Skip to main content

OPINION article

Front. Neurol., 11 October 2013
Sec. Movement Disorders
This article is part of the Research Topic Cognition and Sleep in Parkinson’s disease (PD) View all 6 articles

Sleep microstructure and memory function

  • Sleep Disorders Center, Università Vita-Salute San Raffaele, Milan, Italy

Non-REM Sleep, REM Sleep, and Memory

Several studies investigated the effect of sleep on memory. In humans, arguably the first experimental description of a beneficial role of sleep for memory stabilization was provided in 1924 (1), indicating a protective benefit of sleep in preventing the normal decay-curve of forgetting that develops across time spent awake. Specific stages of sleep appear to be critical for memory consolidation. Memory is commonly divided into a declarative and a non-declarative memory system (2). Declarative memory is defined by memories accessible to conscious recollection, i.e., memories for events in a spatio-temporal context (episodic memory) and fact-based information (semantic memory). Procedural memory for skills is the type of non-declarative memory most thoroughly studied with regard to the effects of sleep. Sleep is characterized by the cyclic occurrence of REM and NREM sleep comprising SWS (sleep stages 3 and 4) and lighter sleep stages 1 and 2. Two hypotheses were proposed regarding sleep stages and memory consolidation (3). The dual process theory assumes that the specific sleep stages support consolidation of different types of memories. SWS supports declarative memory consolidation whereas REM sleep does so for procedural memories (4). The sequential hypothesis, on the other hand, proposes that sleep benefits memory optimally through the cyclic succession of both SWS and REM sleep. The original version of this hypothesis assumed that SWS functions to weaken non-adaptive memory traces whereas REM sleep re-stores the remaining traces (5). The dual process hypothesis received support mainly based on the early late sleep comparison, i.e., an approach comparing effects of retention intervals covering the first (SWS-rich) or the second (REM sleep-rich) half of nocturnal sleep. SWS-rich early sleep consistently found to support consolidation of hippocampus-dependent declarative memories, whereas REM sleep benefited non-declarative types of memory like priming, and memories for visuo-motor skills (6, 7). However, this dichotomy does not fit all results. Several non-declarative tasks, like visual texture discrimination, are also supported by SWS whereas REM sleep in some instances seems to benefit aspects of declarative memory (8).

More recently, Genzel and colleagues (9) tried to clarify the link between specific sleep stages and different types of memory consolidation by suppressing sleep stages. They deprived subjects once each of REM sleep and SWS, and once let them sleep undisturbed through the night. After each night, the authors tested declarative and procedural memory consolidation. Although REM sleep and SWS awakenings led to a significant reduction of the respective sleep stages, memory consolidation remained unaffected. According to the authors, there are two possible explanations for REM deprivation and SWS deprivation not influencing sleep-dependent consolidation of motor tasks: (1) the diminished amount of REM sleep in the REM deprivation condition was still sufficient for sleep-dependent memory consolidation or (2) the memory consolidation is dependent on stage 2 sleep. Another explanation of the results is that sleep-associated processes contributing to memory consolidation requires analyses of polysomnographic phenomena, like sleep spindles and slow-wave activity (SWA). This aspect is very important also for the implications for the cognitive decline by increasing age (10, 11). Other factors that change with age, such as hormones and neurotransmitters, or hypoxia related to sleep-disordered breathing (12, 13), may also play an important role.

Sleep Spindles and Memory

Sleep spindles – a hallmark of stage 2 – are characterized in humans by waxing and waning 11- to 15-Hz oscillations lasting 0.5–3 s. They are generated by the thalamus, which acts as a pacemaker (14) and result from reciprocal rhythmic interactions between reticular and thalamo-cortical cells. Several reports described an association between NREM sleep spindle events and memory, including hippocampal-dependent learning. These short synchronous bursts of activity coincide with hippocampal sharp waves and ripples (15, 16), possibly reflecting reactivation of learned memory representations (17). Moreover, spindle density was associated with next-day memory recall: Meier-Koll and colleagues (18) reported a similar increase in spindles following learning of a hippocampally dependent maze task, and Clemens and colleagues (19) identified a correlation between spindle density and overnight verbal memory retention. The spindle-related memory processing is supported by very recent data showing that the same cortical areas active during learning are also (re)activated during post-encoding sleep spindle events (20). Sleep spindles can be separated into two subtypes based on frequency (21): “slow” (11–13 Hz) and “fast” (13–15 Hz). Concerning the memory consolidation, recent neuroimaging data demonstrated that the occurrence of fast sleep spindles coincide with moments of increased functional connectivity between the hippocampus and areas of neocortex (22), supporting a postulated component of the hippocampal-neocortical model of sleep-dependent memory consolidation. Indeed, several studies described select associations between episodic memory and fast- but not slow-spindle activity (23, 24). It is known that healthy aging is characterized by a reduced numbers of sleep spindles (25), and several studies reported reduced sleep spindles density in patients with Parkinson’s disease (26, 27).

Cyclic Alternating Pattern

Increases in SWA was observed after learning a procedural rotation adaptation task and declarative word-pair learning (28). Increases in SWA after rotation adaptation learning were restricted to the motor cortical areas required for task acquisition. Inducing sleep slow oscillations (<1 Hz) by transcranial application of slowly oscillating potentials intensifies SWS and enhances consolidation of declarative memory (29): following learning of a word-pair list, transcranial direct current stimulation was applied over the prefrontal cortex of participants during early night SWS, inducting slow oscillation-like field potentials (in this case, at 0.75 Hz). Consequently, a greater benefit on overnight retention was observed for the set of information learned prior to sleep. Direct current stimulation not only increased the amount of slow oscillation activity, as well as sleep spindle frequency activity, during the simulation period (and for some time after), but also enhanced next-day word-pair retention. These findings were interpreted in the context of slow oscillations potentially triggering spindle-regulated plasticity in cortex. Slow EEG oscillations are the main components of the cyclic alternating pattern (CAP), that is expression of sleep microstructure. CAP is a periodic EEG activity during NREM sleep, characterized by sequences of abrupt frequency and amplitude variations (Phases A) followed by a return to background activity (Phases B). A CAP sequence is composed of a succession of CAP cycles. A CAP cycle is composed of a phase A and the following phase B. Phases A were subdivided into A1, A2, and A3 subtypes, based on the proportion of SWA and faster EEG (30). In subtype A1, EEG synchrony is the predominant activity and specimens include delta bursts, K-complex sequences, vertex sharp transients. In subtype A2, the EEG activity is a mixture of slow and fast rhythms with 20–50% of phase A occupied by EEG desynchrony. In subtype A3, the EEG activity is predominantly rapid low-voltage rhythms with >50% of phase A occupied by EEG desynchrony. Besides CAP, the other major EEG activity in the frequency range below 1 Hz is the so-called slow oscillation (31). This 0.5–0.9 Hz EEG rhythm, which characterizes states of reduced tonic arousal, was outlined during anesthesia and NREM sleep in animals and humans. The slow oscillation is generated in cortical neurons, and consists of phases of depolarization, characterized by intensive neural firing, followed by long-lasting hyperpolarization. The coalescence of slow rhythms is visible during NREM sleep, and slow waves converge into collectives resulting in the phase A1 subtypes of CAP. CAP A1 subtypes are almost exclusively composed by slow waves, map over the frontal regions of the scalp and correspond to periods in which the functional organization of the EEG network shows enhanced small-world network characteristics, a condition favorable for cognitive processing. These features make CAP A1 subtypes reliable candidates for a significant role in sleep-related cognitive processes. The first study supporting the idea that CAP slow components (A1) may be beneficial for cognitive processing was performed by Ferini-Strambi and colleagues (32), that evaluated a young individual with superior memory performance and found an increased % of CAP A1.

Since Huber and colleagues (33) found that a motor learning task performed during the day was followed by an increase in EEG SWA during the night and that SWA correlated with an improvement in subsequent performance, Ferri and colleagues (34) examined their original recordings and found that the night after the task, the CAP A1 subtypes were significantly increased in number and this increase correlated with improvements in task performance. Furthermore, in subjects with Asperger syndrome, significant correlations between verbal and non-verbal performance and CAP were found, with a general positive correlation with the A1 subtypes and a negative correlation with A2 and A3 subtypes (35). Moreover, dyslexic children have been reported to show a significant positive correlation between A1 subtypes and verbal and full-scale IQ, while CAP rate in NREM sleep stage 3 is positively correlated with verbal IQ (36). Thus, there is accumulating evidence implicating CAP as an important intermediate in cognitive processing. In another more recent publication, Aricò and colleagues (37) reported that CAP rate and A1 subtypes influence cognitive function in healthy subjects. The study illustrates the significant positive correlation between A1 subtypes and neurocognitive tests that characterize frontal lobe cognitive functions (e.g., verbal fluency, working memory, verbal learning). Furthermore, cognitive performance appears negatively correlated with A2 subtypes, whereas A3 subtypes are negatively correlated with planning and motor sequencing. These results provide additional support for the hypothesis that the A1 subtypes are associated with higher cognitive functioning, whereas the A2 and A3 subtypes are associated with impaired neurocognitive functioning. These results were confirmed by another more recent experimental study (38) that explored the association between CAP and neurocognitive performance in a group of normal subjects before and after two nights of experimentally induced sleep fragmentation. After sleep fragmentation, even if the percentage of SWS was dramatically reduced, there was a twofold increase in total CAP rate across all NREM sleep stages, and all phase A indexes significantly increased. Also this study supports that CAP A1 subtypes are associated with higher cognitive functioning, whereas CAP A3 subtypes are associated with lower cognitive functioning in young healthy subjects. Unfortunately, there are no systematic data on the relationship between CAP and cognitive function in healthy elderly, as well as in neurodegenerative disorders. The percentage of A1 subtypes shows clear changes by the age (39), with a decrease from teenagers (71%) to young adults (61%) and middle age (62%), and a marked decline after age 60 (46%). In contrast, subtypes A2 and A3 undergo a linear increase from infants, pre-school children to the old age, similar to the arousal evolution across the life span. A recent study (40) evaluated CAP in patients with idiopathic REM sleep behavior disorder (iRBD), a parasomnia that may be an early manifestation of a neurodegenerative disease. CAP sequences and CAP rate were significantly higher in iRBD patients compared to the controls. The increase in CAP sequences was observed in phase A2 and A3 subtypes while phase A1 subtype was significantly lower in RBD patients. This finding is interesting since a neurocognitive impairment was described in iRBD patients (41). In conclusion, a consistent literature demonstrated the relationship between sleep and memory, and in particular the need for sleep after learning in the consolidation of hippocampal-dependent memory. The importance of the first half of the night for declarative memory consolidation originates from sleep spindles and slow EEG oscillations. These distinct polysomnographic phenomena characterizing sleep microstructure show a decrease as aging progresses.

References

1. Jenkins DA, Dallenbach KM. Obliviscence during sleep and waking. Am J Psychol (1924) 35:605–12. doi: 10.2307/1414040

CrossRef Full Text

2. Squire LR, Zola SM. Structure and function of declarative and nondeclarative memory systems. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A (1996) 93:13515–22.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

3. Diekelmann S, Wilhelm I, Born J. The whats and whens of sleep-dependent memory consolidation. Sleep Med Rev (2009) 13(5):309–21. doi:10.1016/j.smrv.2008.08.002

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

4. Maquet P. The role of sleep in learning and memory. Science (2001) 294:1048–52. doi:10.1126/science.1062856

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

5. Ficca G, Salzarulo P. What in sleep is for memory. Sleep Med (2004) 5:225–30. doi:10.1016/j.sleep.2004.01.018

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

6. Plihal W, Born J. Effects of early and late nocturnal sleep on priming and spatial memory. Psychophysiology (1999) 36:571–82. doi:10.1111/1469-8986.3650571

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

7. Wagner U, Hallschmid M, Verleger R, Born J. Signs of REM sleep dependent enhancement of implicit face memory: a repetition priming study. Biol Psychol (2003) 62:197–210. doi:10.1016/S0301-0511(02)00125-4

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

8. Fogel SM, Smith CT, Cote KA. Dissociable learning-dependent changes in REM and non-REM sleep in declarative and procedural memory systems. Behav Brain Res (2007) 180:48–61. doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2007.02.037

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

9. Genzel L, Dresler M, Wehrle R, Grözinger M, Steiger A. Slow wave sleep and REM sleep awakenings do not affect sleep dependent memory consolidation. Sleep (2009) 32(3):302–10.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

10. Dresler M, Kluge M, Genzel L, Schüssler P, Steiger A. Synergistic effects of age and depression on sleep-dependent memory consolidation. Pharmacopsychiatry (2007) 40:238–9. doi:10.1055/s-2007-991835

CrossRef Full Text

11. Spencer RMC, Gouw AM, Ivry RB. Age-related decline of sleep-dependent consolidation. Learn Mem (2007) 14:480–4. doi:10.1101/lm.569407

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

12. Steiger A. Sleep and the hypothalamo-pituitary-adrenocortical system. Sleep Med Rev (2002) 6:125–38. doi:10.1053/smrv.2001.0159

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

13. Canessa N, Ferini-Strambi L. Sleep-disordered breathing and cognitive decline in older adults. JAMA (2011) 306(6):654–5. doi:10.1001/jama.2011.1124

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

14. Steriade M. Thalamic origin of sleep spindles: Morison and Bassett. J. Neurophysiol. (1995) 73:921–2.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

15. Sirota A, Csicsvari J, Buhl D, Buzsáki G. Communication between neocortex and hippocampus during sleep in rodents. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A (2003) 100:2065–9. doi:10.1073/pnas.0437938100

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

16. Clemens Z, Mölle M, Eross L, Jakus R, Rásonyi G, Halász P, et al. Fine-tuned coupling between human parahippocampal ripples and sleep spindles. Eur J Neurosci (2011) 33:511–20. doi:10.1111/j.1460-9568.2010.07505.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

17. Born J. Slow-wave sleep and the consolidation of long-term memory. World J Biol Psychiatry (2010) 11(Suppl 1):16–21. doi:10.3109/15622971003637637

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

18. Meier-Koll A, Bussmann B, Schmidt C, Neuschwander D. Walking through a maze alters the architecture of sleep. Percept Mot Skills (1999) 88:1141–59. doi:10.2466/pms.1999.88.3c.1141

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

19. Clemens Z, Fabo D, Halasz P. Overnight verbal memory retention correlates with the number of sleep spindles. Neuroscience (2005) 132:529–35. doi:10.1016/j.neuroscience.2005.01.011

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

20. Bergmann TO, Molle M, Diedrichs J, Born J, Siebner HR. Sleep spindle-related reactivation of category-specific cortical regions after learning face-scene associations. Neuroimage (2012) 59:2733–42. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.10.036

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

21. Werth E, Achermann P, Dijk DJ, Borbely AA. Spindle frequency activity in the sleep EEG: individual differences and topographic distribution. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol (1997) 103:535–42. doi:10.1016/S0013-4694(97)00070-9

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

22. Andrade KC, Spoormaker VI, Dresler M, Wehrle R, Holsboer F, Samann PG, et al. Sleep spindles and hippocampal functional connectivity in human NREM sleep. J Neurosci (2011) 31:10331–9. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5660-10.2011

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

23. Saletin JM, Goldstein AN, Walker MP. The role of sleep in directed forgetting and remembering of human memories. Cereb Cortex (2011) 21:2534–41. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhr034

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

24. van der Helm E, Gujar N, Nishida M, Walker MP. Sleep-dependent facilitation of episodic memory details. PLoS One (2011) 6:e27421. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0027421

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

25. Pace-Schott EF, Spencer RM. Age-related changes in the cognitive function of sleep. Prog Brain Res (2011) 191:75–89. doi:10.1016/B978-0-444-53752-2.00012-6

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

26. Puca FM, Bricolo A, Turella G. Effect of L-dopa or amantadine therapy on sleep spindles in Parkinsonism. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol (1973) 35(3):327–30. doi:10.1016/0013-4694(73)90245-9

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

27. Comella CL, Tanner CM, Ristanovic RK. Polysomnographic sleep measures in Parkinson’s disease patients with treatment-induced hallucinations. Ann Neurol (1993) 34(5):710–4. doi:10.1002/ana.410340514

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

28. Molle M, Marshall L, Gais S, Born J. Learning increases human electroencephalographic coherence during subsequent slow sleep oscillations. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A (2004) 101:13963–8. doi:10.1073/pnas.0402820101

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

29. Marshall L, Helgadottir H, Molle M, Born J. Boosting slow oscillations during sleep potentiates memory. Nature (2006) 444:610–3. doi:10.1038/nature05278

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

30. Terzano MG, Parrino L, Sherieri A, Chervin R, Chokroverty S, Guilleminault C, et al. Atlas, rules, and recording techniques for the scoring of cyclic alternating pattern (CAP) in human sleep. Sleep Med (2001) 2(6):537–53. doi:10.1016/S1389-9457(01)00149-6

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

31. Steriade M, Nunez A, Amzica F. A novel slow (<1 Hz) oscillation of neocortical neurons in vivo: depolarizing and hyperpolarizing components. J Neurosci (1993) 13:3252–65.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

32. Ferini-Strambi L, Ortelli P, Castronovo V, Cappa S. Increased periodic arousal fluctuations during non-REM sleep are associated to superior memory. Brain Res Bull (2004) 63:439–42. doi:10.1016/j.brainresbull.2004.01.012

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

33. Huber R, Ghilardi MF, Massimini M, Tononi G. Local sleep and learning. Nature (2004) 430:78–81. doi:10.1038/nature02663

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

34. Ferri R, Huber R, Aricò D, Drago V, Rundo F, Massimini M, et al. The slow-wave components of the cyclic alternating pattern (CAP) have a role in sleep-related learning processes. Neurosci Lett (2008) 432:228–31. doi:10.1016/j.neulet.2007.12.025

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

35. Bruni O, Ferri R, Vittori E, Novelli L, Vignati M, Porfirio MC, et al. Sleep architecture and NREM alterations in children and adolescents with Asperger syndrome. Sleep (2007) 30(11):1577–85.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

36. Bruni O, Ferri R, Novelli L, Finotti E, Terribili M, Troianiello M, et al. Slow EEG amplitude oscillations during NREM sleep and reading disabilities in children with dyslexia. Dev Neuropsychol (2009) 34(5):539–51. doi:10.1080/87565640903133418

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

37. Aricò D, Drago V, Foster PS, Heilman KM, Williamson J, Ferri R. Effects of NREM sleep instability on cognitive processing. Sleep Med (2010) 11:791–8. doi:10.1016/j.sleep.2010.02.009

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

38. Ferri R, Drago V, Aricò D, Bruni O, Remington RW, Stamatakis K, et al. The effects of experimental sleep fragmentation on cognitive processing. Sleep Med (2010) 11(4):378–85. doi:10.1016/j.sleep.2010.01.006

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

39. Parrino L, Ferri R, Bruni O, Terzano MG. Cyclic alternating pattern (CAP): the marker of sleep instability. Sleep Med Rev (2012) 16(1):27–45. doi:10.1016/j.smrv.2011.02.003

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

40. Kutlu A, Işeri P, Selekler M, Benbir G, Karadeniz D. Cyclic alternating pattern analysis in REM sleep behavior disorder. Sleep Breath (2013) 17:209–15. doi:10.1007/s11325-012-0675-5

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

41. Fantini ML, Farini E, Ortelli P, Zucconi M, Manconi M, Cappa S, et al. Longitudinal study of cognitive function in idiopathic REM sleep behavior disorder. Sleep (2011) 34(5):619–25.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Keywords: sleep microstructure, cyclic alternating pattern, memory, sleep spindles, cognitive functions

Citation: Ferini-Strambi L, Galbiati A and Marelli S (2013) Sleep microstructure and memory function. Front. Neurol. 4:159. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2013.00159

Received: 02 September 2013; Accepted: 26 September 2013;
Published online: 11 October 2013.

Edited by:

Giovanni Albani, Istituto Auxologico Italiano, Italy; IRCCS, Italy

Copyright: © 2013 Ferini-Strambi, Galbiati and Marelli. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: ferinistrambi.luigi@hsr.it

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.