Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Neuroanat., 12 January 2018
This article is part of the Research Topic Neuroscience in Africa View all 36 articles

Reductions in Corpus Callosum Volume Partially Mediate Effects of Prenatal Alcohol Exposure on IQ

  • 1Department of Human Biology, Faculty of Health Sciences, University of Cape Town, Cape Town, South Africa
  • 2Department of Psychiatry and Mental Health, Faculty of Health Sciences, University of Cape Town, Cape Town, South Africa
  • 3Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Neurosciences, Wayne State University School of Medicine, Detroit, MI, United States
  • 4MRC/UCT Medical Imaging Research Unit, Division of Biomedical Engineering, Department of Human Biology, Faculty of Health Sciences, University of Cape Town, Cape Town, South Africa

Disproportionate volume reductions in the basal ganglia, corpus callosum (CC) and hippocampus have been reported in children with prenatal alcohol exposure (PAE). However, few studies have investigated these reductions in high prevalence communities, such as the Western Cape Province of South Africa, and only one study made use of manual tracing, the gold standard of volumetric analysis. The present study examined the effects of PAE on subcortical neuroanatomy using manual tracing and the relation of volumetric reductions in these regions to IQ and performance on the California Verbal Learning Test-Children's Version (CVLT-C), a list learning task sensitive to PAE. High-resolution T1-weighted images were acquired, using a sequence optimized for morphometric neuroanatomical analysis, on a Siemens 3T Allegra MRI scanner from 71 right-handed, 9- to 11-year-old children [9 fetal alcohol syndrome (FAS), 19 partial FAS (PFAS), 24 non-syndromal heavily exposed (HE) and 19 non-exposed controls]. Frequency of maternal drinking was ascertained prospectively during pregnancy using timeline follow-back interviews. PAE was examined in relation to volumes of the CC and left and right caudate nuclei, nucleus accumbens and hippocampi. All structures were manually traced using Multitracer. Higher levels of PAE were associated with reductions in CC volume after adjustment for TIV. Although the effect of PAE on CC was confounded with smoking and lead exposure, additional analyses showed that it was not accounted for by these exposures. Amongst dysmorphic children, smaller CC was associated with poorer IQ and CVLT-C scores and statistically mediated the effect of PAE on IQ. In addition, higher levels of PAE were associated with bilateral volume reductions in caudate nuclei and hippocampi, effects that remained significant after control for TIV, child sex and age, socioeconomic status, maternal smoking during pregnancy, and childhood lead exposure. These data confirm previous findings showing that PAE is associated with decreases in subcortical volumes and is the first study to show that decreases in callosal volume may play a role in fetal alcohol-related impairment in cognitive function seen in childhood.

Introduction

Prenatal alcohol exposure (PAE) is associated with a range of neurocognitive and behavioral problems. Fetal alcohol spectrum disorders (FASD) is an umbrella term under which the entire spectrum of outcomes related to PAE falls. Fetal alcohol syndrome (FAS), the most severe of the FASD, is characterized by growth deficits, small head circumference, distinctive craniofacial dysmorphology (small palpebral fissures, flattened philtrum, and thin vermillion) and cognitive problems that relate to specific brain abnormalities (Jones and Smith, 1973; Hoyme et al., 2005). Partial FAS (PFAS) is diagnosed in individuals exhibiting the facial dysmorphology characteristic of FAS, whose mothers are known to have drunk heavily during pregnancy and who exhibit growth deficits, small head circumference or neurobehavioral impairment (Hoyme et al., 2005). Adverse effects on brain development range from microstructural, neurochemical and cellular dysfunction (such as widespread apoptotic neurodegeneration that can result in the death of millions of cells in the forebrain during gestation) to gross structural abnormalities (Ikonomidou, 2000; Olney, 2004; Spadoni et al., 2007; Lebel et al., 2008).

In the U.S. and in Western countries, FAS occurs at a rate estimated to be between 0.05 and 7 per 1,000 live births (May and Gossage, 2001; May et al., 2009). In contrast, certain high-risk communities in South Africa have been found to have an FAS prevalence rating that is 18–148 times greater than in the U.S. (May et al., 2000; Viljoen et al., 2005; Urban et al., 2008). Within the Western Cape province of South Africa, rates are highest amongst the Colored population of mixed Asian, African and European ancestry (May et al., 2000, 2005, 2013; Viljoen et al., 2005).

Magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) allows for non-invasive and quantitative investigation of the structural and functional brain alterations that are observed in FASD. Brain regions that have been consistently implicated in FASD include the corpus callosum (CC), cerebellum, parietal lobe, frontal lobe, temporal lobe, caudate nucleus, hippocampus and increased volume of lateral ventricles (see review papers, Lebel et al., 2011; Donald et al., 2015). While most regions show alcohol-related volume reductions, findings in certain subcortical brain regions have not always been consistent. For example, some studies report bilateral decreases in hippocampal volume (Willoughby et al., 2008; Astley et al., 2009; Coles et al., 2011; Nardelli et al., 2011; Treit et al., 2013) with PAE, while others find relative sparing (Archibald et al., 2001), or even volume increases (Riikonen et al., 2007). Methodological differences, such as automated vs. manual segmentation, failure or different strategies to control for total intracranial volume (TIV), small sample sizes, or large age ranges may explain some of these differences. Notably, one study found increased hippocampal volume relative to brain volume with age (9–15 years) in controls, but not in children with FASD (Willoughby et al., 2008), highlighting the potential contribution of age-related changes in brain development when comparing diagnostic groups and the need for narrow age ranges. It is not yet known to what extent developmental trajectories in children prenatally exposed to alcohol are altered.

Although manual tracing is considered the gold standard for volumetric analysis, yielding the greatest consistency and sensitivity (Morey et al., 2009), few studies employ this method due to high time demands (Archibald et al., 2001; Riikonen et al., 2007; Willoughby et al., 2008; Astley et al., 2009). Here we use manual tracing of study participants within a narrow age range (10.4 ± 0.4 years) to examine the effects of PAE on volumes of select subcortical structures and the CC in 81 children from the Cape Town Longitudinal Cohort (Jacobson et al., 2008), for whom diagnostic and detailed prospective prenatal alcohol and drug exposure data are available. Most previous studies have only been able to examine volumetric differences between diagnostic groups and not dose-dependent effects, since the amount of alcohol used during pregnancy is difficult to recall reliably in retrospective case-controlled studies (Jacobson et al., 2002).

Due to the labor intensive nature of manual tracing, only subcortical structures previously implicated in FASD were investigated (Archibald et al., 2001; Morey et al., 2009; Nardelli et al., 2011). These include the caudate nucleus and hippocampus, which are involved in cognition, memory and emotional networks (Devinsky and D'Esposito, 2004) that are amongst the most affected domains in FASD (Uecker and Nadel, 1996; Mattson et al., 1998; Willoughby et al., 2008; Green et al., 2009; Jacobson et al., 2011; Lewis et al., 2016). Although findings in the nucleus accumbens have rarely been reported in FASD (Archibald et al., 2001), we report it here because the manual tracing protocol we used for the caudate initially involved tracing both structures together. The CC was included as it is clearly visible on MRIs, is easy to delineate, and has been consistently shown to be reduced in FASD (Mattson et al., 1992; Sowell et al., 2001, 2008; Bookstein et al., 2002; Autti-Rämö et al., 2007; Lebel et al., 2008; Astley et al., 2009), even in newborns (Jacobson et al., 2017). PAE has been linked to a larger angle of the splenium (Bookstein et al., 2007), reductions in CC thickness and area in the splenium and anterior third (Yang et al., 2012), and CC volume reductions in the splenium, genu and area just anterior to the splenium (Riley et al., 1995).

In addition, we examined whether alcohol-related regional volumetric changes mediate the effect of PAE on cognitive performance on the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Fourth Edition (WISC-IV) and the California Verbal Learning Test-Children's Version (CVLT-C). Poorer IQ is frequently seen in FASD (Streissguth et al., 1990, 1991; Jacobson et al., 2004) as is impaired performance on the CVLT-C (Delis et al., 1994), a list learning task (Kerns et al., 1997; Mattson et al., 1998; Crocker et al., 2011; Vaurio et al., 2011; Suttie et al., 2013; Lewis et al., 2015).

We hypothesized that increasing PAE would be associated with volumetric reductions in all regions examined and that these effects would remain significant after controlling for reductions in total brain volume. We also hypothesized that volumetric reductions in hippocampus, caudate nucleus and CC would be associated with poorer WISC IQ and CVLT-C performance and that the effects of PAE on these outcomes would be partially mediated by volumetric changes in the regions that were traced.

Methods

Participants

From 1998 to 2002 pregnant women were recruited into the Cape Town Longitudinal Cohort (Jacobson et al., 2008) from an antenatal clinic in a Cape Colored community in Cape Town known to have a high prevalence of alcohol consumption (Croxford and Viljoen, 1999). Detailed alcohol exposure data collected prospectively during pregnancy were available for all of these children. Mothers were interviewed at enrollment and at two subsequent visits using the timeline follow-back (TLFB) approach to record alcohol consumption during pregnancy (Jacobson et al., 2002). We adapted the interview to include information about the type of beverage consumed, container size (including pictures of different containers, bottles, cans, glass size) and sharing (size of container divided by number of women drinking together) to reflect how pregnant women in this community tend to drink. The mother was asked about her drinking on a day-by-day basis during a typical 2-week period around the time of conception, with recall linked to specific times of day and activities. If her drinking had changed since conception, she was also asked about her drinking during the past 2 weeks and when her drinking had changed. At the two follow-up visits, the mother was again asked about her drinking during the previous 2 weeks. Volume was recorded for each type of alcohol beverage consumed each day and converted to oz of absolute alcohol (AA) using multipliers proposed by Bowman et al. (1975) (liquor—0.4, beer—0.05, wine—0.12, cider—0.06). Six summary measures were constructed—average oz AA per day at conception, AA per day averaged across pregnancy, AA per drinking day (quantity per occasion) at conception and across pregnancy, and number of drinking days per week (frequency) at conception and across pregnancy.

Any woman who reported drinking at least 1.0 oz AA per day (≈2 standard drinks per day) or at least 2 instances of binge drinking (≥5 drinks per occasion) during the first trimester was invited to participate. Women initiating antenatal care who reported no binge drinking and minimal alcohol use (<0.5 oz AA/ day) were invited to participate as controls. In this sample all but one of the mothers in the control group reported abstaining from drinking during pregnancy; the single control mother who drank consumed only 2 drinks/occasion on 2–3 days during the pregnancy. Smoking was recorded as the number of cigarettes smoked per day. Participants were excluded if they were <18 years of age, presented with chronic medical problems including epilepsy, diabetes, HIV, or cardiac problems. Infant exclusionary criteria were major chromosomal anomalies, neural tube defects, multiple births, and seizures.

In 2005, we organized a clinic in which the children in the study were each independently examined by two expert dysmorphologists (E.H. Hoyme, M.D., and L.K. Robinson, M.D.) for growth and FAS related dysmorphic features using a standard protocol (Hoyme et al., 2005) based on the Revised Institute of Medicine criteria (Jacobson et al., 2008). Case conferences were held in which the dysmorphologists, SWJ, JLJ, and CDM determined which of the children met criteria for FAS or PFAS. Children who did not meet criteria for FAS of PFAS were designated as either non-syndromal heavily exposed (HE) or controls, depending on the maternal alcohol history. Five children who could not attend the 2005 clinic were examined by another expert FASD dysmorphologist (N. Khaole, M.D.), whose diagnoses were all subsequently confirmed by examinations conducted in follow-up clinics we held with the same dysmorphologists in 2009 and with HEH in 2013 and 2016.

Magnetic Resonance Image (MRI) Acquisition

Magnetic resonance image (MRI) scans were acquired at the Cape Universities Brain Imaging Centre (CUBIC) using a 3T Siemens Allegra MRI scanner (Siemens Medical Systems, Erlangen, Germany) for 81 right-handed children from this cohort at age 9–11 years. High-resolution T1-weighted images were obtained using a volumetric navigated (Tisdall et al., 2012) multi-echo magnetization prepared rapid gradient echo (MEMPRAGE) sequence optimized for morphometric neuroanatomical analysis using FreeSurfer software (van der Kouwe et al., 2008). Imaging parameters were: FOV 256 × 256 mm, 128 sagittal slices, TR 2,530 mm, TE 1.53/3.21/4.89/6.57 mm, TI 1,100 ms, flip angle 7°, voxel size 1.3 × 1.0 × 1.3 mm3, acquisition time 8:07 min. The volumetric navigator provides real-time motion tracking and correction to reduce artifacts resulting from motion.

Human subject participation was approved by the ethics committees of the University of Cape Town Faculty of Health Sciences and Wayne State University. Mothers provided written informed consent and children, oral assent. Examiners were blind with respect to PAE history and FASD diagnosis, except in the most severe cases of FAS. Total intracranial volumes (TIV) were obtained from FreeSurfer (Dale et al., 1999; Fischl et al., 2002). Called eTIV or ICV, within FreeSurfer, the programme calculates TIV by using an atlas scaling factor that it obtains from registering the images from the sample to an average template and using that scaling factor to estimate each participant's TIV (Buckner et al., 2004, see also http://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu/fswiki/eTIV).

Manual Tracing Protocol

The CC and left and right caudate nuclei, nucleus accumbens and hippocampi were manually traced on the MR images using Multitracer (Woods, 2003) software on a tablet laptop (Lenovo ThinkPad X200, Wacom digitizer pen enabled). A single blinded investigator performed all manual tracings (SCB). Tracing was supervised and reviewed by CW, the senior neuroanatomist at the University of Cape Town. All structures were traced in the coronal plane, with the exception of the CC that was traced in the sagittal plane. Orthogonal planes were used to inform border delineation. “Frust” volumes were recorded for each structure, which assumes that a contour applies to the center of the slice on which it is drawn and that the square root of the cross-sectional area changes linearly across the slice thickness (Woods, 2003).

The CC (Figure 1A) is clearly visible as a curved WM structure near the midline slice. The superior border is the cingulate gyrus and the CSF (cerebrospinal fluid) of the lateral and posterior fissure form the inferior border. The midline slices (about slices 127–130) were determined by locating the smallest area of the thalamus. Bookstein et al. (2002) used the term “midline” to denote the slices located most medially in the CC (i.e., along the midline of the brain). The CC was traced on two contiguous midline slices. These were averaged and volume calculated.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. MR image slices of a non-exposed control child showing manual tracings of the (A) corpus callosum, (B) left and right caudate nuclei, (C) left and right nucleus accumbens and (D) left hippocampus. L and R denote left and right, respectively.

The caudate nucleus (Figure 1B) is a gray matter (GM) structure that lies on the lateral wall of the lateral ventricle and is traced until it disappears. The medial border is the lateral ventricle and the lateral border is formed by WM. The inferior border is the nucleus accumbens (Figure 1C), which can only be differentiated histologically by cytoarchitecture, however, it can be removed from the caudate nucleus reliably by drawing a straight line from the inferior border of the lateral ventricle to the bottom-most border of the internal capsule. Laterally, it is separated from the putamen by a line descending from the internal capsule. It was traced until the anterior commissure bridges the midline.

The anterior border of the hippocampus (Figure 1D) is the inner surface of the alveus. Tracing proceeds past the crura of the fornices until hippocampal GM disappears. The medial border includes the subiculum to the supramedial border of WM in the temporal lobe. The lateral border is the inner surface of the alveus within the temporal horn of the lateral ventricle. Inferiorly, the boundary is the interface between the WM and GM.

Three participants (2 HE; 1 control) were excluded due to pathology (unrelated to the research question) and 7 (1 FAS; 2 HE; 4 control) due to compromised image quality. Pathology included one scan where there were hypointense inclusions on the superior surface of the right lateral ventricle (indicative of a potentially conflicting diagnosis), and two scans where the lateral ventricles were occluded, making delineation of caudate nuclei and nucleus accumbens problematic.

Statistical Analyses

Analysis of variance (ANOVA) was performed for each region of interest (ROI) to examine whether structural volumes differed between diagnostic groups. Pearson correlation was used to examine the relations of ROI volumes to each of the potential confounders. Potential control variables included TIV, child's sex and age (years) at scan, socioeconomic status (SES; Hollingshead, 2011), maternal smoking during pregnancy (cigarettes/day), and postnatal lead exposure (ug/dl) obtained from a blood sample at 5 years. ANCOVA was used to examine whether volumetric group differences remained significant after controlling for confounders even weakly (p < 0.10) related to the outcome. Although marijuana (days/week) and cocaine (days/week) were considered, only 5 mothers in this sample reported marijuana use and 1 cocaine. Any observed between-group differences were, therefore, rerun excluding children born to these mothers to see whether the effects persisted without them.

The association between continuous measures of PAE (both at conception and across pregnancy) and structural volumes were examined using Pearson correlation. Hierarchical multiple regression analyses were used to control for confounders. The alcohol measure was entered in the first step of each analysis for each outcome. Total intracranial volume (TIV) was then added to the regressions for the regional volumes to determine if the exposure affected the regional volume over and above its effect on total brain volume. Control variables related to the outcome at less than p < 0.10 were entered in the third step to determine if the effect of the prenatal alcohol measure on structure volume continued to be significant after statistical adjustment for these potential confounders. Observed associations with extent of alcohol exposure were rerun excluding children born to mothers who used marijuana or cocaine during pregnancy to see whether effects remained without them.

Pearson correlations were used to examine whether alcohol-related volumetric changes were associated with performance on the WISC-IV IQ and CVLT-C, two outcomes known to be related to PAE. Mediation of effects of PAE on these cognitive outcomes by structural volumes associated with PAE was examined using multiple regression. The effect of PAE on the cognitive outcome was entered in Step 1 of the regression; the structure volume, in Step 2. Mediation was inferred if the effect of PAE was significantly reduced when the structure volume was entered in Step 2, based on the Clogg test (Clogg et al., 1992). Table 1 shows a list of abbreviations used.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. List of abbreviations used.

Results

Following the exclusions described above, we report data for 71 children. Demographic characteristics are presented in Table 2. Although the FASD diagnostic groups did not differ by sex, prenatal exposure to cigarettes, marijuana, or cocaine, or childhood lead exposure, children in the HE group were slightly older than those in the FAS and PFAS groups. As expected, children in both the FAS and PFAS groups had lower IQs than the HE children; those in the PFAS group also had lower scores than the controls. Total brain volumes were smaller in the children with FAS compared to all other groups. SES was lower for mothers of children with FAS or PFAS than mothers of children from the HE and control groups.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Sample characteristics (N = 71).

As expected, all of the alcohol measures for the control group were statistically lower compared to the alcohol-using groups (Table 2). In addition, children with FAS were more exposed than HE children on all measures. They were also exposed to more alcohol/drinking day at conception than those in the PFAS group. At conception and across pregnancy, AA/day and drinking days/week for the PFAS group were also significantly higher than for the HE group. Although number of days of alcohol use was reduced from 1.5 (at conception) to 1.1 (across pregnancy) day/week on average [t(68) = 3.88, p < 0.001), the number of drinks/occasion remained virtually unchanged, [t(69) = −0.33, p = 0.741]. Moreover, alcohol consumption at conception was highly correlated with alcohol use throughout pregnancy (r = 0.923). Due to the small sample size of the FAS group, the two syndromal groups, FAS and PFAS, were combined for subsequent volumetric analyses.

Volumes of traced regions are reported for each diagnostic group in Table 3 and associations of control variables with structural volumes in Table 4. Children in the FAS/PFAS group had smaller right hippocampi compared to both the HE and control children, but this effect was no longer significant after adjustment for TIV. Volumes of four regions were smaller in girls than in boys. Among the remaining potential confounders, smoking during pregnancy and postnatal lead exposure were related to size in one region—the CC.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Comparison of structure volumes by diagnostic group.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Correlations of control variables with structure volumes.

Table 5 presents the results of the multiple regression analyses examining the relation of continuous measures of PAE to the structural volumes. PAE was associated with volume reductions bilaterally in the caudate and hippocampus and smaller CC, but the effect on the CC was no longer significant after controlling for smoking during pregnancy and postnatal lead exposure. When the regression for CC was rerun controlling only for lead, the effect of absolute alcohol/day remained significant (β = −0.281, p = 0.022). In a regression of the effect of alcohol and smoking on CC volume, the effects of both of these exposures fell just short of significance (alcohol β = −0.236, p = 0.065; smoking β = −0.217, p = 0.089), suggesting that neither effect was attributable to the effect of the other exposure.

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Association of alcohol consumption measures with structure volumes.

Prenatal alcohol exposure (PAE) was associated with lower IQ (r = −0.402; p = 0.001) and CVLT-C (r = −0.248; p = 0.045) scores. Larger CC was associated with higher IQ for the sample as a whole and with better CVLT-C performance for the FAS/PFAS group (Table 6). Unexpectedly, smaller hippocampi were associated with better CVLT-C scores for the FAS/PFAS group.

TABLE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Table 6. Associations of structure volumes with IQ and CVLT-C scores.

The hypothesis that CC size would mediate the effect of PAE on IQ was tested using multiple regression. PAE was entered in Step 1. When CC size was added in Step 2 of the regression analysis, the effect of AA/day on IQ was reduced from −0.40 to −0.33, a reduction that was statistically significant, Clogg (t = −2.86, p < 0.01). These data thus indicate that the effect of PAE on IQ was partially mediated by the alcohol-related reduction in CC size (Figure 2).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Path model showing partial mediation of the association between AA/day and IQ by corpus callosum (CC) volume. The figure shows that the effect of prenatal alcohol exposure on IQ is partially mediated by the fetal alcohol-related corpus callosum volume reduction. When CC size was added in Step 2 of the regression analysis, the effect of AA/day on IQ was reduced from −0.40 to −0.33, a reduction that was statistically significant, Clogg (t = −2.86, p < 0.01).

Discussion

This study shows that increasing PAE, both in terms of quantity and frequency, is associated with disproportionate volume reductions bilaterally in the caudate nuclei and hippocampi and smaller CC. By contrast, volume differences between FASD diagnostic groups were evident only in the right hippocampus. Smaller CC was associated with poorer IQ. Within the FAS/PFAS group, smaller CC was also associated with lower CVLT-C scores, but hippocampal volume reductions were associated with better CVLT-C performance. The results from our mediation analysis suggest that the adverse effect of PAE on IQ is partially attributable to the reductions in CC size.

When the regional volumetric measures were compared across the three diagnostic groups, no significant differences were seen after adjustment for TIV. By contrast, the continuous measures of PAE were associated with volumetric differences in all of the regions examined after adjustment for brain size, except the bilateral NA. These findings are consistent with other neuroimaging studies performed with children from the same Cape Town cohort, which found that continuous measures of PAE were more sensitive than diagnosis to fetal alcohol-related alterations in brain structure and function (De Guio et al., 2014; Meintjes et al., 2014; du Plessis et al., 2015).

Our finding of alcohol-related volumetric reductions bilaterally in the caudate nuclei are consistent with studies reporting decreased basal ganglia volume in FAS (Mattson et al., 1994, 1996, 1999, 2001; Archibald et al., 2001; Roussotte et al., 2012). Animal studies suggest that the physiological mechanisms underlying smaller striatal volumes may be explained by a reduction in dendritic tree formation rather than a reduction in number of parvalbumin interneurons (De Giorgio et al., 2012). The caudate nucleus is involved in many of the neurobehavioural deficits exhibited by children prenatally exposed to alcohol, including executive function domains, such as cognitive flexibility, concept formation and reasoning, planning and response inhibition (Mattson et al., 1999, 2001). This region has also been shown to be activated less by children with FASD during behavioral inhibition tasks (Fryer et al., 2007) and to mediate cognitive control and verbal learning in children with PAE (Fryer et al., 2012). Although the latter study reported that larger caudate nuclei were associated with better performance in verbal learning in alcohol-exposed children, we did not confirm that finding in our study.

The caudate nuclei may be more susceptible to the effects of PAE than the NA. It has been suggested that some midline brain structures are more affected by alcohol exposure during embryological development than others (Sulik et al., 1981; Zhou et al., 2003; Meintjes et al., 2014). Our results are consistent with those of Archibald et al. (2001), who also failed to find alcohol-related volume reductions in the NA using a similar tracing methodology. Because the NA can only truly be differentiated from the caudate nucleus histologically, the lack of association in both these studies may be due to poor sensitivity by these tracing methods to differentiate the NA from the caudate nuclei. Thus, although our results suggest that the NA may be relatively spared in PAE, further investigation using more sensitive methods, such as diffusion tensor imaging or higher resolution structural imaging, is needed.

Prenatal alcohol exposure (PAE) was also associated with volumetric reductions in both the left and right hippocampi–effects that remained significant after control for TIV. Reductions in pyramidal and granule cell number and density were observed in rats exposed prenatally to alcohol (Livy et al., 2003), which suggests that this may be the case in humans. The hippocampal formation has been shown to be particularly sensitive to PAE, showing a diverse range of negative effects and is one of the few areas of the brain which continues to exhibit these deficits as it develops into adulthood (see review, Gil-Mohapel et al., 2010). Associations of smaller hippocampi with better memory performance in typically-developing young adults (Van Petten et al., 2004) suggest a neural pruning process with age that may alter the relation of hippocampal volume in healthy controls as they grow older. Although our findings for the caudate nuclei, NA and CC are consistent with those of Archibald et al. (2001), who used a similar tracing protocol to ours, fetal alcohol-related differences in hippocampal volumes in their study did not survive after controlling for TIV. This disparity in results may be due to the wide range of ages included in that study (7–24 years) compared to the more limited range in ours (9–11 years). The observed association of smaller hippocampal volume with better learning and memory performance in the FAS/PFAS group in the current study was not expected at this age and is difficult to interpret.

Prenatal alcohol exposure (PAE) was associated with smaller CC after adjustment for TIV and postnatal lead exposure. However, regression analyses showed that the effects of smoking and alcohol on CC volume were confounded in this sample, and both fell slightly short of conventional levels of statistical significance when included in the same analysis. The effects of maternal smoking on the child's brain are not well understood, but decreases in CC size and changes in microstructure of other frontal WM structures have been linked with maternal smoking during pregnancy (Jacobsen et al., 2007; Jacobson et al., 2008; Paus et al., 2008). Given that the PAE and maternal smoking effects were similar in magnitude when adjusted for each other in the regression analysis, neither effect appears to be attributable to the effect of the other exposure, and a larger sample would likely have shown their effects to be independent.

The hypothesis that volumetric reductions in the caudate nuclei and hippocampi would mediate effects of PAE on cognitive performance was not supported. Volumetric reductions in the caudate nuclei did not relate to the cognitive outcomes; and, contrary to expectation, the hippocampal volumes in the FAS/PFAS group were inversely related to learning and memory performance.

In this study, larger CC area was associated with higher IQ scores and, amongst children in the FAS/PFAS group, with better learning and memory performance. Mediation analysis indicated that the effect of PAE on IQ was partially mediated by reductions in CC size. Our findings are consistent with several studies that have suggested that WM is more susceptible to the teratogenic effects of PAE than cortical GM (Archibald et al., 2001; Lebel et al., 2008; Spottiswoode et al., 2011). It is of interest that microstructural changes in two regions of the CC have also been found to mediate effects of PAE on IQ (Fan et al., 2016).

Morphology of the CC has been shown to have functional effects, with a thinner midline being associated with poorer motor activity and thicker than average midline being associated with poorer executive function (Bookstein et al., 2002). The CC is particularly important for functional outcomes requiring interhemispheric transfer of information, and it has been shown that even moderate exposure to alcohol may influence this process (Roebuck et al., 2002; Dodge et al., 2009). Since cognitive function assessed on IQ tests depends on a broad range of cognitive domains (Wechsler, 2003; Willoughby et al., 2008) utilizing a range of GM areas that communicate via axons in the CC, poorer interhemispheric transfer could contribute to lower IQ (Dodge et al., 2009). Smaller CC size may be due to either poorer myelination or fewer axons in the CC, resulting in slower signal processing or communication with greater numbers of GM cell bodies, respectively. CC volume reductions may also be due to changes to CC projection neurons, as reductions in size, number and location of these cells were observed in rats prenatally exposed to alcohol (Livy and Elberger, 2008). However, as no one animal model exhibits all the features of FASD (Cudd, 2005), further investigation into the underlying physiological mechanisms in humans is needed to better understand the links between CC and behavioral outcomes. As such, it is not surprising that the most heavily exposed children, those in the FAS/PFAS group, had the smallest CC areas and the lowest IQ scores.

One potential limitation of this study that is common to other longitudinal PAE studies is that it relies on the mother's report to assess alcohol consumption. However, we have previously validated the TLFB interview used in this study in relation to levels of fatty acid ethyl ester metabolites in meconium samples obtained from newborns in this cohort (Bearer et al., 2003). In addition, this TLFB interview has been shown to predict a broad range of behavioral (Jacobson et al., 2002, 2008; Lewis et al., 2015; Lindinger et al., 2016; Fortin et al., 2017) and neuroimaging outcomes (De Guio et al., 2014; Meintjes et al., 2014; du Plessis et al., 2015; Jacobson et al., 2017). A second limitation relates to the difficulty assessing effects of timing of exposure in a human study. Animal models have demonstrated that timing of exposure is a major determinant of specific longterm neurobehavioral outcomes, since different brain regions are susceptible to PAE during unique sensitive periods in their development (Livy et al., 2003; Valenzuela et al., 2012; Sadrian et al., 2014). Unfortunately, multicollinearity between maternal alcohol use at conception and later in pregnancy in this cohort, as in most human samples, was too high to discriminate effects attributable to timing of exposure. Polysubstance abuse is another common limitation in PAE studies. The current study assessed potential confounding by prenatal cocaine, marijuana and smoking exposure in order to address this problem; none of these exposures were related to the outcomes examined. Strengths of this study include the use of a sample size that was large relative to other manual tracing studies, limited age range, a population that is geographically and culturally homogenous albeit diverse in terms of genetic ancestry, and the availability of continuous measures of prospectively collected alcohol consumption.

In conclusion, this study, which used manual tracing, confirms previous reports linking PAE to decreases in subcortical volumes in the caudate nucleus and hippocampus after adjustment for TIV and also showed that PAE is associated with a disproportionate reduction in CC size. This is the first study to provide evidence that fetal alcohol-related reductions in CC partially mediate effects of PAE on IQ. This finding adds to the growing body of evidence suggesting that WM may be particularly vulnerable to the teratogenic effects of PAE on development of the fetal brain.

Ethics Statement

Ethical approval for the study was acquired from the University of Cape Town Faculty of Health Sciences Human Research Ethics Committee and the Wayne State University Institutional Review Board. Informed written consent was obtained from the mothers and oral assent from the children in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki.

Author Contributions

SB performed the tracing of the volumes under the supervision of CW and with the assistance of SR. She also reviewed the literature, performed the data analyses, contributed to the interpretation and wrote up the findings. EM provided overall project supervision, collaborated on the design of the neuroimaging study, the data analysis, interpretation and write-up of the findings. SJ and JJ designed the study, supervised recruitment and maternal interviews and child assessments, collaborated on the data analysis, the interpretation of the findings and write-up of the paper. CM administered the maternal interviews, which included sociodemographic information and alcohol, smoking and drug ascertainment. NL and CL performed neuropsychological assessments.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

This study was supported by NIH/NIAAA grants R01AA016781 and U01AA014790; South African Research Chairs Initiative of the Department of Science and Technology and National Research Foundation of South Africa; Medical Research Council of South Africa; and the Lycaki-Young Fund, State of Michigan. We thank H. Eugene Hoyme, M.D., Luther K. Robinson, M.D., and Nathaniel Khaole, M.D., who conducted the Cape Town dysmorphology examinations in 2005 in conjunction with the NIAAA Collaborative Initiative on FASD and Dr. Hoyme and the other dysmorphologists who participated in the 2009, 2013, and 2016 clinics. We thank R. Colin Carter, M.D./M.M.Sc. for his consultation, the CUBIC radiographers Marie-Louise de Villiers and Nailah Maroof, and our University of Cape Town and Wayne State University research staff Nicolette Hamman, Mariska Pienaar, Maggie September, Emma Makin, Renee Sun, and Neil Dodge. We also thank the parents and children for their long-term participation in and contribution to the study.

References

Archibald, S. L., Fennema-Notestine, C., Gamst, A., Riley, E. P., Mattson, S. N., and Jernigan, T. L. (2001). Brain dysmorphology in individuals with severe prenatal alcohol exposure. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 43, 148–154. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8749.2001.tb00179.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Astley, S. J., Aylward, E. H., Olson, H. C., Kerns, K., Brooks, A., Coggins, T. E., et al. (2009). Magnetic resonance imaging outcomes from a comprehensive magnetic resonance study of children with fetal alcohol spectrum disorders. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 33, 1671–1689. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2009.01004.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Autti-Rämö, I., Autti, T., Korkman, M., Kettunen, S., Salonen, O., and Valanne, L. (2007). MRI findings in children with school problems who had been exposed prenatally to alcohol. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 44, 98–106. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8749.2002.tb00294.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bearer, C. F., Jacobson, J. L., Jacobson, S. W., Barr, D., Croxford, J., Molteno, C. D., et al. (2003). Validation of a new biomarker of fetal exposure to alcohol. J. Pediatr. 143, 463–469. doi: 10.1067/S0022-3476(03)00442-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bookstein, F. L., Connor, P. D., Huggins, J. E., Barr, H. M., Pimentel, K. D., and Streissguth, A. P. (2007). Many infants prenatally exposed to high levels of alcohol show one particular anomaly of the corpus callosum. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 31, 868–879. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2007.00367.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bookstein, F. L., Streissguth, A. P., Sampson, P. D., Connor, P. D., and Barr, H. M. (2002). Corpus callosum shape and neuropsychological deficits in adult males with heavy fetal alcohol exposure. Neuroimage 15, 233–251. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0977

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bowman, R. S., Stein, L. I., and Newton, J. R. (1975). Measurement and interpretation of drinking behavior. I. On measuring patterns of alcohol consumption. II. Relationships between drinking behavior and social adjustment in a sample of problem drinkers. J. Stud. Alcohol 36, 1154–1172. doi: 10.15288/jsa.1975.36.1154

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buckner, R. L., Head, D., Parker, J., Fotenos, A. F., Marcus, D., Morris, J. C., et al. (2004). A unified approach for morphometric and functional data analysis in young, old, and demented adults using automated atlas-based head size normalization: reliability and validation against manual measurement of total intracranial volume. Neuroimage 23, 724–738. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.06.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clogg, C. C., Petkova, E., and Shihadeh, E. S. (1992). Statistical methods for analyzing collapsibility in regression models. J. Educ. Stat. 17, 51–74. doi: 10.2307/1165079

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coles, C. D., Goldstein, F. C., Lynch, M. E., Chen, X., Kable, J. A., Johnson, K. C., et al. (2011). Memory and brain volume in adults prenatally exposed to alcohol. Brain Cogn. 75, 67–77. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2010.08.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crocker, N., Vaurio, L., Riley, E. P., and Mattson, S. N. (2011). Comparison of verbal learning and memory in children with heavy prenatal alcohol exposure or attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 35, 1114–1121. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2011.01444.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Croxford, J., and Viljoen, D. (1999). Alcohol consumption by pregnant women in the Western Cape. S. Afr. Med. J. 89, 962–965.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Cudd, T. A. (2005). Animal model systems for the study of alcohol teratology. Exp. Biol. Med. 230, 389–393. doi: 10.1177/15353702-0323006-06

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dale, A. M., Fischl, B., and Sereno, M. I. (1999). Cortical surface-based analysis. Neuroimage 9, 179–194. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1998.0395

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Giorgio, A., Comparini, S. E., Intra, F. S., and Granato, A. (2012). Long-term alterations of striatal parvalbumin interneurons in a rat model of early exposure to alcohol. J. Neurodev. Disord. 4:18. doi: 10.1186/1866-1955-4-18

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Guio, F., Mangin, J.-F., Rivière, D., Perrot, M., Molteno, C. D., Jacobson, S. W., et al. (2014). A study of cortical morphology in children with fetal alcohol spectrum disorders. Hum. Brain Mapp. 35, 2285–2296. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22327

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Delis, D. C., Kaplan, E., Kramer, J., and Ober, B. (1994). California Verbal Learning Test-Children's Version. New York, NY: Psychological Corporation.

PubMed Abstract

Devinsky, O., and D'Esposito, M. (2004). Neurology of Cognitive and Behavioral Disorders. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Dodge, N. C., Jacobson, J. L., Molteno, C. D., Meintjes, E. M., Bangalore, S., Diwadkar, V., et al. (2009). Prenatal alcohol exposure and interhemispheric transfer of tactile information: detroit and cape town findings. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 33, 1628–1637. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2009.00994.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Donald, K. A., Eastman, E., Howells, F. M., Adnams, C., Riley, E. P., Woods, R. P., et al. (2015). Neuroimaging effects of prenatal alcohol exposure on the developing human brain: a magnetic resonance imaging review. Acta Neuropsychiatr. 27, 251–269. doi: 10.1017/neu.2015.12

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

du Plessis, L., Jacobson, S. W., Molteno, C. D., Robertson, F. C., Peterson, B. S., Jacobson, J. L., et al. (2015). Neural correlates of cerebellar-mediated timing during finger tapping in children with fetal alcohol spectrum disorders. Neuroimage Clin. 7, 562–570. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2014.12.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fan, J., Jacobson, S. W., Taylor, P. A., Molteno, C. D., Dodge, N. C., Stanton, M. E., et al. (2016). White matter deficits mediate effects of prenatal alcohol exposure on cognitive development in childhood. Hum. Brain Mapp. 37, 2943–2958. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23218

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fischl, B., Salat, D. H., Busa, E., Albert, M., Dieterich, M., Haselgrove, C., et al. (2002). Whole brain segmentation: automated labeling of neuroanatomical structures in the human brain. Neuron 33, 341–355. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(02)00569-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fortin, M., Muckle, G., Jacobson, S. W., Jacobson, J. L., and Bélanger, R. E. (2017). Alcohol use among Inuit pregnant women: validity of alcohol ascertainment measures over time. Neurotoxicol. Teratol. 64, 73–78. doi: 10.1016/j.ntt.2017.10.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fryer, S. L., Mattson, S. N., Jernigan, T. L., Archibald, S. L., Jones, K. L., and Riley, E. P. (2012). Caudate volume predicts neurocognitive performance in youth with heavy prenatal alcohol exposure. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 36, 1932–1941. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2012.01811.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fryer, S. L., Tapert, S. F., Mattson, S. N., Paulus, M. P., Spadoni, A. D., and Riley, E. P. (2007). Prenatal alcohol exposure affects frontal-striatal BOLD response during inhibitory control. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 31, 1415–1424. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2007.00443.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gil-Mohapel, J., Boehme, F., Kainer, L., and Christie, B. R. (2010). Hippocampal cell loss and neurogenesis after fetal alcohol exposure: insights from different rodent models. Brain Res. Rev. 64, 283–303. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2010.04.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Green, C. R., Mihic, A. M., Nikkel, S. M., Stade, B. C., Rasmussen, C., Munoz, D. P., et al. (2009). Executive function deficits in children with fetal alcohol spectrum disorders (FASD) measured using the Cambridge Neuropsychological Tests Automated Battery (CANTAB). J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 50, 688–697. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2008.01990.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hollingshead, A. B. (2011). Four factor index of social status. Yale. J. Sociol. 8, 21–51.

Google Scholar

Hoyme, H. E., May, P. A., Kalberg, W. O., Kodituwakku, P., Gossage, J. P., Trujillo, P. M., et al. (2005). A practical clinical approach to diagnosis of fetal alcohol spectrum disorders: clarification of the 1996 institute of medicine criteria. Pediatrics 115, 39–47. doi: 10.1542/peds.2004-0259

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ikonomidou, C. (2000). Ethanol-induced apoptotic neurodegeneration and fetal alcohol syndrome. Science 287, 1056–1060. doi: 10.1126/science.287.5455.1056

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jacobsen, L. K., Picciotto, M. R., Heath, C. J., Frost, S. J., Tsou, K. A., Dwan, R. A., et al. (2007). Prenatal and adolescent exposure to tobacco smoke modulates the development of white matter microstructure. J. Neurosci. 27, 13491–13498. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2402-07.2007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jacobson, S. W., Chiodo, L. M., Sokol, R. J., and Jacobson, J. L. (2002). Validity of maternal report of prenatal alcohol, cocaine, and smoking in relation to neurobehavioral outcome. Pediatrics 109, 815–825. doi: 10.1542/peds.109.5.815

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jacobson, S. W., Jacobson, J. L., Molteno, C. D., Warton, C. M. R., Wintermark, P., Hoyme, H. E., et al. (2017). Heavy prenatal alcohol exposure is related to smaller corpus callosum in newborn MRI scans. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 41, 965–975. doi: 10.1111/acer.13363

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jacobson, S. W., Jacobson, J. L., Sokol, R. J., Chiodo, L. M., and Corobana, R. (2004). Maternal age, alcohol abuse history, and quality of parenting as moderators of the effects of prenatal alcohol exposure on 7. 5-year intellectual function. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 28, 1732–1745. doi: 10.1097/01.ALC.0000145691.81233.FA

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jacobson, S. W., Jacobson, J. L., Stanton, M. E., Meintjes, E. M., and Molteno, C. D. (2011). Biobehavioral markers of adverse effect in fetal alcohol spectrum disorders. Neuropsychol. Rev. 21, 148–166. doi: 10.1007/s11065-011-9169-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jacobson, S. W., Stanton, M. E., Molteno, C. D., Burden, M. J., Fuller, D. S., Hoyme, H. E., et al. (2008). Impaired eyeblink conditioning in children with fetal alcohol syndrome. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 32, 365–372. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2007.00585.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jones, K. L., and Smith, D. W. (1973). Recognition of the fetal alcohol syndrome in early infancy. Lancet 2, 999–1001. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(73)91092-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kerns, K. A., Don, A., Mateer, C. A., and Streissguth, A. P. (1997). Cognitive deficits in nonretarded adults with fetal alcohol syndrome. J. Learn. Disabil. 30, 685–693. doi: 10.1177/002221949703000612

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lebel, C., Rasmussen, C., Wyper, K., Walker, L., Andrew, G., Yager, J., et al. (2008). Brain diffusion abnormalities in children with fetal alcohol spectrum disorder. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 32, 1732–1740. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2008.00750.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lebel, C., Roussotte, F., and Sowell, E. R. (2011). Imaging the impact of prenatal alcohol exposure on the structure of the developing human brain. Neuropsychol. Rev. 21, 102–118. doi: 10.1007/s11065-011-9163-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lewis, C. E., Thomas, K. G., Dodge, N. C., Molteno, C. D., Meintjes, E. M., Jacobson, J. L., et al. (2015). Verbal learning and memory impairment in children with fetal alcohol spectrum disorders. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 39, 724–732. doi: 10.1111/acer.12671

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lewis, C. E., Thomas, K. G. F., Molteno, C. D., Kliegel, M., Meintjes, E. M., Jacobson, J. L., et al. (2016). Prospective memory impairment in children with prenatal alcohol exposure. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 40, 969–978. doi: 10.1111/acer.13045

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lindinger, N. M., Malcolm-Smith, S., Dodge, N. C., Molteno, C. D., Thomas, K. G. F., Meintjes, E. M., et al. (2016). Theory of mind in children with fetal alcohol spectrum disorders. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 40, 367–376. doi: 10.1111/acer.12961

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Livy, D. J., and Elberger, A. J. (2008). Alcohol exposure during the first two trimesters-equivalent alters the development of corpus callosum projection neurons in the rat. Alcohol 42, 285–293. doi: 10.1016/j.alcohol.2008.04.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Livy, D. J., Miller, E. K., Maier, S. E., and West, J. R. (2003). Fetal alcohol exposure and temporal vulnerability: effects of binge-like alcohol exposure on the developing rat hippocampus. Neurotoxicol. Teratol. 25, 447–458. doi: 10.1016/S0892-0362(03)00030-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mattson, S. N., Goodman, A. M., Caine, C., Delis, D. C., and Riley, E. P. (1999). Executive functioning in children with heavy prenatal alcohol exposure. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 23, 1808–1815. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.1999.tb04077.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mattson, S. N., Riley, E. P., and Diego, S. (1998). A review of the neurobehavioral deficits in children with fetal alcohol syndrome or prenatal exposure to alcohol. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 22, 279–294. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.1998.tb03651.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mattson, S. N., Riley, E. P., Jernigan, T. L., Ehlers, C. L., Delis, D. C., Jones, K. L., et al. (1992). Fetal alcohol syndrome: a case report of neuropsychological, MRI and EEG assessment of two children. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 16, 1001–1003. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.1992.tb01909.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mattson, S. N., Riley, E. P., Jernigan, T. L., Garcia, A., Kaneko, W. M., Ehlers, C. L., et al. (1994). A decrease in the size of the basal ganglia following prenatal alcohol exposure: a preliminary report. Neurotoxicol. Teratol. 16, 283–289. doi: 10.1016/0892-0362(94)90050-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mattson, S. N., Riley, E. P., Sowell, E. R., Jernigan, T. L., Sobel, D. F., and Jones, K. L. (1996). A decrease in the size of the basal ganglia in children with fetal alcohol syndrome. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 20, 1088–1093. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.1996.tb01951.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mattson, S. N., Schoenfeld, A. M., and Riley, E. P. (2001). Teratogenic effects of alcohol on brain and behavior. Alcohol Res. Health 25, 185–191.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

May, P. A., Blankenship, J., Marais, A.-S., Gossage, J. P., Kalberg, W. O., Joubert, B., et al. (2013). Maternal alcohol consumption producing fetal alcohol spectrum disorders (FASD): quantity, frequency, and timing of drinking. Drug Alcohol Depend. 133, 502–512. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2013.07.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

May, P. A., Brooke, L., Gossage, J. P., Croxford, J., Adnams, C., Jones, K. L., et al. (2000). Epidemiology of fetal alcohol syndrome in a South African community in the Western Cape Province. Am. J. Public Health 90, 1905–1912. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.90.12.1905

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

May, P. A., and Gossage, J. P. (2001). Estimating the prevalence of fetal alcohol syndrome. a summary. Alcohol Res. Health 25, 159–167.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

May, P. A., Gossage, J. P., Brooke, L. E., Snell, C. L., Marais, A.-S., Hendricks, L. S., et al. (2005). Maternal risk factors for fetal alcohol syndrome in the Western cape province of South Africa: a population-based study. Am. J. Public Health 95, 1190–1199. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2003.037093

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

May, P. A., Gossage, J. P., Kalberg, W. O., Robinson, L. K., Buckley, D., Manning, M., et al. (2009). Prevalence and epidemiologic characteristics of FASD from various research methods with an emphasis on recent in-school studies. Dev. Disabil. Res. Rev. 15, 176–192. doi: 10.1002/ddrr.68

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meintjes, E. M., Narr, K. L., van der Kouwe, A. J., Molteno, C. D., Pirnia, T., Gutman, B., et al. (2014). A tensor-based morphometry analysis of regional differences in brain volume in relation to prenatal alcohol exposure. NeuroImage Clin. 5, 152–160. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2014.04.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morey, R. A., Petty, C. M., Xu, Y., Hayes, J. P., Wagner, H. R., Lewis, D. V., et al. (2009). A comparison of automated segmentation and manual tracing for quantifying hippocampal and amygdala volumes. Neuroimage 45, 855–866. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.12.033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nardelli, A., Lebel, C., Rasmussen, C., Andrew, G., and Beaulieu, C. (2011). Extensive deep gray matter volume reductions in children and adolescents with fetal alcohol spectrum disorders. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 35, 1404–1417. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2011.01476.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Olney, J. W. (2004). Fetal alcohol syndrome at the cellular level. Addict. Biol. 9, 137–149. doi: 10.1080/13556210410001717006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paus, T., Nawazkhan, I., Leonard, G., Perron, M., Pike, G. B., Pitiot, A., et al. (2008). Corpus callosum in adolescent offspring exposed prenatally to maternal cigarette smoking. Neuroimage 40, 435–441. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.10.066

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Riikonen, R., Salonen, I., Partanen, K., and Verho, S. (2007). Brain perfusion SPECT and MRI in foetal alcohol syndrome. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 41, 652–659. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8749.1999.tb00518.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Riley, E. P., Mattson, S. N., Sowell, E. R., Jernigan, T. L., Sobel, D. F., and Jones, K. L. (1995). Abnormalities of the corpus callosum in children prenatally exposed to alcohol. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 19, 1198–1202. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.1995.tb01600.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roebuck, T. M., Mattson, S. N., and Riley, E. P. (2002). Interhemispheric transfer in children with heavy prenatal alcohol exposure. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 26, 1863–1871. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2002.tb02494.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roussotte, F. F., Sulik, K. K., Mattson, S. N., Riley, E. P., Jones, K. L., Adnams, C. M., et al. (2012). Regional brain volume reductions relate to facial dysmorphology and neurocognitive function in fetal alcohol spectrum disorders. Hum. Brain Mapp. 33, 920–937. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21260

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sadrian, B., Lopez-Guzman, M., Wilson, D. A., and Saito, M. (2014). Distinct neurobehavioral dysfunction based on the timing of developmental binge-like alcohol exposure. Neuroscience 280, 204–219. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2014.09.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sowell, E. R., Johnson, A., Kan, E., Lu, L. H., Van Horn, J. D., Toga, A. W., et al. (2008). Mapping white matter integrity and neurobehavioral correlates in children with fetal alcohol spectrum disorders. J. Neurosci. 28, 1313–1319. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5067-07.2008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sowell, E. R., Mattson, S. N., and Thompson, P. M. (2001). Mapping callosal morphology and cognitive correlates: effects of heavy prenatal alcohol exposure. Neurology 57, 235–244. doi: 10.1212/WNL.57.2.235

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spadoni, A. D., McGee, C. L., Fryer, S. L., and Riley, E. P. (2007). Neuroimaging and fetal alcohol spectrum disorders. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 31, 239–245. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2006.09.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spottiswoode, B. S., Meintjes, E. M., Anderson, A. W., Molteno, C. D., Stanton, M. E., Dodge, N. C., et al. (2011). Diffusion tensor imaging of the cerebellum and eyeblink conditioning in fetal alcohol spectrum disorder. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 35, 2174–2183. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2011.01566.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Streissguth, A. P., Aase, J. M., Clarren, S. K., Randels, S. P., LaDue, R. A., and Smith, D. F. (1991). Fetal alcohol syndrome in adolescents and adults. JAMA 265, 1961–1967. doi: 10.1001/jama.1991.03460150065025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Streissguth, A. P., Barr, H. M., and Sampson, P. D. (1990). Moderate prenatal alcohol exposure: effects on child IQ and learning problems at age 7 1/2 years. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 14, 662–669. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.1990.tb01224.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sulik, K. K., Johnston, M. C., and Webb, M. A. (1981). Fetal alcohol syndrome: embryogenesis in a mouse model. Science 214, 936–938. doi: 10.1126/science.6795717

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Suttie, M., Foroud, T., Wetherill, L., Jacobson, J. L., Molteno, C. D., Meintjes, E. M., et al. (2013). Facial dysmorphism across the fetal alcohol spectrum. Pediatrics 131, e779–e788. doi: 10.1542/peds.2012-1371

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tisdall, M. D., Hess, A. T., Reuter, M., Meintjes, E. M., Fischl, B., and van der Kouwe, A. J. (2012). Volumetric navigators for prospective motion correction and selective reacquisition in neuroanatomical MRI. Magn. Reson. Med. 68, 389–399. doi: 10.1002/mrm.23228

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Treit, S., Lebel, C., Baugh, L., Rasmussen, C., Andrew, G., and Beaulieu, C. (2013). Longitudinal MRI reveals altered trajectory of brain development during childhood and adolescence in fetal alcohol spectrum disorders. J. Neurosci. 33, 10098–10109. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5004-12.2013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Uecker, A., and Nadel, L. (1996). Spatial locations gone awry: object and spatial memory deficits in children with fetal alcohol syndrome. Neuropsychologia 34, 209–223. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(95)00096-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Urban, M., Chersich, M., Fourie, L.-A., Chetty, C., Olivier, L., and Viljoen, D. (2008). Fetal alcohol syndrome among grade-one children in the Northern Cape Province: prevalence and risk factors. S. Afr. Med. J. 98, 877–882.

Google Scholar

Valenzuela, C. F., Morton, R. A., Diaz, M. R., and Topper, L. (2012). Does moderate drinking harm the fetal brain? Insights from animal models. Trends Neurosci. 35, 284–292. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2012.01.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van der Kouwe, A. J. W., Benner, T., Salat, D. H., and Fischl, B. (2008). Brain morphometry with multiecho MPRAGE. Neuroimage 40, 559–569. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.12.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Petten, C., Plante, E., Davidson, P. S., Kuo, T. Y., Bajuscak, L., and Glisky, E. L. (2004). Memory and executive function in older adults: relationships with temporal and prefrontal gray matter volumes and white matter hyperintensities. Neuropsychologia 42, 1313–1335. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.02.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vaurio, L., Riley, E. P., and Mattson, S. N. (2011). Neuropsychological comparison of children with heavy prenatal alcohol exposure and an IQ-matched comparison group. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 17, 463–473. doi: 10.1017/S1355617711000063

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Viljoen, D. L., Gossage, J. P., Brooke, L., Adnams, C. M., Jones, K. L., Robinson, L. K., et al. (2005). Fetal alcohol syndrome epidemiology in a South African community: a second study of a very high prevalence area. J. Stud. Alcohol 66, 593–604. doi: 10.15288/jsa.2005.66.593

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wechsler, D. (2003). WISC-IV Administration Manual. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.

Willoughby, K. A., Sheard, E. D., Nash, K., and Rovet, J. (2008). Effects of prenatal alcohol exposure on hippocampal volume, verbal learning, and verbal and spatial recall in late childhood. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 14, 1022–1033. doi: 10.1017/S1355617708081368

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Woods, R. P. (2003). Multitracer: a Java-based tool for anatomic delineation of grayscale volumetric images. Neuroimage 19, 1829–1834. doi: 10.1016/S1053-8119(03)00243-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yang, Y., Phillips, O. R., Kan, E., Sulik, K. K., Mattson, S. N., Riley, E. P., et al. (2012). Callosal thickness reductions relate to facial dysmorphology in fetal alcohol spectrum disorders. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 36, 798–806. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2011.01679.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhou, F. C., Sari, Y., Powrozek, T., Goodlett, C. R., and Li, T.-K. (2003). Moderate alcohol exposure compromises neural tube midline development in prenatal brain. Dev. Brain Res. 144, 43–55. doi: 10.1016/S0165-3806(03)00158-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: fetal alcohol spectrum disorders, MRI, subcortical volumes, corpus callosum, IQ

Citation: Biffen SC, Warton CMR, Lindinger NM, Randall SR, Lewis CE, Molteno CD, Jacobson JL, Jacobson SW and Meintjes EM (2018) Reductions in Corpus Callosum Volume Partially Mediate Effects of Prenatal Alcohol Exposure on IQ. Front. Neuroanat. 11:132. doi: 10.3389/fnana.2017.00132

Received: 28 July 2017; Accepted: 18 December 2017;
Published: 12 January 2018.

Edited by:

Nilesh Bhailalbhai Patel, University of Nairobi, Kenya

Reviewed by:

Corrado Romano, Unit of Pediatrics and Medical Genetics, IRCCS Associazione Oasi Maria Santissima, Italy
Lauren Jantzie, University of New Mexico, United States
Alberto Granato, Università Cattolica del Sacro Cuore, Italy

Copyright © 2018 Biffen, Warton, Lindinger, Randall, Lewis, Molteno, Jacobson, Jacobson and Meintjes. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Stevie C. Biffen, c3RldmllYmlmZmVuQGdtYWlsLmNvbQ==
Ernesta M. Meintjes, ZXJuZXN0YS5tZWludGplc0BnbWFpbC5jb20=

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.