial orientation internal models that combine otolith and SCC cues (46–53). In addition, there is evidence that there are separate streams of processing for the components of rotation that are about an earth-vertical vs. earth-horizontal axes (54–56). More specifically, yaw rotations about an earth-vertical axis only receive useful motion cues from the SCC, while tilts and translation about an earth-horizontal axis require SCC–otolith integration to disambiguate tilt and translation. This may result in our earth-vertical yaw rotation thresholds having lower correlations with the other threshold measures, all of which required SCC–otolith integration.
We note that multiple comparisons correction strictly applies to the case of a small number of rejections of the null hypothesis among a large group in which the null hypothesis is not rejected. In our correlations between thresholds, the null hypothesis was rejected in 17 of 20 comparisons, so the remaining three may not have occurred by chance.
Structure Underlying Variation in Vestibular Thresholds
Principal component analysis found that one component accounts for roughly 60% of the variation in the population. After age adjustment, 21% of variation is explained by aging and 41% of the variation is explained by the first component. The projection of the first component hardly changes after age adjustment, suggesting that lower or higher thresholds across all measures are an important individual trait. A possible interpretation of this component is that is represents (or partially represents) physiologic/biologic age (57, 58), which account for variations not explained by chronologic age (i.e., in years). It could also arise from anatomical or physiological covariation across subjects.
The remaining components (two through five) account for 40% of variation, and are more difficult to interpret, especially since they each explain roughly 10% of the total variation. The second component may allow a decoupling of yaw and roll thresholds, consistent with their low correlation. These components also include measurement imprecision by definition. For example, we have used simulations to estimate that when thresholds are determined using 100 trials and a three-down, one-up staircase, the predicted coefficient of variation is 18% (37). This accounts for only random effects, and does not consider “real world” effects like subjects losing concentration, fatigue and other lapses.
Balance Test Results
From Correlation to Expression of Fundamental Causes
Although correlations do not prove causation, a few factors convince us that the regression results are an expression of roll tilt 0.2 Hz thresholds as a fundamental cause of changes in balance performance. We describe these in detail below, but begin with a summary: (1) the converse relationship, that changes in balance cause changes in vestibular function, seems less likely; (2) among the thresholds we measured, roll tilt 0.2 Hz thresholds assay the cues that provide the most physically relevant sensory feedback for postural control; and (3) if covariate(s) of vestibular thresholds were responsible for the correlation between thresholds and chance of failing condition 4, it is unlikely that they would only covary with roll tilt 0.2 Hz thresholds.
First, while evidence from patients with vestibular deficits shows that vestibular function plays a causal role in the control of balance (3, 4, 6, 17–20), no evidence exists (to the best of our knowledge) that changes in balance cause changes in vestibular function. While vestibular sensory feedback has been shown to affect postural control, it is less clear how changes in balance caused by aging would affect vestibular function.
Second, the physiologic ability to remain upright depends on the sensation of gravitational direction, and thus roll tilt cues would be one of the fundamental cues required to control balance (59, 60). We have shown that roll tilt 0.2 Hz thresholds depend on both otolith cues about the direction of gravity as well as SCC cues about angular rotation (40). Thus, the regression results are consistent with this physical prediction. This argument is strengthened by the lack of statistically significant correlations between chance of failing condition 4 and vestibular cues (e.g., yaw rotation) that do not have a direct theoretical rationale for contributing to postural control.
Third, we consider the possibility that vestibular thresholds are covariates of another variable that is the true primary underlying cause of balance dysfunction, since we did not quantify all of the many contributors to balance (e.g., biomechanics, muscle strength, proprioception, and vision—when available, movement strategies, sensory integration, cognitive processing, etc.) (4). For example, both muscle strength and vestibular function decline with age, and thus are likely correlated across subjects. Since muscle strength is correlated with fall risk (61) and balance performance (62), one could imagine that a regression between vestibular function and balance performance could give a positive result that arises because of covariation between vestibular function and muscle strength. However, if this were true, it is unlikely that only a single vestibular threshold out of five would be correlated with covariates, but we find only a single vestibular threshold is correlated with the chance of failing condition 4. Furthermore, it would be an unlikely coincidence that roll tilt 0.2 Hz threshold would be correlated with the chance of failing condition 4 because of covariates, given the physical explanation described earlier.
We conducted further analyses to provide additional support that covariates are unlikely to explain our correlations. We performed a multiple variable logistic regression between age, Y, Z, yaw, and roll tilt 1 Hz thresholds (i.e. all measures except for roll tilt 0.2 Hz thresholds) and found that none of the thresholds had a statistically significant relationship with the chance of failing condition 4 (Y p = 0.71; Z p = 0.46; yaw p = 0.40; roll tilt 1 Hz p = 0.16). We compared this to the multiple variable logistic regression that included the same variables as well as roll tilt 0.2 Hz thresholds. The difference between these fits was statistically significant (χ2-statistic, p = 0.037) showing that adding the roll tilt 0.2 Hz thresholds as a regression variable yielded significant model fit improvements even after all of the other available variables (including their various covariations) had already been included in the fit. A similar analysis in which each of the four other thresholds was dropped from the model did not yield a statistically significant difference from the model that included the five thresholds (χ2-statistic, p > 0.39).
Sagittal vs. Frontal Plane Motion
Balance requires control of orientation relative to gravity in both the sagittal (anterioposterior) and frontal (mediolateral) planes, and roll tilt thresholds only assay the cues that provide feedback about motion in the frontal plane. Future studies could measure pitch thresholds to determine the cues relevant to sagittal control, and to estimate the relative importance of sagittal and frontal mechanisms to the chance of failing condition 4. There is some evidence that the task we used in which the feet were together primarily challenges frontal plane control, since the rates of failing condition 4 were >20 vs. 1.8% in a similar task with a wide stance (63), which provides less frontal plane challenge and presumably similar sagittal plane challenge. Therefore, we hypothesize that, for the task we used, the chance of failing condition 4 would have a stronger correlation with roll tilt thresholds than pitch thresholds.
Model Comparison
A number of models performed similarly well at predicting the chance of failing condition 4, with BIC values between 84.9 and 86.9: (i) first principal component of thresholds, (ii) age + roll tilt 0.2 Hz thresholds, (iii) age + first principal component of age-adjusted thresholds, (iv) age + roll tilt 0.2 Hz age-adjusted thresholds, and (v) age + first principal component of thresholds. Since age + roll tilt 0.2 Hz thresholds relies on less data than PCA, and since it does not require the additional step of age adjustment, the simplicity of this model may make it preferable.
We found that age had a larger coefficient when roll tilt 0.2 Hz thresholds were age-adjusted vs. when they were unadjusted. Since vestibular age effects were removed from the threshold measure, it is unsurprising that this causes age itself to have a stronger effect on the chance of failing condition 4. Furthermore, age serves as a proxy for the various contributors to balance which we did not measure (i.e., non-vestibular). Thus, we propose that the effects of age on vestibular thresholds also covaried with these non-vestibular factors, and when the covarying age effects were removed from the roll tilt 0.2 Hz thresholds, the age coefficient was adjusted to capture just the non-vestibular age effects on the chance of failing condition 4.
Related to this, we also note that while age is statistically significant in the model that includes roll tilt 0.2 Hz thresholds and age, in the models that include age, sex and all five thresholds, age is not statistically significant (p = 0.10). The most likely explanation for this is that since vestibular thresholds vary with age, the age effect is spread among age and the vestibular thresholds, decreasing the effect size for each variable and making it harder to reach the level of statistical significance. This explanation is supported by the fact that in the same regression using age-adjusted thresholds (Section “Model Comparisons” in Appendix), age is still statistically significant (p = 0.00026). An alternate explanation is that adding additional variables to the model increases variability overall and makes it less likely that each variable will reach statistical significance without a corresponding increase in the number of subjects. Another explanation that we cannot refute at this time is that vestibular thresholds might serve as a biomarker for physiologic/biologic age (57, 58).
We also performed a regression that predicted the chance of failing condition 4 based on age (p = 0.42), roll tilt 0.2 Hz threshold (p = 0.017) and an interaction term (p = 0.054). Since the interaction term did not strictly reach the criteria for statistical significance, we did not present it in the Results. We note that this may have occurred because our population was not extremely large, and because there were not many subjects that were either older with low thresholds, or younger with high thresholds. We speculate that in a study with a larger population, each of these terms might be statistically significant.
We found no evidence that vestibular biases were correlated with the chance of failing condition 4. We can only speculate about the reasons. Perceptual biases may be unrelated to motor errors. The brain may be better at compensating for biases in balance vs. in perception. Alternatively, only large biases may have an effect on balance, and our subjects may not have exhibited large enough biases to demonstrate a relationship. Finally, the measured biases may reflect cognitive processes rather than having a sensory origin (64).
Unique Aspects of Yaw Rotation
We noted a few ways in which yaw rotation threshold responses differ from those of other motions, in addition to those that have been previously reported; we summarize them here. First, while our previous study presented an age regression that fit well across all five threshold measures, when fits were done to individual threshold measures, only yaw rotation did not have a statistically significant (p = 0.087) age effect (1); qualitatively, it also seems that the age cutoff may be higher for yaw rotation. Other studies also reported the lack of statistically significant changes in yaw thresholds with age (65–67). Second, yaw rotation thresholds have the weakest correlation with other threshold measures; specifically, the lowest correlation coefficients were between yaw thresholds and the other four thresholds. Third, velocity storage has different properties for yaw rotation vs. other motions, including less dependence on otolith cues for yaw rotation even with the subject supine (68), and a longer time constant for yaw rotations about an earth-vertical axis (69). Finally, the yaw plane plays a unique role during navigation (70). Different and/or more extensive processing for yaw rotation may result in reduced age effects.
Brief Summary
We investigated correlations between thresholds and multiple variable correlations between thresholds and the chance of failing condition 4 of the Modified Romberg balance test, which increases vestibular reliance by having subjects stand on foam with eyes closed. We found that the chance of failing condition 4 depends significantly on both roll tilt 0.2 Hz thresholds (p = 0.006) and age (p = 0.013). We also found moderate correlations (0.30–0.51) among the five vestibular thresholds, even after using our published aging regression to remove most aging effects.
Ethics Statement
The study was approved by the MEEI Human Studies Committee and written informed consent was obtained from all subjects as dictated by the Declaration of Helsinki.
Author Contributions
MB, TC, and DM designed the study and collected the data. FK, WW, DM, and TC performed statistical analyses and interpretation. FK drafted the manuscript and WW, DM, TC, and MB provided critical feedback on the manuscript.
Conflict of Interest Statement
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Acknowledgments
This research was supported by NIH/NIDCD R01-DC01458, R01-DC014924 and R03-DC013635. The authors also thank Raquel Galvan-Garza, Wangsong Gong, Bob Grimes, Csilla Haburcakova, Kristen Kirk, Rick Lewis, Koeun Lim, and Yongwoo Yi.
Footnote
- ^According to the 2010 national vital statistics report (21), the following are the leading causes of death in United States: (1) heart disease (598,000), (2) cancer (575,000), (3) chronic respiratory diseases (138,000), (4) stroke (129,000), (5) accidents (121,000), (6) Alzheimer’s (83,000), (7) diabetes (69,000), (8) kidney diseases (50,000), (9) influenza and pneumonia (50,000), and (10) suicide (38,000). All death rates are rounded to nearest thousand.
References
1. Bermúdez Rey M, Clark T, Wang W, Leeder T, Bian Y, Merfeld D. Vestibular perceptual thresholds increase above the age of 40. Front Neurol (2016) 7(162):162. doi:10.3389/fneur.2016.00162
2. CDC. Web-Based Injury Statistics Query and Reporting System (WISQARS). Atlanta, GA: National Center for Injury Prevention and Control (2014).
3. Nashner LM, Black FO, Wall C III. Adaptation to altered support and visual conditions during stance: patients with vestibular deficits. J Neurosci (1982) 2(5):536–44.
4. Horak F, Nashner L, Diener H. Postural strategies associated with somatosensory and vestibular loss. Exp Brain Res (1990) 82(1):167–77. doi:10.1007/BF00230848
5. Peterka RJ, Benolken MS. Role of somatosensory and vestibular cues in attenuating visually induced human postural sway. Exp Brain Res (1995) 105(1):101–10. doi:10.1007/BF00242186
6. Peterka RJ. Sensorimotor integration in human postural control. J Neurophysiol (2002) 88(3):1097–118. doi:10.1152/jn.00605.2001
7. Horak FB. Postural orientation and equilibrium: what do we need to know about neural control of balance to prevent falls? Age Ageing (2006) 35(Suppl 2):ii7–11. doi:10.1093/ageing/afl077
8. Goodworth AD, Peterka RJ. Influence of bilateral vestibular loss on spinal stabilization in humans. J Neurophysiol (2010) 103(4):1978–87. doi:10.1152/jn.01064.2009
9. van der Kooij H, Peterka RJ. Non-linear stimulus-response behavior of the human stance control system is predicted by optimization of a system with sensory and motor noise. J Comput Neurosci (2011) 30(3):759–78. doi:10.1007/s10827-010-0291-y
10. Shumway-Cook A, Woollacott MH. Motor Control Translating Research into Clinical Practice. Phiadelphia: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins (2012).
11. Agrawal Y, Carey JP, Della Santina CC, Schubert MC, Minor LB. Disorders of balance and vestibular function in US adults: data from the National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey, 2001-2004. Arch Intern Med (2009) 169(10):938–44. doi:10.1001/archinternmed.2009.66
12. Shumway-Cook A, Horak FB. Assessing the influence of sensory interaction on balance: suggestion from the field. Phys Ther (1986) 66(10):1548–50. doi:10.1093/ptj/66.10.1548
13. Horak FB. Clinical measurement of postural control in adults. Phys Ther (1987) 67(12):1881–5. doi:10.1093/ptj/67.12.1881
14. CDC. Balance Procedures Manual. NHANES (2003). Available from: https://www.cdc.gov/nchs/data/nhanes/ba.pdf
15. Pothula VB, Chew F, Lesser TH, Sharma AK. Falls and vestibular impairment. Clin Otolaryngol Allied Sci (2004) 29(2):179–82. doi:10.1111/j.0307-7772.2004.00785.x
16. Valko Y, Lewis RF, Priesol AJ, Merfeld DM. Vestibular labyrinth contributions to human whole-body motion discrimination. J Neurosci (2012) 32(39):13537–42. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2157-12.2012
17. Allum J, Adkin A, Carpenter M, Held-Ziolkowska M, Honegger F, Pierchala K. Trunk sway measures of postural stability during clinical balance tests: effects of a unilateral vestibular deficit. Gait Posture (2001) 14(3):227–37. doi:10.1016/S0966-6362(01)00132-1
18. Mergner T, Schweigart G, Maurer C, Blumle A. Human postural responses to motion of real and virtual visual environments under different support base conditions. Exp Brain Res (2005) 167(4):535–56. doi:10.1007/s00221-005-0065-3
19. Stapley PJ, Ting LH, Kuifu C, Everaert DG, Macpherson JM. Bilateral vestibular loss leads to active destabilization of balance during voluntary head turns in the standing cat. J Neurophysiol (2006) 95(6):3783–97. doi:10.1152/jn.00034.2006
20. Macpherson JM, Everaert DG, Stapley PJ, Ting LH. Bilateral vestibular loss in cats leads to active destabilization of balance during pitch and roll rotations of the support surface. J Neurophysiol (2007) 97(6):4357–67. doi:10.1152/jn.01338.2006
22. Han BI, Song HS, Kim JS. Vestibular rehabilitation therapy: review of indications, mechanisms, and key exercises. J Clin Neurol (2011) 7(4):184–96. doi:10.3988/jcn.2011.7.4.184
23. Hall CD, Herdman SJ, Whitney SL, Cass SP, Clendaniel RA, Fife TD, et al. Vestibular rehabilitation for peripheral vestibular hypofunction: an evidence-based clinical practice guideline: from the American physical therapy association neurology section. J Neurol Phys Ther (2016) 40(2):124–55. doi:10.1097/NPT.0000000000000120
24. Sienko KH, Vichare VV, Balkwill MD, Wall C III. Assessment of vibrotactile feedback on postural stability during pseudorandom multidirectional platform motion. IEEE Trans Biomed Eng (2010) 57(4):944–52. doi:10.1109/TBME.2009.2036833
25. Chiang B, Fridman GY, Dai C, Rahman MA, Della Santina CC. Design and performance of a multichannel vestibular prosthesis that restores semicircular canal sensation in rhesus monkey. IEEE Trans Neural Syst Rehabil Eng (2011) 19(5):588–98. doi:10.1109/TNSRE.2011.2164937
26. Merfeld DM, Lewis RF. Replacing semicircular canal function with a vestibular implant. Curr Opin Otolaryngol Head Neck Surg (2012) 20(5):386–92. doi:10.1097/MOO.0b013e328357630f
27. Van De Berg R, Guinand N, Guyot J-P, Kingma H, Stokroos RJ. The modified ampullar approach for vestibular implant surgery: feasibility and its first application in a human with a long-term vestibular loss. Front Neurol (2012) 3:18. doi:10.3389/fneur.2012.00018
28. Golub JS, Ling L, Nie K, Nowack A, Shepherd SJ, Bierer SM, et al. Prosthetic implantation of the human vestibular system. Otol Neurotol (2014) 35(1):136. doi:10.1097/MAO.0000000000000003
29. Priplata AA, Patritti BL, Niemi JB, Hughes R, Gravelle DC, Lipsitz LA, et al. Noise-enhanced balance control in patients with diabetes and patients with stroke. Ann Neurol (2006) 59(1):4–12. doi:10.1002/ana.20670
30. Scinicariello AP, Inglis JT, Collins JJ. The effects of stochastic monopolar galvanic vestibular stimulation on human postural sway. J Vestib Res (2002) 12(2–3):77–85.
31. Pal S, Rosengren SM, Colebatch JG. Stochastic galvanic vestibular stimulation produces a small reduction in sway in Parkinson’s disease. J Vestib Res (2009) 19(3–4):137–42. doi:10.3233/VES-2009-0360
32. Mulavara AP, Fiedler MJ, Kofman IS, Wood SJ, Serrador JM, Peters B, et al. Improving balance function using vestibular stochastic resonance: optimizing stimulus characteristics. Exp Brain Res (2011) 210(2):303–12. doi:10.1007/s00221-011-2633-z
33. Goel R, Kofman I, Jeevarajan J, De Dios Y, Cohen HS, Bloomberg JJ, et al. Using low levels of stochastic vestibular stimulation to improve balance function. PLoS One (2015) 10(8):e0136335. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0136335
34. Goodworth AD, Kunsman M, DePietro V, LaPenta G, Miles K, Murphy J. Characterization of how a walking boot affects balance. J Prosthet Orthot (2014) 26(1):54–60. doi:10.1097/JPO.0000000000000014
35. Grabherr L, Nicoucar K, Mast FW, Merfeld DM. Vestibular thresholds for yaw rotation about an earth-vertical axis as a function of frequency. Exp Brain Res (2008) 186(4):677–81. doi:10.1007/s00221-008-1350-8
36. Chaudhuri SE, Karmali F, Merfeld DM. Whole body motion-detection tasks can yield much lower thresholds than direction-recognition tasks: implications for the role of vibration. J Neurophysiol (2013) 110(12):2764–72. doi:10.1152/jn.00091.2013
37. Karmali F, Chaudhuri SE, Yi Y, Merfeld DM. Determining thresholds using adaptive procedures and psychometric fits: evaluating efficiency using theory, simulations, and human experiments. Exp Brain Res (2016) 234(3):773–89. doi:10.1007/s00221-015-4501-8
38. Leek MR. Adaptive procedures in psychophysical research. Percept Psychophys (2001) 63(8):1279–92. doi:10.3758/BF03194543
39. Taylor MM. PEST: efficient estimates on probability functions. J Acoust Soc Am (1967) 41:782–7. doi:10.1121/1.1910407
40. Lim K, Karmali F, Nicoucar K, Merfeld DM. Perceptual precision of passive body tilt is consistent with statistically optimal cue integration. J Neurophysiol (2017) 117(5):2037–52. doi:10.1152/jn.00073.2016
42. Chaudhuri SE, Merfeld DM. Signal detection theory and vestibular perception: III. Estimating unbiased fit parameters for psychometric functions. Exp Brain Res (2013) 225(1):133–46. doi:10.1007/s00221-012-3354-7
43. Benson AJ, Spencer MB, Stott JR. Thresholds for the detection of the direction of whole-body, linear movement in the horizontal plane. Aviat Space Environ Med (1986) 57(11):1088–96.
44. Benson AJ, Hutt EC, Brown SF. Thresholds for the perception of whole body angular movement about a vertical axis. Aviat Space Environ Med (1989) 60(3):205–13.
45. Mathworks. Create Generalized Linear Regression Model by Stepwise Regression. Mathworks (2017). Available from: https://www.mathworks.com/help/stats/stepwiseglm.html
46. Merfeld DM. Modeling the vestibule-ocular reflex of the squirrel monkey during eccentric rotation and roll tilt. Exp Brain Res (1995) 106(1):123–34. doi:10.1007/BF00241361
47. Glasauer S, Merfeld DM. Modeling three-dimensional responses during complex motion stimulation. In: Fetter M, Haslwanter T, Misslisch H, Tweed D, editors. Three-Dimensional Kinematics of Eye, Head and Limb Movements. Amsterdam: Harwood Acad (1997) p. 387–98.
48. Angelaki DE, McHenry MQ, Dickman JD, Newlands SD, Hess BJM. Computation of inertial motion: neural strategies to resolve ambiguous otolith information. J Neurosci (1999) 19(1):316–27.
49. Mergner T, Glasauer S. A simple model of vestibular canal-otolith signal fusion. Ann N Y Acad Sci (1999) 871(1):430–4. doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.1999.tb09211.x
50. Merfeld DM, Zupan LH. Neural processing of gravitoinertial cues in humans. III. Modeling tilt and translation responses. J Neurophysiol (2002) 87(2):819–33. doi:10.1152/jn.00485.2001
51. Zupan LH, Merfeld DM, Darlot C. Using sensory weighting to model the influence of canal, otolith and visual cues on spatial orientation and eye movements. Biol Cybern (2002) 86(3):209–30. doi:10.1007/s00422-001-0290-1
52. Green AM, Angelaki DE. Resolution of sensory ambiguities for gaze stabilization requires a second neural integrator. J Neurosci (2003) 23(28):9265–75.
53. Green AM, Angelaki DE. An integrative neural network for detecting inertial motion and head orientation. J Neurophysiol (2004) 92(2):905–25. doi:10.1152/jn.01234.2003
54. Raphan T, Matsuo V, Cohen B. Velocity storage in the vestibulo-ocular reflex arc (VOR). Exp Brain Res (1979) 35(2):229–48. doi:10.1007/BF00236613
55. Green AM, Shaikh AG, Angelaki DE. Sensory vestibular contributions to constructing internal models of self-motion. J Neural Eng (2005) 2(3):S164–79. doi:10.1088/1741-2560/2/3/S02
56. Angelaki DE, Cullen KE. Vestibular system: the many facets of a multimodal sense. Annu Rev Neurosci (2008) 31:125–50. doi:10.1146/annurev.neuro.31.060407.125555
57. Krishnamurthy J, Torrice C, Ramsey MR, Kovalev GI, Al-Regaiey K, Su L, et al. Ink4a/Arf expression is a biomarker of aging. J Clin Invest (2004) 114(9):1299–307. doi:10.1172/JCI22475
58. Ressler S, Bartkova J, Niederegger H, Bartek J, Scharffetter-Kochanek K, Jansen-Durr P, et al. p16INK4A is a robust in vivo biomarker of cellular aging in human skin. Aging Cell (2006) 5(5):379–89. doi:10.1111/j.1474-9726.2006.00231.x
59. Goodworth AD, Peterka RJ. Influence of stance width on frontal plane postural dynamics and coordination in human balance control. J Neurophysiol (2010) 104(2):1103–18. doi:10.1152/jn.00916.2009
60. Goodworth AD, Peterka RJ. Sensorimotor integration for multisegmental frontal plane balance control in humans. J Neurophysiol (2012) 107(1):12–28. doi:10.1152/jn.00670.2010
61. Moreland JD, Richardson JA, Goldsmith CH, Clase CM. Muscle weakness and falls in older adults: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J Am Geriatr Soc (2004) 52(7):1121–9. doi:10.1111/j.1532-5415.2004.52310.x
62. Fukagawa NK, Wolfson L, Judge J, Whipple R, King M. Strength is a major factor in balance, gait, and the occurrence of falls. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci (1995) 50(Special_Issue):64–7. doi:10.1093/gerona/50A.Special_Issue.64
63. Koo J-W, Chang MY, Woo S-Y, Kim S, Cho Y-S. Prevalence of vestibular dysfunction and associated factors in South Korea. BMJ Open (2015) 5(10):e008224. doi:10.1136/bmjopen-2015-008224
64. Garcia-Perez MA, Alcala-Quintana R. Shifts of the psychometric function: distinguishing bias from perceptual effects. Q J Exp Psychol (Hove). (2013) 66: 319–37. doi:10.1080/17470218.2012.708761
65. Seemungal B, Gunaratne I, Fleming I, Gresty M, Bronstein A. Perceptual and nystagmic thresholds of vestibular function in yaw. J Vestib Res (2004) 14(6):461–6.
66. Roditi RE, Crane BT. Directional asymmetries and age effects in human self-motion perception. J Assoc Res Otolaryngol (2012) 13(3):381–401. doi:10.1007/s10162-012-0318-3
67. Chang NY, Hiss MM, Sanders MC, Olomu OU, MacNeilage PR, Uchanski RM, et al. Vestibular perception and the vestibulo-ocular reflex in young and older adults. Ear Hear (2014) 35(5):565–70. doi:10.1097/AUD.0000000000000052
68. Bockisch CJ, Straumann D, Haslwanter T. Human 3-D aVOR with and without otolith stimulation. Exp Brain Res (2005) 161(3):358–67. doi:10.1007/s00221-004-2080-1
69. Bertolini G, Ramat S. Velocity storage in the human vertical rotational vestibulo-ocular reflex. Exp Brain Res (2011) 209(1):51–63. doi:10.1007/s00221-010-2518-6
70. Taube JS. The head direction signal: origins and sensory-motor integration. Annu Rev Neurosci (2007) 30:181–207. doi:10.1146/annurev.neuro.29.051605.112854
Appendix
A. Model Comparisons
We investigated a number of logistic regression models to find the most appropriate descriptor of our results (Table A1). Models are listed in ascending order of the BIC, with lower values indicating better models. This comparison shows that there are two categories of models that best describe the data, with none of the five models clearly better than the others. The first is age and roll 0.2 Hz, with or without age adjustment. The second is the first PCA component, with or without the age term included, and with or without age adjustment.
Keywords: vestibular, balance, perception, thresholds, aging, multivariate
Citation: Karmali F, Bermúdez Rey MC, Clark TK, Wang W and Merfeld DM (2017) Multivariate Analyses of Balance Test Performance, Vestibular Thresholds, and Age. Front. Neurol. 8:578. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2017.00578
Received: 17 July 2017; Accepted: 13 October 2017;
Published: 08 November 2017
Edited by:
Bernard Cohen, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, United StatesCopyright: © 2017 Karmali, Bermúdez Rey, Clark, Wang and Merfeld. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Faisal Karmali, ZmFpc2FsX2thcm1hbGkmI3gwMDA0MDt5YWhvby5jb20=