- PsyQ Psycho-Medical Programs, Expertise Center Adult ADHD, The Hague, Netherlands
Many adult outpatients with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) report an oversensitivity to light. We explored the link between ADHD and photophobia in an online survey (N = 494). Self-reported photophobia was prevalent in 69% of respondents with, and in 28% of respondents without, ADHD (symptoms). The ADHD (symptoms) group wore sunglasses longer during daytime in all seasons. Photophobia may be related to the functioning of the eyes, which mediate dopamine and melatonin production systems in the eye. In the brain, dopamine and melatonin are involved in both ADHD and circadian rhythm disturbances. Possibly, the regulation of the dopamine and melatonin systems in the eyes and in the brain are related. Despite the study’s limitations, the results are encouraging for further study on the pathophysiology of ADHD, eye functioning, and circadian rhythm disturbances.
Introduction
From clinical experience, we learned that a substantial number of adult patients with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) wear sunglasses throughout the year, even on overcast days. Many of these patients report a sensitivity or even oversensitivity of their eyes to bright light. Besides this clinical observation of photophobia, there are a number of studies indicating a link between ADHD and other optical dysfunctions. In children with ADHD, 76% have reduced visual acuity, caused by more strabismus (cross-eyedness), subnormal stereoacuity (depth detection), convergence insufficiency, and smaller optic disks (1). Another study found a prevalence of 63% myopia or hyperopia, and in total 83% refractive errors in children with ADHD (2). In comparison, in a study among Dutch schoolchildren, the prevalence of myopia and hyperopia was 36% (3). Moreover, young adults with ADHD more often have problems with depth perception, peripheral vision, visual search, visual processing speed, and difficulties with color perception, especially in the blue spectrum, as compared to matched controls (4). Many of these eye dysfunctions are the result of, or affect, the retinal function.
From chronobiology studies, we know that there is a link between retinal function and the circadian rhythm. The intensity of the light reaching the intrinsic photosensitive retinal ganglion cells (ipRGC) in the retina is signaled directly to the suprachiasmatic nuclei (SCN) of the brain, which is then translated into information about the time of the day (5, 6). The SCN in turn orchestrates the internal biological clocks and is accountable for one’s chronotype (i.e., being a “morning” or an “evening” person). Among adults with ADHD, the majority has an (extremely) late chronotype and a delayed sleep phase (7, 8). A delayed sleep phase is, among others, marked by a delayed production of the endogenous “sleep hormone” melatonin in the evening (9). The suppression of melatonin in the morning is initiated by light reaching the retina of the eye (10). Melatonin may be suppressed later or less by the use of sunglasses during the day (11). This may cause the production of melatonin in the evening to be delayed, which in turn further delays the sleep/wake cycle. This is important because late sleep is associated with a short-sleep duration that on a chronic basis has a negative impact on health, with short sleep being associated with increased risks of chronic diseases such as obesity, diabetes, cardiovascular diseases, and cancer (12, 13). It is therefore important to study the potential role of the retinal functioning and its effect on chronotype, sleep, and ADHD symptoms.
The combination of our clinical experience and the findings in the available literature suggest that there may be an increased prevalence of visual problems and photophobia in adults with ADHD. In order to study a possible link between photophobia and ADHD, we initiated a short-online survey in a large group of adults with and without ADHD (symptoms). We also explored if a delayed sleep phase (late chronotype) was related to photophobia. Results from this study can be used as a first exploration of the possible link between ADHD symptoms, chronotype, and eye functioning in general, in adults with and without ADHD (symptoms).
Materials and Methods
Participants
We invited participants with and without ADHD (symptoms) from the Dutch general population to fill in our online survey, via websites of ADHD patient organizations and our social media accounts (Facebook© and Twitter©). Also, therapists from our outpatient Adult ADHD clinic invited their ADHD patients to participate in our study by emailing them the link to the survey.
Material
The study was conducted in July 2013 and consisted of a very short (<5 min) self-composed and non-validated online survey in Dutch, using software by www.thesistools.nl (survey no longer available online). The survey consisted of the complete list of the following questions: a multiple choice question on having a diagnosed ADHD (I have a diagnosis of ADHD; I do not have a diagnosis but I do have ADHD symptoms; I do not have ADHD); an item on having photophobia apart from any migraine episodes (“My eyes are sensitive to light,” yes or no); the effect of light on the eyes if one had photophobia (unpleasant sensation in the eyes/headache or pain in the eyes/dryness of the eyes; each one answered with yes or no); if one had photophobia, the number of years one has had it; if one had photophobia, which measurements one takes (no measurements/using eye drops/wearing sunglasses; each one answered with yes or no; and a text field for open answers); the average number of hours per day in every season one wears sunglasses; and one’s chronotype (“Are you a morning or an evening person?”) that was scored on a five-point Likert scale (1 = definitely a morning type to 5 = definitely an evening type). We collected the following background information: age; gender; type; and dose of medication use (the participants typed in which type and dose of medication they used, or stated that they did not use any medication); and wearing glasses or contacts (yes or no). We also collected information on possible confounders for photophobia that are known from the literature: having an eye disease or disorder (yes or no); comorbidity (migraine/diabetes/head injury/seasonal affective disorder/bipolar depression/chronic fatigue/agoraphobia; each one answered with yes or no); and having had chemotherapy (yes or no) (14). The survey was not considered burdensome and the participants were assigned an anonymous consecutive number in the database. We followed guidelines of the Dutch Personal Data Protection Act. Our study does not fall under the Dutch Medical Research Involving Human Subjects Act and therefore did not need to undergo medical ethical review. Participants gave written informed consent by filling in the online survey.
Analysis
We used Chi-square analysis to compare categorical variables between the groups with and without ADHD (symptoms). In order to determine if any of the main and interaction factors significantly added to the odds of photophobia, we entered the relevant explanatory variables into a single binary logistic regression with photophobia as the dependent variable. The model included the following explanatory variables (reference categories between brackets): female gender (male); age >40 years (≤40 years); ADHD group (controls); extreme evening chronotype (other chronotype); eye disease (no eye disease); contacts or glasses (no contacts or glasses); reporting migraine (no migraine); reporting SAD (no SAD); reporting chronic fatigue (no chronic fatigue); interaction effect of gender and age; interaction effect of ADHD and SAD; interaction effect of extreme evening chronotype and SAD. Because the SPSS Version 20.0 for Windows (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, USA) was used for all statistical analyses, and α = 0.05 was used to indicate statistical significance.
Results
In total, 543 participants filled out the surveys, of which 49 were excluded from analysis due to incomplete surveys (i.e., because they prematurely terminated the survey session). This resulted in a total of N = 494 participants eligible for analysis, see Table 1. Of the participants, 30% reported to have a diagnosis of ADHD, 17% reported ADHD symptoms, and 53% had no ADHD. In Table 1, we merged the ADHD and the ADHD symptoms groups (termed ADHD group) because of their similarities concerning photophobia prevalences, in order to create a larger group and thus increase the power of the statistical tests. There were more males in the ADHD group than in the control group (27 vs. 19%, χ2 = 3.9, df = 1, p = 0.048). The control group was on average slightly older, but not significantly (36.7 vs. 38.4 years, F = 2.8, p = 0.093). The groups did differed in their prevalence of use of glasses or contact lenses (58 vs. 49%, χ2 = 4.0, df = 1, p = 0.045). Some possible confounders that were asked in the survey were very low prevalent (diabetes, chemotherapy, serious head injury, bipolar disorder, and/or agoraphobia), thus, these are reported as “Other confounding factors” in Table 1.
Self-reported photophobia was prevalent in 69% of participants in the ADHD group, vs. 28% in the control group (χ2 = 80.4; df = 1; p < 0.001), see Table 1. There was a tendency toward higher prevalence of extreme evening chronotype in the ADHD group (26 vs. 19%, χ2 = 3.6; df = 1; p = 0.057). Photophobia was a life-long condition in 73% of the photophobia cases, and this was not significantly different between ADHD and non-ADHD groups. Bright light caused an unpleasant sensation in the eyes in 73% of the cases, headache or pain in the eyes in 46% of cases, and dryness of the eyes in 25% of cases. The ADHD group reported to wear sunglasses 2.3 h in spring (vs. 1.2 h in non-ADHD; p < 0.001); 4.0 h in summer (vs. 3.1 h; p = 0.001), 1.4 h in autumn (vs. 0.7 h; p < 0.001), and 1.0 h in winter (vs. 0.4 h; p < 0.001), see Table 1.
We have entered all relevant explanatory variables into a single binary logistic regression model with photosensitivity as the dependent variable, see Table 2. The participants reporting any of the confounding factors for photophobia, which were diabetes, chemotherapy, serious head injury, bipolar disorder, and/or agoraphobia, were excluded from the binary logistic regression, resulting in N = 405 included cases. Migraine was entered in the regression as a separate covariate because of its substantial prevalence. The logistic regression revealed that the main factors for having ADHD (symptoms) (OR 6.1; p < 0.001), and eye disease (OR 6.2; p = 0.002) were related to a higher odds of photophobia. There was a tendency for higher odds of photophobia for the use of contact lenses or glasses (OR 1.6; p = 0.052), and having (seasonal) affective disorder (OR = 2.7, p = 0.069). Contrary to our expectation, extreme evening chronotype had a tendency toward a lower odds of photophobia (OR 0.6; p = 0.096), and migraine was not related to higher odds of having photophobia (apart from any migraine episodes; OR 1.1; p = 0.870). Other factors were also not significantly related to photophobia, and we did not find any interaction effects.
Discussion
This preliminary study shows a strong relationship between photophobia and ADHD (symptoms). Also, the ADHD group more frequently reported the use of contacts and glasses, and wearing sunglasses longer during the day in every season than the non-ADHD group. The photosensitivity was not significantly related to seasonal affective disorder (SAD) in our study; however, there was a tendency. A recent study by Roecklein et al. showed that patients with SAD have a weaker pupil response to blue vs. red light as compared to healthy controls, allowing more light to reach the retina, which may be experienced as oversensitivity to light (15).
These preliminary results should be interpreted in the light of the limitations of the study. The main limitation of our study is that we used an online survey, which has some methodological disadvantages with regards to selection of participants, in which the control group may not reflect the general population and the patient group may not reflect the general ADHD population, and also regarding unknown response quality and unverifiable self-report of diseases. Also, the survey may have lacked clarity in the formulation of photophobia, other than being oversensitive to light in general. We have hypothesized that the oversensitivity to light in ADHD may have been related to a behavioral adaptation such as avoiding daylight and wearing sunglasses, which in turn may prevent entrainment of the biological clock to daylight, leading to a further delay of the already delayed circadian rhythm. In the Van Veen study, there was a prevalence of 78% delayed sleep phase in a group of adults with ADHD (7).
Contrary to our hypothesis, however, in the current study the extreme evening chronotype had a tendency toward a lower odds of photophobia. We speculate that the chronotype question may not have been specific enough: we have asked if one is a morning or an evening type, without any further explanation on the inherited circadian preference for the morning or evening. This may have biased our chronotype results. Also, we did not ask the time of the day the participants wear sunglasses. Wearing sunglasses during the afternoon and early evening prevents people from delaying their circadian rhythm. Further study investigating the use of sunglasses should take the time of the day into account. Lastly, in our survey the participants were not able to specify if they used either glasses or contact lenses, so that we were not able to analyze if there was a relationship between wearing contact lenses, which can cause corneal surface damage, and photophobia, which may be the result of this damage. Similarly, ocular damage as a result of long-term use of ophthalmic solutions with preservatives and its relationship with photophobia cannot be analyzed because we did not consider this possible relationship prior to data sampling. These possible relationships should be examined in future studies.
In all, we found a strong relationship between ADHD and photophobia, but our results deserve a more thorough investigation in a representative sample of patients and healthy controls. It is yet unclear what causes the photophobia in adults with ADHD. There may be a role for the intrinsically photosensitive retinal ganglion cells (ipRGCs), which are involved in the pupillary response to light and the sensitivity of the eye to light at the one hand, and in the synthesis of dopamine and melatonin in the retina at the other hand (16, 17). Possibly, the regulation of the dopamine and melatonin systems in the eyes and in the brain are related. The dopamine system is dysregulated in ADHD, and the melatonin onset is delayed in the majority of adults with ADHD (7, 18, 19). The functioning of the dopamine and melatonin receptors in the retina is possibly impaired as well and may be related to the pathophysiology of ADHD and circadian disturbances. The role of the ipRGCs is therefore an interesting candidate for further research in ADHD. Pupillometry may give us more insight into the effect of light on the eyes, the pupillary reflex, and the relatedness of ipRGC functioning, oversensitivity to light, and the pathophysiology of ADHD.
Conflict of Interest Statement
The Review Editor, Eus J. W. Van Someren declares that, despite having collaborated with authors Denise Bijlenga and J. J. Sandra Kooij, the review process was handled objectively and no conflict of interest exists. Dr. J. J. Sandra Kooij reports having received lecture fees from Janssen B. V. and Lilly B. V. and unrestricted research grants from Janssen B. V. and Shire B. V.; all of these for other studies. Dr. Denise Bijlenga reports no financial interests or potential conflicts of interest.
References
1. Grönlund MA, Aring E, Landgren M, Hellström A. Visual function and ocular features in children and adolescents with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, with and without treatment with stimulants. Eye (2007) 21:494–502. doi: 10.1038/sj.eye.6702240
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
2. Mezer E, Wygnanski-Jaffe T. Do children and adolescents with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder have ocular abnormalities? Eur J Ophthalmol (2012) 22:931–5. doi:10.5301/ejo.5000145
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
3. Hendricks TJ, de Brabander J, Vankan-Hendricks MH, van der Horst FG, Hendrikse F, Knottnerus JA. Prevalence of habitual refractive errors and anisometropia among Dutch schoolchildren and hospital employees. Acta Ophthalmol (2009) 87:538–43. doi:10.1111/j.1755-3768.2008.01251.x
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
4. Kim S, Chen S, Tannock R. Visual function and color vision in adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J Optom (2014) 7:22–36. doi:10.1016/j.optom.2013.07.001
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
5. Roenneberg T, Kuehnle T, Juda M, Kantermann T, Allebrandt K, Gordijn M, et al. Epidemiology of the human circadian clock. Sleep Med Rev (2007) 11:429–38. doi:10.1016/j.smrv.2007.07.005
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
6. Allebrandt KV, Teder-Laving M, Kantermann T, Peters A, Campbell H, Rudan I, et al. Chronotype and sleep duration: the influence of season of assessment. Chronobiol Int (2014) 31:731–40. doi:10.3109/07420528.2014.901347
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
7. van Veen MM, Kooij JJ, Boonstra AM, Gordijn MC, van Someren EJ. Delayed circadian rhythm in adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and chronic sleep-onset insomnia. Biol Psychiatry (2010) 67:1091–6. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.12.032
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
8. Bijlenga D, van der Heijden KB, Breuk M, van Someren EJ, Lie ME, Boonstra AM, et al. Associations between sleep characteristics, seasonal depressive symptoms, lifestyle, and ADHD symptoms in adults. J Atten Disord (2013) 17:261–74. doi:10.1177/1087054711428965
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
9. Lewy AJ, Ahmed S, Jackson JM, Sack RL. Melatonin shifts human circadian rhythms according to a phase-response curve. Chronobiol Int (1992) 9:380–92. doi:10.3109/07420529209064550
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
10. Lewy AJ, Wehr TA, Goodwin FK, Newsome DA, Markey SP. Light suppresses melatonin secretion in humans. Science (1980) 210:1267–9. doi:10.1126/science.7434030
11. Mainster MA. Violet and blue light blocking intraocular lenses: photoprotection versus photoreception. Br J Ophthalmol (2006) 90:784–92. doi:10.1136/bjo.2005.086553
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
12. Blask DE. Melatonin, sleep disturbance and cancer risk. Sleep Med Rev (2009) 13:257–64. doi:10.1016/j.smrv.2008.07.007
13. Gangwisch JE. Epidemiological evidence for the links between sleep, circadian rhythms and metabolism. Obes Rev (2009) 10(Suppl 2):37–45. doi:10.1111/j.1467-789X.2009.00663.x
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
14. Digre KB, Brennan KC. Shedding light on photophobia. J Neuroophthalmol (2012) 32:68–81. doi:10.1097/WNO.0b013e3182474548
15. Roecklein KA, Wong PM, Miller MA, Donofry SD, Kamarck ML, Brainard GC. Melanopsin, photosensitive ganglion cells, and seasonal affective disorder. Neurosci Biobehav Rev (2013) 37:229–39. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.12.009
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
16. Benarroch EE. The melanopsin system: phototransduction, projections, functions, and clinical implications. Neurology (2011) 76:1422–7. doi:10.1212/WNL.0b013e31821671a5
17. de Lima LH, dos Santos KP, de Lauro Castrucci AM. Clock genes, melanopsins, melatonin, and dopamine key enzymes and their modulation by light and glutamate in chicken embryonic retinal cells. Chronobiol Int (2011) 28:89–100. doi:10.3109/07420528.2010.540685
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
18. Faraone SV, Biederman J. Neurobiology of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Biol Psychiatry (1998) 44:951–8. doi:10.1016/S0006-3223(98)00240-6
19. Bijlenga D, Van Someren EJW, Gruber R, Bron TI, Kruithof IF, Spanbroek ECA, et al. Body temperature, activity and melatonin profiles in adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and delayed sleep: a case-control study. J Sleep Res (2013) 22:607–16. doi:10.1111/jsr.12075
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Keywords: adult ADHD, photophobia, photosensitivity, chronotype, sleep, circadian rhythm
Citation: Kooij JJS and Bijlenga D (2014) High prevalence of self-reported photophobia in adult ADHD. Front. Neurol. 5:256. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2014.00256
Received: 10 July 2014; Accepted: 20 November 2014;
Published online: 10 December 2014.
Edited by:
Gregory M. Brown, University of Toronto, CanadaReviewed by:
Eus J. W. Van Someren, Netherlands Institute for Neuroscience, NetherlandsDavid L. Streiner, McMaster University and University of Toronto, Canada
Copyright: © 2014 Kooij and Bijlenga. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Denise Bijlenga, PsyQ Expertise Center Adult ADHD, Carel Reinierszkade 197, 2593 HR The Hague, Netherlands e-mail: d.bijlenga@psyq.nl