Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Neuroimaging, 09 January 2023
Sec. Neuroimaging for Cognitive Neuroscience

Systems-level decoding reveals the cognitive and behavioral profile of the human intraparietal sulcus

  • Department of Molecular Psychology, Institute for Psychology, Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin, Berlin, Germany

Introduction: The human intraparietal sulcus (IPS) covers large portions of the posterior cortical surface and has been implicated in a variety of cognitive functions. It is, however, unclear how cognitive functions dissociate between the IPS's heterogeneous subdivisions, particularly in perspective to their connectivity profile.

Methods: We applied a neuroinformatics driven system-level decoding on three cytoarchitectural distinct subdivisions (hIP1, hIP2, hIP3) per hemisphere, with the aim to disentangle the cognitive profile of the IPS in conjunction with functionally connected cortical regions.

Results: The system-level decoding revealed nine functional systems based on meta-analytical associations of IPS subdivisions and their cortical coactivations: Two systems–working memory and numeric cognition–which are centered on all IPS subdivisions, and seven systems–attention, language, grasping, recognition memory, rotation, detection of motions/shapes and navigation–with varying degrees of dissociation across subdivisions and hemispheres. By probing the spatial overlap between systems-level co-activations of the IPS and seven canonical intrinsic resting state networks, we observed a trend toward more co-activation between hIP1 and the front parietal network, between hIP2 and hIP3 and the dorsal attention network, and between hIP3 and the visual and somatomotor network.

Discussion: Our results confirm previous findings on the IPS's role in cognition but also point to previously unknown differentiation along the IPS, which present viable starting points for future work. We also present the systems-level decoding as promising approach toward functional decoding of the human connectome.

1. Introduction

The human intraparietal sulcus separates the superior (SPL) from the inferior parietal lobule (IPL) within posterior parietal cortex (Binkofski et al., 2016). The IPS is relatively large, covering approximately 17% of the parietal lobe's surface, and can be divided into at least three distinct areas with distinct cytoarchitecture and structural and functional connectivity patterns (Scheperjans et al., 2008a; Uddin et al., 2010; Glasser et al., 2016): hIP1 and the hIP2 cover the most anterior aspects of the IPS (Choi et al., 2006) whereas the hIP3 is located posteriorly to hIP1 and hIP2 (Scheperjans et al., 2008a,b). A large body of literature has addressed the IPS's role in a variety of cognitive functions, yet it remains unclear how different cognitive functions map onto the distinct subdivisions within the IPS.

The IPS has been implicated in many cognitive functions. Among these are numerical cognition, including both: the processing of numerical magnitudes and arithmetic operations (Dehaene et al., 2003; Ansari, 2008; Wu et al., 2009; Pinel and Dehaene, 2010; Arsalidou and Taylor, 2011; He et al., 2015; Liu et al., 2017; Vogel et al., 2017; Price et al., 2018; Castaldi et al., 2020; Roell et al., 2021). Activations within IPS were also found during memory routines. These include encoding and maintaining of visual information in visual short-term memory tasks (Xu and Chun, 2006; Harrison et al., 2010; Offen et al., 2010; Domijan, 2011; Xu and Jeong, 2015; Markett et al., 2018; Sheremata et al., 2018; Duma et al., 2019; Praß and de Haan, 2019; Lefco et al., 2020) as well as familiarity based memory retrieval (Frithsen and Miller, 2014; Hutchinson et al., 2014; Rosen et al., 2015, 2018; Chen et al., 2017). Further, the IPS engages in visuomotor coordination such as grasping, action observation and control of hand and eye movements (Corbetta et al., 1998; Shikata et al., 2001; Grefkes and Fink, 2005; Villarreal et al., 2008; Cavina-Pratesi et al., 2018). This engagement extends to visuospatial processes including navigation and the perception of shapes and motions (Salillas et al., 2009; Tark and Curtis, 2013; Binkofski et al., 2016; Meier et al., 2018; Schultz and Bülthoff, 2019; Li and Shigemasu, 2021). Finally, the IPS plays an important role in the interplay of top-down and bottom-up guided pull of attentional processes (Corbetta et al., 1998, 2008; Corbetta and Shulman, 2002; Katsuki and Constantinidis, 2014; Connolly et al., 2016; Markett et al., 2022). Visuomotor coordination and visuospatial attentional processes are closely tied to saccade planning, in which the IPS is also involved (Corbetta et al., 1998; Grosbras et al., 2005; Morris et al., 2007; Baltaretu et al., 2020). But to the best of our knowledge only few attempts have been made to systematically disentangle the role of the cytoarchitecturally distinct IPS regions in cognitive functioning: While visuomotor functioning (Richter et al., 2019), mathematical operations (Wu et al., 2009; Price et al., 2018; Chang et al., 2019) and visuospatial tasks (Gillebert et al., 2013; Papadopoulos et al., 2018) seem to dissociate between the three IPS regions, no study, however, has covered the whole spectrum of the diverse cognitive operations the IPS is involved in.

With the present study, we aim to systematically disentangle the functional profile of the three cytoarchitecturally distinct IPS regions with Bayesian reverse inference decoding of neuroimaging data in Neurosynth (Yarkoni et al., 2011). The Neurosynth database contains data from thousands of neuroimaging studies, including activation coordinates and a large variety of psychological constructs that allow for a more fine-grained decoding, which facilitates the systematic research on structure-to-function relationships of individual brain regions. Given the IPS' embedding into distinct intrinsic connectivity networks and previous suggestions that the connectivity profile of the IPS is also functionally relevant (Corbetta and Shulman, 2002; Fox et al., 2005; Dosenbach et al., 2007; Corbetta et al., 2008; Power et al., 2011; Vossel et al., 2014), we additionally apply a newly developed systems-level decoding, where we simultaneously decode the functional profile of the different IPS regions and functionally connected cortical regions. This novel decoding strategy is based on co-activations and acknowledges that cognitive processes emerge from the interplay of interconnected regions. It thus complements already existing decoding strategies and should be more sensitive in detecting structure-function relationships. Given that hIP1, hIP2, and hIP3 have shown dissociable connectivity patterns (Uddin et al., 2010), we aim with this systems-level decoding to disentangle the functional spectrum of the three IPS seeds and their participation in putative functional neuronal systems.

2. Materials and methods

2.1. Ethics approval

All analysis used secondary data and no new human participants were generated. Ethics approval was obtained by the authors of the original datasets from their respective ethics review boards.

2.2. Definition of the IPS seed regions

We used the Anatomy Toolbox (Eickhoff et al., 2005) to define seed regions in the left and right IPS. The IPS parcellation in the Anatomy Toolbox represents probability maps driven by cytoarchitectonic properties (e.g., cell bodies in cortical layers) and subdivides the IPS in three different regions in each hemisphere (Choi et al., 2006; Scheperjans et al., 2008a,b) (see Figure 1). By grounding our seed definition on cytoarchitectonic data, we avoid issues with circularity when analyzing structure-to-function associations (Kriegeskorte et al., 2009). The IPS parcellation within the Anatomy Toolbox are based on an observer-independent mapping approach, which accounts for the well-known interindividual variability of the IPS's sulcal segments (Zlatkina and Petrides, 2014) and facilitates meta-analytic decoding based on neuroimaging data (Richter et al., 2019). Extracted seed regions were “downsampled” to 2 mm with FSL flirt (Jenkinson et al., 2012) and thresholded to include only voxels with probability of >25%.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Voxel size, Volume and Center of Mass of the IPS seed regions used in the present study. Colored frames around the values correspond to the colors of the seed regions displayed in the cortical renderings on the right. The cortical renderings were generated with MRIcro GL (Rorden and Brett, 2000).

2.3. Resting state data

We used whole-brain seed-based resting-state functional connectivity (RSFC) in combination with a cortical parcellation (Huang et al., 2022) to identify cortical regions with functional coupling to the six IPS seed regions. We utilized resting-state data (~11 min, TR = 800 ms, 2 mm cubic voxels) from a sample of N = 84 healthy young adults from a previous study (Markett et al., 2022), acquired with the HCP's pulse sequences (Harms et al., 2018) and preprocessed with HCP's minimal processing pipeline (Glasser et al., 2013) including noise-removal with ICA-Fix. Mean timeseries were extracted from each seed region's set of voxels and regressed against the time courses from all other voxels within the brain mask. The resulting individual-level seed-based RSFC maps were then submitted to a random-effects second level analysis in SPM12 and the group-level maps were thresholded to keep the family-wise error below 5% at the voxel level. From the thresholded maps, we calculated the relative overlap with 360 cortical regions from a volumetric version of HCP's multimodal parcellation and retained lists of functionally coupled cortical regions with at least 80% spatial overlap for each seed regions. Cortical regions overlapping with any seed region were excluded from the lists. Full details on all acquisition and preprocessing steps are described in Markett et al. (2022).

2.4. Functional decoding of IPS seed regions

We utilized the NeurosynthDecoder as implemented in the Neuroimaging Meta-Analysis Research Environment [NiMARE; (Salo et al., 2021)] to perform the functional decoding for each of the IPS seed regions. The NeurosynthDecoder allows for reverse inference decoding of activation-term associations, which infers the probability by which different behaviors were executed while a given brain region was active. The (~3,000) terms included in the database are a result from automated parsing through abstracts of published neuroimaging studies (Yarkoni et al., 2011) and concern psychological constructs (e.g., “working memory,” “attention”), anatomical regions (e.g., “intraparietal,” “parietal”) or terms that defy a clear categorization (e.g., “greater extent,” “relied”).

Calculation of reverse inferences provides posterior probabilities of a term given activation of the region AND given the prior probability of a term (i.e., the prevalence of the term in the database). Formally, the posterior probability is computed as P(term|activation, p) = p(P(activation|term)/P(activation|term, p) where the prior probability p is set beforehand to 0.5 (i.e., 50% chance of a brain region experiencing the brain state described by the term). This rather conservative approach equates the possibly distinct baserate of terms within the database, which enables the handling of otherwise very different posterior probabilities, that are used for the interpretation of functional decoding results (Yarkoni et al., 2011). For clarification, P(activation|term, p) represents the forward inference and gets computed as P(activation|term, p) = pP(activation|term) + (1p)P(activation|not having the term). In addition to the posterior probabilities (i.e., the effect sizes of the reverse inference decoding), a two-way chi-square was performed to determine if the presence of the label and the selection of a term are statistically independent (<0.05). Correction for multiple comparisons was done by applying a Benjamini-Hochberg FDR correction. More details are given in the NIMARE documentation, accessible at: https://nimare.readthedocs.io/en/latest/decoding.html).

In the present study, we performed the decoding for each individual IPS seed, by submitting each seed separately to the NeurosynthDecoder. This resulted in a list of associated terms and their posterior probabilities. Of these terms, we selected the 30 terms with highest posterior probability for each seed region to ensure comparability. The automatic NeurosynthDecoder lists all terms, irrespective of whether they passed the multiple comparison significance testing or not. We therefore excluded those terms that did not pass the multiple comparison significance testing post-hoc using an in-house MATLAB (2021) script.

2.5. System-level decoding of IPS seed-cortical pairs

The core operation of the systems decoding includes the simultaneous decoding of a given IPS seed and a functionally connected brain region, from which only those terms are retained that were not already obtained in the functional decoding of the connected region alone. Iterating across all seed-cortical pairs, the NeurosynthDecoder was applied twice: The first step decoded the seed region in conjunction with the cortical region, the second step decoded the cortical region alone. This resulted in two sets of terms which were compared with a logical-and-conjunction to produce a list of unique terms present in both sets and a set difference to produce a list of terms specific to the IPS seed-cortical pairs. The entire systems-level decoding framework enabled us to generate reverse inference maps for all seed-cortical pairs and to display all brain regions in that seed-cortical system that were associated with a particular term.

2.5.1. Grouping of the systems-level decoding results in Neurosynth topics

The Neurosynth database contains a large variety of terms, including psychological constructs, from which we derived the posterior probability of structure-to-function relationships. To ease interpretability, we aggregated the terms into larger topics, as provided on the Neurosynth webpage (Yarkoni, 2018). In brief, the topics were originally defined by text mining over neuroimaging article's abstracts by using latent Dirichlet allocation (LDA). LDA is a topic modeling technique that is suited to extract hidden (i.e., latent) topics form large corpus of text data (Blei et al., 2003). Details on the topic modeling in Neurosynth are described in the original publication (Poldrack et al., 2012). We used the most recent version of 50 topics, derived by LDA of the abstracts of 14,371 articles in the Neurosynth database. We visualize topic-wise surface maps that represent the cortical regions involved with a given topic. Then, in the final step of the systems-level decoding, we calculated the percentage of overlaps between the topic surface maps and the 7Networks (Yeo et al., 2011) (and vice versa) to compare our cortical systems to an established intrinsic cortical network organization of the human brain, including the dorsal attention and the frontoparietal networks, to whom the IPS has been consistently associated with. A detailed description of all analysis steps of the entire systems-level decoding is given in the Supporting information S1.

3. Results

3.1. Resting-state-functional- connectivity

We overlaid the seed-connectivity map of each IPS seed with the HCPex-MMP atlas parcellation (Huang et al., 2022) to identify cortical regions connected to the IPS regions. In the following, we will use the grouping scheme proposed by Glasser et al. (2016) who have assigned the 180 regions per hemisphere to 22 cortices (e.g., dorsal stream, …) and five cortical areas (i.e., anterior cortices, posterior cortices, early and intermediate visual cortex, sensorimotor areas, and auditory regions).

The left hIP1 was functionally connected to 90 distinct regions. These were mainly divided over anterior, posterior, and sensorimotor areas of the cortex (see Figure 2), and smaller portions in dorsal areas of the visual cortex. Left hIP2 was also functionally connected to 90 regions. The only difference between hIP1 and hIP2 was the absence of overlap with somatosensory and motor cortices for hIP2. Left hIP3 was functionally connected with 123 regions. These regions were similar to left hIP1 and hIP2 with the main difference of a larger overlap with dorsal and ventral visual cortices of hIP3.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Surface maps created with connectome workbench commands (Marcus et al., 2011) displaying resting-state functional connectivity of the IPS seed regions with cortical regions. Heatmaps on the right display the relative percentage of overlap with the 22 cortices by Glasser et al. (2016).

The right hIP1 was functionally connected to 77 regions that mainly encompassed anterior and posterior regions of the cortex. Sensorimotor areas were only connected within the ipsilateral hemisphere and functional connections with visual and temporal areas were sparse. Right hIP2 that was functionally connected to 101 regions and right hIP3 to 144 regions. For both seeds, the connected regions were divided over anterior, posterior, sensorimotor and visual areas and to a smaller extent over auditory and temporal areas of the cortex. The major difference between right hIP2 and hIP3 were a smaller engagement of visual areas for hIP2.

3.2. Functional characterization of IPS seed regions

For each seed region, we selected the 30 decoding terms with the highest Bayesian reverse posterior probabilities, filtered to include only psychological constructs (see Supporting information S2 for a full list of all significant terms with a reverse probability over >0.05). Bayesian reverse posterior probabilities are estimates of the probability of the occurrence of a term in the database, given activation foci within a seed region. Common terms for the left hemispheric seeds (i.e., left hIP1, hIP2, and hIP3) were “arithmetic,” “symbolic,” “calculation” and “numbers.” For the right hemispheric seeds, common terms associated with all IPS seeds were “arithmetic,” “symbolic,” “numbers,” “calculation,” and “memory load.” We also found evidence for functional segregation within IPS: The decoding revealed ten terms associated with several but not all seed regions and 12 terms that were uniquely assigned to one seed region only. A full list of terms denoting psychological constructs, ordered by the frequency of their assignment to all seed regions is given in Figure 3.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Neurosynth terms denoting psychological constructs resulting from the Bayesian reverse inference decoding summarized in a heatmap. The visualization is hierarchically ordered by the number of term appearances across the seed regions.

3.3. Systems-level decoding

The aim of the systems-level decoding was to decode structure-function relationships with respect to the distinct IPS regions and their embedding into cortical systems. In the following, we summarize the results by grouping all resulting terms into larger topics (see methods) and by visualizing the IPS-centered cortical systems associated with each topic as surface maps, including a spatial comparison with the 7Networks described in Yeo et al. (2011). A full table with all terms of the systems-level decoding, and a full table with all topics containing at least one term denoting psychological constructs in the systems-level decoding is given in the Supporting informations S3, S4.

3.3.1. Seed cortical topic surface maps

The systems-level decoding revealed nine cognitive systems grounded within IPS: a working memory and a numeric cognition system with connections to all three hIP seeds in both hemispheres, an attention system with connections to all three hIP but with preference for the right hemisphere, and six more focal systems with stronger dissociations among the three seeds and hemispheres: language, grasping, recognition memory, rotation, detection of motions/shapes and navigation. In Figure 4 we display the nine functional systems and the terms denoting psychological constructs produced by the systems-level decoding for each IPS seed that were grouped into a given topic. We present topological maps of the nine systems in Figure 5, together with information how the systems segregate across the seven canonical RSN (Yeo et al., 2011), the extent of the topological overlap with each RSN, and information on the contribution of each hIP region to the systems. A figure displaying the different contributions of every seed region within the topic surface maps is in the Supporting information S7.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Heatmaps of the systems associated with the IPS seeds. Here, we display all systems-level decoding terms denoting psychological constructs grouped into a given topic. The visualization is hierarchically ordered by the number of term appearances across the seed regions.

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Each panel displays a cognitive/behavioral system's topology projected in an inflated cortical surface. Color gradings of the surface maps correspond the number of seed regions that build a center of a given system, with less seed regions in redder hues. The top bar plots flanking the surface maps to the left and right show the relative number of system regions per seed and hemisphere. Below the surface maps we present the percentage overlaps of the systems with the 7Networks separately for each seed region (plotted to the left). The complimentary information (percent overlap of each 7Network with the system) is plotted to the right. The coloring scheme for the 7Networks is the usual scheme following the original publication (see legend). Detailed statistics and precise numbers are documented in the Supporting informations S5, S6.

The working memory and numeric cognition systems were similarly centered in all three IPS regions in both hemispheres and showed a high degree of topological overlap with each other. This was also apparent in their overlap with the 7Networks: In both systems, co-activations of each seed showed the largest overlap with the frontoparietal (FPN) and the dorsal attention network (DAN), and were absent for the limbic network. Overlap with the ventral attention network (VAN) followed a gradient across seed regions in both hemispheres, with increasing overlap from hIP1 to hIP3. Overlaps with the default mode network (DMN) were largest for bilateral hIP1, and overlap with the visual and somatomotor networks was most apparent for bilateral hIP3. The comparison with the 7Networks also revealed differences between the two systems, indicating that they are not isomorphic: The numeric cognition system showed relatively more overlap with the DAN relative to the FPN, the gradient of VAN contributions across seed regions was more pronounced in the working memory system, and right IPS showed more co-activations with the SMN in numeric cognition when compared to working memory. The numerical cognition system also had higher internal consistency by having very similar associations with the different terms across seed regions, while different working memory terms were more distributed across seed regions (see Figure 4).

The attention system was lateralized, with more contributions from seed regions in the right hemisphere. Co-activations of all three seeds in both hemispheres overlapped strongly with the FPN. Differences between the seed regions were apparent regarding the visual network and SMN: overlap with the visual overlap was relatively larger for the posterior seeds (hIP2 and hIP3) and overlap with the SMN was only visible for the posterior seeds in the right hemisphere. While all seed regions co-activated with regions within the DAN and the VAN, DAN associations were more pronounced for right hemispheric seeds and VAN associations were more pronounced in the left. Within the left hemisphere, the overlap with DAN and VAN shifted across seed regions: posterior seeds showed more DAN overlap, anterior seeds more overlap with the VAN. The terms were heterogeneously distributed over the IPS seed regions, with right hIP3 as having the largest number of term associations. Interestingly, the terms describe mostly spatial and executive (e.g., “spatial attention,” “visual attention,” “response selection,” “orienting”) processes and do not cover aspects of attentional alerting.

The IPS-centered language system displayed a strong lateralization toward the left hemisphere. We only observed right hemispheric contributions from hIP1. The terms building the corpus of this system can be mainly assigned to second language learning and bilingualism (e.g., “native,” “bilinguals,” “Chinese,” “English”), however, with additional terms describing visual and phonological related language processes (see Figure 4). The overlaps with the 7Networks were similar for all four implicated seed regions: The largest overlap was with the FPN, followed by DAN, VAN, and DMN. The language system showed only negligible overlaps with the visual and limbic networks and the SMN.

The IPS-centered grasping system was also more lateralized to the left hemisphere but more centered on posterior seeds. Co-activations of the three main contributing seed regions (left hIP2 and hIP3, right hIP3) showed similar topological overlap with the 7Networks, which was most strong for DAN, VAN, and the visual networks. Co-activations with the SMN was largely restricted to the most posterior hIP3.

The mental rotation system was the most focal system and only contained the term “rotation,” while all IPS seeds (except for left hIP1) contributed to the system, the most pronounced contribution was observed for right hIP2. The co-activations of this region were also more distributed across the 7Networks with most co-activations in the FPN, DAN, and VAN. The other four seed regions showed only co-activations with the DAN and the visual network.

The navigation system was more lateralized toward the right hemisphere. Of note, the major seed regions on the right were hIP1 and hIP3, while the major seed region on left was hIP2. While the co-activations of all seed regions in the navigation system overlapped most dominantly with the FPN, there were also differences between the seeds with more posterior seeds showing stronger coactivations with DAN, VAN, SMN, and the visual network, and more anterior seeds co-activating more extensively with regions in the DMN.

The recognition memory system was one of the smallest IPS-centered systems: The system was mainly centered in right hIP1 with minor links to the three seeds on the left. Co-activations of the seed region were most dominantly in the FPN and DMN, followed by the VAN.

The other small system, the motions/shapes system, was mainly centered in right hIP3 an left hIP2. For both seeds, co-activations overlapped with the DAN, FPN, and VAN. Right hIP3 showed also strong coactivations with the visual network, while the co-activations with left hIP2 were also pronounced within the DMN.

4. Discussion

The main goal of the present study was to disentangle the functional profiles of three cytoarchitecturally distinct bilateral IPS regions through meta-analytic decoding across the neuroimaging literature in two complementary ways: We conducted a standard Bayesian reverse inference decoding on the IPS regions individually, and a systems-level decoding that reveals joint seed-cortical co-activations as retrieved from the Neurosynth database. The systems-level decoding unraveled several IPS-centered functional cortical systems involved in a variety of cognitive functions. Our decoding framework revealed both similarities and dissimilarities between the different IPS regions and the behavioral relevance of their co-activation profiles.

4.1. Functional characterization of the IPS seeds

The standard meta-analytic Bayesian reverse inference decoding revealed that all three seeds of both hemispheres are commonly associated with terms related to numeric cognition. The engagement of the IPS in these processes was consistently found and is well-described in the neuroimaging literature (Menon et al., 2000; Dehaene et al., 2003; Pinel and Dehaene, 2010; Price et al., 2018). Among the theoretical considerations about the phenomenology of numeric cognition, evidence from cognitive neuroscience suggests that the IPS subserves the visual (mental) representation, manipulation and estimation of the magnitude of a number (Dehaene et al., 2003; Liu et al., 2017; Yeo et al., 2017; Bloechle et al., 2018). Furthermore, the IPS is not only linked to symbol-quantity assessments but also to basic arithmetic operations (e.g., addition, subtraction) (Wu et al., 2009; Arsalidou and Taylor, 2011; Matejko and Ansari, 2019; Castaldi et al., 2020; Roell et al., 2021). In line with these findings, the terms associated with the IPS seeds can be either be described as arithmetic operations (e.g., “calculation”) or more abstract forms of numerical representations (i.e., “symbolic,” “numbers”).

The few studies that aimed at the disentangling of the three cytoarchitecturally distinct IPS seeds in arithmetic tasks did not detect clear dissociable activation patterns in between the seeds in different arithmetic tasks. Wu et al. (2009) showed comparable levels of activation in all IPS regions during mental arithmetics and Chang et al. (2019) were able to link arithmetic aspects of word problem-solving to all three bilateral IPS seeds. Our present results confirm these findings and support the notion that all three cytoarchitecturally distinct IPS regions contribute to numeric operations.

Several studies have posed the question, whether different aspects of numeric cognition are reflected in different levels of lateralization within the IPS. In this line of reasoning, the right IPS is supposed to be involved in format-independent (i.e., irrespective whether stimuli are Arabic numerals or an array of dots) of quantity representations, and the left IPS in symbolic (e.g., Arabic numerals) processing (Venkatraman et al., 2005; Piazza et al., 2007; Holloway and Ansari, 2010). This idea is supported by stronger left IPS activation for verbal representation of arithmetic facts (Dehaene et al., 2003). We, however, did not observe lateralization differences of the IPS in numeric cognition. All three seed regions were commonly associated either with terms such as “arithmetic” and “calculation” or “numbers” and “symbolic” (see Figure 3). However, it needs to be noted that the terms in the Neurosynth database are a product of automated parsing of the literature and may be not as concise enough to distinguish between non-symbolic and symbolic arithmetic operations.

Solving arithmetic tasks depend strongly on working memory (Matejko and Ansari, 2021), so it does not come as a surprise, that the IPS has been consistently implicated in such processes. Accordingly, all seeds were associated with terms describing working memory (e.g., “working memory,” “memory wm,” “verbal working,” “memory load”). Working memory relies on three main processes: The cognitive control over the representation of information kept online, the temporary storage and the retrieval of that information (Baddeley, 1986). It has been hypothesized that the IPS plays an important role in the in the storage aspects of working memory (Christophel et al., 2012). Damage to the right IPS leads to impaired (visual) working memory (Ferber et al., 2020) and evidence from neuroimaging in healthy individuals found stronger activation in the right IPS (McNab and Klingberg, 2008). However, as for numeric cognition, we did not find a discrepancy between the IPS seeds in left and right hemispheres. The storage of information and the visuospatial attentional shift toward the location of the stored information are often described as overlapping processes (Awh et al., 1998; Awh and Jonides, 2001; Silk et al., 2010). Furthermore, focusing the mind on particular events while ignoring possible distractors can be specified as top-down guided attention, which is a cognitive function the IPS has been consistently associated with, especially in the visuospatial domain (Corbetta et al., 1998; Corbetta and Shulman, 2002; Vossel et al., 2014). The results of the functional decoding of the individual IPS seed regions are in accordance with these findings: Left hIP1 was associated with the term “attending,” and left hIP1 and hIP3 with “shifting” and left and right hIP3 with “visuospatial.”

Other terms denoting psychological constructs in this study were less homogenous in the association with the distinct IPS seeds. The term “grasping” appeared for left hIP2 and hIP3 and right hIP2. Grasping depends on the visual representation of objects in the outside world and the precise reaching for and manipulation of that object under permanent visual control. Research in primates and humans located two different sites in the brain related to grasping: Activation in the anterior bank of the IPS (AIP) is supposed to be engaged in visually controlled hand movements, whereas activation in the caudal bank (CIP) was associated with three-dimensional surface orientation and mental representation of objects (Shikata et al., 2001, 2003; Begliomini et al., 2007; Makin et al., 2007). Notably, AIP represent a putative homolog of the human hIP1, whereas the hIP3 might serve as a homolog of the medial intraparietal area (MIP) (Caspers and Zilles, 2012). However, in the present study the term grasping did not show a significant association with neither left nor right hIP1. As for the more caudal regions hIP2 and hIP3 we found terms associated with the mental representation of visual objects such as “mental imagery” and (mental) “rotation,” which might be an indicator in favor of the aforementioned distinctive roles of the IPS regions in visually guided hand movements.

4.2. Systems-level decoding

With the systems-level decoding we were able to unravel two large and overlapping functional systems associated with terms in the spectrum of working memory and numeric cognition that have not been described in the literature in comparable detail. We found a large attention system with a tendency toward a lateralization to the right hemisphere that complements the discussion of the overall involvement of the IPS in attentional processes. Finally, six smaller and more heterogenous systems were detected that shed new lights on the individual contribution of the IPS seeds toward cognitive functions such as language, recognition memory and navigation.

4.2.1. IPS-centered brain systems involved in working memory and numeric cognition

The systems-level decoding revealed two large functional systems–working memory and numeric cognition with similar cortical topology and little differentiation between the cytoarchitecturally distinct IPS-seed regions. Both the working memory system and the system involved in numeric cognition display large overlaps with the frontoparietal and the dorsal attention network and have a similar involvement in the visual network through left and right hIP3.

The large overlap between the two systems is not surprising given the detailed evidence on the interplay between working memory and numeric cognition. Working memory predicts mathematical abilities across the lifespan (Bull and Scerif, 2001; Friso-van den Bos et al., 2013; Allen et al., 2021; Nelwan et al., 2022), and decreased working memory performances has been linked to difficulties solving mathematical problems (Swanson and Jerman, 2006; Andersson, 2008; Zhang et al., 2018). Furthermore, performing different kinds of arithmetic tasks (e.g., calculation and word-problem solving) rely on several cognitive subsystems including working memory (DeStefano and LeFevre, 2004; Raghubar et al., 2010; Cragg et al., 2017). Solving a multidigit calculation, for instance, requires the processing of the task at hand, the storage of intermediate steps of the calculation, and the manipulation of this kind of information in mind (DeStefano and LeFevre, 2004; Peng et al., 2016). Further evidence, for the pivotal role of working memory in numeric cognition comes from the neuroimaging literature, which documents overlapping activations during arithmetic and working memory tasks (Owen et al., 2005; Arsalidou and Taylor, 2011; Rottschy et al., 2012; Fedorenko et al., 2013; Matejko and Ansari, 2021). These overlapping regions build a frontoparietal circuit that includes the IPS, the supramarginal gyrus, premotor cortex and the lateral prefrontal cortex (Menon, 2016), which are a subset of the regions that we uncovered through he systems-level decoding.

Both systems showed nearly identical patterns of overlaps with the frontoparietal DAN and the FPN across both hemispheres. While both ICNs feature the IPS as putative hub (Fox et al., 2005; Dosenbach et al., 2007; Power et al., 2011), previous work has rather focused on functional differences between the two networks (Vincent et al., 2008; Spreng et al., 2010; Fedorenko et al., 2013; Cole et al., 2014), for example in regard to attention (Corbetta and Shulman, 2002; Corbetta et al., 2008; Vossel et al., 2014; Markett et al., 2022), cognitive control (Cole and Schneider, 2007; Power et al., 2011; Marek and Dosenbach, 2018), cognitive flexibility (Cohen et al., 2014), but also to working memory and numeric cognition (Champod and Petrides, 2010; Fedorenko et al., 2013; Markett et al., 2018; Chang et al., 2019). In this light, the almost identical coactivations pattern of the IPS with both ICN during working memory and numeric cognition suggests more similarity between the two ICN than previously thought.

Another goal of this study was to disentangle the role of the cytoarchitectural distinct IPS region regarding their functional roles. In contrast to the standard decoding, where we found subtle differences between the seed regions, and mainly bilateral hIP1 to be associated with the terms “working memory” and “memory wm,” the systems-level decoding revealed associations with the term “memory wm” for all seed regions. This discrepancy suggests a direct involvement of hIP1 in working memory while hIP2 and hIP3 participate in working memory through their co-activation with other cortical areas. An examination of the distinct roles of the IPS seeds in cognition, also includes an examination of differences between the hemispheres. We did not find signs of lateralization in working memory & numeric cognition, which matches the results from the standard decoding but does not align with previous studies on frontoparietal networks (Matejko and Ansari, 2021) or individual brain regions in working memory and/or numeric cognition (Dehaene et al., 2003; Piazza et al., 2007). While we found an engagement of all three IPS regions in both systems, and thus little evidence for dissociating roles of different areas along the IPS, there were subtle differences that still suggest different roles of IPS subregions. First, regions in the DMN showed a preference of overlaps with bilateral hIP1. Second, the bilateral hIP3 showed greater overlap with the visual network. This finding is consistent with earlier attempts in dissociating the connectivity of the three IPS seeds, where the connectivity between the hIP3 and occipital regions were found to be greater in extent than for hIP1 and hIP2 (Uddin et al., 2010; Price et al., 2018) and where the posterior caudal IPS is jointly involved with visual areas in the processing of three-dimensional information (Jastorff et al., 2016; Welchman, 2016). The latter and the fact that this difference in connectivity is not equivalent in respect to the terms that build the corpus of the two functional IPS centered systems (see Figure 4), might be in line with earlier hypotheses stating that the hIP3 is involved in spatial representation of both format-independent quantity and symbolic numerical representations [cf., (Uddin et al., 2010)].

4.2.2. IPS-centered attention system

The IPS is well-known for its role in multimodal attention, particularly through its role as a putative hub in the DAN. Previous research suggests that the DAN is involved in the top-down goal-directed attentional selection based on internal goals or expectations (Corbetta et al., 1998, 2008; Corbetta and Shulman, 2002; Fox et al., 2005). In line with this work, we found strong meta-analytical associations between attention and coactivations of the IPS with large parts of the DAN. Previous work also suggests that the DAN interacts dynamically with regions in ventral frontoparietal areas that form a functional brain system labeled as ventral attention network (Corbetta et al., 2008), salience network (Seeley et al., 2007) or as cingulo-opercular network (Dosenbach et al., 2008). We observed attention-related coactivations between the IPS and the VAN, but these coactivations were less widespread than the coactivations with the DAN. Across different IPS regions, we also observed a negative relationship between DAN- and VAN-coactivations: Particularly in the left hemisphere, co-activations fell either in the DAN or in the VAN. This would be in line with the proposed dissociation of the two ICN in attention and their complex interplay during top-down and bottom-up attention (Katsuki and Constantinidis, 2014; Vossel et al., 2014; Suo et al., 2021). Previous work also suggests a dissociation between the FPN and the DAN during attention: The FPN is thought to be involved in executive attentional control while the DAN is involved in the spatial orienting of attention (Petersen and Posner, 2012). We, however, observed a similar degree of co-activation of both networks with IPS regions during attention. On the term-level, we also found associations that suggest an involvement of the IPS-centered attention system in both spatial orienting and selective executive attention. These findings support previous suggestions that the FPN interacts closely with the DAN during visuospatial attention (Spreng et al., 2010; Dixon et al., 2017, 2018) and that different attention functions dissociate less across ICN than previously thought (Markett et al., 2022).

Only few works have addressed the role of different IPS divisions in attention. We observed stronger co-activation of posterior hIP3 with visual regions and stronger co-activations of hIP1 with the default mode network. This fits into previous reports on within-IPS differentiations (Gillebert et al., 2013) and supports the idea that the attention system is distinct from the sensory (visual) areas that it modulates (Posner and Dehaene, 1994). A more direct link between the default mode network and visual processing and attention has also been suggested previously (Szinte and Knapen, 2020; Markett et al., 2022). We also found the attention system to be lateralized toward the right hemisphere, which fits into the neuropsychological literature on attention deficits after right-hemispheric lesions (Corbetta and Shulman, 2011; Vossel et al., 2014).

4.2.3. Heterogenous IPS-centered functional systems

The IPS-centered language system displayed a lateralization toward the left hemisphere, in line with this hemisphere's dominance in language functioning. We found overlaps between the left IPS seeds and the right hIP1 and the FPN, the DAN and the VAN. The terms building the corpus of this system can be mainly assigned to second language learning (e.g., “native,” “bilinguals,” “Chinese,” “English”). This fits previous work that implicated the left intraparietal lobule (i.e., the angular gyrus) in language learning (Della Rosa et al., 2012; Barbeau et al., 2017) and highlighted a language control network consisting of anterior cingulate/pre-SMA, thalamus, basal ganglia, left intraparietal lobule (i.e., left angular gyrus) and frontal control regions (i.e., left prefrontal cortex) in switching between multiple languages (Abutalebi et al., 2013; Abutalebi and Green, 2016; Yuan, 2021). The results of the systems-level decoding suggests equally important involvement of the three left IPS subregions, presumably through the control of eye movements accomplished by DAN and VAN components (Corbetta et al., 1998, 2008; Corbetta and Shulman, 2002; Henderson et al., 2018), if language stimuli are presented visually (e.g., reading) as done in many natural language tasks.

A similar tendency toward lateralization to the left hemisphere was existent for the grasping system we found in the present study. Grasping requires the visually guided control of arm or hand movements, associated with frontoparietal areas (Binkofski et al., 1999, 2016; Grefkes and Fink, 2005; Filimon, 2010; Cavina-Pratesi et al., 2018) which might explain the large overlaps of this system with the DAN, the VAN and FPN. Neurons in the caudal bank of the macaque brain react to visual and somatosensory stimuli (Matelli and Luppino, 2001), which might serve as a further explanation for the large overlaps with the visual network and SMN in the left hIP3 that presents a putative homolog of the macaque MIP (Caspers and Zilles, 2012). The results in the present study represent, however, a viable starting point for future work, examining the specialized role of cytoarchitectural distinct IPS seeds in grasping.

By contrast, the IPS-centered system involved in navigation showed a lateralization to the right hemisphere with a peak (i.e., in respect to the spatial extent) on right hIP3. This right hemispheric dominance was accompanied by substantial overlaps with the VAN, the FPN, the DAN, visual areas and the SMN. Spatial navigation depends on the integration of exogenous sensory (i.e., especially visual) information and a controlled course of action if a visual cue is behaviorally relevant. Both actions are accomplished by the interactions of brain regions located in the VAN, the DAN and the FPN (Corbetta and Shulman, 2002; Corbetta et al., 2008; Chica et al., 2014), with a hypothesized right lateralization of the VAN as found in the present study.

Another system with the largest spatial extent on right hIP3, is involved with the detection of objects in the environment based on motions and shapes, which aligns with assumptions about the role of the IPS in object recognition [for reviews see Grefkes and Fink, 2005; Binkofski et al., 2016]. The detection of objects in the environment depends heavily on the processing of visual information, the maintenance of this information in visual short-term memory, attending to the stored information and the selection of an adequate response, accomplished by the interaction of distributed brain areas in prefrontal cortex, visual areas and the IPS (Xu and Chun, 2006; Xu, 2009; Harrison et al., 2010; Christophel et al., 2012; Xu and Jeong, 2015). The overlaps of the right hIP3-centered system components in this study with the visual network, the DAN & FPN and of the left hIP2 components with the FPN are thus in line with recent work. However, we found the left hIP2 components to be associated with the term “stimulus response” which presents a cognitive process which is not exclusively tied to the detection of motions and shapes. Moreover, the left hIP1 components were associated with “visual motion” but showed almost no significant overlaps with any of the 7Networks. Thus, further research is needed to elaborate on the dissociation between left hIP and right hIP3 as found in the present study.

Interestingly, except from left hIP1 all seed regions were associated with the term “rotation,” while the respective IPS-centered system is mainly based on right hIP2 displaying large overlaps with the DAN, the FPN, the VAN and the visual and the somatomotor network. Activation during mental rotation was robustly found in frontoparietal areas including the IPS (Podzebenko et al., 2005; Weiss et al., 2009). Studies aiming at a differentiation of the IPS seeds as used in our study, found stronger connectivity between hIP3 and visual areas (Uddin et al., 2010; Price et al., 2018), while previous work also highlighted the role of this subdivision in the manipulation of visual information (Papadopoulos et al., 2018) and visuospatial attention (Gillebert et al., 2013), which aligns with the general findings presented here.

Finally, the recognition memory system centered on right hIP1 (with some limitations also on the left hIP1) consisted of regions of the FPN, the DAN, the DMN and VAN. Notably, the IPS has been consistently associated with familiarity-related processes of memory recollection (Wagner et al., 2005; Yonelinas, 2005; Berryhill et al., 2007; Johnson et al., 2013). Allocating resources to the recollection of a memory are initiated by top-down guided attentional control processes involving regions in the DAN, while activation in the VAN reflects the bottom-up sensory guided pull of attention induced by the retrieved memory (Cabeza et al., 2008; Ciaramelli et al., 2008; Hutchinson et al., 2014; Monge et al., 2018). Finally, activation in the DMN have as well be associated with episodic memory retrieval (Spreng et al., 2010).

Notably, common to all systems presented here, is a strong engagement of cortical areas in either/or the DAN, FPN and the VAN. We therefore propose that the IPS plays a domain-general role in mediating between these systems, for instance, by allocating attentional resources in interaction with frontal executive areas. The systems-level decoding also unraveled differences in between the participation of the different seed regions in cortical functioning, which might serve as a promising lead for future studies. For instance, we found stronger co-activations of visual and sensorimotor areas with hIP3 seed regions in working memory, numeric cognition, attention, grasping, navigation and in the detection of motions and shapes in comparison to hIP1 and hIP2, while the association with (e.g.,) recollection memory seems to be restricted to a single IPS seed (i.e., bilateral hIP1).

4.3. Conclusion

The novel systems-level decoding approach proved useful in recovering structure-function associations of the IPS beyond the localized associations from the standard Bayesian reverse decoding framework. The cognitive profile of the IPS is heterogeneous but also quite uniform across different subregions. Not surprisingly for a putative cortical hub region, the IPS's functional repertoire becomes most visible in conjunction with its functional interactions with widespread cortical regions across several intrinsic networks. The present results provide a reference for the interpretation of cognitive neuroscience experiments and highlights several starting points for future investigations for the connectome-wide representation of cognitive and behavioral functions.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

All analysis used secondary data and no new human participants were generated. Ethics approval was obtained by the authors of the original datasets from their respective ethics review boards.

Author contributions

Material preparation and data collection was performed by OB and SM. OB implemented the computer code, supporting algorithms, and analyzed the data. OB drafted the manuscript and final revisions were done by SM. All authors contributed to the study conception, design, read, and approved the final manuscript.

Funding

The current work was partially funded by Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft, Grant/Award Number: MA-6792/3-1. The article processing charge was funded by the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG, German Research Foundation) – 491192747 and the Open Access Publication Fund of Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnimg.2022.1074674/full#supplementary-material

References

Abutalebi, J., Della Rosa, P. A., Ding, G., Weekes, B., Costa, A., Green, D. W. (2013). Language proficiency modulates the engagement of cognitive control areas in multilinguals. Cortex 49, 905–911. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2012.08.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Abutalebi, J., Green, D. W. (2016). Neuroimaging of language control in bilinguals: neural adaptation and reserve. Biling. Lang. Cogn. 19, 689–698. doi: 10.1017/S1366728916000225

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Allen, K., Giofr,è, D., Higgins, S., Adams, J. (2021). Using working memory performance to predict mathematics performance 2 years on. Psychol. Res. 85, 1986–1996. doi: 10.1007/s00426-020-01382-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Andersson, U. (2008). Working memory as a predictor of written arithmetical skills in children: The importance of central executive functions. Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 78, 181–203. doi: 10.1348/000709907X209854

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ansari, D. (2008). Effects of development and enculturation on number representation in the brain. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 9, 278–291. doi: 10.1038/nrn2334

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arsalidou, M., Taylor, M. J. (2011). Is 2+2=4? Meta-analyses of brain areas needed for numbers and calculations. NeuroImage 54, 2382–2393. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.10.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Awh, E., Jonides, J. (2001). Overlapping mechanisms of attention and spatial working memory. Trends Cogn. Sci. 5, 119–126. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01593-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Awh, E., Jonides, J., Reuter-Lorenz, P. A. (1998). Rehearsal in spatial working memory. J. Exp. Psychol. 24, 780–790. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.24.3.780

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baddeley, A. (1986). Working Memory. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Baltaretu, B. R., Monaco, S., Velji-Ibrahim, J., Luabeya, G. N., Crawford, J. D. (2020). Parietal cortex integrates saccade and object orientation signals to update grasp plans. J. Neurosci. 4525–4535. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0300-20.2020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barbeau, E. B., Chai, X. J., Chen, J. K., Soles, J., Berken, J., Baum, S., et al. (2017). The role of the left inferior parietal lobule in second language learning: an intensive language training fMRI study. Neuropsychologia 98, 169–176. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2016.10.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Begliomini, C., Caria, A., Grodd, W., Castiello, U. (2007). Comparing natural and constrained movements: new insights into the visuomotor control of grasping. PLoS ONE 2, e1108. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0001108

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berryhill, M. E., Phuong, L., Picasso, L., Cabeza, R., Olson, I. R. (2007). Parietal lobe and episodic memory: bilateral damage causes impaired free recall of autobiographical memory. J. Neurosci. 27, 14415–14423. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4163-07.2007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Binkofski, F., Buccino, G., Posse, S., Seitz, R. J., Rizzolatti, G., Freund, H.-J. (1999). A fronto-parietal circuit for object manipulation in man: evidence from an fMRI-study: Fronto-parietal circuit for object manipulation. Eur. J. Neurosci. 11, 3276–3286. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.1999.00753.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Binkofski, F. C., Klann, J., Caspers, S. (2016). “On the neuroanatomy and functional role of the inferior parietal lobule and intraparietal sulcus,” in Neurobiology of Language, eds G. Hickok and S. L. Small (San Diego, CA: Academic Press), 35–47. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-12-407794-2.00004-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blei, D. M., Ng, A. Y., Jordan, M. I. (2003). Latent dirichlet allocation. J. Mach. Learn. Res. 3, 993–1022.

Google Scholar

Bloechle, J., Huber, J. F., Klein, E., Bahnmueller, J., Rennig, J., Moeller, K., et al. (2018). Spatial arrangement and set size influence the coding of non-symbolic quantities in the intraparietal sulcus. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 12, 54. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2018.00054

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bull, R., Scerif, G. (2001). Executive functioning as a predictor of children's mathematics ability: inhibition, switching, and working memory. Dev. Neuropsychol. 19, 273–293. doi: 10.1207/S15326942DN1903_3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cabeza, R., Ciaramelli, E., Olson, I. R., Moscovitch, M. (2008). The parietal cortex and episodic memory: an attentional account. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 9, 613–625. doi: 10.1038/nrn2459

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Caspers, S., Zilles, K. (2012). “Posterior parietal cortex: Multimodal association cortex,” in The Human Nervous System, eds, Mai, J. K. and Paxinos, G. (Boston, MA: Elsevier Academic Press). 1036–1055.

Google Scholar

Castaldi, E., Vignaud, A., Eger, E. (2020). Mapping subcomponents of numerical cognition in relation to functional and anatomical landmarks of human parietal cortex. NeuroImage 221, 117210. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.117210

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cavina-Pratesi, C., Connolly, J. D., Monaco, S., Figley, T. D., Milner, A. D., Schenk, T., et al. (2018). Human neuroimaging reveals the subcomponents of grasping, reaching and pointing actions. Cortex 98, 128–148. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2017.05.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Champod, A. S., Petrides, M. (2010). Dissociation within the frontoparietal network in verbal working memory: a parametric functional magnetic resonance imaging study. J. Neurosci. 30, 3849–3856. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0097-10.2010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chang, T., Lung, T., Ng, C., Metcalfe, A. W. S. (2019). Fronto-insular-parietal network engagement underlying arithmetic word problem solving. Hum. Brain Mapp. 40, 1927–1941. doi: 10.1002/hbm.24502

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, H.-Y., Gilmore, A. W., Nelson, S. M., McDermott, K. B. (2017). Are there multiple kinds of episodic memory? An fMRI investigation comparing autobiographical and recognition memory tasks. J. Neurosci. 37, 2764–2775. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1534-16.2017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chica, A. B., Bourgeois, A., Bartolomero, P. (2014). On the role of the ventral attention system in spatial orienting. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8, 235. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00235

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Choi, H.-J., Zilles, K., Mohlberg, H., Schleicher, A., Fink, G. R., Armstrong, E., et al. (2006). Cytoarchitectonic identification and probabilistic mapping of two distinct areas within the anterior ventral bank of the human intraparietal sulcus. J. Comp. Neurol. 495, 53–69. doi: 10.1002/cne.20849

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Christophel, T. B., Hebart, M. N., Haynes, J.-D. (2012). Decoding the contents of visual short-term memory from human visual and parietal cortex. J. Neurosci. 32, 12983–12989. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0184-12.2012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ciaramelli, E., Grady, C. L., Moscovitch, M. (2008). Top-down and bottom-up attention to memory: A hypothesis (AtoM) on the role of the posterior parietal cortex in memory retrieval. Neuropsychologia 46, 1828–1851. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.03.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen, J. R., Gallen, C. L., Jacobs, E. G., Lee, T. G., D'Esposito, M. (2014). Quantifying the reconfiguration of intrinsic networks during working memory. PLoS ONE 9, e106636. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0106636

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cole, M. W., Bassett, D. S., Power, J. D., Braver, T. S., Petersen, S. E. (2014). Intrinsic and task-evoked network architectures of the human brain. Neuron 83, 238–251. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.05.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cole, M. W., Schneider, W. (2007). The cognitive control network: integrated cortical regions with dissociable functions. NeuroImage 37, 343–360. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.03.071

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Connolly, J. D., Kentridge, R. W., Cavina-Pratesi, C. (2016). Coding of attention across the human intraparietal sulcus. Exp. Brain Res. 234, 917–930. doi: 10.1007/s00221-015-4507-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Corbetta, M., Akbudak, E., Conturo, T. E., Snyder, A. Z., Ollinger, J. M., Drury, H. A., et al. (1998). A common network of functional areas for attention and eye movements. Neuron 21, 761–773. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(00)80593-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Corbetta, M., Patel, G., Shulman, G. L. (2008). The reorienting system of the human brain: from environment to theory of mind. Neuron 58, 306–324. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.04.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Corbetta, M., Shulman, G. L. (2002). Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 3, 201–215. doi: 10.1038/nrn755

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Corbetta, M., Shulman, G. L. (2011). Spatial neglect and attention networks. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 34, 569–599. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-061010-113731

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cragg, L., Richardson, S., Hubber, P. J., Keeble, S., Gilmore, C. (2017). When is working memory important for arithmetic? The impact of strategy and age. PLoS ONE 12, e0188693. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0188693

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dehaene, S., Piazza, M., Pinel, P., Cohen, L. (2003). Three parietal circuits for number processing. Cogn. Neuropsychol. 20, 487–506. doi: 10.1080/02643290244000239

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Della Rosa, P. A., Videsott, G., Borsa, V. M., Canini, M., Weekes, B., Franceschini, R., et al. (2012). A neural interactive location for multilingual talent. Cortex 49, 605–608. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2012.12.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

DeStefano, D., LeFevre, J. (2004). The role of working memory in mental arithmetic. Eur. J. Cogn. Psychol. 16, 353–386. doi: 10.1080/09541440244000328

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dixon, M. L., Andrews-Hanna, J. R., Spreng, R. N., Irving, Z. C., Mills, C., Girn, M., et al. (2017). Interactions between the default network and dorsal attention network vary across default subsystems, time, and cognitive states. NeuroImage 147, 632–649. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.12.073

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dixon, M. L., De La Vega, A., Mills, C., Andrews-Hanna, J., Spreng, R. N., Cole, M. W., et al. (2018). Heterogeneity within the frontoparietal control network and its relationship to the default and dorsal attention networks. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 115, E1598–E1607. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1715766115

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Domijan, D. (2011). A computational model of fMRI activity in the intraparietal sulcus that supports visual working memory. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 11, 573–599. doi: 10.3758/s13415-011-0054-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dosenbach, N. U. F., Fair, D. A., Cohen, A. L., Schlaggar, B. L., Petersen, S. E. (2008). A dual-networks architecture of top-down control. Trends Cogn. Sci. 12, 99–105. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2008.01.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dosenbach, N. U. F., Fair, D. A., Miezin, F. M., Cohen, A. L., Wenger, K. K., Dosenbach, R. A. T., et al. (2007). Distinct brain networks for adaptive and stable task control in humans. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 104, 11073–11078. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0704320104

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Duma, G. M., Mento, G., Cutini, S., Sessa, P., Baillet, S., Brigadoi, S., et al. (2019). Functional dissociation of anterior cingulate cortex and intraparietal sulcus in visual working memory. Cortex 121, 277–291. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2019.09.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eickhoff, S. B., Stephan, K. E., Mohlberg, H., Grefkes, C., Fink, G. R., Amunts, K., et al. (2005). A new SPM toolbox for combining probabilistic cytoarchitectonic maps and functional imaging data. NeuroImage 25, 1325–1335. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.12.034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fedorenko, E., Duncan, J., Kanwisher, N. (2013). Broad domain generality in focal regions of frontal and parietal cortex. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 110, 16616–16621. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1315235110

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ferber, S., Ruppel, J., Danckert, J. (2020). Visual working memory deficits following right brain damage. Brain Cogn. 142, 105566. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2020.105566

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Filimon, F. (2010). Human cortical control of hand movements: parietofrontal networks for reaching, grasping, and pointing. Neuroscientist 16, 388–407. doi: 10.1177/1073858410375468

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fox, M. D., Snyder, A. Z., Vincent, J. L., Corbetta, M., Van Essen, D. C., Raichle, M. E. (2005). The human brain is intrinsically organized into dynamic, anticorrelated functional networks. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 102, 9673–9678. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0504136102

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Friso-van den Bos, I., van der Ven, S. H. G., Kroesbergen, E. H., van Luit, J. E. H. (2013). Working memory and mathematics in primary school children: a meta-analysis. Educ. Res. Rev. 10, 29–44. doi: 10.1016/j.edurev.2013.05.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frithsen, A., Miller, M. B. (2014). The posterior parietal cortex: comparing remember/know and source memory tests of recollection and familiarity. Neuropsychologia 61, 31–44. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.06.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gillebert, C. R., Mantini, D., Peeters, R., Dupont, P., Vandenberghe, R. (2013). Cytoarchitectonic mapping of attentional selection and reorienting in parietal cortex. NeuroImage 67, 257–272. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.11.026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Glasser, M. F., Coalson, T. S., Robinson, E. C., Hacker, C. D., Harwell, J., Yacoub, E., et al. (2016). A multi-modal parcellation of human cerebral cortex. Nature 536, 171–178. doi: 10.1038/nature18933

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Glasser, M. F., Sotiropoulos, S. N., Wilson, J. A., Coalson, T. S., Fischl, B., Andersson, J. L., et al. (2013). The minimal preprocessing pipelines for the human connectome project. NeuroImage 80, 105–124. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.04.127

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grefkes, C., Fink, G. R. (2005). The functional organization of the intraparietal sulcus in humans and monkeys. J. Anat. 207, 3–17. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7580.2005.00426.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grosbras, M. H., Laird, A. R., Paus, T. (2005). Cortical regions involved in eye movements, shifts of attention, and gaze perception. Hum. Brain Mapp. 25, 140–154. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20145

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harms, M. P., Somerville, L. H., Ances, B. M., Andersson, J., Barch, D. M., Bastiani, M., et al. (2018). Extending the human connectome project across ages: Imaging protocols for the lifespan development and aging projects. Neuroimage. 183, 972–984. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.09.060

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harrison, A., Jolicoeur, P., Marois, R. (2010). “What” and “Where” in the Intraparietal Sulcus: An fMRI Study of Object Identity and Location in Visual Short-Term Memory. Cereb. Cortex 20, 2478–2485. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhp314

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

He, L., Zhou, K., Zhou, T., He, S., Chen, L. (2015). Topology-defined units in numerosity perception. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 112, E5647–55. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1512408112

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Henderson, J. M., Choi, W., Luke, S. G., Schmidt, J. (2018). Neural correlates of individual differences in fixation duration during natural reading. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 1–33. doi: 10.1080/17470218.2017.1329322

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Holloway, I. D., Ansari, D. (2010). Developmental specialization in the right intraparietal sulcus for the abstract representation of numerical magnitude. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 22, 2627–2637. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21399

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huang, C.-C., Rolls, E. T., Feng, J., Lin, C.-P. (2022). An extended Human Connectome Project multimodal parcellation atlas of the human cortex and subcortical areas. Brain Struct. Funct. 227, 763–778. doi: 10.1007/s00429-021-02421-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hutchinson, J. B., Uncapher, M. R., Weiner, K. S., Bressler, D. W., Silver, M. A., Preston, A. R., et al. (2014). Functional heterogeneity in posterior parietal cortex across attention and episodic memory retrieval. Cereb. Cortex 24, 49–66. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs278

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jastorff, J., Abdollahi, R. O., Fasano, F., Orban, G. A. (2016). Seeing biological actions in 3 D : an f MRI study. Hum. Brain Mapp. 37, 203–219. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jenkinson, M., Beckmann, C. F., Behrens, T. E. J., Woolrich, M. W., Smith, S. M. (2012). FSL. NeuroImage 62, 782–790. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.09.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnson, J. D., Suzuki, M., Rugg, M. D. (2013). Recollection, familiarity, and content-sensitivity in lateral parietal cortex: a high-resolution fMRI study. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7, 219. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00219

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Katsuki, F., Constantinidis, C. (2014). Bottom-up and top-down attention: different processes and overlapping neural systems. Neuroscientist 20, 509–521. doi: 10.1177/1073858413514136

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kriegeskorte, N., Simmons, W. K., Bellgowan, P. S. F., Baker, C. I. (2009). Circular analysis in systems neuroscience: the dangers of double dipping. Nat. Neurosci. 12, 535–540. doi: 10.1038/nn.2303

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lefco, R. W., Brissenden, J. A., Noyce, A. L., Tobyne, S. M., Somers, D. C. (2020). Gradients of functional organization in posterior parietal cortex revealed by visual attention, visual short-term memory, and intrinsic functional connectivity. NeuroImage 219, 117029. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.117029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, Z., Shigemasu, H. (2021). Unique neural activity patterns among lower order cortices and shared patterns among higher order cortices during processing of similar shapes with different stimulus types. Percept. 12, 204166952110182. doi: 10.1177/20416695211018222

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, J., Zhang, H., Chen, C., Chen, H., Cui, J., Zhou, X. (2017). The neural circuits for arithmetic principles. NeuroImage 147, 432–446. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.12.035

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Makin, T. R., Holmes, N. P., Zohary, E. (2007). Is that near my hand? multisensory representation of peripersonal space in human intraparietal sulcus. J. Neurosci. 27, 731–740. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3653-06.2007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marcus, D. S., Harwell, J., Olsen, T., Hodge, M., Glasser, M. F., Prior, F., et al. (2011). Informatics and data mining tools and strategies for the human connectome project. Front. Neuroinformatics 5, 4. doi: 10.3389/fninf.2011.00004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marek, S., Dosenbach, N. U. F. (2018). The frontoparietal network: function, electrophysiology, and importance of individual precision mapping. Dialogues Clin. Neurosci. 20, 133–140. doi: 10.31887/DCNS.2018.20.2/smarek

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Markett, S., Nothdurfter, D., Focsa, A., Reuter, M., Jawinski, P. (2022). Attention networks and the intrinsic network structure of the human brain. Hum. Brain Mapp. 43, 1431–1448. doi: 10.1002/hbm.25734

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Markett, S., Reuter, M., Heeren, B., Lachmann, B., Weber, B., Montag, C. (2018). Working memory capacity and the functional connectome - insights from resting-state fMRI and voxelwise centrality mapping. Brain Imaging Behav. 12, 238–246. doi: 10.1007/s11682-017-9688-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Matejko, A. A., Ansari, D. (2019). The neural association between arithmetic and basic numerical processing depends on arithmetic problem size and not chronological age. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 37, 100653. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2019.100653

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Matejko, A. A., Ansari, D. (2021). Shared neural circuits for visuospatial working memory and arithmetic in children and adults. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 33, 1003–1019. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_01695

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Matelli, M., Luppino, G. (2001). Parietofrontal circuits for action and space perception in the macaque monkey. Neuroimage 14, S27–32. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0835

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

MATLAB (2021). Version 9.10.0.1602886 (R2021a). Natick, MA: The MathWorks Inc.

Google Scholar

McNab, F., Klingberg, T. (2008). Prefrontal cortex and basal ganglia control access to working memory. Nat. Neurosci. 11, 103–107. doi: 10.1038/nn2024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meier, K., Partanen, M., Giaschi, D. (2018). Neural correlates of speed-tuned motion perception in healthy adults. Perception 47, 660–683. doi: 10.1177/0301006618771463

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Menon, V. (2016). Memory and cognitive control circuits in mathematical cognition and learning. Prog. Brain Res. 227, 159–186. doi: 10.1016/bs.pbr.2016.04.026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Menon, V., Rivera, S. M., White, C. D., Glover, G. H., Reiss, A. L. (2000). Dissociating prefrontal and parietal cortex activation during arithmetic processing. NeuroImage 12, 357–365. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2000.0613

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Monge, Z. A., Wing, E. A., Stokes, J., Cabeza, R. (2018). Search and recovery of autobiographical and laboratory memories: shared and distinct neural components. Neuropsychologia 110, 44–54. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.07.030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morris, A. P., Chambers, C. D., Mattingley, J. B. (2007). Parietal stimulation destabilizes spatial updating across saccadic eye movements. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 104, 9069–9074. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0610508104

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nelwan, M., Friso-van den Bos, I., Vissers, C., Kroesbergen, E. (2022). The relation between working memory, number sense, and mathematics throughout primary education in children with and without mathematical difficulties. Child Neuropsychol. 28, 143–170. doi: 10.1080/09297049.2021.1959905

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Offen, S., Gardner, J. L., Schluppeck, D., Heeger, D. J. (2010). Differential roles for frontal eye fields (FEFs) and intraparietal sulcus (IPS) in visual working memory and visual attention. J. Vis. 10, 28–28. doi: 10.1167/10.11.28

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Owen, A. M., McMillan, K. M., Laird, A. R., Bullmore, E. (2005). N-back working memory paradigm: A meta-analysis of normative functional neuroimaging studies. Hum. Brain Mapp. 25, 46–59. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20131

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Papadopoulos, A., Sforazzini, F., Egan, G., Jamadar, S. (2018). Functional subdivisions within the human intraparietal sulcus are involved in visuospatial transformation in a non-context-dependent manner. Hum. Brain Mapp. 39, 354–368. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23847

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peng, P., Namkung, J., Barnes, M., Sun, C. (2016). A meta-analysis of mathematics and working memory: Moderating effects of working memory domain, type of mathematics skill, and sample characteristics. J. Educ. Psychol. 108, 455–473. doi: 10.1037/edu0000079

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Petersen, S. E., Posner, M. I. (2012). The attention system of the human brain: 20 years after. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 35, 73–89. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-062111-150525

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Piazza, M., Pinel, P., Le Bihan, D., Dehaene, S. (2007). A magnitude code common to numerosities and number symbols in human intraparietal cortex. Neuron 53, 293–305. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.11.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pinel, P., Dehaene, S. (2010). Beyond hemispheric dominance: brain regions underlying the joint lateralization of language and arithmetic to the left hemisphere. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 22, 48–66. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21184

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Podzebenko, K., Egan, G. F., Watson, J. D. G. (2005). Real and imaginary rotary motion processing: functional parcellation of the human parietal lobe revealed by fMRI. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 17, 24–36. doi: 10.1162/0898929052879996

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Poldrack, R. A., Mumford, J. A., Schonberg, T., Kalar, D., Barman, B., Yarkoni, T. (2012). Discovering relations between mind, brain, and mental disorders using topic mapping. PLoS Comput. Biol. 8, e1002707. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1002707

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Posner, M. L., Dehaene, S. (1994). Attentional networks. Trends Neurosci. 17, 75–79. doi: 10.1016/0166-2236(94)90078-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Power, J. D., Cohen, A. L., Nelson, S. M., Wig, G. S., Barnes, K. A., Church, J. A., et al. (2011). Functional network organization of the human brain. Neuron 72, 665–678. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.09.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Praß, M., de Haan, B. (2019). Multi-target attention and visual short-term memory capacity are closely linked in the intraparietal sulcus. Hum. Brain Mapp. 40, 3589–3605. doi: 10.1002/hbm.24618

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Price, G. R., Yeo, D. J., Wilkey, E. D., Cutting, L. E. (2018). Prospective relations between resting-state connectivity of parietal subdivisions and arithmetic competence. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 30, 280–290. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2017.02.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raghubar, K. P., Barnes, M. A., Hecht, S. A. (2010). Working memory and mathematics: a review of developmental, individual difference, and cognitive approaches. Learn. Individ. Differ. 20, 110–122. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2009.10.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Richter, M., Amunts, K., Mohlberg, H., Bludau, S., Eickhoff, S. B., Zilles, K., et al. (2019). Cytoarchitectonic segregation of human posterior intraparietal and adjacent parieto-occipital sulcus and its relation to visuomotor and cognitive functions. Cereb. Cortex 29, 1305–1327. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhy245

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roell, M., Cachia, A., Matejko, A. A., Houdé, O., Ansari, D., Borst, G. (2021). Sulcation of the intraparietal sulcus is related to symbolic but not non-symbolic number skills. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 51, 100998. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2021.100998

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rorden, C., Brett, M. (2000). Stereotaxic display of brain lesions. Behav. Neurol. 12, 191–200. doi: 10.1155/2000/421719

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rosen, M. L., Stern, C. E., Devaney, K. J., Somers, D. C. (2018). Cortical and subcortical contributions to long-term memory-guided visuospatial attention. Cereb. Cortex 28, 2935–2947. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhx172

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rosen, M. L., Stern, C. E., Michalka, S. W., Devaney, K. J., Somers, D. C. (2015). Influences of long-term memory-guided attention and stimulus-guided attention on visuospatial representations within human intraparietal sulcus. J. Neurosci. 35, 11358–11363. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1055-15.2015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rottschy, C., Langner, R., Dogan, I., Reetz, K., Laird, A. R., Schulz, J. B., et al. (2012). Modelling neural correlates of working memory: a coordinate-based meta-analysis. NeuroImage 60, 830–846. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.11.050

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Salillas, E., Basso, D., Baldi, M., Semenza, C., Vecchi, T. (2009). Motion on numbers: transcranial magnetic stimulation on the ventral intraparietal sulcus alters both numerical and motion processes. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 21, 2129–2138. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2008.21157

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Salo, T., Yarkoni, T., Nichols, T. E., Poline, J. B. K., James, D., Gorgolewski, K. J., et al. (2021). neurostuff/NiMARE: 0.0.12 2022. doi: 10.5281/zenodo.6885551

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scheperjans, F., Eickhoff, S. B., Homke, L., Mohlberg, H., Hermann, K., Amunts, K., et al. (2008a). Probabilistic maps, morphometry, and variability of cytoarchitectonic areas in the human superior parietal cortex. Cereb. Cortex 18, 2141–2157. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhm241

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scheperjans, F., Hermann, K., Eickhoff, S. B., Amunts, K., Schleicher, A., Zilles, K. (2008b). Observer-independent cytoarchitectonic mapping of the human superior parietal cortex. Cereb. Cortex 18, 846–867. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhm116

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schultz, J., Bülthoff, H. H. (2019). Perceiving animacy purely from visual motion cues involves intraparietal sulcus. NeuroImage 197, 120–132. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.04.058

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seeley, W. W., Menon, V., Schatzberg, A. F., Keller, J., Glover, G. H., Kenna, H., et al. (2007). Dissociable intrinsic connectivity networks for salience processing and executive control. J. Neurosci. 27, 2349–2356. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5587-06.2007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sheremata, S. L., Somers, D. C., Shomstein, S. (2018). Visual short-term memory activity in parietal lobe reflects cognitive processes beyond attentional selection. J. Neurosci. 38, 1511–1519. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1716-17.2017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shikata, E., Hamzei, F., Glauche, V., Knab, R., Dettmers, C., Weiller, C., et al. (2001). Surface orientation discrimination activates caudal and anterior intraparietal sulcus in humans: an event-related fMRI study. J. Neurophysiol. 85, 1309–1314. doi: 10.1152/jn.2001.85.3.1309

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shikata, E., Hamzei, F., Glauche, V., Koch, M., Weiller, C., Binkofski, F., et al. (2003). Functional properties and interaction of the anterior and posterior intraparietal areas in humans: Properties of AIP and CIP. Eur. J. Neurosci. 17, 1105–1110. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.2003.02540.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Silk, T. J., Bellgrove, M. A., Wrafter, P., Mattingley, J. B., Cunnington, R. (2010). Spatial working memory and spatial attention rely on common neural processes in the intraparietal sulcus. NeuroImage 53, 718–724. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.06.068

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spreng, R. N., Stevens, W. D., Chamberlain, J. P., Gilmore, A. W., Schacter, D. L. (2010). Default network activity, coupled with the frontoparietal control network, supports goal-directed cognition. NeuroImage 53, 303–317. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.06.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Suo, X., Ding, H., Li, X., Zhang, Y., Liang, M., Zhang, Y., et al. (2021). Anatomical and functional coupling between the dorsal and ventral attention networks. NeuroImage 232, 117868. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2021.117868

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Swanson, H. L., Jerman, O. (2006). Math disabilities: a selective meta-analysis of the literature. Rev. Educ. Res. 76, 249–274. doi: 10.3102/00346543076002249

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Szinte, M., Knapen, T. (2020). Visual organization of the default network. Cereb. Cortex 30, 3518–3527. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhz323

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tark, K.-J., Curtis, C. E. (2013). Deciding where to look based on visual, auditory, and semantic information. Brain Res. 1525, 26–38. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2013.06.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Uddin, L. Q., Supekar, K., Amin, H., Rykhlevskaia, E., Nguyen, D. A., Greicius, M. D., et al. (2010). Dissociable connectivity within human angular gyrus and intraparietal sulcus: evidence from functional and structural connectivity. Cereb. Cortex 20, 2636–2646. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhq011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Venkatraman, V., Ansari, D., Chee, M. W. L. (2005). Neural correlates of symbolic and non-symbolic arithmetic. Neuropsychologia 43, 744–753. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.08.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Villarreal, M., Fridman, E. A., Amengual, A., Falasco, G., Gerscovich, E. R., Ulloa, E. R., et al. (2008). The neural substrate of gesture recognition. Neuropsychologia 46, 2371–2382. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.03.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vincent, J. L., Kahn, I., Snyder, A. Z., Raichle, M. E., Buckner, R. L. (2008). Evidence for a frontoparietal control system revealed by intrinsic functional connectivity. J. Neurophysiol. 100, 3328–3342. doi: 10.1152/jn.90355.2008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vogel, S. E., Goffin, C., Bohnenberger, J., Koschutnig, K., Reishofer, G., Grabner, R. H., et al. (2017). The left intraparietal sulcus adapts to symbolic number in both the visual and auditory modalities: evidence from fMRI. NeuroImage 153, 16–27. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.03.048

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vossel, S., Geng, J. J., Fink, G. R. (2014). Dorsal and ventral attention systems: distinct neural circuits but collaborative roles. The Neuroscientist 20, 150–159. doi: 10.1177/1073858413494269

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wagner, A. D., Shannon, B. J., Kahn, I., Buckner, R. L. (2005). Parietal lobe contributions to episodic memory retrieval. Trends Cogn. Sci. 9, 445–53. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.07.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weiss, M. M., Wolbers, T., Peller, M., Witt, K., Marshall, L., Buchel, C., et al. (2009). Rotated alphanumeric characters do not automatically activate frontoparietal areas subserving mental rotation. NeuroImage 44, 1063–1073. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.09.042

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Welchman, A. E. (2016). The human brain in depth: how we see in 3D. Annu. Rev. Vis. Sci. 2, 345–376. doi: 10.1146/annurev-vision-111815-114605

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wu, S. S., Chang, T. T., Majid, A., Caspers, S., Eickhoff, S. B., Menon, V. (2009). Functional heterogeneity of inferior parietal cortex during mathematical cognition assessed with cytoarchitectonic probability maps. Cereb. Cortex 19, 2930–2945. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhp063

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Xu, Y. (2009). Distinctive neural mechanisms supporting visual object individuation and identification. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 21, 511–518. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2008.21024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Xu, Y., Chun, M. M. (2006). Dissociable neural mechanisms supporting visual short-term memory for objects. Nature 440, 91–95. doi: 10.1038/nature04262

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Xu, Y., Jeong, S. K. (2015). “The contribution of human superior intraparietal sulcus to visual short-term memory and perception,” in Mechanisms of Sensory Working Memory, eds P. Jolicoeur, C. Lefebvre, and J. Martinez-Trujillo (San Diego, CA: Academic Press), 33–42. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-12-801371-7.00004-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yarkoni, T. (2018). Neurosynth - Topic-based meta-analyses. Available online at: https://neurosynth.org/analyses/topics/ (accessed April 5, 2022).

Google Scholar

Yarkoni, T., Poldrack, R. A., Nichols, T. E., Van Essen, D. C., Wager, T. D. (2011). Large-scale automated synthesis of human functional neuroimaging data. Nat. Methods 8, 665–670. doi: 10.1038/nmeth.1635

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yeo, B. T., Krienen, F. M., Sepulcre, J., Sabuncu, M. R., Lashkari, D., Hollinshead, M., et al. (2011). The organization of the human cerebral cortex estimated by intrinsic functional connectivity. J. Neurophysiol. 106, 1125–1165. doi: 10.1152/jn.00338.2011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yeo, D. J., Wilkey, E. D., Price, G. R. (2017). The search for the number form area: a functional neuroimaging meta-analysis. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 78, 145–160. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.04.027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yonelinas, A. P. (2005). Separating the brain regions involved in recollection and familiarity in recognition memory. J. Neurosci. 25, 3002–3008. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5295-04.2005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yuan, Q. (2021). Patterns and networks of language control in bilingual language production. Brain Struct. Funct. 226, 963–977. doi: 10.1007/s00429-021-02218-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, H., Chang, L., Chen, X., Ma, L., Zhou, R. (2018). Working memory updating training improves mathematics performance in middle school students with learning difficulties. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 12, 154. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2018.00154

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zlatkina, V., Petrides, M. (2014). Morphological patterns of the intraparietal sulcus and the anterior intermediate parietal sulcus of Jensen in the human brain. Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 281, 20141493. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2014.1493

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: resting state functional connectivity, intraparietal sulcus, systems-level decoding, neuroinformatics, Neurosynth

Citation: Boeken OJ and Markett S (2023) Systems-level decoding reveals the cognitive and behavioral profile of the human intraparietal sulcus. Front. Neuroimaging 1:1074674. doi: 10.3389/fnimg.2022.1074674

Received: 19 October 2022; Accepted: 19 December 2022;
Published: 09 January 2023.

Edited by:

Qi Chen, South China Normal University, China

Reviewed by:

Mingming Zhang, Liaoning Normal University, China
Dixiu Liu, Gannan Medical University, China

Copyright © 2023 Boeken and Markett. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Ole Jonas Boeken, yes b2xlam9uYXMuYm9la2VuJiN4MDAwNDA7Z21haWwuY29t

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.